Abstract
Although the neurocognitive mechanisms of non-affective language comprehension have been studied extensively, relatively less is known about how the emotional meaning of language is processed. In this study, electrophysiological responses to affectively positive, negative and neutral words, presented within non-constraining, neutral contexts, were evaluated under conditions of explicit evaluation of emotional content (Experiment 1) and passive reading (Experiment 2). In both experiments, a widely distributed Late Positivity was found to be larger to negative than to positive words (a ‘negativity bias’). In addition, in Experiment 2, a small, posterior N400 effect to negative and positive (relative to neutral) words was detected, with no differences found between N400 magnitudes to negative and positive words. Taken together, these results suggest that comprehending the emotional meaning of words following a neutral context requires an initial semantic analysis that is relatively more engaged for emotional than for non-emotional words, while a later, more extended, attention-modulated process distinguishes the specific emotional valence (positive versus negative) of words. Thus emotional processing networks within the brain appear to exert a continuous influence, evident at several stages, on the construction of the emotional meaning of language.
GENERAL INTRODUCTION
During language comprehension, the final representation of meaning that is generated is dependent on multiple neurocognitive processes that come into play at different time points. The meaning of each incoming word is initially integrated with that of its preceding context and with respect to pre-existing knowledge, and then, under some circumstances, additional cognitive processes are engaged in order to more fully evaluate the meaning constructed. However, the mechanisms underlying the construction of meaning of emotionally-evocative language are poorly understood. The present study used event-related potentials (ERPs) – a direct index of online neural activity – to examine the neurocognitive processes underlying the comprehension of emotional language, both when participants paid explicit attention to emotional content and under more passive reading conditions. Our focus was on the modulation by emotion of two ERP components – the N400 and the Late Positivity. Below we review these two components in relation to both the comprehension of language and emotional information.
Building up meaning during language comprehension
The N400 and semantic memory
It has been known since the early 1980s that there is a distinct neural measure that is sensitive to the semantic contextual relationship between an eliciting word and its preceding context: the N400 (Kutas and Hillyard 1980; Kutas and Hillyard 1984). The N400 is a centro-posteriorly distributed, negative-going waveform that starts at approximately 300 msec and peaks at approximately 400 msec after the onset of a critical word. The amplitude of this waveform is larger (more negative) to critical words that are semantically unassociated (versus associated) with preceding single words in semantic priming paradigms (Bentin et al 1985; Rugg 1985) and that are incongruous (versus congruous) with their preceding sentence contexts (Kutas and Hillyard 1980; Kutas and Hillyard 1984) or global discourse contexts (van Berkum et al 1999). In addition to being evoked by frank semantic anomalies (Kutas and Hillyard 1980), the amplitude of the N400 is larger to plausible words that are unpredictable (versus highly predictable) with respect to their preceding context (Kutas and Hillyard 1984), to implausible words that are categorically unrelated (versus categorically related) to a predicted word (Federmeier and Kutas 1999; Kutas and Hillyard 1984), and to words that are inconsistent (versus consistent) with our knowledge of what is commonly encountered in the world (Hagoort et al 2004; Kuperberg et al 2003). Taken together, this previous work suggests that the N400 reflects, at least in part, the process of directly mapping an evolving representation of meaning on to stored knowledge, whether this be stored associations about what we know to be true (Hagoort et al 2004), what types of events we think are likely to occur (Kuperberg et al 2003), or stored semantic associations (Van Petten 1993) and categorical and featural relationships (Federmeier and Kutas 1999) between individual words – a process that has been referred to, generally, as a semantic memory-based analysis (Kuperberg 2007; Kutas and Federmeier 2000; Kutas et al 2006).
Late Positivities in language processing
In addition to a semantic memory-based analysis, under certain conditions, additional cognitive processes may be engaged in a further effort to integrate and evaluate a word with respect to its preceding context. This additional analysis is often manifested by a centro-posteriorly distributed Late Positive component that onsets and peaks at a later time-point than the N400 and that can extend until approximately 800 or 900 msec after stimulus onset. The Late Positivity is evoked by words that violate the syntactic or thematic structure of their preceding context (the P600) (Hagoort 1993; Kuperberg 2007; Osterhout and Holcomb 1992), and is also seen in various other situations, such as during the comprehension of metaphor (Coulson and Van Petten 2002) and jokes (Coulson and Kutas 2001). In all of these cases, a continued analysis (or reanalysis) of the relationship between the eliciting stimulus and the context are required in order to recover a full and accurate representation of meaning.
The degree to which the Late Positivity reflects particular linguistic processes (Kuperberg 2007; Osterhout and Hagoort 1999), or more general processes (Coulson et al 1998; Kolk and Chwilla 2007) is debated. Late Positivities have often been viewed as part of the P300 family of ERP components that are modulated by the subjective probability of the eliciting stimuli (with more positive amplitudes with greater task-relevance) and index the degree to which a working model of the environment requires updating (Donchin and Coles 1988). Nonetheless, although the Late Positivity, like the P300, is modulated by task and, in some situations, by the probability of the eliciting stimulus, it can be present and modulated even during passive reading and listening, without additional task requirements, suggesting that the processes it reflects can be inherent to naturalistic online comprehension. In sum, although it is not clear whether the linguistic and non-linguistic Late Positivities index the same or overlapping cognitive processes, what is not controversial is that the Late Positivity reflects additional cognitive processing that continues after the N400 time window and that such processes are distinct from the semantic memory-based analysis reflected by the N400.
Building up and evaluating emotional meaning
Semantic memory and emotion
How representations of emotional meaning are stored within semantic memory remains poorly understood (Niedenthal 2007). Studies examining patterns of evaluative ratings of words have found evidence for a structure of emotion knowledge that includes dimensions of valence, arousal, and potency or dominance (Kissler et al 2006; Osgood et al 1957). Since valence and arousal appear to account for much of the variance in these ratings, a two-dimensional model of affective meaning has been largely adopted (Feldman 1995; Lang 1979; Russell 1980). These dimensions of word meaning also apply to non-linguistic representations, such as pictures and sounds (Bradley et al 2001; Bradley and Lang 2000), suggesting that they may be represented amodally within semantic memory. An additional source of evidence that emotion concepts are stored and structured within semantic memory comes from affective semantic priming studies in which the processing of target words with emotional meaning is facilitated by the presence of prime words of similar affective valence, and slowed by the presentation of words with a dissimilar valence (Bargh et al 1996; Fazio et al 1986), although the precise mechanisms by which this occurs are debated (Klinger et al 2000; Spruyt et al 2007).
Other studies have suggested that emotional information is represented in the brain in a distributed, ‘embodied’ manner—the meaning of emotional information is retrieved via a simulation of the emotionally-evocative stimulus or overall experience (Glenberg et al 2005; Niedenthal 2007). Evidence in support of this model comes from studies showing that mood and bodily states can ‘prime’ the comprehension (Glenberg et al 2005) and classification (Niedenthal et al 1999) of representations that are emotionally congruent with the body or mood state. For example, in one study, reading times of emotionally-laden sentences were faster when participants were induced to have mood-congruent (versus mood-incongruent) facial expressions without their awareness (Havas et al 2007), suggesting that such emotion knowledge can rapidly influence online language comprehension.
Despite this evidence for a structured organization of emotion knowledge that can interact with language comprehension, and the known sensitivity of the N400 to semantic memory-based processes, there have been relatively few studies examining the relationship between emotional meaning and the N400 ERP component. A number of studies have observed an N400 effect in response to words that are emotionally incongruous (versus congruous) with the emotional prosody of a preceding, spoken sentence (Schirmer et al 2002; Schirmer et al 2005) or with the prosody of how the word itself was spoken (Schirmer and Kotz 2003). In another study, subjects, having undergone a mood induction procedure, read short, highly constraining, emotionally-evocative stories that ended with a word that was either positive or negative in valence, or highly incongruous with the preceding story (Chung et al 1996). Participants in a negative mood state produced a more negative N400 to positive than to negative words, while the opposite pattern was observed in subjects in a positive mood. In addition, an overall facilitatory effect of positive mood state on general semantic retrieval has been found (Federmeier et al 2001), providing further evidence for an influence of mood state on semantic processing within the N400 time window. However, prior studies have not attempted to isolate emotional processing by comparing the modulation of the N400 by emotional and affectively neutral (but otherwise semantically congruous) words within a neutral context; thus, it is unclear whether previous findings of N400 effects to emotional words reflect unique aspects of affective semantic processing, or represent additional instances of N400 effects to more general semantic incongruity.
The Late Positivity and emotional processing
A Late Positivity is evoked in response to a variety of emotional (relative to non-emotional) stimuli including emotionally-evocative pictures (Johnson et al 1986; Palomba et al 1997; Schupp et al 2000), photographs of human faces with emotional expressions (Krolak-Salmon et al 2001; Schupp et al 2004b; Vanderploeg et al 1987), and words with affective meanings (Herbert et al 2006; Kanske and Kotz 2007; Kiehl et al 1999; Naumann et al 1992; Naumann et al 1997). Because the emotional Late Positivity is modulated by the degree of emotional similarity between the eliciting stimuli and its surrounding stimuli (Cacioppo et al 1994), as well as by the task-relevance of the stimuli (Carretie et al 1996; Naumann et al 1997; Schupp et al 2007a), it has often been considered part of the P300 family. Consistent with this notion is its modulation by the amount of attention allocated to the emotional content of the stimuli, which is, in turn, influenced by how arousing (and intrinsically attention-grabbing) the stimuli are (Schupp et al 2004a; Schupp et al 2007b). However, there is some evidence that the emotional Late Positivity represents a more specific index of emotional processing. Several studies have reported that the Late Positivity to negatively-valenced stimuli is larger than to positively-valenced stimuli (Bernat et al 2001; Ito et al 1998; Kanske and Kotz 2007; Kiehl et al 1999), even in situations in which attention to affective meanings is limited or absent (Bernat et al 2001), or when the arousing quality of the positive and negative stimuli are equated (Ito et al 1998).
This sensitivity of the Late Positivity to affective valence (negative vs. positive) has been demonstrated in experiments measuring responses to emotional words (Bernat et al 2001; Kanske and Kotz 2007; Kiehl et al 1999), faces (Schupp et al 2004b) and pictures (Ito et al 1998). It supports a more general hypothesis about affective processing that emerged initially from findings in social psychology: that negatively-valenced information gives rise to more complex representations and requires more cognitive processing than positively-valenced information, leading to a “negativity bias” (Dahl 2001; Ito et al 1998; Pratto and John 1991). This negativity bias has been defined as the increased output from the negative motivational system, relative to the positive one, given a comparable amount of motivational input (Ito et al 1998). It has been observed in numerous behavioral studies reporting longer response latencies to negative than to positive words (Dahl 2001; Graves et al 1981; Kiehl et al 1999; Pratto and John 1991; Strauss 1983). Some of these studies found that this bias is only present when subjects are explicitly attending to the emotional meaning of the words (Dahl 2001; White 1996), while others have found this effect even when subjects are performing non-affective operations such as lexical decisions or Stroop interference tasks (Pratto and John 1991; Strauss 1983; Wurm and Vakoch 1996).
As is the case with the Late Positivity evoked by affectively neutral language stimuli, it is unclear exactly what cognitive process(es) are reflected by the emotional Late Positivity. It has usually been interpreted as an allocation of attentional resources to the emotional stimulus and, consistent with its conceptualization as part of the P300 family, as reflecting a re-evaluation of the eliciting stimulus with respect to its surrounding context. Such a re-evaluation may be particularly important in processing emotional stimuli, given the importance of emotional responses for signaling changes in strategy during the pursuit of rewards and avoidance of harm (Mathews and MacLeod 1994; Williams et al 1996). It has been proposed that, in general, negatively-valenced stimuli are prioritized over positively-valenced stimuli because, in a short time frame, it is more important to avoid harmful events, such as injury or death, than to pursue pleasurable ones (Taylor 1991).
The present study
In the present study, we used ERPs to elucidate the neurocognitive mechanisms mediating the processing of emotional information during the comprehension of short, two-sentence scenarios. Participants read pairs of sentences in which negative, positive and neutral words were preceded by a neutral, ambiguous and non-constraining context, e.g. “Sandra’s old boyfriend stopped by her apartment today. This time he brought a rose/gun/letter (positive, negative, neutral word, respectively) with him.” All of the sentences were plausible, and the critical word was not predictable in any of the three conditions. Two experiments were conducted: one in which participants made explicit valence categorization judgments (Experiment 1) and one in which ERPs were measured as participants processed affective information incidentally, passively reading the sentence pairs with occasional checks for comprehension (Experiment 2). In both experiments, ERPs were time-locked to the emotional or neutral words (e.g. rose/gun/letter – the critical word) within the second sentence.
Based on the evidence suggesting that emotion-related knowledge has a specific organization within the brain, such that emotionally-laden information is segregated from (or prioritized with respect to) non-emotional information (Bower 1981; Innes-Ker and Niedenthal 2002), we predicted that encountering an affectively valent word (versus a neutral word), following a neutral (and therefore affectively inconsistent) context, would lead to increased difficulty of semantic processing and an increased N400, even if the overall meaning was plausible.
Based on previous evidence (as described above) for Late Positivities elicited during the processing of neutral language and emotional stimuli across several modalities, we also predicted that, in addition to an N400 effect, the motivational significance of emotional words, within a non-constraining neutral context, would lead to additional prolonged processing, reflected by a Late Positivity effect (larger Late Positivity amplitudes for negative and positive words relative to neutral words).
We also aimed to determine whether the amplitude of the Late Positivity would be modulated by the type of affective valence of the critical word. Based on previous electrophysiological evidence (Bernat et al 2001; Kanske and Kotz 2007; Kiehl et al 1999), we hypothesized that we would find a more positive Late Positivity to negatively-valenced than positively-valenced words in context.
EXPERIMENT 1: VALENCE CATEGORIZATION TASK
METHODS
Participants
18 Tufts psychology undergraduate students participated (9 men; mean age = 22.4 ± 3.5). Individuals with histories of psychiatric or neurological disorders, who had learned languages other than English before age 5, or who were left-handed were excluded. Each participant provided written informed consent in accordance with the procedures of the Institutional Review Board of Tufts University. Prior to the ERP experiment, demographic information was collected about each participant and levels of trait anxiety was measured using the Speilberger State and Trait Anxiety Assessment Inventory (STAI) (Spielberger et al 1983).
Stimuli
Two-sentence descriptions of scenarios which described a situation involving one person or more (see Table 1 for examples) were generated for each of three experimental conditions. For each sentence pair, the first sentence (8–11 words long) was neutral and ambiguous in content, providing a non-constraining context for the second sentence. There was a neutral, positively-valenced, or negatively-valenced word (the critical word) in the second sentence. The second sentence was 6–9 words long, and the critical word was either the fourth, fifth or sixth word in the sentence and was never the sentence final word.
Table 1.
first sentence | second sentence (without the critical word) | neutral critical word | positive critical word | negative critical word |
---|---|---|---|---|
Nancy’s son ended up just like his father. | He was already a _____ by age 25. | husband | millionaire | criminal |
Stephen owned a lot of nineteenth century art. | Everyone knew that he _____ paintings of old masters. | bought | loved | forged |
An unfamiliar man rang Lenora’s doorbell one day. | He had come to _____ her. | register | congratulate | arrest |
Every day, the local newspaper wrote about the mayor. | They always said _____ things about him. | factual | marvelous | damaging |
Mr. Jenners planned to move his family to New York. | The reason for this was _____ to the children. | obvious | reassuring | hidden |
Cheryl’s baby cried when she took him to bed. | She quieted him with a _____ that night. | pacifier | lullaby | drug |
Pretests of Stimuli
Pretests were conducted in participants who did not participate in the ERP experiments in order to obtain more objective measures of whether the sentence-pairs were positive, negative or neutral in valence (24 raters), and to collect cloze probabilities (24 raters) and plausibility ratings (15 raters), as well as concreteness, valence and arousal measurements of the critical words (10–12 raters for each). Concreteness, valence, arousal, and plausibility ratings were made using 7-point Likert scales (7 = most concrete, most pleasant, most arousing and most likely). For cloze probability ratings, raters were presented with each scenario without the critical word and the words that followed it and were ask to produce the most likely next word. The cloze probability for each critical word reflected the proportion of times that the word was used to complete the sentence. For plausibility ratings, raters were presented with each sentence pair, ending with the critical word, and were asked to rate how likely it was that this situation would happen in real life.
Using the sentence pair average valence ratings, scenarios for which all three conditions met criteria for affective valence (positive condition: valence>5; negative condition: valence<3; neutral condition: valence ≥3 and ≤5) were selected. Within this subset, additional scenarios were eliminated so that the three conditions were matched with respect to the concreteness, number of letters and Kucera-Francis frequency of the critical word (see Table 2). This left a final stimulus set of 405 sentence pairs. As expected, the three conditions differed significantly with respect to the mean affective valence of the sentence pairs and critical words (positive > neutral > negative) (see Table 2). They also differed with respect to subjective arousal level (negative > positive > neutral) of the critical words, consistent with relative arousal levels of negative and positive words found within naturalistic language (Lewis et al 2007) and the increased slope of activation of the negative (relative to the positive) motivational system (Ito et al 1998). Overall, the critical words were unpredictable (with an overall average cloze probability of 2 %), but all the sentences were rated as plausible (with an overall average plausibility rating of 5.4). However, the three conditions differed slightly with respect to mean cloze probability (positive (3.5) > negative (1.4) and neutral (1.1)) and mean plausibility (negative (4.7) < positive (5.8) and neutral (5.6)). These differences among the conditions in arousal, cloze probability and plausibility likely reflect inherent, naturalistic aspects of emotional processing (see Discussion of Experiment 2), but to investigate their role, we conducted additional analyses, in addition to the main analyses, in which these different factors were matched across conditions by excluding a subset of the stimuli.1
Table 2.
Positive mean (SD) | Negative mean (SD) | Neutral mean (SD) | |
---|---|---|---|
Affect of the sentence pair* | 5.73 (0.4) | 2.11 (0.5) | 4.21 (0.5) |
Affect of the critical word* | 5.43 (0.8) | 2.19 (0.7) | 4.30 (0.7) |
Arousal of the critical word* | 4.26 (1.1) | 4.64 (0.9) | 3.37 (0.9) |
Concreteness of the critical word | 3.76 (1.3) | 3.85 (1.1) | 4.03 (1.1) |
Cloze probability percentage* | 3.52 (7.93) | 1.42 (5.12) | 1.14 (4.21) |
Plausibility* | 5.82 (1.56) | 4.72 (1.55) | 5.63 (1.12) |
Number of letters in the critical word | 7.30 (2.4) | 6.76 (2.3) | 7.27 (2.2) |
K-F Frequency of the critical word | 46.55 (101. 4) | 38.42 (99.6) | 54.44 (57.4) |
For the ERP experiments, the final stimuli set were divided into three lists, using a Latin Square design, which were counterbalanced between participants such that no participant encountered the same sentence pair more than once and such that, across subjects, all two-sentence scenarios were seen in all three conditions. Each of the three lists included 135 sentence pairs, with 45 sentence pairs for each condition.
Stimulus Presentation
Participants sat in a comfortable chair in a dimly lit room. Stimuli were presented to participants on a computer monitor, in white font, centered on a black background, and subtended at a visual angle of about 5°. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the six lists. All trials began with a white fixation cross (500 msec, interstimulus interval (ISI) 100 msec). Then the first sentence was presented as a whole (3.5 seconds, ISI 100 msec). The second sentence was presented one word at a time (500 msec, ISI 100 msec). A 750 msec blank-screen interval followed the final word of the second sentence, followed by the next trial. The subjects’ task was to press one of three buttons with their right thumb (button order was counterbalanced across subjects), depending on their judgment of whether the sentence pair depicted a pleasant, unpleasant or neutral person, place or situation.
Data Analysis
Behavioral data analysis
The percentage of participants’ responses which were consistent with the prior classifications of the three conditions (obtained during the pretests of the stimuli, see above) were compared across conditions and between the two groups using ANOVAs, and significant main effects and condition by group interactions were followed up by planned, paired Student’s t-tests (α = .05).
ERP data analyses
All analyses were conducted on the mean amplitudes of ERPs evoked by critical words (using a 100 msec prestimulus baseline), within selected time windows (see Results).
Repeated measures ANOVAs were performed on the midline, medial, lateral, and peripheral electrode columns (see Figure 1) as previously described (Kreher et al 2006; Kuperberg et al 2007; Kuperberg et al 2003). In all analyses, AP distribution (electrode position along the anterior-posterior plane) and Affect (positive, negative, and neutral) were entered as within-subjects factors. For the medial, lateral, and peripheral column analyses, Hemisphere (left, right) was an additional within-subjects factor. Overall ANOVAs were followed up with simple effects ANOVAs comparing each sentence type with one another, and follow-up t-tests at individual electrode sites. A Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied to all analyses with more than one degree of freedom in the numerator (Geisser and Greenhouse 1959). In these cases, we report the original degrees of freedom with the corrected p value.
RESULTS
The percentage of sentence pairs in which the participants’ valence classifications corresponded to those of the pretest conducted in a separate sample (see Methods), for the positive, negative and neutral sentence pairs was 90.7%, 94.9% and 77.9% (SDs: 5.5, 3.8, 13.39), respectively. A repeated measures ANOVA revealed a highly significant effect of Affect for the rate of correspondence between the participants’ classifications and those of the prior pretests (F( 2,34)= 20.53; p<1 × 10−6). Participants’ classifications of the negative sentence pairs were significantly more likely to correspond to those of the pretests than their classifications of the neutral (t(17)=4.96; p=.0001) and positive (t(17)=3.09; p=.006) sentence pairs, and participants’ classifications of the positive sentence pairs were significantly more likely to correspond to those of the pretests than their classifications of the neutral sentence pairs (t(17)= 4.18; p=.0006).
Grand-average ERPs elicited by the critical words in the three types of sentence pairs and maps of voltage differences between the conditions are shown in Figure 2. An N1-P2 complex can be seen in the first 300 msec after word onset. There was little evidence for modulation of this complex across the three conditions, as reflected by the absence of a main effect of Affect or Affect by AP distribution interaction in the first 300 msec (with analyses carried out at separate 0–100 msec, 100–200 msec, and 200–300 msec time windows). However, there was an Affect by Hemisphere interaction for the 0–100 msec time window for the negative versus positive contrast in the peripheral column only, which was due to a greater positivity in response to the negative words than the positive words at T5 (t(17) = 2.187, p = 0.043; ps >.12 for other electrodes in the peripheral columns).
These early components were followed by a negativity and then a large Late Positivity, which began at ~375 ms, peaking between 500 and 700 ms.2 Because of the dominance of the Late Positivity and its onset within the N400 time window, an N400 effect could not be evaluated statistically (see Figure 2A). For the Late Positivity, overall ANOVAs revealed significant main effects of Affect, and Affect by AP distribution interactions at all electrode columns (see Table 3). Planned, pair-wise simple effects ANOVAs also revealed significant main effects of Affect and Affect by AP distribution interactions, reflecting significantly larger Late Positivities to the negative relative to the positive critical words that were, in turn significantly larger than to the neutral critical words, particularly at posterior sites. There were no significant Affect by Hemisphere interactions, or Affect by Hemisphere by AP distribution interactions.
Table 3.
Main effect of Affect | Interactions between Affect and AP distribution | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
F (degrees of freedom) | P value | F (degrees of freedom) | P value | |
A. Overall effects | ||||
Midline | 17.093 (2, 34) | 1.29×10−5 | 8.386 (8, 136) | 4.00×10−5 |
Medial | 18.382 (2, 34) | 6.78×10−6 | 5.868 (4, 68) | 0.004 |
Lateral | 16.228 (2, 34) | 2.47×10−5 | 9.337 (6, 102) | 2.23 ×10−5 |
Peripheral | 13.868 (2, 34) | 6.88×10−5 | 8.685 (8, 136) | 4.16 ×10−4 |
B. Negative versus Neutral | ||||
Midline | 31.537 (1, 17) | 3.09×10−5 | 14.368 (4, 68) | 7.23 ×10−6 |
Medial | 31.631 (1, 17) | 3.04×10−5 | 9.963 (2, 34) | 0.003 |
Lateral | 25.073 (1, 17) | 1.08×10−4 | 15.298 (3, 51) | 1.05 ×10−5 |
Peripheral | 22.205 (1, 17) | 2.01×10−4 | 17.314 (4, 68) | 4.02×10−5 |
C. Positive versus Neutral | ||||
Midline | 6.669 (1, 17) | 0.019 | 6.441 (4, 68) | 0.003 |
Medial | 5.535 (1, 17) | 0.031 | 10.211 (2, 34) | 5.45×10−4 |
Lateral | 4.865 (1, 17) | 0.041 | 12.386 (3, 51) | 3.96 ×10−5 |
Peripheral | 5.191 (1, 17) | 0.036 | 12.402 (4, 68) | 6.87×10−4 |
D. Negative versus Positive | ||||
Midline | 12.479 (1, 17) | 0.003 | 4.899 (4, 68) | 0.022 |
Medial | 16.871 (1, 17) | 7.34×10−4 | 0.096 (2, 34) | 0.812 |
Lateral | 16.347 (1, 17) | 8.44×10−4 | 0.894 (3, 51) | 0.397 |
Peripheral | 11.718 (1, 17) | 0.003 | 1.589 (4, 68) | 0.225 |
The statistical results for the analyses of the three datasets that were matched for arousal, cloze probability and plausibility were the same as those of the original analyses, with the exception that for the plausibility matched analysis, the neutral versus positive contrast showed Affect by AP distribution interactions at all columns but no main effects of Affect. Grand average ERPs and voltage maps for these arousal, cloze and plausibility – matched analyses are shown in Figures S1, S2 and S3 in Supplementary Materials (see www.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/kuperberglab/materials.htm).
DISCUSSION
In this first experiment, specific modulation of the N400 by emotion could not be measured due to the presence of a large overlapping positivity which began at about 375 ms. This Late Positivity was found to be largest to the negative words, smaller to the positive words and smallest to the neutral words. Both the valence categorization task and the emotional content of the words likely contributed to this pattern of modulation, since each alone can elicit positivities. Although it is not possible to determine definitively whether the categorization task and emotional content influenced the same or distinct neural mechanism(s), studies examining the effects of varying attention on neurophysiological responses to emotional faces and scenes have found evidence for interactions between attention and emotion within the Late Positivity window (Vuilleumier and Pourtois 2007), suggesting that attention and emotional content engage the same or overlapping neural circuitry.
The fact that the relative magnitudes of the Late Positivities to the three conditions patterned with the differences among the conditions in mean arousal levels in the original dataset suggests a role for arousal in the emotional Late Positivity: the intrinsic motivational significance of affective material may be reflected in its capacity to elicit physiologic arousal, and explicit, top-down attention to emotional meaning may have further increased arousal levels. This explanation would also be consistent with a simulation-based comprehension mechanism (Glenberg et al 2005); a bodily state of arousal may reflect a rapid activation of limbic networks and the peripheral autonomic nervous system that occurs when the emotionally-salient event or object depicted by the word or text is re-experienced. However, previous studies have been contradictory with respect to this issue, with some support for an arousal modulation of the Late Positivity that is independent of valence (Schupp et al 2004a) and also for the reverse relationship (Ito et al 1998; Kanske and Kotz 2007). In the present study, differences in arousal between the positive and negative stimuli cannot fully explain the differences in modulation of the Late Positivity to these stimuli, as this affect-mediated difference in modulation of the Late Positivity was not altered when these two conditions were equated for arousal. Future studies which measure physiologic arousal directly, using skin conductance or pupillary responses, may help to clarify this issue further.
Our finding of a larger Late Positivity to negative than to positive words, even within the arousal, plausibility and cloze-matched datasets, suggests that additional processing resources are recruited to re-evaluate and integrate the meaning of negative words within a neutral context, relative to those required for positive words. This finding of a ‘negativity bias’ during the rapid online evaluation of emotional information is consistent with the results of previous work using non-linguistic stimuli (Ito et al 1998; Schupp et al 2004b) and single words (Bernat et al 2001; Kiehl et al 1999). In the current study, the negativity bias may have been additionally influenced by the preceding neutral context. The magnitude of the Late Positivity elicited by emotional stimuli has been found to correlate with its ‘affective distance’ from contextual stimuli, i.e. the difference between the affect ratings of the target and context (Cacioppo et al 1994). Thus a difference in affect or arousal between the neutral context and critical word that is greater for the negative relative to the positive critical words could produce a differential neural response, reflected in the Late Positivity. Alternatively, since the slope of the valence versus arousal function of the negative motivational system is steeper than that of the positive motivational system (Ito et al 1998), this bias may be present even when the affective distance from the context is equated for negative and positive stimuli. In the current study the affective content and arousal level of the context were not manipulated; the relative contributions to the emotional Late Positivity of the affective distance between context and target and the intrinsic emotional meaning of the stimuli can be explored further in follow-up experiments which systematically vary these two components.
Also, in the current study we focused on the neurophysiological effects of general affect categories (negative and positive) rather than on the effects of specific emotion categories within a general affect category (i.e., within the negative affect category: angry, sad, fearful, etc). The perception of specific emotion may be particularly influenced by contextual information (Barrett et al 2007); thus, it will be important in future work to determine whether there is further modulation of the emotional Late Positivity by these specific emotion categories.
In order to dissociate the role of explicit attention to affective meaning from the effects of affective meaning itself on ERPs during the comprehension of emotional words, we conducted a second experiment in which a separate set of participants passively read these same sentence pairs for comprehension. Based on the results of numerous previous studies, we predicted that the absence of an evaluative task would lead to smaller magnitudes of late positivities to emotional (versus neutral) words. However, given behavioral (Pratto and John 1991; Strauss 1983; Wurm and Vakoch 1996) and ERP (Bernat et al 2001) evidence for the persistence of the negativity bias even when participants are not explicitly attending to affective content, we predicted that this bias would be preserved under passive reading conditions. Finally, we also predicted that, because the intrinsic salience of emotional words increases the demands of semantically integrating their meanings, we would detect, during passive comprehension, an N400 effect to emotional (versus neutral) words.
EXPERIMENT 2
METHODS
Participants
18 Tufts psychology undergraduate students participated (9 men; mean age = 21.1 ± 3.2 (mean ± SD). None had participated in the first experiment. Inclusion and exclusion criteria and the characterization procedure were identical to those of the first experiment.
Stimuli and Procedure
The sentence pairs were the same as those in Experiment 1, except that probe trials were now included (see below). The experimental procedure was identical to that of Experiment 1, except for the task.
Construction of probes
After a variable number of trials, participants viewed a “probe” in order to ensure that they attended to the sentences and read them for comprehension. In each of the three original lists, 24 probe phrases, that each was either congruous (12) or incongruous (12) with a theme or concept described in the preceding sentence pair, was inserted. For example: After many years Barbara finally telephoned her father. She wanted to forgive him now. Probe phrase: “talking to family” (congruous) or “interested in politics” (incongruous).
The probes were then counterbalanced across the three original lists such that, across all subjects, a probe that was incongruous in one list was used as a congruous probe for the same condition in a different list, as well as the reverse, giving rise to six lists. These probes were rated for congruency in 10 raters who did not participate in the ERP experiment; these raters were 98% accurate in identifying the congruous and incongruous probes.
After a variable number of trials (on average, after every 6 trials), participants viewed the probe phrase in yellow. Participants were told to read all of the sentences and, when they saw a probe phrase, to decide whether this phrase was congruous or incongruous in meaning with the preceding sentence pair. The probe phrase remained on the screen until the subject made a response with their right or left thumb (counterbalanced across participants) at which point the next trial started.
Data Acquisition and Analysis
Data acquisition and analysis were identical to that in Experiment 1.
Also, Spearman’s correlations were used to conduct exploratory analyses to determine whether participants’ level of trait anxiety impacted modulation of the N400 by emotion at Pz (the electrode which has shown the largest N400 effects in previous studies). Alpha was set at .05.
RESULTS
The mean accuracy rate (averaged over the 18 subjects) in correctly identifying the congruous and incongruous probes was 89 % (SD=23). No participant showed less than 70% accuracy.
Grand-average ERPs elicited by the critical words in the three types of sentence pairs and maps of voltage differences between conditions are shown in Figure 3. An N1-P2 complex can be seen in the first 300 msec after word onset. In both the 0–100 msec time window and the 100–200 msec time window, there were no differences between the three conditions. In the 200–300 msec time window, the P2 appeared to be slightly smaller (more negative) to the positive relative to both the neutral and the negative critical words at lateral sites, reflected by interactions between Affect and AP distribution (positive vs. neutral: F(6, 102) = 4.426, p = 0.035) and Affect, AP distribution and Hemisphere (positive vs. negative: F(6, 102) = 4.281, p = 0.023). However, follow-up at individual electrode sites failed to reveal any significant effects (all ps > 0.13).
The N1-P2 was followed by a distinct negativity – the N400. This was immediately followed by a late positivity at many of the same posterior electrode sites where the N400 appeared largest. To characterize the modulation of the N400 across the three conditions, and yet minimize overlap from this later positivity, we selected a relatively early time window for statistical analysis of the N400: 325–425 msec. This encompassed the peak of the N400 as seen on the grand average waveforms.3
Overall ANOVAs showed no main effects of Affect, but there were significant Affect by AP distribution interactions at the midline, medial and lateral electrode columns (see Table 4). Planned pair-wise ANOVAs were carried out to follow up these effects. These again revealed significant Affect by AP distribution interactions: the negative words evoked a larger negativity than the neutral words at the midline and lateral columns, particularly at central and posterior sites4 (CP5, p=.03; T5, p=.03; trends at CP1, P3, Pz, P4, O1, Oz, O2, T6, ps<.12). The positive words also evoked a slightly larger negativity than the neutral words at the midline, medial, lateral and peripheral columns, particularly at occipital sites (Oz, p=.03; O2, p=.03; O1, p=.07), but a slightly larger, but non-significant, positivity than to the neutral words at more anterior sites. There were no significant differences in amplitude between the N400 evoked by negative and positive words at any electrode column. There were also no Affect by Hemisphere, or Affect by Hemisphere by AP distribution interactions.
Table 4.
Main effect of emotion | Interactions between Affect and AP distribution | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
F (degrees of freedom) | P value | F (degrees of freedom) | P value | |
A. Overall effects | ||||
Midline | 0.266 (2, 34) | 0.768 | 4.302 (8, 136) | 0.012 |
Medial | 0.771 (2, 34) | 0.469 | 3.938 (4, 68) | 0.043 |
Lateral | 1.070 (2, 34) | 0.353 | 4.745 (6, 102) | 0.018 |
Peripheral | 1.104 (2, 34) | 0.342 | 2.578 (8, 136) | 0.066 |
B. Negative versus Neutral | ||||
Midline | 0.337 (1, 17) | 0.569 | 6.088 (4, 68) | 0.011 |
Medial | 1.156 (1, 17) | 0.297 | 4.217 (2, 34) | 0.053 |
Lateral | 1.433 (1, 17) | 0.248 | 5.088 (3, 51) | 0.030 |
Peripheral | 1.310 (1, 17) | 0.268 | 3.242 (4, 68) | 0.068 |
C. Positive versus Neutral | ||||
Midline | 0.001 (1, 17) | 0.973 | 5.283 (4, 68) | 0.020 |
Medial | 0.014 (1, 17) | 0.908 | 6.356 (2, 34) | 0.016 |
Lateral | 0.000 (1, 17) | 0.992 | 5.801 (3, 51) | 0.019 |
Peripheral | 0.012 (1, 17) | 0.912 | 3.804 (4, 68) | 0.046 |
D. Negative versus Positive | ||||
Midline | 0.420 (1, 17) | 0.526 | 0.088 (4, 68) | 0.896 |
Medial | 1.090 (1, 17) | 0.311 | 0.527 (2, 34) | 0.504 |
Lateral | 1.610 (1, 17) | 0.222 | 0.531 (3, 51) | 0.548 |
Peripheral | 1.755 (1, 17) | 0.203 | 0.104 (4, 68) | 0.892 |
The large positivity which followed the N400 peaked between 500 and 700 ms.5 For this Late Positivity, the overall ANOVAs revealed main effects of Affect at all columns except the peripheral column (see Table 5), but no significant interactions involving AP distribution or Hemisphere in any electrode column. Follow-up pairwise ANOVAs revealed a larger Late Positivity to the negative words relative to the neutral words at the midline column. In contrast, there were no differences between Late Positivity amplitudes elicited by the positive and neutral words. Finally, planned pair-wise simple effects ANOVAs comparing the negative and positive words revealed main effects of Affect at all columns, reflecting significantly larger Late Positivities to the negative than to the positive words.
Table 5.
Main effect of Affect | Interactions between Affect and AP distribution | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
F (degrees of freedom) | P value | F (degrees of freedom) | P value | |
A. Overall effects | ||||
Midline | 5.226 (2, 34) | 0.012 | 1.326 (8, 136) | 0.236 |
Medial | 4.621 (2, 34) | 0.018 | 1.017 (4, 68) | 0.373 |
Lateral | 4.142 (2, 34) | 0.028 | 1.356 (6, 102) | 0.270 |
Peripheral | 2.296 (2, 34) | 0.127 | 1.182 (8, 136) | 0.325 |
B. Negative versus Neutral | ||||
Midline | 4.866 (1, 17) | 0.041 | 1.859 (4, 68) | 0.175 |
Medial | 4.278 (1, 17) | 0.054 | 1.279 (2, 34) | 0.279 |
Lateral | 2.849 (1, 17) | 0.110 | 1.447 (3, 51) | 0.251 |
Peripheral | 1.336 (1, 17) | 0.264 | 2.898 (4, 68) | 0.072 |
C. Positive versus Neutral | ||||
Midline | 0.406 (1, 17) | 0.533 | 0.610 (4, 68) | 0.535 |
Medial | 0.639 (1, 17) | 0.435 | 0.348 (2, 34) | 0.676 |
Lateral | 0.866 (1, 17) | 0.365 | 0.371 (3, 51) | 0.617 |
Peripheral | 0.584 (1, 17) | 0.455 | 0.113 (4, 68) | 0.887 |
D. Negative versus Positive | ||||
Midline | 12.186 (1, 17) | 0.003 | 1.441 (4, 68) | 0.252 |
Medial | 10.822 (1, 17) | 0.004 | 1.164 (2, 34) | 0.316 |
Lateral | 10.697 (1, 17) | 0.005 | 2.134 (3, 51) | 0.155 |
Peripheral | 6.656 (1, 17) | 0.019 | 1.035 (4, 68) | 0.356 |
Analyses of the plausibility-matched dataset indicated that differences between the negative and neutral conditions in levels of plausibility did not account for the negative (versus neutral) N400 effect; in this dataset, all of the previously found interactions remained significant, with an additional significant effect found in the medial column. Similarly, in the arousal-matched dataset, all of the original Affect by AP distribution interactions for the N400 time window remained significant, with additional significant effects in the medial and peripheral columns for both the negative vs. neutral and positive vs. neutral contrasts, with no significant differences between the negative and positive conditions in any column. Finally, the analysis of the cloze-matched data set indicated that the slightly increased cloze probability of the positive relative to the negative and neutral critical words was not likely to account for the original findings, e.g., the contrast between positive and neutral words in the cloze-matched dataset revealed Affect by AP distribution interactions for the N400 time window that approached significance in the midline, medial and lateral columns (all Fs> 3.5; ps<0.07).
Also, analyses conducted in the datasets matched for arousal, cloze probability and plausibility demonstrated that the negative vs. positive Late Positivity effect was not due to differences in these factors; the original pattern of results remained intact, with main effects of Affect in all columns, with the exception of the peripheral column for the arousal-matched analysis which showed an Affect by Hemisphere by AP distribution interaction instead. Similarly, the negative vs. neutral Late Positivity effect was not affected by the difference in plausibility between the negative and neutral words, since this effect became more robust in the plausibility-matched analysis (attaining significance in the medial, lateral and peripheral columns). Grand average ERPs and voltage maps for these arousal, cloze and plausibility – matched analyses are shown in Figures S4, S5 and S6 in Supplementary Materials (see www.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/kuperberglab/materials.htm).
The difference between N400 amplitudes to the negative and neutral words at Pz (as well as at CP5 and T5, where the N400 effect to negative versus neutral words was largest) correlated with levels of trait anxiety (Pz: Rho=.51, p=.030; CP5: Rho=.53, p=.023; T5: Rho=.48, p=.042) (see Figure 4), i.e., higher levels of trait anxiety led to smaller (less negative) N400 effects. There were no other significant correlations between Pz N400 amplitudes and trait anxiety (all ps > 0.12).
DISCUSSION
In this second experiment, we found that, during the passive comprehension of emotional words following a neutral, non-constraining context, a localized, posteriorly distributed N400 to negative and positive words was larger (more negative) than the N400 to neutral words. However, there was no significant difference between the amplitude of the N400 to positive and negative words. Also, similar to Experiment 1, a Late Positivity was significantly larger (more positive) to negative words than to both neutral and positive words. However, during passive reading conditions, there were no differences between the amplitudes of the Late Positivity to positive and neutral words. The same pattern of results was obtained when differences between the conditions in arousal, cloze probability or plausibility were taken into account.
One possible interpretation of the N400 effect to negative and positive words is that it arose from a lower predictability and/or plausibility of emotionally-laden events. Pretests of the stimuli used here showed that the positive endings to the sentence pairs were slightly more predictable than the neutral and negative ones, and that the negative endings were slightly less plausible than both the neutral and positive endings. These differences may reflect naturalistic aspects of processing emotional meaning, particularly since they are consistent with evidence that healthy people tend to, on average, expect positive outcomes and to believe that unpleasant events are relatively unlikely to occur (an ‘optimism bias’ about future events) (Armor and Sackett 2006; Hoch 1984; Sharot et al 2007; Weinstein 1980). However, these differences are unlikely to fully explain the observed pattern of findings for two reasons. First, the pattern of N400 modulation across the three conditions did not pattern with the small differences in cloze probability or plausibility ratings across the three conditions: given the ratings, the N400 to the positive words should have been smaller or equal to that of the neutral words – not larger at some posterior sites. Second, the N400 effect to negative and positive (versus neutral) words remained evident in the analyses of datasets which were matched across the conditions for cloze probability and plausibility.
We therefore suggest that this small, localized modulation of the N400 by emotion arose from a more specific influence of emotion on semantic integration and that, as soon as the affective meaning of the critical words was decoded, the consequent activation of emotional processing networks increased the demands of integrating their meanings within their neutral contexts. This might occur simply because distinct neural representations were activated by the emotional critical words and the neutral context (Cacioppo et al 1994; Schupp et al 2000). Alternatively, it may have been driven by the greater capacity of the emotional words to elicit arousal and attract attention (Lang 1995), directly recruiting additional processing resources for more detailed semantic analysis of emotionally-salient stimuli (also see Van Berkum, in press for a similar interpretation). Note that both these explanations assume that decoding the meaning of emotional words leads to a rapid activation of emotional processing networks within the brain, consistent with embodied frameworks of semantic memory (Glenberg et al 2005; Zwaan and Radvansky 1998).
Both of the explanations outlined above also assume an important role of the neutral context in modulating the N400 to the emotional words in the present study – a role that is illuminated by a comparison with previous studies presenting emotional words either in isolation or in emotionally congruous contexts: Kanske and Kotz (2007) demonstrated, in contrast to these findings, less negative N400 amplitudes (as well as shorter reaction times) to emotional words relative to neutral words presented in isolation as participants performed a lexical decision task. Also, several studies have found reductions in N400 amplitudes to emotional words when they were primed by sentences spoken in congruent emotional prosody (Schirmer et al 2002; Schirmer et al 2005); in contrast to the present study, in these studies, the automatic activation of emotional processing networks by the emotionally congruent context may have facilitated integration.
We also found an inverse correlation between the size of the N400 effect to negative words and trait anxiety, i.e., a smaller (less negative) N400 effect to negative versus neutral words in participants with more trait anxiety. This finding should be considered preliminary as we did not find a complementary enhancement by trait anxiety of the N400 effect to positive words, and we did not explicitly manipulate participants’ mood or examine the role of positive affect. However, it does support the possibility that predominant trait mood can influence the semantic mapping process within the N400 time window: participants with predominantly negative mood may have either perceived the negative (versus the neutral) events as more expected, and/or the neutral context may have been interpreted as negative, decreasing any affective mismatch between the context and the negative critical word. Consistent with this explanation, an N400 effect has been reported to words within sentence contexts that are morally inconsistent (versus consistent) with a participant’s individual belief system (Van Berkum et al submitted) and also to words which are inconsistent (versus consistent) with a positive self-view (Watson et al 2007). Taken together, these findings support the hypothesis that current mood state influences the processing of emotional input in a manner that is analogous to the influence of the immediate context on general, on-line language comprehension (Kutas and Federmeier 2000), and that the comprehension of emotionally salient information is also influenced by deeply ingrained, emotionally-laden beliefs about the self and the world that are rapidly retrieved from memory stores specialized for emotional and/or autobiographical information. Follow-up studies which specifically manipulate mood state, characterize long-standing affective dispositions and belief systems of participants, and then examine the effects of these factors on predictability and plausibility (and ease of semantic integration) of emotionally-laden events, can more fully explore the roles of mood, temperament and autobiographical knowledge in the comprehension of emotional language.
Also, in this experiment, like in Experiment 1, we found a larger Late Positivity to negative words, compared to positive and neutral words. However, unlike in Experiment 1, under conditions of passive reading, there were no differences between the Late Positivities to positive and neutral words. This suggests that in Experiment 1 there was an interaction between the task and the intrinsic salience of the stimuli, augmenting the Late Positivity overall and leading to differentiation between the positive and neutral conditions. Interestingly, the magnitude of the negativity bias (the difference between the negative and positive conditions) was not markedly affected by task, with a similar, widespread distribution in both experiments. Thus the negativity bias may represent a manifestation of the intrinsic, attention-attracting capacity of emotional information (which is greater for negative than positive stimuli) relative to a neutral context.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
The overall goal of these two experiments was to study the neurocognitive mechanisms governing the comprehension of emotional meaning of language. We asked whether encountering an emotional word within a neutral context (a) influences a semantic memory-based process, as indexed by the N400 and (b) leads to the engagement of additional cognitive processes that are known to be recruited during the processing of emotional stimuli, as indexed by the Late Positivity. We found that, during passive comprehension of sentences that built up an affectively neutral context, words with emotional meaning (relative to affectively neutral words) evoked a small, posterior N400 effect, providing evidence for an intensified or more difficult process of mapping the evolving representation of meaning. However, the amplitude of the N400 elicited by emotional words was not significantly modulated by the specific valence of the words (i.e. by whether their affective content was positive versus negative). Rather, a Late Positivity showed such valence sensitivity, with a larger amplitude to negative than to positive words, during both conditions of explicit affective evaluation and passive reading. Taken together, these findings suggest that a semantic memory-based mechanism (indexed by the N400) was influenced by the overall emotional salience of a word relative to its neutral context, while the precise valence of the word modulated a later stage of evaluation (indexed by the Late Positivity).
Here we have shown that a word which is generally semantically plausible (i.e. makes sense) but affectively incongruous (versus congruous) with its preceding context evokes an N400 effect. The ease of semantic integration, as reflected by the N400, is thought to be determined both by the degree to which the preceding context paves the way towards a word’s meaning and the accessibility of the word’s meaning within long-term semantic memory stores (Kutas and Federmeier 2000). We have suggested that the increased N400 to emotional (relative to neutral) words embedded in a neutral context, may reflect the increased demands of mapping the emotional meaning of a word with the neutral meaning of the preceding sentence, with respect to long-term semantic knowledge (including emotionally-laden associations), either because of the relative distance between emotional and neutral semantic knowledge stores (Osgood et al 1957) (each subserved by distinct neural networks, with emotional processing networks centered in the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex) and/or because emotion or arousal rapidly focuses attention for more in-depth or intensified semantic memory-based analysis. This intensified, early analysis may be followed by a more detailed evaluation of the emotional event or situation depicted, during the Late Positivity time window.
Several previous studies have not observed modulation of the Late Positivity by emotion during the incidental processing of emotional words (Carretie et al 1996; Naumann et al 1997), arguing that explicit attention to emotional meaning is necessary to evoke a Late Positivity. However, here we found, during passive reading conditions, a Late Positivity to negative words that was significantly larger than the Late Positivity to both positive and neutral words in a neutral context. These findings suggest that, at least when embedded in a neutral context, the intrinsic motivational significance of negatively-valenced information attracts attentional resources. Of note, however, during passive comprehension, emotional content per se did not lead to a general emotional modulation of the Late Positivity, since only negatively-valenced information led to amplification of the Late Positivity. In contrast, during the explicit evaluation of affective meaning, both a general emotion effect (emotional greater than neutral) and a valence effect (negative greater than positive) were demonstrated. This observation is consistent with the findings of ERP studies that have varied either spatial or task-related attention to the emotional content of emotionally-evocative pictures or human faces with emotional expressions (Schupp et al 2007b; Vuilleumier and Pourtois 2007), providing evidence for attention-emotion interactions during later stages of processing.
Our finding of a negativity bias in the Late Positivity to emotional words is consistent with a large body of previous evidence indicating that negatively-valenced information typically consumes more processing resources than positively-valenced information. Like the emotional Late Positivity overall, the negativity bias here was not task-dependent—it was seen in both experiments. This task-independence is inconsistent with the findings of some behavioral studies which found that response latencies were longer to negative than to positive words only when their meaning was explicitly processed (Dahl 2001; White 1996). But our result is in line with the ERP findings of Bernat and colleagues who demonstrated a negativity bias in the Late Positivity during subliminal as well as during supraliminal exposures to emotional words (Bernat et al 2001). Taken together, it is likely that the greater temporal sensitivity and greater proximity to the relevant neural processes of ERPs, relative to reaction times, provided the power to detect differences between responses to negative and positive words regardless of task. Our finding of a negativity bias to emotional words during the appraisal of social vignettes is consistent with evidence from social psychology (Taylor 1991) that this bias is present during the comprehension of affectively meaningful, real-world events.
The demonstration of a Late Positivity to emotional words in a neutral context adds to our growing understanding of Late Positivities which are evoked by different triggers during the comprehension of sentences and discourse. While it is likely that the continued analyses reflected by such positivities represent multiple different cognitive processes that are subserved by different neural networks, it has been suggested that they may share a common function—to re-evaluate or monitor the input (Kolk and Chwilla 2007). This model is in line with the present findings; a trigger to re-evaluate a neutral context after encountering an emotionally meaningful stimulus (leading to an alternative and possibly more accurate interpretation of the input) clearly serves a primary goal of the emotional response system—to maximize perceptual or interpretive accuracy in certain, prioritized situations, for the purpose of promoting survival.
The fact that we found little evidence for early modulation of ERPs by affective meaning is inconsistent with reports of effects of the emotional content of words on amplitudes of the P1, N1 and P2 components (Bernat et al 2001; Kissler et al 2006; Smith et al 2003). However, these early effects have been reported inconsistently and may be more likely to emerge if stimuli are repeated or presented transiently (Kissler et al 2006). Also, in the current study, the overall greater comprehension demands of evaluating whole sentences, relative to single words, may have delayed the impact of emotional content on semantic processing. In light of this possibility, the emotion-related enhancement of an early posterior negativity (EPN) to single words and non-linguistic stimuli, which occurs between 150 and 350 msec (Herbert et al 2008; Junghofer et al 2001; Kissler et al 2007; Schupp et al 2003; Schupp et al 2007b), may be related to the N400 effect found here. Additional studies, which compare the neurophysiological effects of emotional meaning on words presented alone to words presented in context, will be able to clarify the relationships among these effects further.
Summary
We have shown evidence that the passive comprehension of emotional, plausible words within a neutral context involves an intensified process of semantic integration, which likely results from an interaction between the neural circuits subserving emotional and other types of semantic memory-based retrieval during the comprehension of emotional language. In addition, both passive and active comprehension of such words led to a subsequent, attention-modulated, prolonged analysis of emotional words, similar to that found during the processing of non-linguistic emotional information.
We have suggested that the earliest stages of mapping the meaning of an incoming word on to its context, as reflected by the N400, can be modulated by an initial assessment of whether the meaning of a stimulus is emotionally significant or not. Stimuli that are assessed as aversive, or potentially harm-inducing, then trigger more extensive processing, as reflected by an augmented Late Positivity. This distinction between neurocognitive responses that are sensitive to ‘emotion’ (the N400) and more specifically to ‘valence’ (the Late Positivity) is broadly reminiscent of results of functional imaging studies which have described brain regions that respond to overall emotional salience, such as the amygdala and ventral striatum (Zald 2003; Zink et al 2003), and valence-sensitive areas of the ventral prefrontal cortex (Kringelbach 2005; Small et al 2003). In future studies, it will be interesting to determine, using combined ERP/functional MRI or magnetic encephalography methods, whether the rapid modulation of language processing by emotional meaning demonstrated here using ERPs is mediated by the same emotion-sensitive subcortical and valence-sensitive prefrontal networks.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Abigail Swain, Tatiana Sitnikova, Molly Butterworth, Catherine Garth, and George Nitzburg for their invaluable technical assistance and Jos Van Berkum and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on the manuscript. This research was funded by grants from NIMH (R01 MH071635 (GRK), K23-MH076054 (DJH)), NARSAD (DJH, GRK), the Sidney R. Baer, Jr. Foundation (DJH, GRK), and the Institute for Mental Illness and Neuroscience Discovery (MIND). DJH was also supported by the Clinical Research Training Program, the GlaxoSmithKline Severe Mental Illness Award and the Jerome Lyle Rappaport Charitable Foundation. SKL was supported by a grant from the Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, USDOJ, to the Harvard Medical School Center for Mental Health and Media.
Footnotes
In order to match the conditions for arousal, cloze probability and plausibility, we excluded 12, 17 and 47 scenarios, respectively, of the original 135. In the arousal matched set, only the positive and negative conditions were matched (the neutral condition remained lower in arousal than the other two conditions).
The grand average difference waveform calculated between the negative and neutral conditions peaked at 520 msec.
This was confirmed by constructing a grand average difference waveform for the negative minus neutral critical words at Pz and calculating the peak latency of this difference waveform from 300–500 msec: the mean peak latency fell at: 375 msec.
Although examination of the voltage map suggested that the interaction may have, in part, been driven by an anterior positivity to the negative versus the neutral critical words, post-hoc analyses revealed no significant effects at any anterior sites.
The grand average difference waveform calculated between the negative and neutral conditions peaked at 520 msec.
References
- Armor DA, Sackett AM. Accuracy, error, and bias in predictions for real versus hypothetical events. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2006;91:583–600. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bargh JA, Chaiken S, Raymond P, Hymes C. The automatic evaluation effect: Unconditional automatic attitude activation with a pronunciation task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1996;32:104–128. [Google Scholar]
- Barrett LF, Lindquist KA, Gendron M. Language as context for the perception of emotion. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:327–332. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bentin S, McCarthy G, Wood CC. Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1985;60:343–355. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(85)90008-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernat E, Bunce S, Shevrin H. Event-related brain potentials differentiate positive and negative mood adjectives during both supraliminal and subliminal visual processing. Int J Psychophysiol. 2001;42:11–34. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(01)00133-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bower GH. Mood and memory. Am Psychol. 1981;36:129–148. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.36.2.129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley MM, Codispoti M, Cuthbert BN, Lang PJ. Emotion and motivation I: defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion. 2001;1:276–298. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Affective reactions to acoustic stimuli. Psychophysiology. 2000;37:204–215. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo JT, Crites SL, Jr, Gardner WL, Bernston GG. Bioelectrical echoes from evaluative categorizations: I. A late positive brain potential that varies as a function of trait negativity and extremity. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67:115–125. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.67.1.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carretie L, Iglesias J, Garcia T, Ballesteros M. N300, P300 and the emotional processing of visual stimuli. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1996;103:298–303. doi: 10.1016/s0013-4694(96)96565-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chung G, Tucker DM, West P, Potts GF, Liotti M, Luu P, Hartry AL. Emotional expectancy: brain electrical activity associated with an emotional bias in interpreting life events. Psychophysiology. 1996;33:218–233. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb00419.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coulson S, King J, Kutas M. Expect the unexpected: Event-related brain responses to morphosyntactic violations. Language and Cognitive Processes. 1998;13:71–74. [Google Scholar]
- Coulson S, Kutas M. Getting it: human event-related brain response to jokes in good and poor comprehenders. Neurosci Lett. 2001;316:71–74. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(01)02387-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coulson S, Van Petten C. Conceptual integration and metaphor: an event-related potential study. Mem Cognit. 2002;30:958–968. doi: 10.3758/bf03195780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dahl M. Asymmetries in the processing of emotionally valenced words. Scand J Psychol. 2001;42:97–104. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donchin E, Coles MGH. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? Behavioral and Brain Science. 1988;11:355–372. [Google Scholar]
- Fazio RH, Sanbonmatsu DM, Powell MC, Kardes FR. On the automatic activation of attitudes. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;50:229–238. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.50.2.229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Federmeier KD, Kirson DA, Moreno EM, Kutas M. Effects of transient, mild mood states on semantic memory organization and use: an event-related potential investigation in humans. Neurosci Lett. 2001;305:149–152. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(01)01843-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Federmeier KD, Kutas M. A rose by any other name: long-term memory structure and sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language. 1999;41:469–495. [Google Scholar]
- Feldman LA. Variations in the circumplex structure of mood. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1995;21:806–817. [Google Scholar]
- Glenberg AM, Havas D, Becker R, Rinck M. Grounding Language in Bodily States: The Case for Emotion. In: Zwaan RA, Pecher D, editors. The grounding of cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Graves R, Landis T, Goodglass H. Laterality and sex differences for visual recognition of emotional and non-emotional words. Neuropsychologia. 1981;19:95–102. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(81)90049-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagoort P. Impairments of lexical-semantic processing in aphasia: evidence from the processing of lexical ambiguities. Brain Lang. 1993;45:189–232. doi: 10.1006/brln.1993.1043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagoort P, Hald L, Bastiaansen M, Petersson KM. Integration of word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension. Science. 2004;304:438–441. doi: 10.1126/science.1095455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Havas D, Glenberg AM, Rinck M. Emotion simulation during language comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2007;14:436–441. doi: 10.3758/bf03194085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herbert C, Junghofer M, Kissler J. Event related potentials to emotional adjectives during reading. Psychophysiology. 2008;45:487–498. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00638.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herbert C, Kissler J, Junghofer M, Peyk P, Rockstroh B. Processing of emotional adjectives: Evidence from startle EMG and ERPs. Psychophysiology. 2006;43:197–206. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00385.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoch S. Counterfactual reasoning and accuracy in predicting personal events. J Exp Psychol. 1984;11:719–731. [Google Scholar]
- Innes-Ker A, Niedenthal PM. Emotion concepts and emotional states in social judgment and categorization. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2002;83:804–816. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ito TA, Larsen JT, Smith NK, Cacioppo JT. Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: the negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;75:887–900. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.75.4.887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson VS, Miller DR, Burleson MH. Multiple P3s to emotional stimuli and their theoretical significance. Psychophysiology. 1986;23:684–693. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00694.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Junghofer M, Bradley MM, Elbert TR, Lang PJ. Fleeting images: a new look at early emotion discrimination. Psychophysiology. 2001;38:175–178. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanske P, Kotz SA. Concreteness in emotional words: ERP evidence from a hemifield study. Brain Res. 2007;1148:138–148. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiehl KA, Hare RD, McDonald JJ, Brink J. Semantic and affective processing in psychopaths: an event-related potential (ERP) study. Psychophysiology. 1999;36:765–774. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kissler J, Assadollahi R, Herbert C. Emotional and semantic networks in visual word processing: insights from ERP studies. Prog Brain Res. 2006;156:147–183. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56008-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kissler J, Herbert C, Peyk P, Junghofer M. Buzzwords: early cortical responses to emotional words during reading. Psychol Sci. 2007;18:475–480. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01924.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klinger MR, Burton PC, Pitts GS. Mechanisms of unconscious priming: I. Response competition, not spreading activation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. 2000;26:441–455. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.26.2.441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolk H, Chwilla D. Late positivities in unusual situations. Brain Lang. 2007;100:257–261. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreher DA, Holcomb PJ, Kuperberg GR. An electrophysiological investigation of indirect semantic priming. Psychophysiology. 2006;43:550–563. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00460.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kringelbach ML. The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:691–702. doi: 10.1038/nrn1747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krolak-Salmon P, Fischer C, Vighetto A, Mauguiere F. Processing of facial emotional expression: spatio-temporal data as assessed by scalp event-related potentials. Eur J Neurosci. 2001;13:987–994. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01454.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuperberg GR. Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: Challenges to syntax. Brain Res. 2007;1146:23–49. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.12.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuperberg GR, Kreher DA, Sitnikova T, Caplan DN, Holcomb PJ. The role of animacy and thematic relationships in processing active English sentences: evidence from event-related potentials. Brain Lang. 2007;100:223–237. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuperberg GR, Sitnikova T, Caplan D, Holcomb PJ. Electrophysiological distinctions in processing conceptual relationships within simple sentences. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2003;17:117–129. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(03)00086-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kutas M, Federmeier KD. Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4:463–470. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01560-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kutas M, Hillyard SA. Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science. 1980;207:203–205. doi: 10.1126/science.7350657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kutas M, Hillyard SA. Brain potentials during reading reflect word expectancy and semantic association. Nature. 1984;307:161–163. doi: 10.1038/307161a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kutas M, Van Petten C, Kluender R. Psycholinguistics electrified II: 1994–2005. In: Gernsbacher MTMA, editor. Handbook of Psycholinguistics, second edition. New York: Elsevier; 2006. pp. 659–724. [Google Scholar]
- Lang PJ. Presidential address, 1978. A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery. Psychophysiology. 1979;16:495–512. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1979.tb01511.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lang PJ. The emotion probe. Studies of motivation and attention. Am Psychol. 1995;50:372–385. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.50.5.372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis PA, Critchley HD, Rotshtein P, Dolan RJ. Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affective words. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:742–748. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhk024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathews A, MacLeod C. Cognitive approaches to emotion and emotional disorders. Annu Rev Psychol. 1994;45:25–50. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.45.020194.000325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naumann E, Bartussek D, Diedrich O, Laufer ME. Assessing cognitive and affective information processing functions of the brain by means of the late positive complex of the event-related potential. Journal of Psychophysiology. 1992;6:285–298. [Google Scholar]
- Naumann E, Maier S, Diedrich O, Becker G, Bartussek D. Structural, semantic and emotion-focussed processing of neutral and negative nouns: Event-related potential correlates. Journal of Psychophysiology. 1997;11:158–172. [Google Scholar]
- Niedenthal PM. Embodying emotion. Science. 2007;316:1002–1005. doi: 10.1126/science.1136930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niedenthal PM, Halberstadt JB, Innes-Ker AM. Emotional response categorization. Psychological Review. 1999;106:337–361. [Google Scholar]
- Osgood CE, Suci GJ, Tannenbaum PH. The measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press; 1957. [Google Scholar]
- Osterhout L, Hagoort P. A superficial resemblence does not necessarily mean you are part of the family: conterarguments to Couson, King and Kutas (1998) in the P600/SPS=P300 debate. Language and Cognitive Processes. 1999;14:1–14. [Google Scholar]
- Osterhout L, Holcomb PJ. Event-related potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language. 1992;31:785–806. [Google Scholar]
- Palomba D, Angrilli A, Mini A. Visual evoked potentials, heart rate responses and memory to emotional pictorial stimuli. Int J Psychophysiol. 1997;27:55–67. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(97)00751-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pratto F, John OP. Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;61:380–391. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.61.3.380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rugg MD. The effects of semantic priming and work repetition on event-related potentials. Psychophysiology. 1985;22:642–647. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1985.tb01661.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell JA. A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1980;6:1161–1178. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.79.2.286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schirmer A, Kotz SA. ERP evidence for a sex-specific Stroop effect in emotional speech. JCogn Neurosci. 2003;15:1135–1148. doi: 10.1162/089892903322598102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schirmer A, Kotz SA, Friederici AD. Sex differentiates the role of emotional prosody during word processing. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2002;14:228–233. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00108-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schirmer A, Kotz SA, Friederici AD. On the role of attention for the processing of emotions in speech: sex differences revisited. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;24:442–452. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schupp HT, Cuthbert BN, Bradley MM, Cacioppo JT, Ito T, Lang PJ. Affective picture processing: the late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology. 2000;37:257–261. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schupp HT, Junghofer M, Weike AI, Hamm AO. Attention and emotion: an ERP analysis of facilitated emotional stimulus processing. Neuroreport. 2003;14:1107–1110. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200306110-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schupp HT, Junghofer M, Weike AI, Hamm AO. The selective processing of briefly presented affective pictures: an ERP analysis. Psychophysiology. 2004a;41:441–449. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00174.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schupp HT, Ohman A, Junghofer M, Weike AI, Stockburger J, Hamm AO. The facilitated processing of threatening faces: an ERP analysis. Emotion. 2004b;4:189–200. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.4.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schupp HT, Stockburger J, Bublatzky F, Junghofer M, Weike AI, Hamm AO. Explicit attention interferes with selective emotion processing in human extrastriate cortex. BMC Neurosci. 2007a;8:16. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-8-16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schupp HT, Stockburger J, Codispoti M, Junghofer M, Weike AI, Hamm AO. Selective visual attention to emotion. J Neurosci. 2007b;27:1082–1089. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3223-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharot T, Riccardi AM, Raio CM, Phelps EA. Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias. Nature. 2007;450:102–105. doi: 10.1038/nature06280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Small DM, Gregory MD, Mak YE, Gitelman D, Mesulam MM, Parrish T. Dissociation of neural representation of intensity and affective valuation in human gustation. Neuron. 2003;39:701–711. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00467-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith NK, Cacioppo JT, Larsen JT, Chartrand TL. May I have your attention, please: electrocortical responses to positive and negative stimuli. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:171–183. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00147-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, California: Consulting Psychologists’ Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Spruyt A, Hermans D, De Houwer J, Vandromme H, Eelen P. On the nature of the affective priming effect: effects of stimulus onset asynchrony and congruency proportion in naming and evaluative categorization. Mem Cognit. 2007;35:95–106. doi: 10.3758/bf03195946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strauss E. Perception of emotional words. Neuropsychologia. 1983;21:99–103. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(83)90104-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SE. Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin. 1991;110:67–85. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Berkum JJ. The neuropragmatics of ‘simple’ utterance comprehension: An ERP review. In: Sauerland U, Yatsushiro K, editors. Semantic and Pragmatics: From Experiment to Theory. In press. [Google Scholar]
- Van Berkum JJ, Hagoort P, Brown CM. Semantic integration in sentences and discourse: evidence from the N400. J Cogn Neurosci. 1999;11:657–671. doi: 10.1162/089892999563724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Berkum JJA, Holleman B, Nieuwland MS, Otten M, Murre JM. Moral disagreement affects early semantic and emotional processing in the brain: An EEG study on answering opinion poll questions Submitted. [Google Scholar]
- Van Petten C. A comparison of lexical and sentence-level context effects in event-related potentials. Special issue: Event-related brain potentials in the study of language. Language and Cognitive Processes. 1993;8:485–531. [Google Scholar]
- Vanderploeg RD, Brown WS, Marsh JT. Judgments of emotion in words and faces: ERP correlates. Int J Psychophysiol. 1987;5:193–205. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(87)90006-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vuilleumier P, Pourtois G. Distributed and interactive brain mechanisms during emotion face perception: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:174–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson LA, Dritschel B, Obonsawin MC, Jentzsch I. Seeing yourself in a positive light: brain correlates of the self-positivity bias. Brain Res. 2007;1152:106–110. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.03.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinstein ND. Unrealistic optimism about future life events. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39:806–820. [Google Scholar]
- White M. Automatic affective appraisal of words. Cognition and Emotion. 1996;10:199–211. [Google Scholar]
- Williams JM, Mathews A, MacLeod C. The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychol Bull. 1996;120:3–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wurm LE, Vakoch DA. Dimensions of speech perception: semantic associations in the affective lexicon. Cognition and Emotion. 1996;10:409–423. [Google Scholar]
- Zald DH. The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2003;41:88–123. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(02)00248-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zink CF, Pagnoni G, Martin ME, Dhamala M, Berns GS. Human striatal response to salient nonrewarding stimuli. JNeurosci. 2003;23:8092–8097. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-22-08092.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zwaan RA, Radvansky GA. Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychol Bull. 1998;123:162–185. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.123.2.162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.