Skip to main content
Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology logoLink to Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology
. 2011 Aug;21(4):321–330. doi: 10.1089/cap.2010.0045

Preschool Onset Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Course and Predictors of Stability over 24 Months

Mini Tandon 1,, Xuemei Si 1, Joan Luby 1
PMCID: PMC3157747  PMID: 21851190

Abstract

Objective

The present study examined the course of ADHD over 24 months in a preschool population.

Method

n=48 preschoolers with ADHD, aged 3.0-5.11 years, subjects included in a larger sample of preschoolers with depression and other disorders (n=306) were comprehensively assessed at 3 annual time points over 24 months in a prospective longitudinal follow-up study.

Results

Baseline diagnoses of preschool MDD, ODD, and CD were risk factors for ADHD diagnosis over 24 months in this preschool population. Among older preschoolers and after controlling for key demographic variables, ADHD predicted later ADHD diagnosis, along with other significant risk factors - baseline diagnosis of ODD, and/or family history of disruptive disorders, and stressful life events.

Conclusions

ADHD showed greater homotypic continuity at later rather than earlier preschool ages. Other disruptive comorbidities also emerged as key predictors of stable ADHD course. Study findings may help to inform which preschool ADHD populations to target for early intervention. Larger sample sizes are needed to confirm these findings and to further explore the stability, course, and predictors of outcome of preschool onset ADHD.

Introduction

Validity of ADHD in preschoolers

Empirical studies in the area of preschool psychopathology have increased markedly over the last decade (Lavigne et al. 2009). The availability of age appropriate measures that accurately ascertain symptoms as they manifest at this early age has facilitated systematic investigations of preschool mental disorders (Egger et al. 2006). Data validating several Axis I psychiatric disorders arising during the preschool period have now become available (Egger and Angold 2006). These have included investigations of preschool depression (Kashani et al. 1997; Kashani and Carlson 1987; Kashani et al. 1986; Luby et al. 2002; Luby et al. 2003; Luby et al. 2009; Stalets and Luby 2006), posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Scheeringa et al. 2005), other anxiety disorders (Spence et al. 2001; Warren 2004), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), conduct disorder (CD) (Keenan et al. 2007), and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Lahey et al. 2004).

Following the Robins and Guze (1970) criteria used to establish the validity of psychiatric disorders, the validity of preschool ADHD has been supported to date by stability and specificity of symptoms. Persistence of symptoms has been demonstrated in several longitudinal investigations from independent research groups (Campbell et al. 1986; Lahey et al. 2004; Lahey et al. 1998; McGee et al. 1991). Academic, social, and global impairment as reported by teacher and parent measures have been reported both cross-sectionally and longitudinally (Gadow et al. 2001; Kadesjo et al. 2001; Lahey et al. 2004; Tandon et al. 2009). The level of impairment of preschoolers with ADHD, has been reported to be similar to that of affected school age children (Wilens et al. 2002). More specifically, preschoolers with ADHD show poorer academic readiness for school entry, poorer fine motor skills, and are socially impaired relative to healthy controls (DuPaul 2001). Other related problems include preschool expulsion, difficulty maintaining babysitters and/or childcare and subsequently missed family work days and personal time (Barkley 2006). Finally, the validity of preschool ADHD is also supported by a “greater family history of related disorder,” and genetics is estimated to account for 80% of the variance in heritability of the disorder (Barkley 2006). While the diagnosis of ADHD in preschoolers has been reported at relatively high rates in clinical settings (Luby and Morgan 1997; Wilens et al. 2002), surprisingly, and in contrast to other preschool disorders noted above, there is a relative dearth of validity data for this preschool disorder.

Defining clinical level hyperactivity is more challenging during the preschool period as population-based studies indicate that this symptom alone is quite common among typically developing preschoolers (Smidts and Oosterlaan 2007). While high activity levels are also normative among preschoolers, clinical symptoms of ADHD have been observed in less than 10% of preschoolers (Egger and Angold 2006), which is noteworthy given recent concerns for overdiagnosis of ADHD at early ages (Evans et al. 2010). Further, other symptoms that are common and developmentally normative, such as “often interrupts,” occur at much higher rates in preschoolers with ADHD (Egger et al. 2006). For example, rates of ADHD symptom levels were reported as 4–8 times higher in clinic referred preschoolers with ADHD compared to age matched, non-ADHD preschoolers in a study of 3 to 7 year old children (Kadesjo et al. 2001).

Stability and Predictive Validity of ADHD in Preschoolers

Several longitudinal studies suggest symptoms of ADHD in preschoolers persist into elementary school (Beitchman et al. 1987; Campbell 1995; Lahey et al. 2004; Posner et al. 2007). These investigations informed symptom continuity (i.e., hyperactivity) or “problem behavior” rather than categorical diagnostic stability per se (Campbell 1995; Fischer et al. 1993). These studies suggested that problem behavior or hyperactivity as a symptom in preschoolers, predicted problematic behavior through adolescence. Other studies have suggested that while hyperactivity manifests as problematic behavior observed over several years, a myriad of other poor outcomes, including but not limited to ADHD are evident at increased rates by adolescence (McGee et al. 1991). This heterotypic continuity has also been shown in a more recent investigation by Lahey et al who reported not only homotypic continuity, but also that ADHD predicted higher severity of anxiety, depression and conduct symptoms, especially in females (i.e. heterotypic outcomes) (Lahey et al. 2007). In a prior report of a 3-year longitudinal investigation of preschoolers, ages 4 to 6 with ADHD, the majority of the sample (n=255) continued to demonstrate impairing ADHD (Lahey et al. 2004). In addition to longitudinal studies of specific symptoms in preschool populations, several investigations have demonstrated the stability of preschool disruptive disorders such as ODD and CD. For example, Lavigne et al (1998; 1998; 2001) reported that the 2-year stability of any disruptive disorder (including ADHD) was 50% for 2–3 year olds and 65% for 4–5 year old preschoolers.

Comorbidities and Preschool Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

Disruptive disorders in general have been found to be highly co-morbid with ADHD in several investigations (Gadow and Nolan 2002; Keenan 2000; Schwebel et al. 2002; Wakschlag et al. 2007). For example, oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) has been found to be co-morbid with ADHD in 60% of referred preschoolers (Wilens et al. 2002; Wilens et al. 2002). The diagnosis of ODD has shown to be both reliable and valid in preschool populations (Keenan et al. 2007; Lavigne et al. 1998; Lavigne et al. 2001). In addition to the high co-morbidity rates with ADHD, co-morbid ODD has also been associated with other poor outcomes such as a higher rate of injuries when compared to those with ADHD alone (Schwebel et al. 2002). Further studies are now needed to elucidate the longitudinal stability of preschool onset ADHD and to inform the key factors that contribute to course.

Current Investigation

The current investigation examined the diagnostic stability of DSM-IV ADHD in preschoolers ages 3.0 to 5.11 over a 2-year period. The following analysis was conducted using data from a sample of children ascertained for a study of preschool MDD for which a disruptive comparison group was also sought using the same ascertainment methods. It was hypothesized that ADHD diagnosed in preschoolers would show diagnostic stability at school age. Potential contributors to stability of preschool ADHD were examined accounting for demographic variables, medication use, comorbidities, parental psychopathology and stressful life events. Preschoolers were included in a no disorder group if they did not meet criteria for any DSM-IV psychiatric disorder based on parent report on the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA) (Egger et al. 1999), an age-appropriate, interviewer-based comprehensive diagnostic interview for the assessment of preschoolers.

Methods

Subjects

A sample of n=306 preschoolers, aged 3–5.11 years, was ascertained from multiple community sites in the St. Louis metropolitan area. To obtain an ethnically and socioeconomically diverse sample, primary care, pediatric clinics, and preschools/daycares were selected at random using a geographically stratified method (Luby et al. 2009). The group was oversampled for depressive disorders on the basis of parent responses on a previously validated screening checklist, the Preschool Feelings Checklist (PFC) (Luby et al. 2004); however, children with disruptive symptoms were also sought and included as a psychiatric comparison group. A score of greater than 3 on the PFC, while highly sensitive to capturing depressive disorders is less specific and also useful to capture those at high risk for disruptive disorders (Luby et al. 2004). Therefore the disruptive group was ascertained using the same methodology as the depressed group. All diagnostic group designations were based on parent report on the PAPA. This disruptive group was of primary interest for the current investigation. Preschoolers with chronic medical or neurological problems, mental retardation, or autistic spectrum disorders were excluded. Concerns that age of presentation at follow-up could bias likelihood of diagnosis was addressed by controlling for age at baseline (91% of the children attended both follow-up periods). Approval for the study was obtained from the Washington University School of Medicine Institutional Review Board. Consent was obtained for parent and child's participation from legal guardians, and assent was obtained for children six years of age or older.

Measures

The DSM-IV diagnoses were based on parent report on the PAPA (Egger et al. 1999), an age-appropriate, interviewer-based comprehensive diagnostic interview for the assessment of preschoolers with established test-retest reliability (Egger et al. 2006). As is standard for this measure, final diagnoses were derived using computerized DSM-IV based algorithms. The PAPA was administered by bachelor or masters-level clinicians who have undergone a 4-day formal training and subsequent calibration process. Interviewers were blind to diagnoses of the subject from prior study waves. Information about the use of psychotropic medication in the study sample was obtained from the PAPA as well as from parent reports on the Health and Behavior Questionnaire (HBQ) (Armstrong et al. 2003). Both stressful (eg parental divorce, change in daycare/school, death of a pet) and traumatic life events (eg death of a parent, a natural disaster, attacked by an animal) were assessed using the PAPA. Family history was ascertained from primary caregivers using the Family Interview for Genetic Studies (Maxwell 1992), a widely-used and validated measure of family psychiatric history. For the current study, family history of psychiatric disorders represented the proportion of all first- and second-degree biological relatives reported to have disorders [major depressive disorder (MDD), anxiety (anxiety disorders included separation anxiety disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, and PTSD), bipolar disorder (BP), CD, and ADHD]. Family history of disruptive disorders was ascertained but distinctions between ODD and CD were not made. Subjects were assessed at 3 annual study waves conducted over 24 months.

Data analysis

Demographic Characteristics of Diagnostic Groups at Baseline

Cross tabulation (χ2) tests and analyses of variance (ANOVA) were used to examine demographic and risk factor differences between diagnostic groups at baseline. In addition, cross tabulation (χ2) tests and Mann-Whitney U tests were conducted as appropriate to examine differences in the same factors between preschoolers who attended later study waves and those who dropped out.

Risk Factors Contributing to Stability of Preschool ADHD

Because repeated observations of an individual may be correlated, a Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE) model, which accounts for correlations, was used in this longitudinal study to examine risk factors such as family history of psychiatric diagnoses, life events, baseline diagnosis of MDD, anxiety, ODD, and CD, contributing to the stability of preschool ADHD. Demographic factors were controlled in the model.

Likelihood of Chronicity of Preschool ADHD

Multinomial logistic regression analyses were conducted to test the likelihood that preschoolers in the ADHD group vs those in the no-disorder or psychiatric comparison groups would have ADHD, no disorders, or another psychiatric disorder at follow up {baseline (T1, ages 3–5.11) vs. 1 year follow up (T3, ages 4–6.11); T1 vs. 2 year follow up (T5, ages 5–7.11); and T3 vs. T5. Logistic regression models were conducted to test whether an ADHD diagnosis at an earlier wave predicted an ADHD diagnosis at follow-up (e.g. T1 to T3, T1 to T5, and T3 to T5) while statistically controlling for demographic variables, co-morbid diagnoses (split into separate MDD, anxiety, CD, and ODD categories), stressful life events, and family history of MDD/anxiety, bipolar and disruptive disorders at baseline. The severity scores of ADHD were examined as well.

Differences between Preschoolers with ADHD who Recovered vs. had Stable Course at School Age

Cross tabulation (χ2) and nonparametric (Mann Whitney) tests were used to examine the differences between preschoolers who had a diagnosis of ADHD at preschool age (younger than 71 months) and also had data for school age ADHD status (yes vs. no); a further multivariate logistic regression model was conducted to examine factors that were associated with the differences. (SPSS software, version 17.0 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Illinois), was used to conduct all statistical analyses)

Results

Diagnostic groups and sample characteristics

Using baseline diagnoses, preschoolers were categorized into 1 of 3 hierarchical diagnostic groups: (1) the ADHD group was composed of those who met criteria for ADHD with or without any other co-morbidity (n=48); (2) the psychiatric group was composed of those who met criteria for other disorders except ADHD (n=106), including MDD, any anxiety (anxiety disorders included SAD, GAD, and PTSD) and/or disruptive disorders (disruptive disorders included ODD and CD); (3) preschoolers were included in a no disorder group (n=146) if they did not meet criteria for any DSM-IV psychiatric disorder based on parent report on the PAPA. Two preschoolers met symptom criteria only for bipolar disorder type I at baseline and were excluded due to the current lack of clarity on this diagnosis in preschool children, and 4 preschoolers had excessive missing data; therefore, these 6 subjects were excluded from the analyses that follow.

Results from a 1-way ANOVA with post hoc pair-wise comparisons indicated that the number of life events the preschoolers experienced prior to baseline (for stressful life events, F2, 296=4.24, p=.015, for traumatic life events, F2, 296=4.89, p=.008) and the proportion of family members with a history of bipolar disorder (collected at baseline, F2, 287=3.17, p=.043) were significantly different among diagnostic comparison groups. Specifically, preschoolers in the ADHD group had experienced significantly more life events (both stressful and traumatic) at baseline than preschoolers in the no disorder group (p=.012 for both) (Table 1). Cross tabulation χ2 test results indicated that caregivers of preschoolers in the ADHD group had a lower education level than caregivers in both the psychiatric (χ23=9.80, p=.020) and the no disorder (χ23=17.29, p=.001) groups. Total family income was significantly lower in the ADHD group compared with the psychiatric (χ23=10.05, p=.018) and the no disorder (χ23=15.61, p=.001) groups. Preschoolers in the ADHD group were less likely to come from married parents compared with the no disorder group (χ24=12.70, p=.013). Of the caregivers of preschoolers with baseline ADHD, 38% reported married, and 36% reported cohabitation over 6 months, which was included in the never married category. No other significant demographic or risk factor differences were found between diagnostic groups at baseline. Preschoolers in the ADHD group had a marginally higher proportion of family members with a history of bipolar disorder than preschoolers in the no-disorder group (non significant, p=.091).

Table 1.

Characteristics of the Sample at Baseline

 
Patients, No. (%)
 
 
Characteristic ADHD (n=48) Other Psychiatric Disorder (n=106) No Disorder (n=146) χ2 p-value
Age, y       9.41 0.052
 3 21 (44) 25 (24) 47 (32)    
 4 13 (27) 48 (45) 66 (45)    
 5 14 (29) 33 (31) 33 (23)    
Ethnicity       5.53 0.237
 White 21 (44) 51 (53) 87 (60)    
 African American 18 (37) 35 (33) 45 (31)    
 Other 9 (19) 15 (14) 13 (9)    
Sex       2.34 0.311
 M 29 (60) 56 (53) 70 (48)    
 F 19 (40) 50 (47) 76 (52)    
Total family income, $       16.26 0.012
 ≤20,000 18 (37) 20 (21) 25 (18)    
 20,001–40,000 13 (27) 18 (19) 20 (15)    
 40,001–60,000 8 (17) 16 (26) 27 (20)    
≥60,001 9 (19) 42 (44) 63 (47)    
Parent education       19.92 0.003
 High school diploma 14 (29) 16 (15) 27 (18)    
 Some college 25 (52) 45 (42) 42 (29)    
 4 year college degree 3 (6) 22 (21) 35 (24)    
 ≥Graduate education 6 (13) 23 (22) 42 (29)    
Parent marital status       15.99 0.043
 Married 18 (38) 57 (55) 94 (66)    
 Widowed 0 0 1 (1)    
 Separated 1 (2) 4 (4) 2 (1)    
 Divorced 5 (10) 10 (9) 7 (5)    
 Never married 24 (50) 33 (32) 39 (27)    
No. of life events, mean (SD)          
 Stressful life events 4.13 (2.20) 3.45 (1.85) 3.19 (1.91) F=4.24 0.015
 Traumatic life events 1.83 (1.39) 1.56 (1.24) 1.24 (1.14) F=4.89 0.008
 Total 5.96 (3.11) 5.01 (2.46) 4.43 (2.49) F=6.51 0.002
Age, mean (SD), mo 52.04 (10.55) 54.36 (9.70) 52.12 (9.15) F=1.90 0.151
Proportion of family history disruptive disorder, mean (SD) 5.92 (9.00) 6.11 (10.50) 5.31 (12.62) F=0.16 0.853
Proportion of family history MDD/ANX, mean (SD) 13.50 (13.71) 15.48 (15.33) 12.95 (13.90) F=0.93 0.394
Proportion of family history BP, mean (SD) 3.70 (5.43) 3.10 (5.48) 1.83 (4.69) F=3.17 0.043

SD=standard deviation; MDD=major depressive disorder; ANX=anxiety; BP=bipolar disorder.

Two hundred seventy-three subjects (38 with ADHD, 100 with psychiatric disorders, and 135 with no disorders) were retained during the longitudinal study (subjects who returned for at least one of the two annual follow-up waves). The Mann-Whitney U test and cross tabulations (χ2 tests) results indicated that within the no-disorder group, preschoolers who had parents with higher education levels were less likely to drop out (p=.011). Within the ADHD group, preschoolers who had no baseline diagnosis of CD (p=.003) and who had less family history of bipolar disorder at baseline (p=.028) were more likely to be retained in the study. No other statistically significant differences were found. The use of medications was examined and found to be non-significant.

Risk factors contributing to stability of preschool ADHD

Results from the GEE model indicated that preschoolers' ADHD diagnoses were relatively stable across 24 months. There was no significant difference among rates of ADHD diagnosis at different waves (Wald χ2=2.457, df=2, p=.293). There were four risk factors contributing to preschoolers' ADHD diagnoses: baseline ODD (Wald χ2=27.433, OR: 6.196, 95% CI 3.131 –12.261, p<.001), baseline CD (Wald χ2=11.270, OR: 3.537, 95% CI 1.692 – 7.396, p=.001), baseline MDD (Wald χ2=5.852, OR: 2.287, 95% CI 1.170 – 4.470, p=.016), and family history of bipolar disorder at baseline (Wald χ2=4.765, OR: 1.051, 95% CI 1.005 – 1.099, p=.029) (see Table 2).

Table 2.

Generalized Estimating Equations (GEE) Examining Risk Factors for ADHD Diagnosis over 24 Months

Variables entered into the modela Wald χ2 df OR (95% CI) p-value
Child's age, mo 0.036 1 1.003 (0.971-1.036) .849
Sex (M=1/F=0), Male 0.501 1 0.800 (0.432-1.483) .479
Ethnicity 3.933 2   .140
 White     1 [Reference]  
 Black 0.363 1 0.781 (0.350-1.743) .547
 Other 1.867 1 2.006 (0.739-5.442) .172
Family income, $ 2.209 3   .530
 ≤20,000     1 [Reference]  
 20,001–40,000 1.513 1 0.582 (0.245-1.379) .219
 40,001–60,000 0.52 1 0.693 (0.239-2.012) .500
 ≥60,001 2.22 1 0.535 (0.209-1.373) .194
Maternal education 5.378 3   .146
 High school     1 [Reference]  
 Some college education 2.173 1 1.788 (0.826-3.870) .140
 4-year college degree 0.741 1 0.595 (0.182-1.941) .389
 ≥Graduate education 0.494 1 1.447 (0.516-4.054) .482
Major depressive disorder 5.852 1 2.287 (1.170-4.470) .016
Anxiety disorder 0.681 1 0.750 (0.378-1.486) .409
Conduct disorder 11.270 1 3.537 (1.692-7.396) .001
Oppositional defiant disorder 27.433 1 6.196 (3.131-12.261) <.001
No. of stressful life events 0.024 1 0.993 (0.910-1.084) .877
Family history of disruptive disorder 0.280 1 1.006 (0.983-1.030) .597
Family history of MDD/anxiety disorder 1.005 1 1.010 (0.991-1.030) .316
Family history of bipolar disorder 4.765 1 1.051 (1.005-1.099) .029
Wave 2.457 2   .293
 Wave=3 vs. Wave=1 1.959 1 0.735 (0.263-1.249) .162
 Wave=3 vs. Wave=2 0.234 1 0.864 (0.480-1.558) .628
 Wave=2 vs. Wave=1 2.108 1 0.663 (0.381-1.155) .147

CI=confidence interval; OR=odds ratio; MDD=major depressive disorder.

a

The model included a total of 270 subjects, all variables were recorded at baseline, except for stressful life events, which were the total events the child had encountered prior to each study period.

Likelihood of stability of preschool ADHD: Homotypic vs. heterotypic ADHD outcomes

In this study sample, there were n=32 preschoolers diagnosed with ADHD at baseline who attended both subsequent waves. N=13 out of these (40.6%) preschoolers were diagnosed with ADHD at T3. N=10 (n=8 from T3 ADHD group, n=1 from T3 other psychiatric disorder group, and n=1 from T3 no disorder group) were diagnosed with ADHD at T5 (10/32=31.3%). Together there were n=15 (46.9%) preschoolers diagnosed with ADHD at follow-up waves. There were n=12 (12/32=37.5%) preschoolers diagnosed with other psychiatric disorders (no ADHD) at follow-up waves (n=9 –1=8 from T3 other psych group, and 4 more at wave T5 from the T3 no disorder group). Only n=5 (5/32=15.6%) preschoolers who had an ADHD diagnosis at baseline recovered at later waves (no disorder at both follow-up waves).

Baseline (T1) vs. 12 month follow-up (T3) diagnostic groups

Compared to preschool children with no disorder at baseline, preschool children with baseline ADHD were 18.2 times as likely to have a diagnosis of ADHD at T3 and 5.3 times as likely to have a diagnosis of ADHD rather than another psychiatric disorder at T3. Further, preschool children with baseline ADHD were 6 times as likely to have a diagnosis of ADHD at T3 compared to preschool children with other psychiatric diagnoses at baseline (see Table 3 for Odds Ratios along with their 95% confidence intervals for all comparisons).

Table 3.

Odds Ratios for Homotypic Versus Heterotypic Continuity of Preschool ADHD and Other Psychiatric Disorders

T1 group T3 group OR 95% CI
ADHD vs No disorder ADHD vs No disorder 18.20*** 5.61-59.09
  ADHD vs Other Psychiatric disorder 5.25* 1.47-18.77
  Other Psychiatric disorder vs No disorder 3.47*** 1.22-9.85
ADHD vs Other Psychiatric disorder ADHD vs No disorder 6.00** 1.99-18.08
  ADHD vs Other Psychiatric disorder 6.00** 1.89-19.04
  Other Psychiatric disorder vs No disorder NS  
Other Psychiatric disorder vs No disorder ADHD vs No disorder 3.03* 1.02-9.05
  ADHD vs Other Psychiatric disorder NS  
  Other Psychiatric disorder vs No disorder 3.47*** 1.82-6.61
T1 group T5 group OR 95% CI
ADHD vs No disorder ADHD vs No disorder 22.27*** 5.97-83.08
  ADHD vs Other Psychiatric disorder 4.44* 1.08-18.36
  Other Psychiatric disorder vs No disorder 5.01** 1.79-14.00
ADHD vs Other Psychiatric disorder ADHD vs No disorder 4.46** 1.49-13.30
  ADHD vs Other Psychiatric disorder 3.44* 1.09-10.86
  Other Psychiatric disorder vs No disorder NS  
Other Psychiatric disorder vs No disorder ADHD vs No disorder 5.00** 1.49-16.75
  ADHD vs Other Psychiatric disorder NS  
  Other Psychiatric disorder vs No disorder 3.88*** 1.94-7.76
T3 group T5 group OR 95% CI
ADHD vs No disorder ADHD vs No disorder 314.7*** 35.55-2785
  ADHD vs Other Psychiatric disorder 6.52** 1.93-22.02
  Other Psychiatric disorder vs No disorder 48.27*** 5.82-400.4
ADHD vs Other Psychiatric disorder ADHD vs No disorder 304.0*** 25.70-3596
  ADHD vs Other Psychiatric disorder 22.22*** 4.24-116.4
  Other Psychiatric disorder vs No disorder 13.68* 1.63-114.7
Other Psychiatric disorder vs No disorder ADHD vs No disorder NS  
  ADHD vs Other Psychiatric disorder NS  
  Other Psychiatric disorder vs No disorder 3.47*** 1.79-6.96

T1=baseline, T3=12 month follow-up, T5=24 month follow-up.

*

p<.05 **p<.01 ***p<.001.

ADHD=attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; OR=odds ratios; CI=confidence interval.

Baseline vs. 24 month follow-up (T5) diagnostic groups

Compared to preschool children with no disorder at baseline, preschool children with baseline ADHD were 22.3 times as likely to have a diagnosis of ADHD at T5 and 4.4 times as likely to have a diagnosis of ADHD rather than another psychiatric disorder at T5. Further, preschool children with baseline ADHD were 4.5 times as likely to have a diagnosis of ADHD at T5 compared to preschool children with other psychiatric diagnoses at baseline (see Table 3).

T3 vs. T5 diagnostic groups

Compared to preschool children with no disorder at T3, preschool children with T3 ADHD were 315 times as likely to have T5 ADHD and 6.5 times as likely to have a diagnosis of ADHD rather than other psychiatric disorder at T5. (see Table 3).

Examining ADHD Stability Accounting for Demographics, Comorbidities and other Risk/Protective Factors

All 3 logistic regression models were significant, Cox & Snell R2=.26, χ2=65.82, df=17, p<.001 for the model T3 ADHD with T1 ADHD as the predictor, Cox & Snell R2=.23, χ2=55.73, df=17, p<.001 for the model T5 ADHD with T1 ADHD as the predictor, and Cox & Snell R2=.30, χ2=75.64, df=17, p<.001 for the model T5ADHD with T3 ADHD as the predictor. Table 4 displays the results of these analyses with variables grouped into logical categories. Demographic variables, co-morbid disorders, other risk factors, and ADHD diagnosis were entered simultaneously. Five variables were significant using the model T3 ADHD with T1 ADHD as the predictor. The diagnosis of preschool ADHD at T1 (baseline) (OR: 10.06, p=.001) was the strongest predictor of T3 ADHD, followed by ODD at baseline (OR: 4.18, p=.03), family history of bipolar disorder at baseline (OR: 1.12, p=.01), age in months at baseline (OR: 1.07, p=.04), and family history of disruptive disorder (CD and/or ADHD) at baseline (OR: 1.05, p=.01) (see Table 4a). Three variables were significant when predicting T5 ADHD with baseline ADHD. The diagnosis of preschool ODD at baseline (OR: 15.4, p<.001), ADHD at baseline (OR: 8.05, p=.01) and family history of bipolar disorder at baseline (OR: 1.12, p=.03), were significant predictors of T5 ADHD even when controlling for all other risk factors (see Table 4a). When examining the model T5 ADHD with T3 ADHD, the diagnosis of T3ADHD (OR: 85.57, p<.001) was the strongest predictor of T5 ADHD, followed by ODD at baseline (OR: 16.85, p=.001) (see Table 4b).

Table 4a.

Logistic Regression of Preschool ADHD and Subsequent ADHD Controlling for Key Risk Factorsa

 
 
Model 1: T3ADHD as response variable
Model 2: T5ADHD as response variable
Variables entered into modelb % Wald χ2 OR (95% CI) p-value Wald χ2 OR (95% CI) p-value
Child's age, mean (SD) mo 53.09 (9.52) 4.31 1.07 (1.00-1.15) .04 3.45 1.07 (1.00-1.14) .06
Sex (M=1/F=0), Male 49.1 0.29 1.41 (0.41-4.84) .59 0.01 0.95 (0.27-3.36) .94
Family income, $
 ≤20,000 19.4 1.89 1 [Reference] .60 2.38 1 [Reference] .50
 20,001–40,000 16.2 0.90 0.39 (0.06-2.71) .34 1.27 0.25 (0.02-2.83) .26
 40,001–60,000 21.3 1.80 0.26 (0.04-1.85) .18 0.00 1.07 (0.12-9.41) .95
 ≥60,001 43.1 0.75 0.42 (0.06-2.95) .39 0.08 1.38 (0.14-13.33) .78
Maternal education
 High school 15.3 3.28 1 [Reference] .35 3.12 1 [Reference] .37
 Some college education 35.6 2.80 7.05 (0.72-69.29) .09 0.08 1.39 (0.13-14.77) .78
 4-year college degree 22.2 2.53 8.92 (0.60-132.3) .11 0.20 0.52 (0.03-9.07) .65
 ≥Graduate education 26.9 2.92 9.92 (0.71-138.0) .09 0.67 2.87 (0.23-35.56) .41
Major depressive disorder 21.3 1.42 2.19 (0.60-7.94) .23 0.09 1.23 (0.32-4.65) .76
Anxiety disorder 21.3 0.07 0.82 (0.19-3.61) .79 2.04 0.25 (0.04-1.68) .15
Conduct disorder 11.6 3.81 5.24 (0.99-22.67) .05 1.05 2.99 (0.37-24.34) .31
Oppositional defiant disorder 22.7 4.57 4.18 (1.13-15.51) .03 13.90 15.40 (3.66-64.79) <.001
No. of stressful life events, mean (SD) 8.95 (4.51) 0.01 1.01 (0.88-1.16) .92 0.19 0.97 (0.85-1.11) .66
Family history of disruptive disorder, mean (SD), % 5.41 (11.32) 7.51 1.05 (1.02-1.09) .01 0.02 0.99 (0.92-1.07) .89
Family history of MDD/anxiety disorder, mean (SD), % 14.18 (14.63) 0.04 1.01 (0.96-1.05) .84 1.82 1.03 (0.99-1.07) .18
Family history of bipolar disorder, mean (SD), % 2.57 (5.12) 6.89 1.12 (1.03-1.22) .01 4.97 1.12 (1.01-1.23) .03
Preschool ADHD 13.4 10.45 10.06 (2.48-40.80) .001 8.00 8.05 (1.90-34.19) .01
Table 4b.

Logistic Regression of 12 and 24 month follow-up ADHD Controlling for Risk Factorsa

 
T5ADHD as response variable
Variables entered into modelb Wald χ2 OR (95% CI) p-value
Child's age, mean (SD) mo 1.15 1.05 (0.96-1.14) .28
Sex (M = 1/F = 0), Male 0.89 0.49 (0.11-2.18) .35
Family income, $
 ≤20,000 2.53 1 [Reference] .47
 20,001–40,000 0.17 0.55 (0.03-9.50) .68
 40,001–60,000 0.86 3.37 (0.26-43.57) .35
 ≥60,001 0.82 3.76 (0.21-65.98) .37
Maternal education
 High school 2.63 1 [Reference] .45
 Some college education 0.44 0.39 (0.03-6.20) .51
 4-year college degree 1.33 0.13 (0.00-4.18) .25
 ≥Graduate education 0.03 0.75 (0.04-14.71) .85
Major depressive disorder 0.08 0.80 (0.16-3.94) .78
Anxiety disorder 1.75 0.18 (0.01-2.32) .19
Conduct disorder 0.08 1.53 (0.08-29.82) .78
Oppositional defiant disorder 11.42 16.85 (3.27-86.72) .001
No. of stressful life events, mean (SD) 0.92 0.92 (0.78-1.09) .34
Family history of disruptive disorder, mean (SD), % 0.31 0.98 (0.92-1.05) .58
Family history of MDD/anxiety disorder, mean (SD), % 1.61 1.03 (0.98-1.08) .20
Family history of bipolar disorder, mean (SD), % 0.93 1.06 (0.94-1.19) .33
T3 ADHD 19.80 85.57 (12.06-607.4) <.001

CI=confidence interval; OR=odds ratio.

a

All three models included a total of 216 subjects with valid data for all 13 variables. Model 1 χ217 = 65.82, p < .001, Model 2 χ217 = 55.73, p < .001, Model 3 χ217 = 75.64, p < .001.

b

All variables were recorded at baseline, except for stressful life events, which were the total events the child had encountered both prior to and during study periods, and T3ADHD is the diagnosis at 12 month follow-up.

Severity of ADHD was measured using number of symptoms met per DSMIV and was found to predict later wave ADHD; however, there were no significant clinical differences in use of summed symptoms vs. categorical diagnosis to predict later ADHD diagnosis. The same logistic regression models were conducted using baseline ADHD severity scores to predict T3 and T5 ADHD, using T3 ADHD severity scores to predict T5 ADHD. Baseline ADHD severity score (Wald χ2=12.89, OR: 1.33, 95% CI 1.14 – 1.56, p<.001), family history of disruptive disorders (Wald χ2=4.81, OR: 1.04, 95% CI 1.00 – 1.08, p=.028) and family history of bipolar disorder (Wald χ2=4.71, OR: 1.11, 95% CI 1.01 – 1.21, p=.030) were 3 significant factors for predicting T3 ADHD; baseline ADHD severity score (Wald χ2=12.04, OR: 1.31, 95% CI 1.13 – 1.53, p=.001) and baseline ODD (Wald χ2=11.89, OR: 14.07, 95% CI 3.13 – 63.22, p=.001) were significant factors for predicting T5 ADHD; T3 ADHD severity score (Wald χ2=16.36, OR: 1.69, 95% CI 1.31 – 2.17, p<.001) and baseline ODD (Wald χ2=9.84, OR: 20.83, 95% CI 3.12 – 138.8, p=.002) were significant factors for predicting T5 ADHD.

Differences between Preschoolers with ADHD who Recovered vs. had Stable Course at School Age

A subsample of n=27 subjects (n=17 from T1 and n=10 from T3) had a diagnosis of ADHD at preschool age (younger than 71 months) and who also had data available for school age ADHD status (n=16 had school age ADHD, n=11 did not). Cross tabulation (χ2 tests) and nonparametric methods (Mann Whitney U tests) were used to examine the differences between these two groups; factors examined included demographic variables, family history, preschool age life events, ADHD, ODD, and CD. Results indicated that compared with preschoolers with ADHD who had recovered from ADHD by school age, those who continued to have ADHD at school age had more maternal history of affective disorder, more preschool traumatic life stressors, and were more likely to have an ODD diagnosis at preschool age. Further multivariate analysis were used with all three factors entered in a logistic regression model; using stepwise selection, results indicated that preschool age ODD was significant (Wald χ2=4.90, OR: 17.62, 95% CI 1.39 – 223.81, p=.027), and preschool traumatic life stressors showed trend-level significance (Wald χ2=3.30, OR: 3.43, 95% CI 0.91 – 12.99, p=.069); mother's affective disorder was no longer significant.

Discussion

The present study utilized a sample of preschoolers with ADHD (ascertained as a comparison group for a longitudinal investigation of preschool depression) to investigate the longitudinal course of preschool ADHD. The current investigation examined the course of ADHD at three time points over two years, including at baseline (T1, ages 3–5.11), 12 months later (T3, ages 4–6.11), and 24 months later (T5, ages 5–7.11). The odds of retaining a diagnosis of ADHD were 9 times greater between T3 and T5 than between T1 and T3, providing strongest support for homotypic continuity of ADHD at later waves. These findings converge with Tandon et al (2009) in which performance based attentional deficits were more highly correlated with ADHD diagnoses made at later rather than earlier preschool waves. The findings also call into question the stability of early diagnoses in younger preschoolers using DSM-IV criteria as they stand and calls for caution in diagnosis of very young children supporting recent evidence for such caution in independent studies (Evans et al. 2010). Overall, study findings support the stability of diagnosis of ADHD from the preschool period into school age. In the current study, any interpretation of age itself as predictor of stability of ADHD must be made cautiously given that each study subject may present at a different age within the same wave of the study. For example one subject presented to the study for the first time at age 3.0, while another may have presented for the first time at age 5.0. However, greater stability was evident between later rather than earlier waves which suggests age may be a factor. Overall findings inform important risk factors and demonstrate stability of the ADHD diagnosis between the preschool and school age period.

A baseline diagnosis of ODD also emerged as an important risk factor for persistence of ADHD in our study. These findings are similar to those previously reported by Lavigne et al (2001), who showed a significant relationship between baseline ODD and subsequent diagnosis of ADHD. Furthermore, family history findings were noteworthy in the current investigation. The odds of a later diagnosis of ADHD increased in children with a family history of disruptive disorder (includes CD and/or ADHD; data for family history of ODD was not obtained). Interestingly, those preschoolers with family history of BP at baseline also had higher odds of T3 and T5 ADHD; however, when examining risk factors between T3 and T5 ADHD, family history of BP at baseline was no longer significant. The mechanism by which a family history of bipolar disorder confers risk for ADHD to preschoolers or school age children remains unclear (Birmaher et al. 2009; Chang et al. 2003). However, given the high rates of co-morbidity of these disorders in childhood reported by multiple research groups (DuPaul and Stoner 2003; Geller et al. 2000; Wilens et al. 2002), this risk relationship is an area worthy of future investigation.

Conversely, there were no significant associations between family history of MDD or Anxiety to later ADHD. Of note, older age at baseline was associated with an increased likelihood of a later diagnosis of ADHD at T3. This stands in contrast to longitudinal findings investigating ODD in which younger age at baseline was associated with later ADHD (Lavigne et al. 2001). Also in contrast to the importance of gender in the longitudinal course of ODD reported by Lavigne et al (Lavigne et al. 2001), no gender differences were associated with later diagnosis of ADHD in the present study.

There are several noteworthy limitations to the study. First, given the limited sample size of children with a diagnosis of ADHD in this study, the potential for a Type I error cannot be ruled out. ADHD diagnosis was made using parent informant only and did not include teacher reports. It is possible that the use of only parent informant in this study may have either inflated or deflated the number of children diagnosed with ADHD; however, parents were queried as to symptom presence in at least two settings. Next, the study sample was oversampled for depressive disorders, which limits the overall generalizability to the general population. Another limitation was the small sample size due to the low rate of retention of preschoolers with ADHD. However, when analyses were run with imputed data, no significant differences in the results were found. Another limitation of the study is that while the use of medications was examined and found to be non-significant, the study was not a controlled treatment study; therefore, any conclusion about the impact of treatment should be taken as speculative. Further, the impact of non-pharmacological interventions on diagnoses was not examined. Future investigations examining interventions such as parent management training and course of ADHD are certainly warranted.

In conclusion, while limited by a small sample size of preschoolers with ADHD (due to the main focus of the study being on mood disorders) the current study findings provide relatively greater support for homotypic 46.9% (15/32) rather than heterotypic 37.5% (12/32) continuity of baseline ADHD. This finding is supported by Lahey et al (2004); however, the positive predictive value of ADHD predicting later ADHD was lower in the current study than in that reported by Lahey. Several reasons for this discrepancy are possible. Unlike the study by Lahey et al. (2004), in which study subjects were ascertained for ADHD symptoms at baseline from psychiatric clinics, the current study utilized a community sample. Therefore the Lahey sample was likely to have had more severe baseline symptomotology than those in the current study, accounting for the higher stability rates and homotypic continuity than found in this study. Of note, current study findings also inform the likelihood of no disorder 15.6% (5/32) at follow-up (both T3 and T5) after diagnosis of baseline ADHD.

Clinical Implications

This investigation of continuity suggests that an early diagnosis of ADHD is clinically important from a general mental health risk standpoint. Findings also add to the literature that the subgroup of preschoolers with ADHD and one or more additional risk factors as outlined (co-morbid ODD, family history of disruptive disorders) would be the most important group to target for early intervention given the much greater likelihood for stable symptoms. This is underscored by the notion that earlier intervention in mental disorders may be more effective, although ADHD intervention should be specifically addressed (Greenhill et al. 2008; Jones et al. 2007; McGoey et al. 2002; Rappaport et al. 1998).

The study findings of relatively greater homotypic continuity of ADHD specifically in those older preschoolers with co-morbid diagnosis of ODD, may help elucidate a particular subset of preschoolers at increased risk for stable course into school age, and hence a group to target for early intervention. Study findings suggesting lesser stability of ADHD diagnoses made at earlier preschool ages are noteworthy and call for caution in diagnosis, as well as need for further investigations to inform clinicians and caregivers, particularly prior to any pharmacologic or other treatment interventions. Finally, a detailed family history of disruptive disorders and bipolar disorder may further inform likelihood of persistence of preschool ADHD. The recruitment of a larger sample of preschoolers with ADHD is now needed to better inform the longitudinal trajectory of this disorder. Further studies that elucidate the longitudinal course of preschool ADHD are also warranted to determine clinical course and outcomes into school age and early adolescence (Posner et al. 2007).

Disclosures

Drs. Tandon and Luby and Ms. Si have no conflicts of interest or financial ties to disclose.

References

  1. Barkley RA. ADHD in adults: Developmental course and outcome of children with ADHD, and ADHD in clinic-referred adults. In: Barkley RA, editor. Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, a Handbook for Diagnosis and Treatment. 3RD. New York: Guilford Press; 2006. pp. 248–253. [Google Scholar]
  2. Beitchman JH. Wekerle C. Hood J. Diagnostic continuity from preschool to middle childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1987;26:694–699. doi: 10.1097/00004583-198709000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Birmaher B. Axelson D. Monk K. Kalas C. Goldstein B. Hickey MB. Obreja M. Ehmann M. Iyengar S. Shamseddeen W. Kupfer D. Brent D. Lifetime psychiatric disorders in school-aged offspring of parents with bipolar disorder: The Pittsburgh Bipolar Offspring study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:287–296. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Campbell SB. Behavior problems in preschool children: A review of recent research. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1995;36:113–149. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01657.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Campbell SB. Ewing LJ. Breaux AM. Szumowski EK. Parent-referred problem three-year-olds: Follow-up at school entry. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1986;27:473–488. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1986.tb00635.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chang K. Steiner H. Dienes K. Adleman N. Ketter T. Bipolar offspring: A window into bipolar disorder evolution. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53:945–951. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00061-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. DuPaul GJ. McGoey KE. Eckert TL. VanBrakle J. Preschool children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Impairments in behavioral, social, and achool functioning. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:508–515. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200105000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. DuPaul GJ. Stoner G. Overview of ADHD. In: Dupaul GJ, editor; Stoner G, editor. ADHD in the Schools: Assessment and Intervention Strategies. 2nd. New York: Guilford Press; 2003. pp. 4–21. [Google Scholar]
  9. Egger H. Erkanli A. Keeler G. Potts E. Walter B. Angold A. Test-retest reliability of the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA) J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;45:538–549. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000205705.71194.b8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Egger HL. Angold A. Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: Presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:313–337. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Egger HL. Kondo D. Angold A. The epidemiology and diagnostic issues in preschool attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A review. Infants & Young Children: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Special Care Practices. 2006;19:109–122. [Google Scholar]
  12. Evans WN. Morrill MS. Parente ST. Measuring inappropriate medical diagnosis and treatment in survey data: The case of ADHD among school-age children. J Health Econ. 2010;29:657–673. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2010.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fischer M. Barkley RA. Fletcher KE. Smallish L. The adolescent outcome of hyperactive children: Predictors of psychiatric, academic, social, and emotional adjustment. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1993;32:324–332. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199303000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gadow KD. Nolan EE. Differences between preschool children with ODD, ADHD, and ODD+ADHD symptoms. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002;43:191–201. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gadow KD. Sprafkin J. Nolan EE. DSM-IV symptoms in community and clinic preschool children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:1383–1392. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200112000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Geller B. Zimerman B. Williams M. Bolhofner K. Craney JL. Delbello MP. Soutullo CA. Diagnostic characteristics of 93 cases of a prepubertal and early adolescent bipolar disorder phenotype by gender, puberty and comorbid attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2000;10:157–164. doi: 10.1089/10445460050167269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Greenhill LL. Posner K. Vaughan BS. Kratochvil CJ. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in preschool children. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2008;17:347–366. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2007.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Jones K. Daley D. Hutchings J. Bywater T. Eames C. Efficacy of the Incredible Years Basic parent training programme as an early intervention for children with conduct problems and ADHD. Child Care Health and Dev. 2007;33:749–756. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2007.00747.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kadesjo C. Kadesjo B. Hagglof B. Gillberg C. ADHD in Swedish 3- to 7-year-old children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:1021–1028. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200109000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kashani JH. Allan WD. Beck NC., Jr. Bledsoe Y. Reid JC. Dysthymic disorder in clinically referred preschool children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:1426–1433. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199710000-00025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kashani JH. Carlson GA. Seriously depressed preschoolers. Am J Psychiatry. 1987;144:348–350. doi: 10.1176/ajp.144.3.348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kashani JH. Holcomb WR. Orvaschel H. Depression and depressive symptoms in preschool children from the general population. Am J Psychiatry. 1986;143:1138–1343. doi: 10.1176/ajp.143.9.1138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Keenan K. Emotion dysregulation as a risk factor for child psychopathology. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. 2000;7:418–434. [Google Scholar]
  24. Keenan K. Wakschlag LS. Danis B. Hill C. Humphries M. Duax J. Donald R. Further evidence of the reliability and validity of DSM-IV ODD and CD in preschool children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46:457–468. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31803062d3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lahey BB. Hartung CM. Loney J. Pelham WE. Chronis AM. Lee SS. Are there sex differences in the predictive validity of DSM-IV ADHD among younger children? J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2007;36:113–126. doi: 10.1080/15374410701274066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lahey BB. Pelham WE. Loney J. Kipp H. Ehrhardt A. Lee SS. Willcutt EG. Hartung CM. Chronis A. Massetti G. Three-year predictive validity of DSM-IV attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children diagnosed at 4-6 years of age. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:2014–2020. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.11.2014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lahey BB. Pelham WE. Stein MA. Loney J. Trapani C. Nugent K. Kipp H. Schmidt E. Lee S. Cale M. Gold E. Hartung CM. Willcutt E. Baumann B. Validity of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder for younger children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;37:695–702. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199807000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lavigne JV. Arend R. Rosenbaum D. Binns HJ. Christoffel KK. Gibbons RD. Psychiatric disorders with onset in the preschool years: I. Stability of diagnoses. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;37:1246–1254. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199812000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lavigne JV. Arend R. Rosenbaum D. Binns HJ. Christoffel KK. Gibbons RD. Psychiatric disorders with onset in the preschool years: II. Correlates and predictors of stable case status. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;37:1255–1261. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199812000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lavigne JV. Cicchetti C. Gibbons RD. Binns HJ. Larsen L. DeVito C. Oppositional defiant disorder with onset in preschool years: Longitudinal stability and pathways to other disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:1393–1400. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200112000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lavigne JV. LeBailly SA. Hopkins J. Gouze KR. Binns HJ. The prevalence of ADHD, ODD, depression, and anxiety in a community sample of 4-year-olds. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2009;38:315–328. doi: 10.1080/15374410902851382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Luby J. Heffelfinger A. Koenig-McNaught A. Brown K. Spitznagel E. The preschool feelings checklist: A brief and sensitive screening measure for depression in young children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;43:708–717. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000121066.29744.08. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Luby J. Heffelfinger A. Mrakrotsky C. Hessler M. Brown K. Hildebrand T. Preschool major depressive disorder: preliminary validation for developmentally modified DSM-IV criteria. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41:928–937. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200208000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Luby J. Heffelfinger AK. Mrakotsky C. Brown KM. Hessler MJ. Wallis JM. Spitznagel EL. The clinical picture of depression in preschool children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;42:340–348. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200303000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Luby J. Morgan K. Characteristics of an infant/preschool psychiatric clinic sample: Implications for clinical assessment, nosology. Inf Mental Hlth J. 1997;18 [Google Scholar]
  36. Luby JL. Si X. Belden AC. Tandon M. Spitznagel E. Preschool depression: Homotypic continuity and course over 24 months. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:897–905. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Maxwell E. Manual for the FIGS. Clinical Neurogenetics Branch, Intramural Research Program, National Institute of Mental Health. 1992.
  38. McGee R. Partridge F. Williams S. Silva PA. A twelve-year follow-up of preschool hyperactive children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1991;30:224–232. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199103000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. McGoey KE. Eckert TL. DuPaul GJ. Early intervention for preschool-age children with ADHD: A literature review. J Emot Behav Disord. 2002;10:14–28. [Google Scholar]
  40. Posner K. Melvin GA. Murray DW. Gugga SS. Fisher P. Skrobala A. Cunningham C. Vitiello B. Abikoff HB. Ghuman JK. Kollins S. Wigal SB. Wigal T. McCracken JT. McGough JJ. Kastelic E. Boorady R. Davies M. Chuang SZ. Swanson JM. Riddle MA. Greenhill LL. Clinical presentation of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in preschool children: The Preschoolers with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Treatment Study (PATS) J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2007;17:547–562. doi: 10.1089/cap.2007.0075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Rappaport GC. Ornoy A. Tenenbaum A. Is early intervention effective in preventing ADHD? Israel, J Psychiatry Relat Sci. 1998;35:271–279. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Robins E. Guze SB. Establishment of diagnostic validity in psychiatric illness: Its application to schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1970;126:983–987. doi: 10.1176/ajp.126.7.983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Scheeringa MS. Zeanah CH. Myers L. Putnam FW. Predictive validity in a prospective follow-up of PTSD in preschool children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44:899–906. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000169013.81536.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Schwebel DC. Speltz ML. Jones K. Bardina P. Unintentional injury in preschool boys with and without early onset of disruptive behavior. J Pediatr Psychol. 2002;27:727–737. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/27.8.727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Smidts DP. Oosterlaan J. How common are symptoms of ADHD in typically developing preschoolers? A study on prevalence rates and prenatal/demographic risk factors. Cortex. 2007;43:710–717. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70500-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Spence SH. Rapee R. McDonald C. Ingram M. The structure of anxiety symptoms among preschoolers. Behav Res Ther. 2001;39:1293–1316. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(00)00098-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Stalets MM. Luby JL. Preschool depression. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2006;15:899–917. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2006.05.011. viii-ix, [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Tandon M. Si X. Belden A. Luby J. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in preschool children: An investigation of validation based on visual attention performance. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2009;19:137–146. doi: 10.1089/cap.2008.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Wakschlag LS. Briggs-Gowan MJ. Carter AS. Hill C. Danis B. Keenan K. McCarthy KJ. Leventhal BL. A developmental framework for distinguishing disruptive behavior from normative misbehavior in preschool children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48:976–987. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01786.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Wilens TE. Biederman J. Brown S. Monuteaux M. Prince J. Spencer TJ. Patterns of psychopathology and dysfunction in clinically referred preschoolers. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2002;23:S31–S36. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200202001-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Wilens TE. Biederman J. Brown S. Tanguay S. Monuteaux MC. Blake C. Spencer TJ. Psychiatric comorbidity and functioning in clinically referred preschool children and school-age youths with ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41:262–268. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200203000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology are provided here courtesy of Mary Ann Liebert, Inc.

RESOURCES