Skip to main content
Physical Therapy logoLink to Physical Therapy
. 2011 May 26;91(8):1198–1207. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20100372

Associations Between Physical Performance and Executive Function in Older Adults With Mild Cognitive Impairment: Gait Speed and the Timed “Up & Go” Test

Ellen L McGough 1,, Valerie E Kelly 2, Rebecca G Logsdon 3, Susan M McCurry 4, Barbara B Cochrane 5, Joyce M Engel 6, Linda Teri 7
PMCID: PMC3145896  PMID: 21616934

Abstract

Background

Older adults with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) are at higher risk for developing Alzheimer disease. Physical performance decline on gait and mobility tasks in conjunction with executive dysfunction has implications for accelerated functional decline, disability, and institutionalization in sedentary older adults with aMCI.

Objectives

The purpose of this study was to examine whether performance on 2 tests commonly used by physical therapists (usual gait speed and Timed “Up & Go” Test [TUG]) are associated with performance on 2 neuropsychological tests of executive function (Trail Making Test, part B [TMT-B], and Stroop-Interference, calculated from the Stroop Word Color Test) in sedentary older adults with aMCI.

Design

The study was a cross-sectional analysis of 201 sedentary older adults with memory impairment participating in a longitudinal intervention study of cognitive function, aging, exercise, and health promotion.

Methods

Physical performance speed on gait and mobility tasks was measured via usual gait speed and the TUG (at fast pace). Executive function was measured with the TMT-B and Stroop-Interference measures.

Results

Applying multiple linear regression, usual gait speed was associated with executive function on both the TMT-B (β=−0.215, P=.003) and Stroop-Interference (β=−0.195, P=.01) measures, indicating that slower usual gait speed was associated with lower executive function performance. Timed “Up & Go” Test scores (in logarithmic transformation) also were associated with executive function on both the TMT-B (β=0.256, P<.001) and Stroop-Interference (β=0.228, P=.002) measures, indicating that a longer time on the TUG was associated with lower executive function performance. All associations remained statistically significant after adjusting for age, sex, depressive symptoms, medical comorbidity, and body mass index.

Limitations

The cross-sectional nature of this study does not allow for inferences of causation.

Conclusions

Physical performance speed was associated with executive function after adjusting for age, sex, and age-related factors in sedentary older adults with aMCI. Further research is needed to determine mechanisms and early intervention strategies to slow functional decline.


Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is considered a transitional state that is less severe than dementia, but beyond that of typical age-related cognitive changes.1 Mild cognitive impairment is defined as impairment (adjusted for age and education) in one or more domains of cognition, with relative sparing of global cognitive functions.24 Although MCI is associated with only mild decline in cognition, the onset of dementia is characterized by overt difficulties in multiple domains of cognitive function as well as performance of daily activities.2 Even in the presence of MCI, reduced function has been identified in executive function tasks,5,6 instrumental activities of daily living7,8 and physical performance tasks.9,10 There are 2 major subclassifications of MCI—amnestic MCI (aMCI) and nonamnestic MCI (naMCI)—the more common of which is aMCI.4,11 Older adults with aMCI, involving early memory loss, are at higher risk for Alzheimer disease (AD),4,11 and reduced executive function may be associated with early physical decline in people with aMCI. Identifying whether physical performance decline is associated with reduced executive function is important for developing physical therapy management strategies aimed at slowing the progression of functional decline and associated disability in older adults with aMCI.

The worsening of executive function in older adults with aMCI is associated with the conversion to AD.5 The degenerative processes in aMCI involve medial temporal lobe structures, as observed in early stages of AD, but also may include the frontal lobe, the part of the brain involved in executive function.4,5 Executive function involves higher-order cognitive processes necessary for implementation of goal-directed behaviors,12 and reliance on executive function is elevated with increasing difficulty of motor tasks,13,14 especially in novel or demanding situations.15 Medication adherence, cooking, housekeeping, and motor tasks performed in a complex environment are examples of goal-directed activities that are vulnerable to decline in executive function.12 Executive function is thought to rely strongly on the prefrontal cortex and includes multiple cognitive processes such as planning, tracking, judgment, initiation, scanning, sequencing, problem solving, and cognitive flexibility.12,16 The notion that executive function is multifaceted in nature is supported by evidence from functional magnetic resonance studies indicating that different aspects of executive function rely on different parts of the prefrontal cortex.17

Declining physical performance in conjunction with cognitive decline has been associated with increased risk for dementia and disability in population-based studies of older adults.18,19 In a prospective, longitudinal study of older adults who were healthy, slower self-selected gait speed was associated with cognitive impairment at the 6-year follow-up.20 In the Sydney Older Persons Study of people who did not have dementia at baseline, the presence of slowed gait speed in combination with cognitive deficits was associated with increased odds of progression to dementia.19 The combination of impaired physical performance and executive dysfunction may be more predictive of dementia risk; therefore, it has implications for accelerated functional decline, disability, and institutionalization in older adults with aMCI.

Studies of physical performance in individuals with MCI support the notion that physical performance impairment is present prior to the onset of dementia,21,22 especially in older adults who demonstrate executive dysfunction.9,23 Executive dysfunction is predictive of functional decline and increased risk for dementia in community-dwelling older adults.24,25 Early pathology, consistent with AD, may contribute to physical performance impairment through alterations in memory, attention, and executive function networks.26,27 Alternatively, age and age-related comorbid conditions may be responsible for declining physical performance and executive dysfunction in older adults with memory impairment. It is unclear whether an association between physical performance and executive function remains after adjusting for age and age-related factors that are known to affect both physical performance and executive function in older adults with aMCI.

Because older adults with both physical and cognitive impairment are at higher risk for dementia and disability,28 identifying whether physical performance decline is associated with executive dysfunction is important for developing physical therapy early intervention strategies for older adults with aMCI. The purpose of this study was to determine whether performance on 2 tests that are commonly used by physical therapists (usual gait speed and the Timed “Up & Go” Test [TUG]) are associated with performance on 2 neuropsychological tests of executive function (the Trail Making Test, part B [TMT-B], and Stroop-Interference, calculated from the Stroop Word Color Test) in sedentary older adults with aMCI after adjusting for age, sex, depressive symptoms, medical comorbidity, and body mass index (BMI). We hypothesized that slower physical performance speed would be associated with lower executive function after adjusting for factors that are known to affect both physical performance and executive function.

Method

Participants

This study involved analysis of baseline data from the Resources and Activities for Life-Long Independence (RALLI) Study, a longitudinal intervention study of cognitive function, aging, exercise, and health promotion in sedentary older adults with aMCI. Participants were volunteers living in independent retirement residences who reported mild memory problems. Study flyers were distributed, and a presentation was given to residents of 28 independent retirement living centers in the Seattle, Washington, metropolitan region. Residents who were interested in volunteering for the RALLI Study contacted the study coordinator (Figure). The sample size was determined based on a power analysis conducted for the randomized controlled trial.

Figure.

Figure.

Flow chart of participant recruitment and screening.

Participants enrolled in the study were aged 70 years and older, were sedentary, and were classified as having aMCI based on screening interviews and a consensus meeting. Study recruitment and screening consisted of: (1) a telephone screening interview, (2) an in-home screening evaluation that consisted of a semistructured interview and neuropsychological screening tests, and (3) an expert consensus panel to review screening data. Petersen criteria1,4 were applied using a combination of cognitive test scores, screening interview data, and consensus case review to identify people with memory problems that would be consistent with a clinical subtype of aMCI (single or multiple domain). Petersen criteria included: (1) memory complaint, (2) impaired memory for age and education, (3) preserved general cognitive function, (4) essentially preserved activities of daily living, and (5) not already diagnosed with dementia. Participants were enrolled in the study from July 2007 through December 2009.

Cognitive function tests and clinical criteria used to determine whether participants met the aMCI classification criteria included: (1) the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for global cognition,29 (2) the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised (WMS-R) Logical Memory I and II subtests for immediate and delayed recall,30 and (3) the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale for severity rating of cognitive impairment.31 Memory impairment was determined by a Clinical Dementia Rating Scale score of 0.5 (consistent with MCI), a score on the WMS-R Logical Memory subtests that was 1 standard deviation below age- and education-adjusted norms,32 problems on the memory recall items of the MMSE, or observed difficulty with everyday recall during the assessment interview. Because the classification of aMCI involves a synthesis of information obtained through neuropsychological assessment, observations of daily activities, and clinical judgment,2,3 each participant was reviewed through a consensus process to determine eligibility for the study. The above neuropsychological test scores, performance on specific memory tasks, and evidence indicating intact ability to perform activities of daily living were examined by 2 clinical psychologists at a consensus meeting. Because aMCI is a clinical classification for which there is no single, definitive diagnostic test, a series of neuropsychological tests as well as an expert clinician's observations and judgment are critical in identifying people at risk for dementia.3 Sedentary lifestyle was defined as performance of less than 150 minutes of moderate-intensity exercise per week (over the previous month), as recommended by the American College of Sports Medicine and the American Heart Association.33

Potential participants were excluded from the study if they: (1) did not meet aMCI criteria; (2) were unable to walk independently with an assistive device; (3) were expecting to move away from the area; (4) had a known terminal illness; (5) were actively suicidal, hallucinating, or delusional; (6) had been hospitalized within the previous 12 months; (7) had an uncontrolled chronic medical condition; (8) were blind or deaf; or (9) had a known central nervous system condition associated with dementia. Upon enrollment in the study, participants completed 2 in-home baseline evaluations administered by trained research assistants. During these evaluations, testing was completed for demographic and health-related information, physical performance measures, and executive function measures as described below. Each participant gave consent prior to the screening process.

Demographic and Health-Related Information

Demographic and health-related information was collected via self-report responses. Medical comorbidity, assessed with the Self-Administered Comorbidity Questionnaire,34 was defined as having any of the following conditions: heart disease, hypertension, diabetes, pulmonary disease, kidney disease, peripheral vascular disease, osteoarthritis, rheumatoid arthritis, or back pain. Symptoms of depression were assessed using the Geriatric Depression Scale (range of scores=0–15).35 Body mass index (kg/m2) was calculated using height and weight measured at baseline.

Physical Performance Measures

Usual gait speed was calculated from an 8-foot (approximately 2.4 m) walk test in which participants walked at their comfortable pace. The 8-foot walk test was completed inside the participant's apartment or in a nearby hallway on a level surface with low-pile or indoor/outdoor carpet. The time to walk 8 feet was averaged over 2 trials and converted to gait speed (meters per second). Comfortable walking speed measurements have been reported to be highly reliable (r=.903) in individuals who were healthy and ranging in age from 20 to 79 years.36 Usual gait speed is comparable to the entire Short Physical Performance Battery in predicting disability in older adults.37

The TUG38 was performed at a fast pace to measure mobility speed.39 Participants were asked to move as quickly but as safely as possible to rise from an armchair (45.72-cm [18-in] seat height), walk 3 m, turn around a cone, walk back to the chair, and sit down. Time to complete the TUG was averaged over 2 trials. When performed at a comfortable pace, TUG scores have good interrater and intrarater reliability as well as a high correlation with the Berg Balance Scale scores (r=−.81), gait speed (r=−.61) and Barthel Index of Activities of Daily Living scores (r=−.78), and normative values have been reported.36,40 When performed as quickly and as safely as possible, the TUG has demonstrated high sensitivity and specificity in identifying older adults who are prone to falling.39

Executive Function Measures

The TMT-B was used to evaluate the components of executive function that represent complex visual scanning, speed, attention, and ability to shift sets.41,42 To complete this test, participants used a pencil to connect 25 encircled numbers and letters in numerical and alphabetical order, alternating between numbers and letters.43 The maximum amount of time allowed to complete the TMT-B is 300 seconds; longer times indicate worse performance in executive function. The TMT-B has been widely used in studies of older adults, and normative data have been reported.44,45 The TMT-B was used in this study because it is considered to be specific to executive function processes due to its requirements for switching sets and mental tracking throughout the task.46

The Stroop Word Color Test was used to assess components of executive function representing a person's ability to deal with conflicting stimuli.47 This test involves pairs of conflicting stimuli that are presented simultaneously, that is, the name of one color printed in another color. There are 3 portions to the Stroop Word Color Test: word naming (W), color naming (C), and color interference (CW). Although there are variations in test length and scoring methods,48,49 the version selected for this study involved recording the number of correct responses in 45 seconds for each portion of the test.50 A difference in the number of words printed in black ink compared with colors named correctly for words printed in a different color (ie, blue ink for the word “red”) is interpreted as interference of color stimuli. An overall Stroop-Interference score, as introduced by Golden,51 was calculated for this study using the formula: [CW − (W × C)/(W + C)]. In a previous study comparing older adults with aMCI with older adults with noncognitive impairments and mild AD, those with aMCI performed less well than those who were noncognitively impaired and better than the AD group on the color interference condition.52 Normative values for the raw scores from the 3 portions of the Stroop Word Color Test have been reported.44,53

Data Analysis

We used SPSS statistical software, version 16.0,* for descriptive statistics and data analysis. To examine the association between physical performance and components of executive function, linear regression was applied and model fit was evaluated. A curvilinear relationship was present between the TUG and executive function (both TMT-B and Stroop-Interference measures). With the understanding that the model is not intended for prediction, but rather to determine whether a relationship exists, we made the decision to log transform TUG scores. Upon transformation, we found that a linear relationship was present between log(TUG) and each executive function variable.

To assess whether executive function, as measured by the TMT-B and Stroop-Interference, was associated with usual gait speed after adjusting for age, sex, depressive symptoms, medical comorbidity, and BMI, we created 2 multiple linear regression models. Covariates known to influence both walking speed and cognitive functions, including age, sex, depressive symptoms, medical comorbidity, and BMI, were entered into each model. The covariate variables were added first to each usual gait speed model, followed by the executive function variable. Although performance on the TMT-B and the Stroop Word Color Test have been associated with age and years of education in older adults,45,53 education was not included as a covariate in the multiple regression analysis because the majority of our sample had 12 years of more of education (97% had >12 years of education, and 79.6% had >13 years of education).

To assess whether executive function, as measured by the TMT-B and Stroop-Interference, was associated with the TUG after adjusting for covariates, 2 models were created using log(TUG) as the outcome. The same covariates as above were entered into each model because they are known to influence both mobility speed and cognitive functions. The covariate variables were added first to each TUG model, followed by the executive function variable.

A dichotomous variable was created for comorbidity (none versus one or more medical conditions). Sex was coded 0 (male) or 1 (female). Correlations and the variance inflation factor for multicollinearity were used to identify whether covariates were strongly correlated. The contribution of the executive function variable in each model was assessed by the change in R2 values from the model with the covariates only to the model with the covariates and the executive function variable. Residual analysis for each multiple linear regression model included normal probability plots and scatter plots of standardized residuals.

Role of the Funding Source

Dr McGough received support through a National Institutes of Health Rehabilitation Sciences predoctoral fellowship (grant 2T32-HD-00742416A1), a National Institute of Nursing Research/National Institutes of Health post-doctoral fellowship (grant T32 NR007106), and the de Tornyay Healthy Aging Doctoral Scholarship (School of Nursing, University of Washington). This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging/National Institutes of Health (grant 2RO1 AG14777- 06A2).

Results

Data for demographic and health-related variables are summarized in Table 1. Participants had a mean age of 84.6 years (SD=5.7), were 80.1% female, and were 91% Caucasian. The initial sample was composed of 201 participants; however, 19 participants did not complete the TMT-B (16 due to vision problems and 3 due to missing data), and 25 participants did not complete the Stroop Word Color Test (22 due to vision problems or color blindness and 3 due to missing data). There also were missing data on the GDS (n=2), TUG (n=5), usual gait speed (n=2), MMSE (n=1), and logical memory (n=1). After accounting for all data entered into the multiple linear regression models, 179 cases were analyzed for associations between physical performance and the TMT-B, and 173 cases were analyzed for associations between physical performance and the Stroop-Interference measure. Sixteen participants (8.0% of the entire sample and 10.8% of those in the final analysis) reached the maximum time (300 seconds) on the TMT-B.

Table 1.

Descriptive Statisticsa

graphic file with name zad00811-3078-t01.jpg

a

TUG=Timed “Up & Go” Test; TMT-B=Trail Making Test, part B; WMS-R=Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised; MMSE=Mini-Mental State Examination; CDR=Clinical Dementia Rating Scale; BMI=body mass index.

b 10.8% of participants (n=16) reached the maximum TMT-B time of 300 seconds.

Usual gait speed was statistically significantly associated with executive function in both the unadjusted analysis (Tab. 2) and after adjusting for covariates (Tab. 3). In the unadjusted analysis, usual gait speed was associated with the TMT-B (β=−.267, P<.001) and Stroop-Interference (β=−.214, P=.004) measures. The change in R2 values attributed to executive function was .07 for the TMT-B and .05 for the Stroop-Interference measure. After adjusting for covariates, the TMT-B (β=−.215, P=.003) and Stroop-Interference (β=−.195, P=.01) findings were statistically significant, indicating that slower usual gait speed was associated with lower executive function performance on both measures. The change in R2 values attributed to the addition of the TMT-B (the difference between the full model and the model with covariates only) was .044. The overall change in R2 values was .084; therefore, the full model explained 54.5% more variance than the unadjusted model. The change in R2 attributed to the addition of the Stroop-Interference measure to the model was .034. The overall change in R2 values was .102; therefore, the full model explained 67.1% more of the variance than the unadjusted model. In the full model for usual gait speed, age and depressive symptoms were statistically significant when the TMT-B and Stroop-Interference measures were in the models, with slower usual gait speed associated with older age and depressive symptoms.

Table 2.

Linear Regression for Usual Gait Speed and Timed “Up & Go” Test (TUG) (Log Transformed)

graphic file with name zad00811-3078-t02.jpg

Table 3.

Linear Regression for Gait Speed (m/s)

graphic file with name zad00811-3078-t03.jpg

a The R2 value for the model not including the executive function variable.

b Change in R2 value was statistically significant at the .05 level when adding the executive function variable to the model.

Log(TUG) was statistically significantly associated with executive function in both the unadjusted analysis (Tab. 2) and after adjusting for covariates (Tab. 4). In the unadjusted analysis, log(TUG) was associated with the TMT-B (β=.290, P=<.001) and Stroop-Interference (β=.251, P=.001) measures. The change in R2 values attributed to the executive function variable was .08 for the TMT-B and .06 for the Stroop-Interference measure. Log(TUG) was associated with both executive function measures after adjusting for covariates. The TMT-B (β=.256, P<.001) and Stroop-Interference (β=.228, P=.002) findings were statistically significant after adjusting for the other variables, indicating that slower TUG times were associated with lower executive function performance on both measures.

Table 4.

Linear Regression for Timed “Up & Go” Test (Log Transformed)

graphic file with name zad00811-3078-t04.jpg

a The R2 value for the model not including the executive function variable.

b Change in R2 value was statistically significant at the .05 level when adding the executive function variable to the model.

The results indicate that a longer time to complete the TUG was associated with lower executive function, that is, a longer time to perform the TMT-B and higher Stroop-Interference scores. The change in R2 values attributed to the addition of the TMT-B to the model was .063 (the difference between the full model and the model with covariates only). The overall change in R2 values was .13; therefore, the full model explained 61.6% more variance than the unadjusted model. The change in R2 values attributed to the addition of the Stroop-Interference measure to the model was .043. The overall change in R2 values was .087; therefore, the full model explained 59.2% more of the variance than the unadjusted model. In the full models for log(TUG), age, depressive symptoms, and BMI were statistically significant covariates, with higher values of log(TUG) (and, therefore, slower performance on the TUG) associated with higher values of BMI and depressive symptoms.

Examination of multicollinearity among the explanatory variables using the variance inflation factor resulted in values close to 1, indicating no collinearity. Analysis of residuals for each model using normal q-plots and scatter plots of residuals by the estimated values showed that the model fit the data appropriately.

Discussion

In this study of sedentary older adults with aMCI, an association between physical performance speed and executive function on the TMT-B and Stroop-Interference measures was demonstrated after adjusting for age, sex, depressive symptoms, and BMI. Slower usual walking speed was associated with lower performance on a test of mental flexibility (TMT-B) and with reduced ability to manage conflicting stimuli (Stroop-Interference). Similarly, performance on a functional mobility task (TUG at fast pace) was associated with both measures of executive function. The results of this study demonstrate a consistent relationship between 2 commonly used physical therapy assessment tools and 2 measures of executive function. This finding is clinically relevant in older adults with memory impairment because impairments in physical and cognitive domains increase the risk for accelerated functional decline and disability, especially in the presence of executive dysfunction.24

The prevalence of slowed gait speed is evident when working memory is challenged in older adults with MCI,54 thus supporting the notion that gait is not entirely automatic, but instead requires attentional resources.13,55 Physical performance is particularly challenged when older adults are asked to concurrently perform a cognitive task, suggesting that allocation of attention is necessary in older adults with and without cognitive impairment.56 Associations between physical performance and cognitive function have been reported in previous studies in the areas of gait speed, balance, and fall risk in older adults with MCI,9,57 and they are especially robust in the presence of executive dysfunction.23 Declining executive function may be an early indicator of overall functional decline in older adults. For example, in a prospective study of older women with intact cognition at baseline, executive function decline occurred 3 years prior to memory decline over a 9-year follow-up period, and executive function decline occurred more often than any other cognitive impairment.58 Sedentary older adults with aMCI may be particularly vulnerable to executive function and mobility impairment and, therefore, at higher risk for subsequent functional decline and falls.

Slowed physical performance may be a compensatory strategy to maintain accuracy in older adults with aMCI.59 People with MCI performed daily activities at slower speeds, but maintained accuracy on a series of daily activities.60 Older adults with probable AD who were asked to perform a cognitive task (repeating random digits) while walking demonstrated slower walking and greater variability in their walking pattern, possibly due to reduced ability to divide or prioritize attention.55 A similar phenomenon may be occurring in older adults with aMCI, with a slowing of task speed in an effort to maintain accuracy even under conditions of relatively low cognitive or environmental challenge, as implemented in our study. Therefore, older adults with aMCI may be particularly vulnerable to physical performance decline and fall risk on tasks that require attention and learning, such as attending to a new walking route or other nonroutine activities. Although age and age-related comorbid conditions may contribute to declining physical performance and executive dysfunction in older adults with memory impairment, the statistically significant associations that remain after adjusting for these factors in our study suggest that other mechanisms, such as brain pathology, may be contributing to this relationship.

Medial temporal lobe structures, which are responsible for memory and learning, are the first brain regions affected by AD pathology, followed by other cortical and subcortical regions with disease progression.61,62 Pathology consistent with AD has been reported in the brains of older adults with aMCI63 and may contribute to physical performance impairment through alterations in memory, attention, and executive function networks.26,27 Alternatively, in older adults with aMCI, pathological mechanisms associated with declining physical performance may result from pathology not typically associated with AD, but instead with other dementia syndromes (eg, Parkinson disease, vascular disease) that interfere with frontal-subcortical circuits.27,64 Therefore, further research is needed to identify neuropathological mechanisms involved in the association between physical performance speed and executive dysfunction in older adults with aMCI.

This study had a defined sample of sedentary older adults with aMCI and valid and reliable measures of physical performance and executive function. There were, however, several limitations. A ceiling effect on the TMT-B occurred with 8.0% of participants (final analysis) reaching the 300-second maximum, so we lack an estimate of the slowest performance possible on the TMT-B. The cross-sectional nature of this study does not allow for inferences of causation. Nevertheless, consistent associations were demonstrated, suggesting that combining physical performance and executive function assessments may be clinically useful in detecting early functional decline in older adults with MCI. Although efforts were made to minimize bias through the selection of valid tests, consideration of potential confounders, and recruitment practices,65 a potential source of bias remains because this sample of older adults was recruited from independent retirement living centers. Future longitudinal studies to assess the predictive value of executive function measures on physical performance in people with aMCI are needed.

Conclusions

Slower physical performance was associated with lower executive function in our sample of sedentary older adults with aMCI, and associations remained statistically significant after adjusting for age, sex, depressive symptoms, medical comorbidity, and BMI. Slower gait and mobility associated with reduced executive function in sedentary older adults with aMCI have implications for accelerated functional decline, disability, and institutionalization. Further research is needed to determine mechanisms for this association and whether early intervention strategies are effective in slowing functional decline and disability in sedentary older adults with aMCI. Early intervention strategies that focus on enhancing executive function as well as physical performance (eg, exercise) should be studied in sedentary older adults with aMCI.

The Bottom Line

What do we already know about this topic?

Older adults with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) are at higher risk for dementia and associated disability. Functional decline often is accelerated in the presence of both physical and cognitive impairments.

What new information does this study offer?

In this study of sedentary older adults with amnestic MCI (memory loss), slower physical performance on gait and mobility tasks was associated with lower performance on executive function tasks, such as those involving planning and judgment.

If you're a patient or caregiver, what might these findings mean for you?

Comprehensive prevention and rehabilitation strategies that enhance both cognitive and physical function are important in reducing functional decline and disability in older adults.

Footnotes

Dr McGough, Dr Kelly, Dr Logsdon, Dr McCurry, and Dr Teri provided concept/idea/research design. All authors provided writing. Dr McGough, Dr Logsdon, Dr McCurry, and Dr Teri provided data collection. Dr McGough, Dr McCurry, and Dr Teri provided data analysis. Dr Logsdon, Dr McCurry, and Dr Teri provided project management, fund procurement, participants, and facilities/equipment. Dr Teri provided institutional liaisons. Dr Kelly, Dr Logsdon, Dr McCurry, Dr Cochrane, Dr Engel, and Dr Teri provided consultation (including review of manuscript before submission). The authors thank the Northwest Research Group on Aging, Ken Pike, PhD, for statistical support, and June van Leynseele, MA, for study coordination.

The University of Washington Institutional Review Board approved the study procedures.

A poster presentation of this research was given at the Combined Sections Meeting of the American Physical Therapy Association; February 17–20, 2010; San Diego, California.

Dr McGough received support through a National Institutes of Health Rehabilitation Sciences predoctoral fellowship (grant 2T32-HD-00742416A1), a National Institute of Nursing Research/National Institutes of Health post-doctoral fellowship (grant T32 NR007106), and the de Tornyay Healthy Aging Doctoral Scholarship (School of Nursing, University of Washington). This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging/National Institutes of Health (grant 2RO1 AG14777-06A2).

*

SPSS Inc, 233 S Wacker Dr, Chicago, IL 60606.

References

  • 1. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, et al. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome [erratum in: Arch Neurol. 1999;56:760]. Arch Neurol. 1999;56:303–308 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Gauthier S, Reisberg B, Zaudig M, et al. Mild cognitive impairment. Lancet. 2006;367:1262–1270 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Petersen RC, Negash S. Mild cognitive impairment: an overview. CNS Spectr. 2008;13:45–53 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004;256:183–194 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Rozzini L, Chilovi BV, Conti M, et al. Conversion of amnestic mild cognitive impairment to dementia of Alzheimer type is independent to memory deterioration. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007;22:1217–1222 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Rosenberg PB, Mielke MM, Appleby B, et al. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in MCI subtypes: the importance of executive dysfunction. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011;26:364–372 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Perneczky R, Pohl C, Sorg C, et al. Complex activities of daily living in mild cognitive impairment: conceptual and diagnostic issues. Age Ageing. 2006;35:240–245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Mariani E, Monastero R, Ercolani S, et al. Influence of comorbidity and cognitive status on instrumental activities of daily living in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: results from the ReGAI project. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;23:523–530 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Liu-Ambrose TY, Ashe MC, Graf P, et al. Increased risk of falling in older community-dwelling women with mild cognitive impairment. Phys Ther. 2008;88:1482–1491 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Verghese J, Robbins M, Holtzer R, et al. Gait dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment syndromes. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008;56:1244–1251 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Reisberg B, Ferris SH, Kluger A, et al. Mild cognitive impairment (MCI): a historical perspective. Int Psychogeriatr. 2008;20:18–31 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Royall DR, Lauterbach EC, Cummings JL, et al. Executive control function; a review of its promise and challenges for clinical research: a report from the Committee on Research of the American Neuropsychiatric Association. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2002;14:377–405 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Sheridan PL, Hausdorff JM. The role of higher-level cognitive function in gait: executive dysfunction contributes to fall risk in Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007;24:125–137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov Disord. 2008;23:329–342 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Stuss DT. Biological and psychological development of executive functions. Brain Cogn. 1992;20:8–23 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Lezak MD. Domains of behavior from a neuropsychological perspective: the whole story. Nebr Symp Motiv. 1994;41:23–55 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Huizinga M, Dolan CV, van der Molen MW. Age-related change in executive function: developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:2017–2036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Aggarwal NT, Wilson RS, Beck TL, et al. Motor dysfunction in mild cognitive impairment and the risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2006;63:1763–1769 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Waite LM, Grayson DA, Piguet O, et al. Gait slowing as a predictor of incident dementia: 6-year longitudinal data from the Sydney Older Persons Study. J Neurol Sci. 2005;229–230:89–93 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Marquis S, Moore MM, Howieson DB, et al. Independent predictors of cognitive decline in healthy elderly persons. Arch Neurol. 2002;59:601–606 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Kluger A, Gianutsos JG, Golomb J, et al. Patterns of motor impairement in normal aging, mild cognitive decline, and early Alzheimer's disease. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1997;52:P28–P39 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Kluger A, Gianutsos JG, Golomb J, et al. Clinical features of MCI: motor changes. Int Psychogeriatr. 2008;20:32–39 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Persad CC, Jones JL, Ashton-Miller JA, et al. Executive function and gait in older adults with cognitive impairment. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008;63:1350–1355 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Johnson JK, Lui LY, Yaffe K. Executive function, more than global cognition, predicts functional decline and mortality in elderly women. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2007;62:1134–1141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Blacker D, Lee H, Muzikansky A, et al. Neuropsychological measures in normal individuals that predict subsequent cognitive decline. Arch Neurol. 2007;64:862–871 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Buckner RL. Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron. 2004;44:195–208 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Tekin S, Cummings JL. Frontal-subcortical neuronal circuits and clinical neuropsychiatry: an update. J Psychosom Res. 2002;53:647–654 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. von Bonsdorff M, Rantanen T, Laukkanen P, et al. Mobility limitations and cognitive deficits as predictors of institutionalization among community-dwelling older people. Gerontology. 2006;52:359–365 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHughs PR. “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Weschler D. Weschler Memory Scale. 3rd ed. In: Corporation TP. (ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997 [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL, et al. A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry. 1982;140:566–572 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Steinberg BA, Bieliauskas LA, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ. Mayo's older Americans normative studies: age- and IQ-adjusted norms for the Wechsler memory scale—revised. Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;19:378–463 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Nelson ME, Rejeski WJ, Blair SN, et al. Physical activity and public health in older adults: recommendation from the American College of Sports Medicine and the American Heart Association. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2007;39:1435–1445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Sangha O, Stucki G, Liang MH, et al. The Self-administered Comorbidity Questionnaire: a new method to assess comorbidity for clinical and health services research. Arthritis Rheum. 2003;49:156–163 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, et al. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res. 1982;17:37–49 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Bohannon RW. Comfortable and maximum walking speed of adults aged 20–79 years: reference values and determinants. Age Ageing. 1997;26:15–19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Guralnik JM, Ferrucci L, Pieper CF, et al. Lower extremity function and subsequent disability: consistency across studies, predictive models, and value of gait speed alone compared with the short physical performance battery. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2000;55:M221–M231 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Podsiadlo D, Richardson S. The timed “Up & Go”: a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1991;39:142–148 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Shumway-Cook A, Brauer S, Woollacott M. Predicting the probability for falls in community-dwelling older adults using the Timed Up & Go Test. Phys Ther. 2000;80:896–903 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Steffen TM, Hacker TA, Mollinger L. Age- and gender-related test performance in community-dwelling elderly people: Six-Minute Walk Test, Berg Balance Scale, Timed Up & Go Test, and gait speeds. Phys Ther. 2002;82:128–137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Reitan RM, Herring S. A short screening device for identification of cerebral dysfunction in children. J Clin Psychol. 1985;41:643–650 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Greenlief CL, Margolis RB, Erker GJ. Application of the Trail Making Test in differentiating neuropsychological impairment of elderly persons. Percept Mot Skills. 1985;61(3 pt 2):1283–1289 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Administration and interpretation of the Trail Making Test. Nat Protoc. 2006;1:2277–2281 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Steinberg BA, Bieliauskas LA, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ. Mayo's older Americans normative studies: age- and IQ-adjusted norms for the Trail Making Test, the Stroop Test, and MAE Controlled Oral Word Association Test. Clin Neuropsychol. 2005;19:329–377 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Zalonis I, Kararizou E, Triantafyllou NI, et al. A normative study of the Trail Making Test A and B in Greek adults. Clin Neuropsychol. 2008;22:842–850 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Arbuthnott K, Frank J. Trail Making Test, part B as a measure of executive control: validation using a set-switching paradigm. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000;22:518–528 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reaction. J Exp Psychol. 1936;18:634–662 [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MP, Van Breukelen GJ, Jolles J. The Stroop Color-Word Test: influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment. 2006;13:62–79 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Valentijn SA, van Boxtel MP, van Hooren SA, et al. Change in sensory functioning predicts change in cognitive functioning: results from a 6-year follow-up in the Maastricht Aging Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:374–380 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Golden CJ, Freshwater SM. Stroop Color and Word Test: A Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting Co; 2002 [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Golden CJ. Identification of brain disorders by the Stroop Color and Word Test. J Clin Psychol. 1976;32:654–658 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Kramer JH, Nelson A, Johnson JK, et al. Multiple cognitive deficits in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2006;22:306–311 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Zalonis I, Christidi F, Bonakis A, et al. The stroop effect in Greek healthy population: normative data for the Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2009;24:81–88 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Montero-Odasso M, Bergman H, Phillips NA. Dual-tasking and gait in people with mild cognitive impairment: the effect of working memory. BMC Geriatr. 2009;9:41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Sheridan PL, Solomont J, Kowall N, Hausdorff JM. Influence of executive function on locomotor function: divided attention increases gait variability in Alzheimer's disease. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51:1633–1637 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Kelly VE, Schrager MA, Price R, et al. Age-associated effects of a concurrent cognitive task on gait speed and stability during narrow-base walking. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008;63:1329–1334 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Eggermont LH, Gavett BE, Volkers KM, et al. Lower-extremity function in cognitively healthy aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2010;91:584–588 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Carlson MC, Xue QL, Zhou J, Fried LP. Executive decline and dysfunction precedes declines in memory: the Women's Health and Aging Study II. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64:110–117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Scherder E, Eggermont L, Swaab D, et al. Gait in ageing and associated dementias; its relationship with cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2007;31:485–497 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Wadley VG, Okonkwo O, Crowe M, Ross-Meadows LA. Mild cognitive impairment and everyday function: evidence of reduced speed in performing instrumental activities of daily living. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;16:416–424 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 1991;82:239–259 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Braak H, Alafuzoff I, Arzberger T, et al. Staging of Alzheimer disease-associated neurofibrillary pathology using paraffin sections and immunocytochemistry. Acta Neuropathol. 2006;112:389–404 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Petersen RC, Parisi JE, Dickson DW, et al. Neuropathologic features of amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. 2006;63:665–672 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Blahak C, Baezner H, Pantoni L, et al. Deep frontal and periventricular age related white matter changes but not basal ganglia and infratentorial hyperintensities are associated with falls: cross-sectional results from the LADIS study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2009;80:608–613 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Portney LG, Watkins WM. Foundations of Clinical Research Applications to Practice. 3rd ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education Inc; 2009 [Google Scholar]

Articles from Physical Therapy are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES