Abstract
Functional connectivity of the brain has been studied by analyzing correlation differences in time courses among seed voxels or regions with other voxels of the brain in patients versus controls. The spatial extent of strongly temporally coherent brain regions co-activated during rest has also been examined using independent component analysis (ICA). However, the weaker temporal relationships among ICA component time courses, which we operationally define as a measure of functional network connectivity (FNC), have not yet been studied. In this study, we propose an approach for evaluating FNC and apply it to functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data collected from persons with schizophrenia and healthy controls. We examined the connectivity and latency among ICA component time courses to test the hypothesis that patients with schizophrenia would show increased functional connectivity and increased lag among resting state networks compared to controls. Resting state fMRI data were collected and the inter-relationships among seven selected resting state networks (identified using group ICA) were evaluated by correlating each subject’s ICA time courses with one another. Patients showed higher correlation than controls among most of the dominant resting state networks. Patients also had slightly more variability in functional connectivity than controls. We present a novel approach for quantifying functional connectivity among brain networks identified with spatial ICA. Significant differences between patient and control connectivity in different networks were revealed possibly reflecting deficiencies in cortical processing in patients.
INTRODUCTION
Developments in functional imaging in the past two decades have allowed for significant advances in our understanding of the complex relationships and interactions among distributed brain regions underlying cognition. An active area of neuroimaging research involves examining the “functional connectivity” of spatially remote brain regions. Functional connectivity analyses allow the characterization of inter-regional neural interactions during particular cognitive or motor tasks or merely from spontaneous activity during rest. Previous functional connectivity analysis approaches have relied on choosing individual seed voxels and subsequently constructing cross-correlation maps of other voxels with respect to the chosen seed voxels (Biswal, et al. 1995; Biswal, et al. 1997; Cordes, et al. 2002; Cordes, et al. 2000; Lowe, et al. 1998).
Another useful method to examine functional connectivity is independent component analysis (ICA) (Calhoun, et al. 2001b; Esposito, et al. 2005; Garrity, et al. 2007; McKeown, et al. 1998), particularly as applied to “resting state” scans, which are relatively easy to obtain and do not suffer from performance confounds in cognitively-impaired patient groups (Beckmann, et al. 2005; Greicius, et al. 2004; Kivinienri, et al. 2001). ICA is a method for recovering underlying signals from linear mixtures of these signals and draws upon higher-order signal statistics to determine a set of “components” that are maximally independent of each other (Calhoun and Adali 2006). The use of ICA in these studies effectively finds and characterizes functional networks in data collected during the performance of a task as well as in resting state fMRI data. For instance, van de Ven, et al., used spatial differences across ICA-generated components’ intensity to determine functional connectivity levels (Van de Ven, et al. 2004). ICA has been found to be useful and able to capture the complex nature of fMRI time courses while also producing consistent spatial components (Turner and Twieg 2005). Rakapakse et al., performed analyses using structural equation modeling to find connectivity between regions identified using spatial ICA in healthy individuals while performing a task (Rajapakse, et al. 2006).
While these techniques are effective for analyzing dysfunctional integration of activations in various regions’ time series in brains, to date there has been no study of group differences in the temporal relationship among spatial components. Within a given component, the regions are by definition strongly temporally coherent due to the ICA assumption of linear mixing. In this paper we focus not upon these strongly coherent time courses, rather we consider weaker dependencies among components. In spatial ICA the images are maximally independent, but the time courses are not independent and can exhibit considerable temporal dependencies. These temporal dependencies among components are significant, but not as large as those between regions within a component (were this the case they would likely have been included within a single component) (Calhoun, et al. 2003). A technique to determine functional temporal connectivity among components and to evaluate group differences in these relatively weaker connections is proposed in this paper. In this paper, we define functional network connectivity (FNC) as the temporal dependency among the ICA components. In contrast to connectivity studies which focus upon the correlation between a single seed region of interest and the other brain regions, we focus instead upon the temporal connectivity among functional networks (components) estimated from ICA.
In order to show the practical relevance of our technique, we apply the methods described in this paper to compare FNC in patients with schizophrenia versus healthy controls. Schizophrenia is a chronic, disabling mental disorder that is diagnosed on the basis of a constellation of psychiatric symptoms and longitudinal course (APA 2000). The disease impairs multiple cognitive domains including memory, attention and executive function (Heinrichs and Zakzanis 1998). Although the causes and mechanisms of schizophrenia are still unclear, a hypothesis of neural network ‘disconnection’ has been proposed. This proposal assumes that schizophrenia arises from dysfunctional integration of a distributed network of brain regions (Friston and Frith 1995) or a misconnection syndrome of neural circuitry leading to an impairment in the smooth coordination of mental processes, sometimes described as “cognitive dysmetria” (Andreasen, et al. 1999).
Many researchers have examined the possibility of ’disconnection’ in psychiatric groups by analyzing brain function with functional connectivity methods (Bokde, et al. 2006; Friston 1995; Friston and Frith 1995; Frith, et al. 1995; Herbster, et al. 1996; Josin and Liddle 2001; Liang, et al. 2006; Liddle, et al. 1992; Mikula and Niebur 2006). For example, in a sample of patients with schizophrenia, Liang, et al., found disrupted functional integration of widespread brain areas, including a decreased connectivity among insula, temporal lobe, prefrontal lobe and corpus striatum and an increased connectivity between the cerebellum and other brain areas, during resting-state by analyzing correlations between brain regions (Liang, et al. 2006). Similarly, Meyer-Lindenberg, et al., reported pronounced disruptions of distributed cooperative activity in frontotemporal interactions in schizophrenia in selected regions of interest in positron emission tomography (PET) brain scans on working memory task (Meyer-Lindenberg, et al. 2001). Other task-related studies reported a lack of interaction between right anterior cingulate and other brain regions (Boksman, et al. 2005), disrupted integration between medial superior frontal gyrus and both the anterior cingulate and the cerebellum (Honey, et al. 2005), as well as reduced functional connectivity in frontotemporal regions of subjects with schizophrenia (Lawrie, et al. 2002). Although these studies help identify problems with typical functional integration among important brain regions, they do not examine patients to see if there is disruption in the relationship of activity within one large networks of brain regions with another. It follows that patients with schizophrenia may not only have deficits in the relationship of one brain region to another, but that their cognitive and behavioral deficits might be related to dysfunction of entire networks of regions failing to properly communicate with one another.
In the study, we focused on examining FNC differences between a group of patients with schizophrenia and a demographically-matched control sample. Based upon two of our recent studies showing less task-specific activation (Calhoun, et al. 2006a) and more high frequency fluctuations in the default mode (Garrity, et al. 2007) in schizophrenia patients versus healthy controls, we hypothesized that patients would show increased connectivity among ICA component networks, possibly reflecting less specialized cognitive processing. Also, based on a prior study showing delayed hemodynamic brain activity in schizophrenia (Ford, et al. 2005), we predicted that patients would show increased latency of peak hemodynamic signal change in each network compared to activity changes in healthy controls. Although we do not directly test the disconnection hypothesis in this work, the proposed methods would permit such tests using the proper experimental paradigm. Indeed, the results of this analysis are consistent with the existence of misconnected neural circuitry in subjects with schizophrenia, through findings that likely depict an increased dependence among a wider array of less efficient brain regions in schizophrenia, a possible manifestation of a generalized cognitive deficit.
Our general approach can be outlined as follows: we first use ICA to extract resting state network time courses in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls (Beckmann, et al. 2005; Garrity, et al. 2007). After additional data filtering for noise removal, the time series between all pair-wise combinations of these networks are further analyzed to determine the maximal lagged correlation between networks (this was done both to mitigate the impact of latency difference upon the correlation and also to provide the possibility of evaluating the latency differences). Significant correlations within groups, differences in correlation between groups, and differences in lags between groups were computed. A resampling technique was used to validate the significance of the group differences. We also tested the robustness of the detected differences in connectivity by testing multiple independent sub-samples of patients and controls.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants and Paradigm description
Data from 29 patients with schizophrenia [age 38.36±11.26 years (19–58 years) and 25 healthy controls (age 38.62±11.127 years (20–59 years)] were drawn from IRB-approved studies, with written subject consents, at the Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center (ONRC). Prior to inclusion in the study, healthy participants were screened to ensure they were free from DSMIV Axis I or Axis II psychopathology [assessed using the SCID-IV (Spitzer, et al. 1996)] and also interviewed to determine that there was no history of psychosis in any first-degree relatives. Patients met DSM-IV criteria for schizophrenia on the basis of a structured clinical interview (SCID-IV) (First, et al. 1995) and review of the case file. The control group included 18 males and 7 females; the patient group consisted of 22 males and 7 females. Eighty percent of controls and 89.7% of patients were right-handed. The two groups were matched for mean age (p>0.811), proportion gender (p>0.747) and handedness (p>0.248). We selected slightly more participants in the patient group due to reported presence of greater variance in patterns in schizophrenic subjects with different symptom profiles (Liddle 1992).
Image Acquisition
FMRI scans were collected using a 3.0 Tesla Siemens Allegra scanner, equipped with 40 mT/m gradients and a standard quadrature head coil. The functional scan was acquired using gradient-echo echo-planar imaging with the following parameters (repeat time (TR) = 1.86s, echo time (TE) = 27 ms, field of view (FOV) = 24 cm, acquisition matrix = 64×64, flip angle (FA) = 70°, slice thickness (ST) = 3 mm, gap = 1mm). After excluding the first six dummy scans, during which magnetization steady state was being reached, 162 scans were used for analysis. During the scan, all subjects were instructed to rest quietly in the scanner, without sleeping. Post-scan questionnaire confirmed subjects’ alertness level during the scan.
Preprocessing
Data were preprocessed using the Statistical Parametric Mapping software package, SPM2 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). Data were motion corrected, spatially smoothed with a 10 mm3 full width half-maximum Gaussian kernel, spatially normalized into the standard Montreal Neurological Institute space, and then converted to the standard space of Talairach and Tournoux (Talairach and Tournoux 1988) using an algorithm developed by Matthew Brett (http://imaging.mrccbu.cam.ac.uk/imaging/MniTalairach).
Component Identification
Group spatial ICA was conducted for all 54 participants using the infomax algorithm (Bell and Sejnowski 1995). Two separate group spatial ICAs were also conducted on controls and patients to ensure that the resulting components had similar resting state fluctuations in the two groups as in the resulting components attained from all 54 participants combined. Data were decomposed into thirty components using the GIFT software (http://icatb.sourceforge.net/, version 1.3b). Dimension estimation, to determine the number of components, was performed using the minimum description length criteria, modified to account for spatial correlation (Li, et al. 2006). Single subject time courses and spatial maps were then computed (called back-reconstruction), during which the aggregate components and the results from data reduction are used to compute the individual subject components (Calhoun, et al. 2001a).
Component Selection
A systematic process was used to inspect and select the components of interest from the 30 estimated components (Stevens, et al. 2006). The association of each component’s spatial map with a priori probabilistic maps of gray matter, white matter, and cerebral spinal fluid within standardized brain space (MNI templates provided in SPM2) helped to identify those components whose patterns of correlated signal change were largely consisted of gray matter versus non-gray matter. Components with high correlation to a priori localized CSF or white matter, or with low correlation to gray matter, suggested that they may be artifactual rather than representing hemodynamic change. Correlation analysis indicated that no component corresponded to the spatial distribution of white matter. Visual inspection of discarded components suggested that they represented eye movements, head motion, or cardiac-induced pulsatile artifact at the base of the brain. Seven components were selected as of interest for further analysis.
Analysis in GIFT also produced time courses for the 7 selected components for each of the 54 subjects. Prior to computing correlations, component time courses were filtered through a band-pass Butterworth filter, with cut-off frequencies set at 0.0372 Hz and 0.372 Hz, the frequency range implicated in previous resting state analysis (Cordes, et al. 2001).
Correlation and lag computation for patients and controls
The ICA algorithm assumes that the time courses of cortical areas within one component are synchronous (Calhoun, et al. 2004). Though the components are spatially independent, significant temporal correlations can exist between them. We examined this possibility directly by computing a constrained maximal lagged correlation. Using this approach, we computed a correlation and a lag value for each subject.
The time courses from the seven components for all subjects were first interpolated to enable detection of sub TR hemodynamic delay differences in patients with schizophrenia (Calhoun, et al. 2000; Ford, et al. 2005). The maximal lagged correlation was then examined between all pair-wise combinations where the number of combinations of 7 components, taken 2 at a time results in 7!/(2!(7–2)!) = 21 possible combinations. In other words, let ρ represent correlation between two time courses, X̄ and Ȳ of dimension T×1 units, where T represents the number of time points in the time course. Let io represent the starting reference of the two original time courses, and Δi represent the non-integer change in time (seconds). Assume X̄ at initial reference point io (X̄io), and Ȳ circularly shifted Δi units from reference point of io (Ȳio+Δi), then ρio+Δi at this configuration of the two time courses can be calculated as follows:
The lag between time courses X̄io and Ȳio+Δi is Δi (commonly denoted as δ) in seconds. Therefore, vectors ρ̄ can be calculated between time courses X̄ and Ȳ, at each instance when Ȳ is circularly shifted Δi units from −5 to +5 seconds. The maximal correlation value from ρ̄ and corresponding lag, δρ̄max, was saved for time courses X̄ and Ȳ.
Correlation and lag values were calculated for all subjects and were later averaged for control and patient group separately, where correlation values represented the dependency of two (out of 7) resting state networks on each other. Using the Student’s t-test, statistically significant correlation combinations from the 21 possible combinations were extracted for patients and controls separately (p < 0.05), resulting in maps of functional network connectivity for each group. Lag values corresponding to the significant correlation combinations were also extracted for each group. These lag values represent the amount of delay between two correlated component time courses averaged across patients and averaged across controls. We predicted that patients would exhibit more significantly correlated functional networks than would controls where length represents the number of significantly correlated connections in each group.
Correlation and lag computation for group difference
Five statistically significant differences in correlations values for patients versus controls were identified using a two sample t-test. To control for multiple comparisons, amore conservative p-value of p<0.01 [corrected for multiple comparisons using the false discovery rate (Genovese, et al. 2002)] was used for significance cut-off. We again predicted that most of the significant connectivity found from group difference would consist of higher correlation for patients than controls (i.e. ρpatients > ρcontrols).
Furthermore, connectivity combinations with statistically significant (p<0.01) lag values,δ̄ρ̄max, were also identified using a two sample t-test of the difference between averaged control and patient lags. This test not only enabled us to visualize the sequence of hemodynamic activity between two components in patients versus controls but also revealed the amount of mean lag during activation from one component to the other for both groups. Due to the delayed brain activity in patients with schizophrenia found in a previous study (Ford, et al. 2005), we predicted that patients will show greater lag between significantly correlated components than controls (i.e. δρcontrol < δρpatient).
Connectivity Validation
Because we used a relatively large sample size of fifty-four subjects, we were able to validate the results by repeating the experiment on subsets of the data to determine whether the same types of connectivity were present. Thus, to test for consistency, the methods described above were repeated twenty times. In each of the twenty trials, functional connectivity was observed between fifteen randomly selected patients and fifteen random controls drawn from the full set of 54 subjects. Significant connectivity was recorded for each group during each trial including significant correlations within groups, differences in correlation between groups, and differences in lags between groups.
In addition, we used a resampling technique to determine whether the mean correlation in controls is significantly different than the mean correlation in patients. We randomly relabeled patients and controls and computed, the mean correlation difference between shuffled controls and patients group in order to build a null distribution. We then calculated a student t-test determined whether the result was significantly different than the mean correlation difference in actual controls and patients group.
RESULTS
Component Selection and Visualization
Figure 1 shows the seven components (A–G) selected for connectivity analysis.Table 1 summarizes the components selected, along with the regions of activation as well as the Brodmann areas (BA) in which activations occur. The results from the separate patient and control ICA analyses were similar to what is show in Figure 1 and indeed led to the same results as the full analysis.
Table 1 (a–g). Regions Activated in Controls and Patients during Resting State.
Region | Brodmann Area | Volume (cc) L/R |
Random Effects: max T (x,y,z) L/R |
---|---|---|---|
Component A: (Default Mode) | |||
Precuneus | 31;7;23;39;19 | 19.9/17.6 | 26.3(0,−46,31)/26.4(3,−46,31) |
Cingulate Gyrus | 31;23;24 | 8.7/5.5 | 25.4(−3,−43,31)/25.0(3,−42,35) |
Posterior Cingulate | 31;23;30;29;18 | 10.6/8.7 | 24.2(−7,−57,24)/22.2(7,−50,27) |
Cuneus | 7;30;18;19;23;31;17 | 6.2/4.5 | 22.2(−3,−66,32)/22.2(3,−63,32) |
Superior Temporal Gyrus | 39;22;13;38 | 8.2/5.4 | 19.9(−52,−56,28)/22.1(49,−60,28) |
Middle Temporal Gyrus | 39;19;22;21 | 4.7/6.1 | 17.1(−52,−60,24)/20.7(45,−60,28) |
Supramarginal Gyrus | 40;39 | 3.8/2.6 | 20.5(−52,−56,32)/20.1(52,−56,32) |
Angular Gyrus | 39;40 | 2.2/2.4 | 19.2(−52,−60,32)/19.8(52,−60,32) |
Inferior Parietal Lobule | 40;39;7 | 5.1/5.2 | 18.5(−52,−56,36)/19.4(45,−59,36) |
Parahippocampal Gyrus | 30;27;18;19;36;35 | 2.5/1.8 | 15.0(−14,−47,7)/14.9(14,−47,7) |
Superior Parietal Lobule | 7 | 0.8/2.7 | 8.6(−7,−62,53)/14.9(38,−66,45) |
Lingual Gyrus | 19;18;30 | 2.8/1.8 | 14.0(−14,−47,2)/12.6(10,−54,3) |
Medial Frontal Gyrus | 10;11;8;9;6 | 11.4/8.4 | 12.0(0,54,1)/11.7(3,54,1) |
Superior Frontal Gyrus | 10;9;8;11 | 8.1/6.4 | 10.7(−21,43,39)/11.0(7,54,−3) |
Superior Occipital Gyrus | 39;19 | 0.0/1.0 | NS/10.2(38,−73,29) |
Middle Frontal Gyrus | 8;9 | 1.5/1.3 | 8.8(−24,36,40)/7.1(38,19,40) |
Paracentral Lobule | 5;31;4;6 | 1.1/0.4 | 8.4(0,−38,48)/8.1(3,−38,48) |
Anterior Cingulate | 32;42 | 1.1/0.9 | 7.9(0,47,−2)/8.0(3,47,−2) |
Thalamus | 0.3/0.4 | 7.8(−10,−37,10)/6.7(7,−34,2) | |
Inferior Temporal Gyrus | 20;21 | 1.3/1.2 | 7.1(−59,−8,−19)/7.0(59,−8,−22) |
Fusiform Gyrus | 20 | 0.4/0.1 | 6.6(−59,−5,−26)/5.8(59,−15,−22) |
Insula | 13 | 0.5/0.0 | 6.3(−28,−40,19)/5.1(28,−40,19) |
Caudate | Caudate Tail | 0.4/0.0 | 6.1(−24,−40,19)/NS |
Component B: Parietal | |||
Superior Parietal Lobule | 7;40;5 | 11.6/10.0 | 25.8(−7,−65,57)/22.0(3,−65,57) |
Precuneus | 7;19;39;31 | 29.1/23.4 | 25.5(−7,−62,61)/23.6(3,−72,49) |
Postcentral Gyrus | 7;5;2;40;1;3 | 10.7/8.7 | 20.6(−7,−55,65)/20.1(3,−54,69) |
Paracentral Lobule | 5;31;6;4;7 | 6.0/3.5 | 18.5(0,−38,48)/16.8(3,−45,60) |
Cingulate Gyrus | 31;32;24;23 | 12.3/5.0 | 18.2(−7,−45,44)/14.4(3,−32,43) |
Inferior Parietal Lobule | 40;39;7 | 17.8/10.9 | 17.3(−45,−42,52)/13.8(42,−42,52) |
Cuneus | 19;7;18;31 | 3.6/3.3 | 13.7(−10,−66,32)/13.7(7,−76,37) |
Angular Gyrus | 39 | 1.7/0.1 | 13.1(−31,−59,36)/7.4(38,−73,33) |
Sub−Gyral | 7;31;40;39 | 0.6/0.9 | 11.5(−21,−52,56)/11.5(10,−32,43) |
Superior Occipital Gyrus | 19;39 | 1.0/0.8 | 11.0(−35,−77,33)/11.1(35,−77,33) |
Middle Frontal Gyrus | 9;10;46;6;11 | 8.1/0.9 | 10.6(−38,46,27)/6.6(38,42,27) |
Medial Frontal Gyrus | 6;9;32;8;25 | 1.4/0.1 | 10.4(−7,−21,47)/5.8(7,−21,47) |
Middle Temporal Gyrus | 39;19 | 0.9/0.1 | 9.7(−38,−73,29)/5.8(38,−80,25) |
Superior Frontal Gyrus | 10;9;11 | 3.8/0.9 | 9.5(−38,49,22)/6.6(38,39,27) |
Supramarginal Gyrus | 40 | 0.9/0.4 | 8.5(−55,−36,35)/7.4(38,−42,35) |
Anterior Cingulate | 32;24 | 1.3/0.3 | 8.2(−7,32,23)/6.7(3,22,24) |
Posterior Cingulate | 29;23;30 | 1.5/0.1 | 7.9(−7,−44,6)/5.5(3,−33,22) |
Superior Temporal Gyrus | 22;38 | 1.0/0.0 | 7.7(−55,13,−4)/NS |
Inferior Frontal Gyrus | 9;10;47;46;11;44 | 1.7/0.1 | 7.3(−55,18,24)/5.7(24,30,−20) |
Parahippocampal Gyrus | 30;36;34;35 | 0.4/0.2 | 7.1(−7,−40,6)/5.3(31,−32,−25) |
Uncus | 34;28 | 0.3/0.0 | 6.8(−17,6,−19)/NS |
Component C: Visual Cortical Areas | |||
Lingual Gyrus | 18;19;17;30 | 17.8/16.8 | 20.1(−7,−78,0)/23.4(7,−64,3) |
Cuneus | 30;18;23;17;19;7;31 | 21.6/20.9 | 20.8(−14,−67,16)/23.3(10,−64,7) |
Posterior Cingulate | 30;31;23;29;18 | 8.1/6.9 | 21.0(−14,−64,16)/22.6(10,−64,11) |
Precuneus | 31;23;19;18;7;39 | 12.7/11.9 | 19.6(−10,−67,20)/19.5(7,−70,20) |
Middle Occipital Gyrus | 18;19;37 | 13.9/12.3 | 18.2(−14,−88,17)/18.1(21,−88,13) |
Inferior Occipital Gyrus | 18;17;19 | 3.8/3.9 | 12.5(−21,−89,−7)/16.8(21,−85,−3) |
Parahippocampal Gyrus | 19;18;30;37;36;27 | 5.7/3.3 | 16.1(−21,−58,−5)/13.9(21,−58,−1) |
Fusiform Gyrus | 19;18;37;20 | 10.4/6.0 | 15.8(−24,−61,−4)/14.7(24,−65,−4) |
Superior Occipital Gyrus | 19;39 | 1.3/1.0 | 14.5(−28,−80,25)/12.0(31,−80,25) |
Middle Temporal Gyrus | 39;19;37;22;21 | 8.6/8.9 | 14.2(−42,−74,12)/12.0(38,−74,16) |
Sub−Gyral | 19;39 | 0.4/0.2 | 10.0(−14,−44,−5)/13.2(17,−55,−5) |
Inferior Temporal Gyrus | 37;19;20 | 1.1/1.0 | 10.4(−49,−68,0)/9.5(42,−71,0) |
Cingulate Gyrus | 31 | 0.4/0.2 | 10.1(0,−60,24)/9.3(3,−60,28) |
Superior Temporal Gyrus | 22;41;39;21;42;29;13 | 6.0/9.1 | 8.9(−55,−27,5)/9.7(49,−34,6) |
Transverse Temporal Gyrus | 41;42 | 0.3/1.2 | 6.3(−49,−27,10)/9.5(45,−30,10) |
Superior Parietal Lobule | 7 | 0.3/0.3 | 7.6(−24,−73,45)/7.8(24,−73,45) |
Insula | 29;13;22;40 | 0.0/0.5 | NS/7.8(45,−27,14) |
Precentral Gyrus | 6;4 | 0.0/0.9 | NS/5.8(59,−5,33) |
Component D: Frontal Temporal Parietal | |||
Superior Parietal Lobule | 7;40 | 8.2/10.2 | 14.1(−42,−59,53)/16.8(38,−59,53) |
Inferior Parietal Lobule | 40;7;39 | 12.3/14.8 | 13.4(−42,−55,53)/15.7(45,−55,53) |
Precuneus | 7;19;39 | 5.7/9.9 | 11.9(−42,−69,41)/15.3(28,−69,49) |
Angular Gyrus | 39;40 | 1.9/2.1 | 9.5(−45,−66,36)/11.9(45,−63,36) |
Postcentral Gyrus | 5;40;2;1;3;7 | 1.9/4.5 | 9.8(−59,−38,52)/10.8(42,−45,60) |
Middle Frontal Gyrus | 11;10;46;47;9;6;8 | 9.7/20.3 | 10.1(−38,54,−14)/10.7(45,40,−17) |
Sub-Gyral | 39;10;37;40 | 0.0/0.4 | NS/10.4(31,−59,40) |
Inferior Frontal Gyrus | 47;46;10;44;9;45;11 | 1.5/8.6 | 9.4(−45,37,−17)/9.4(52,38,2) |
Superior Frontal Gyrus | 8;11;10;9;6 | 1.8/3.1 | 7.4(−35,50,−14)/9.2(42,16,49) |
Middle Temporal Gyrus | 39;20;21;37 | 2.2/4.3 | 8.6(−62,−45,−13)/8.9(49,−60,32) |
Supramarginal Gyrus | 40;39 | 0.6/1.1 | 7.6(−55,−56,36)/8.9(52,−49,36) |
Cuneus | 19;7 | 0.0/0.5 | 5.2(−28,−83,37)/8.7(28,−83,37) |
Superior Occipital Gyrus | 19;39 | 0.2/0.6 | 6.6(−38,−77,33)/8.6(35,−77,33) |
Superior Temporal Gyrus | 39;38 | 0.3/0.9 | 5.8(−55,−63,28)/8.3(49,−56,32) |
Inferior Temporal Gyrus | 20;37 | 1.1/1.5 | 7.9(−59,−48,−13)/7.6(62,−51,−9) |
Precentral Gyrus | 9;4;6 | 0.0/0.6 | NS/7.4(45,19,36) |
Fusiform Gyrus | 37 | 0.1/0.3 | 5.7(−55,−52,−13)/6.2(55,−48,−17) |
Medial Frontal Gyrus | 8;6 | 0.2/0.1 | 6.0(0,33,40)/5.6(3,33,40) |
Component E: Frontal Parietal Subcortical | |||
Inferior Parietal Lobule | 40;39;7 | 15.2/6.0 | 14.5(−49,−49,40)/9.7(45,−52,44) |
Insula | 13;47 | 1.5/0.4 | 13.6(−31,17,−5)/9.1(31,17,−5) |
Middle Frontal Gyrus | 10;46;11;6;8;9;47 | 39.4/6.0 | 13.5(−35,51,2)/9.2(35,47,−6) |
Inferior Frontal Gyrus | 47;46;10,45;13;9;44 | 15.6/1.5 | 13.0(−31,20,−5)/9.2(38,48,2) |
Supramarginal Gyrus | 40 | 4.0/1.1 | 12.7(−42,−46,35)/8.3(38,−46,35) |
Superior Frontal Gyrus | 10;8;6,9;11 | 19.9/2.0 | 12.7(−31,54,−3)/8.3(28,51,−6) |
Sub-Gyral | 47;40;10;39;8;37;9;32 | 1.0/0.6 | 12.6(−35,48,2)/9.4(35,48,2) |
Angular Gyrus | 39 | 1.4/0.3 | 11.5(−38,−59,40)/6.5(35,−59,36) |
Precentral Gyrus | 9;6;44 | 1.9/0.0 | 11.4(−42,19,40)/NS |
Extra-Nuclear | 47;13 | 0.5/0.4 | 11.3(−31,17,−8)/8.7(31,17,−8) |
Precuneus | 39;19;7;31 | 5.3/1.7 | 10.8(−38,−63,36)/7.0(31,−63,40) |
Claustrum | 1.3/0.6 | 10.7(−28,17,−1)/7.7(28,13,−4) | |
Superior Parietal Lobule | 7;40 | 3.9/1.9 | 10.7(−38,−55,48)/9.5(38,−59,49) |
Superior Temporal Gyrus | 39;22 | 1.2/0.1 | 9.7(−38,−56,32)/6.0(31,−53,32) |
Medial Frontal Gyrus | 9;6;10;8;32 | 6.9/0.2 | 9.2(−10,36,31)/6.6(24,44,6) |
Cingulate Gyrus | 31;32;23;24;6 | 5.4/0.7 | 8.8(−7,−25,34)/7.1(3,−26,30) |
Anterior Cingulate | 32;10;24;42;9 | 4.1/0.3 | 8.7(−14,35,23)/5.9(24,41,2) |
Postcentral Gyrus | 2;1;40 | 0.6/0.0 | 8.5(−55,−29,39)/NS |
Middle Temporal Gyrus | 39;37;21;20;22 | 3.9/0.2 | 8.3(−45,−60,32)/5.2(55,−51,−5) |
Lingual Gyrus | 18 | 0.3/0.7 | 5.6(−3,−83,−19)/7.7(10,−79,−19) |
Lentiform Nucleus | 1.6/1.3 | 7.6(−24,17,−1)/7.6(21,17,−8) | |
Inferior Temporal Gyrus | 20;37 | 0.9/0.0 | 6.6(−49,−28,−10)/NS |
Posterior Cingulate | 23 | 0.2/0.0 | 6.5(−7,−29,26)/NS |
Thalamus | 0.1/0.4 | 5.4(−14,−6,9)/5.9(10,−7,0) | |
Caudate | 0.3/0.3 | 5.9(−14,14,−1)/5.9(10,7,0) | |
Fusiform Gyrus | 37;18;19 | 0.0/0.2 | 5.8(−45,−34,−6)/5.6(17,−83,−22) |
Component F: Frontal | |||
Superior Frontal Gyrus | 8;6;9;10 | 31.7/30.1 | 24.1(0,23,49)/21.6(3,23,49) |
Medial Frontal Gyrus | 8;6;9;10;32 | 18.7/17.4 | 23.2(0,26,48)/22.3(3,19,45) |
Cingulate Gyrus | 32;24;6;31;23 | 11.4/8.4 | 20.1(0,29,32)/21.2(3,29,32) |
Anterior Cingulate | 32;24;33;42 | 6.1/5.2 | 17.7(0,35,23)/17.8(3,35,23) |
Middle Frontal Gyrus | 9;8;6;10;46;11;47 | 27.2/29.5 | 14.2(−45,19,40)/16.6(38,26,36) |
Precentral Gyrus | 9;6;4;44 | 4.8/9.7 | 13.6(−45,19,36)/16.5(38,22,36) |
Sub-Gyral | 8;32;9;6;4;40 | 1.4/1.6 | 11.7(−14,23,44)/12.2(17,26,44) |
Paracentral Lobule | 31;6 | 1.0/0.5 | 11.4(0,−11,42)/8.8(3,−11,46) |
Inferior Frontal Gyrus | 9;45;44;47;46;6;13 | 6.8/11.5 | 10.1(−52,12,33)/11.3(49,18,24) |
Superior Temporal Gyrus | 38;22;39 | 0.0/1.1 | 5.5(−49,20,−12)/7.8(52,17,−8) |
Caudate | 2.7/1.9 | 7.6(−14,−3,17)/7.4(14,4,12) | |
Inferior Parietal Lobule | 40;39;7 | 0.0/4.2 | NS/7.4(45,−52,40) |
Thalamus | 1.2/1.2 | 7.0(−14,−3,13)/6.8(10,−3,13) | |
Postcentral Gyrus | 3;4 | 0.0/1.0 | NS/6.6(21,−28,55) |
Supramarginal Gyrus | 40 | 0.0/0.5 | NS/6.5(52,−49,36) |
Lentiform Nucleus | 0.3/0.2 | 6.5(−14,4,8)/5.7(14,0,8) | |
Insula | 47;13 | 0.0/0.3 | 6.5(−14,4,8)/5.7(14,0,8) |
Component G: Temporal | |||
Insula | 13;47;29;40;22;45 | 7.2/8.4 | 28.2(−42,10,−4)/20.1(45,0,0) |
Inferior Frontal Gyrus | 47;45;13;44;10;46;9 | 11.1/8.3 | 26.7(−42,13,−4)/16.8(42,13,−4) |
Superior Temporal Gyrus | 22;38;42;41;13;29;21 | 19.7/17.3 | 26.0(−49,10,−8)/22.6(52,0,0) |
Extra-Nuclear | 13;47 | 0.6/0.7 | 23.9(−42,10,−8)/16.7(42,6,−8) |
Claustrum | 3.4/4.7 | 20.8(−35,10,−1)/18.7(38,−7,−3) | |
Transverse Temporal Gyrus | 42;41 | 1.4/1.7 | 20.0(−59,−17,9)/18.7(59,−17,9) |
Postcentral Gyrus | 40;43;2;1;3 | 4.2/3.9 | 19.9(−62,−23,18)/18.3(62,−23,14) |
Precentral Gyrus | 6;44;13;43 | 3.9/4.5 | 17.8(−49,−10,9)/19.4(55,4,8) |
Sub-Gyral | 13;21;38 | 1.0/0.9 | 19.1(−42,3,−8)/15.9(42,0,−8) |
Middle Temporal Gyrus | 21;22;38;39 | 3.0/1.1 | 17.9(−59,0,−4)/15.2(59,−4,−4) |
Inferior Parietal Lobule | 40 | 2.2/2.6 | 17.3(−62,−23,22)/14.2(55,−33,22) |
Lentiform Nucleus | 2.3/1.8 | 12.6(−31,3,0)/11.4(31,−10,−3) | |
Cingulate Gyrus | 32;24 | 2.1/0.6 | 10.9(0,9,41)/9.7(3,9,41) |
Middle Frontal Gyrus | 47;11;46 | 0.5/0.3 | 10.7(−49,37,−2)/9.2(45,34,−5) |
Subcallosal Gyrus | 34 | 0.0/0.2 | NS/9.9(24,3,−11) |
Anterior Cingulate | 32;24;42 | 1.8/0.6 | 9.6(−3,32,19)/8.6(0,38,2) |
Medial Frontal Gyrus | 32;6 | 0.3/0.1 | 9.5(0,6,45)/8.2(3,6,45) |
Thalamus | 0.4/0.3 | 9.4(−7,−13,5)/9.1(3,−13,5) | |
Parahippocampal Gyrus | 0.0/0.4 | 8.9(−31,−1,−11)/9.1(31,−4,−11) | |
Caudate | 0.1/0.1 | 8.4(−10,4,4)/8.8(35,−17,−7) | |
Superior Frontal Gyrus | 6 | 0.2/0.0 | 8.5(−3,6,49)/NS |
Correlation and lag computation for Patients and Controls
Figure 2 shows a functional network connectivity (FNC) diagram for controls in which significantly correlated components are represented by an arrow. Figure 3 shows a similar connectivity diagram for patients. The direction of each arrow represents the lag between the two connected components. For example, in Figure 2, an arrow connects components B and D, representing that component B lags component D by certain time units. Both controls and patients show significant connectivity between components B and D, components E and F as well as between components B and G. Connectivity of components A and F is only found in patients, while the connectivity of components B and C is only found in controls. The direction of lag differs between patients and controls for components A and C as well as C and G. It is also noticed that controls show an additional connection with component B, while patients show an additional connection with component A.
Correlation and lag computation for group difference
Figure 4 represents the significant correlation found among group differences. In the figure, a dotted line represents connectivity in which patients had higher mean correlation than controls, while a solid line represents connectivity with higher correlation for controls. For example, components A and F, which show significant correlation difference, are connected with a dotted line to indicate that patients had greater mean correlation values than controls for this connection.
Figure 5 shows the significant lag among components for group differences. In the figure, dotted and solid lines are used to represent significant lag networks in patients and controls, respectively. The arrow in the lines represents the lag between the connecting components. For example, a dotted line between component D and F shows that in patients, component F becomes activated after the activation of component D.
Connectivity Validation
Twenty trials of the experiment were performed in which ten control and ten patients were randomly drawn a subjected to a t-test analysis. Figure 6 shows the number of times each of the 21 combination was selected as significant in the 20 trials for patients (p<0.05). Figure 7 shows the histogram of significant correlations for controls. For example, the connection between B and G was significant at many instances in both controls and patients. FNC with default mode (A) and various other components (D, E, and F) were mostly significant in patients, but not so much in controls. Overall, patients show more variance in the FNC combinations selected at each of the 20 trials than controls. For example, controls show 12 combinations to have occurred once or never; while in patients, many fewer combinations occurred once or less than 1 time (8).Similarly, in controls, four combinations occurred 19 or more (20) times, while inpatients, only 1 combination occurred at least 19, or greater, times.
Furthermore, the group difference analysis was also validated. Figure 8 shows the number of times the 5 connections in Figure 4 were selected as significant during the 20 trials. The numbers of times the connections occurred varied from 10 to 13. The validation confirmed (with 100% accuracy) that patients indeed showed greater correlation in 4 of the 5 connections, while controls showed greater correlation in connections B and G. The repetition of significant lag analysis in Figure 5 revealed consistent results in that components D–E and D–F appeared significant (p<0.01) 13 and 14 times, respectively, in the 20 trials. In the occurrences, component D lagged E in controls 92%, while component E lagged D in patients 85% of the time. Similarly, component D lagged F in controls 100%, while component F lagged D in patients 79% of the time.
Significance levels of group difference analysis were also calculated from the resampling approach. Table 2 shows resulting significant mean correlation difference between controls and patients. The columns report results from boot strapping technique in which 13 controls and patients are randomly shuffled from one group to the other and the means and standard deviations of the correlation differences are again computed. Finally, a one-sample t-test was performed to compare whether the mean difference correlation of shuffled groups was significantly different than the mean difference correlation of actual groups (p>0.01). Out of 21 combinations, the top 5 significantly different combinations (p<3.5E-08, uncorrected) are the same as the 5 significantly different combinations in Figure 4; in support of our finding that the patient and control group indeed has significantly different correlations for the 5 combinations selected.
Table 2. Correlation Validation Using Resampling Technique.
Component Combination |
Mean (all controls - patients) |
Standard Deviation (all controls- patients) |
Mean (mixed controls- patients) 20 runs |
Standard Deviation (Mixed controls- patients) 20 runs |
Lilliefors test (checks for normal distribution in 20 runs) [0 = accept Ho] |
t-test (compares means from 20 runs to single actual mean (column 2) [0 = accept Ho] |
p_values from t-test (uncorrected) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
a,e | −0.17912 | 0.06073 | 0.01217 | 0.03923 | 0 | 1 | 4.71E-11 |
a,f | −0.15501 | 0.00122 | 0.00425 | 0.04400 | 0 | 1 | 1.60E-10 |
b,g | 0.16687 | 0.11875 | −0.00257 | 0.03804 | 0 | 1 | 4.40E-09 |
c,e | −0.13941 | 0.03925 | 0.00919 | 0.02929 | 0 | 1 | 1.77E-09 |
c,f | −0.10562 | 0.03844 | −0.00765 | 0.01821 | 0 | 1 | 3.41E-08 |
DISCUSSION
ICA was successfully used to identify resting-state components in healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia and to identify differences in functional network connectivity among these components. We were able to identify several inter-connected networks present during resting and then examine temporal dependencies between them by computing the maximal lagged temporal correlation between the ICA time courses.
The identified resting state networks are included in Figure 1 (a–g). Figure 1a represents the "default-mode" network, which reflects an ensemble of cortical regions typically deactivated during demanding cognitive tasks in fMRI studies (Raichle, et al. 2001). Using functional connectivity, this network can be conceptualized and studied as a "stand-alone" function or system. Major regions in component A include the precuneus, anterior cingulate and posterior cingulate gyri. Additional regions include the superior and middle temporal and frontal gyri, the inferior parietal lobule and parahippocampal gyrus. Briefly, these regions are thought to be related to subject’s recognition, social communication, working memory and some auditory demands. The fluctuations in this network have been cited in numerous studies to increase during resting state and suspend during specific goal-directed behaviors (Garrity, et al. 2007; Raichle, et al. 2001).
Component B (in Figure 1b), in general, represents resting state hemodynamic activity fluctuations in the parietal regions. Along with the parietal lobule, significant regions include postcentral, anterior and posterior cingulate gyri, along with precuneus and middle frontal cortex. In general, the parietal lobule is used during visuo-spatial interaction. Regions present in component C include major visual cortical areas, such as the occipital gyrus along with activations in other areas such as lingual and fusiform gyri, cuneus and precuneus. Component D includes regions in frontal, temporal and parietal gyri, while component E shows covarying regions not only in frontal and temporal gyri, but also in subcortical regions, which include thalamus, caudate, and other basal ganglia regions. The function of the basal ganglia can be described as a brain relay station where commands, such as “stop reading”, get forwarded to the appropriate brain regions for processing. The dominant regions in component D are related to visual perceptual abnormalities reported in schizophrenia (Levy, et al. 2000).
The medial frontal gyrus dominates component F. Other covarying regions in component F include anterior cingulate and precentral gyrus as well as superior frontal gyrus. In general, these regions control the brain’s executive functions and have been cited to show abnormalities in schizophrenia brains (Chan, et al. 2006). Regions in component G include superior temporal and inferior frontal gyrus, responsible for auditory processing and language comprehension, along with insula which is the affective sensory region. Several studies cite frontal and temporal gyrus as partially responsible for auditory hallucinations found in schizophrenia (Gaser, et al. 2004).
Apart from just identifying neural networks present during ‘resting state’, the primary purpose of this paper was to examine functional network connectivity (FNC), or the temporal relationships among the identified components. Connectivity of the ‘default mode’ component (A) with other components was present more consistently in patients than controls. Although we did not explicitly examine connectivity strength in relation to measurements of active psychosis, it is reasonable to speculate that the increased default mode FNC in patients may be the results of distraction due to hallucinatory experiences and/or delusional preoccupations. Increased FNC of the default mode with other components in patients also could indicate greater dependency of brain regions in the default mode network on the function of other neural circuits (or vice versa) during resting state. We also observed directional differences in lag among components (i.e. C→A in patients, while A→C in controls).
There were other group differences in the relationship among component time courses. Although controls showed greater correlations than patients in one functional network (B–G), more networks existed in which patient correlations were significantly higher than controls. This trend of higher correlation in patients (i.e. ρcontrols < ρpatients) might be related to the attentional deficits in schizophrenia (Jorm, et al. 2005). Controls, on the other hand, may have a better ability to persist in a single mental state with patients more variable. This is also consistent with our recent findings showing more rapid fluctuations in the default mode network in patients verses controls (Garrity, et al. 2007). This idea is indirectly supported by the significant connection B→C in only controls because the brain regions in component B have been linked to focused attention and decision making (Paulus, et al. 2002). In addition, reduced fluctuations in frontal and parietal regions have been attributed as a possible concomitant of deficit symptoms in schizophrenia (Pearlson 2000). In contrast, brain regions identified in component C assist in visual image processing and recognition (Kim, et al. 2005). In particular, fusiform gyrus and other object/face recognition areas are abnormal in schizophrenia (Dickey, etal. 2003). Therefore, the absence of functional connection between B and C may hint higher order control deficits over sensory association process in patients that appears intact in healthy controls, however this would need to be directly tested in a future study.
Furthermore, the B–G connectivity shows higher correlation in controls than patients, which agrees with previous studies since these components are related to mental timekeeping and self-ordered behavior, commonly disturbed in schizophrenia (Ganzevles and Haenen 1995). Two of the 4 connections among components in which patients show higher correlation than controls relate to component E. This is also consistent with previous studies finding abnormal function of basal ganglia regions in schizophrenia minds show dysfunction and decreased activations in the basal ganglia (Gaser, et al.2004; Menon, et al. 2001). The increased connectivity of these networks with that depicted in component E may suggest a need for dependency on other components to make up for the lack of function in component E’s regions.
The validation of our results through analysis of subsets of data lends additional support to our various conclusions. The large-scale (54 subjects) and small-scale (15 subjects/trial for 20 trials) analysis of functional connectivity in controls showed very consistent results. The five connectivity networks found in the large-scale analysis of correlation group difference (A and E, A and F, B and G, C and E, C and F) also manifested significantly in the small-scale analysis of multiple trials; however, the smaller-scale model of the study showed patients connectivity to be more scattered, with more connections occurring between 3 and 18 times, than never or always occurring (0–2 or 19–20 times), while control connectivity was more consistent, with most occurrences either less than 2 times, or greater than 19 times. We had anticipated this greater variance in patterns in schizophrenic subjects on account of the previously reported differences in patterns of brain activity in patients with different symptom profiles (Liddle 1992). Regardless of the few differences in the large-scale and small-scale analysis of correlation, the consistent similarities suggest this technique may identify important differences between patients and controls, which may also be useful for classification (although further work is needed to test this). Furthermore, the repeatability of results between two diagnostic groups also implies robustness of results, regardless of gender, age or education. Further validation of our results using the resampling technique confirmed that the mean correlation differences between patients and controls were indeed significantly different.
Group difference results for lag calculations (Figure 5) were not analyzed in detail because the lag difference is only meaningful if the correlations between the processes are also significant (i.e. if the shape of the two time courses is similar). Since the combinations with significant lag do not also have significant correlations for either controls or patients (i.e. connections D–E and D–F are not present in Figure 2 or Figure 3), the lags differences are not reported.
In summary, we describe a general method for studying network connectivity, which is demonstrated in a study of patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. In agreement with our hypotheses, patients showed slightly greater number of significant correlation connectivity as controls in 54 subject analysis, as well as in individual runs for the 20 trial validation Furthermore, patients had higher correlation values than controls in most of the significant functional networks. We also found increased functional network connectivity in patients versus controls with respect to the default mode which has been described as involved in “internal” versus “external” focus (Raichle, et al. 2001). The increased connectivity of other networks to the default mode network may be related to hallucinations, although future work is needed to confirm this speculation.
An advantage of our approach to study the dependencies between functional networks was that it allowed us to examine weak, but significant, connectivity among strongly connected networks. We plan to explore the covariation between the symptom expression and the outcome measures within the schizophrenia group in future work. Although we used a correlational approach in this paper, one could also use other dependency measures, such as mutual information or Granger causality, to study the differences in FNC in healthy controls versus patients with schizophrenia. Furthermore, structural equation modeling techniques will be considered in future work to study multiple dependencies among networks. We also hope to apply this method to ICA results from EEG data and incorporate the newly derived FNC diagrams with the ones found through fMRI analysis (Calhoun, et al. 2006b; Liu and Calhoun 2007). Covariations between ICA components has previously been studied using EEG in the context of a visual task, although in this case short-term covariation was studied (Makeig, et al. 2004). In summary, we propose a general method for studying functional network connectivity (weak, but significant temporal dependencies between temporally coherent networks) and demonstrate it in a study of patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Our approach revealed several novel findings, and may help improve our understanding of schizophrenia as well as other mental disorders.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The authors would like to thank the research staff at the Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center for their help with data collection and also Drs. Jingyu Liu, Michal Assaf, Cyrus Eierud, and Andy Mayer for their insightful comments. This research was supported in part by the National Institutes of Health, under grants 1 R01 EB 000840, 1 R01 EB 005846 (to VDC) and NIMH, 2 RO1 MH43775 MERIT Award, 5 RO1 MH52886 and a NARSAD Distinguished Investigator Award (to GP).
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
REFERENCES
- Andreasen NC, Nopoulos P, O'Leary DS, Miller DD, Wassink T, Flaum M. Defining the phenotype of schizophrenia: cognitive dysmetria and its neural mechanisms. Biological Psychiatry. 1999;46(7):908–920. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00152-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. American Psychiatry Publishing Inc; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Beckmann CF, De Luca M, Devlin JT, Smith SM. Investigations into resting-state connectivity using Independent Component Analysis. Philos.Trans.R.Soc.Lond B Biol.Sci. 2005;360(1457):1001–1013. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell AJ, Sejnowski TJ. An information maximisation approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution. Neural Comput. 1995;7(6):1129–1159. doi: 10.1162/neco.1995.7.6.1129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn.Res.Med. 1995;34(4):537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biswal BB, Van Kylen J, Hyde JS. Simultaneous assessment of flow and BOLD signals in resting-state functional connectivity maps. NMR Biomed. 1997;10(4–5):165–170. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-1492(199706/08)10:4/5<165::aid-nbm454>3.0.co;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bokde AL, Lopez-Bayo P, Meindl T, Pechler S, Born C, Faltraco F, Teipel SJ, Moller HJ, Hampel H. Functional connectivity of the fusiform gyrus during a face-matching task in subjects with mild cognitive impairment. Brain. 2006;129(Pt 5):1113–1124. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boksman K, Theberge J, Williamson P, Drost DJ, Malla A, Densmore M, Takhar J, Pavlosky W, Menon RS, Neufeld RW. A 4.0-T fMRI study of brain connectivity during word fluency in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2005;75(2–3):247–263. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Adali T. 'Unmixing' Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging with Independent Component Analysis. IEEE Eng.in Medicine and Biology. 2006;25(2):79–90. doi: 10.1109/memb.2006.1607672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Adali T, Kiehl KA, Astur RS, Pekar JJ, Pearlson GD. A Method for Multi-task fMRI Data Fusion Applied to Schizophrenia. Hum.Brain Map. 2006a;27(7):598–610. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Adali T, Kraut M, Pearlson GD. A Weighted-Least Squares Algorithm for Estimation and Visualization of Relative Latencies in Event-Related functional MRI. Magn.Res.Med. 2000;44(6):947–954. doi: 10.1002/1522-2594(200012)44:6<947::aid-mrm17>3.0.co;2-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Adali T, Pearlson GD, Pekar JJ. A Method for Making Group Inferences from Functional MRI Data Using Independent Component Analysis. Hum.Brain Map. 2001a;14(3):140–151. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Adali T, Pearlson GD, Pekar JJ. Spatial and temporal independent component analysis of functional MRI data containing a pair of task-related waveforms. Hum Brain Mapp. 2001b;13(1):43–53. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Adali T, Pekar JJ, Pearlson GD. Latency (in)sensitive ICA: Group Independent Component Analysis of fMRI Data in the Temporal Frequency Domain. NeuroImage. 2003;20(3):1661–1669. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00411-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Kiehl KA, Liddle PF, Pearlson GD. Aberrant localization of synchronous hemodynamic activity in auditory cortex reliably characterizes schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55(8):842–849. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Pearlson GD, Kiehl KA. Neuronal Chronometry of Target Detection: Fusion of Hemodynamic and Event-related Potential Data. NeuroImage. 2006b;30(2):544–553. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan RC, Chen EY, Law CW. Specific executive dysfunction in patients with first-episode medication-naive schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2006;82(1):51–64. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cordes D, Haughton V, Carew JD, Arfanakis K, Maravilla K. Hierarchical clustering to measure connectivity in fMRI resting-state data. Magn Reson Imaging. 2002;20(4):305–317. doi: 10.1016/s0730-725x(02)00503-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cordes D, Haughton VM, Arfanakis K, Carew JD, Turski PA, Moritz CH, Quigley MA, Meyerand ME. Frequencies contributing to functional connectivity in the cerebral cortex in "resting-state" data. AJNR Am.J.Neuroradiol. 2001;22(7):1326–1333. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cordes D, Haughton VM, Arfanakis K, Wendt GJ, Turski PA, Moritz CH, Quigley MA, Meyerand ME. Mapping functionally related regions of brain with functional connectivity MR imaging. AJNR Am.J.Neuroradiol. 2000;21(9):1636–1644. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickey CC, McCarley RW, Voglmaier MM, Niznikiewicz MA, Seidman LJ, Frumin M, Toner S, Demeo S, Shenton ME. A MRI study of fusiform gyrus in schizotypal personality disorder. Schizophr Res. 2003;64(1):35–39. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(02)00529-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esposito F, Scarabino T, Hyvarinen A, Himberg J, Formisano E, Comani S, Tedeschi G, Goebel R, Seifritz E, Di SF. Independent component analysis of fMRI group studies by self-organizing clustering. Neuroimage. 2005;25(1):193–205. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders-patient edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0) New York: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Ford JM, Johnson MB, Whitfield SL, Faustman WO, Mathalon DH. Delayed hemodynamic responses in schizophrenia. Neuroimage. 2005;26(3):922–931. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ. Functional and Effective Connectivity in Neuroimaging: A sysnthesis. Human Brain Mapping. 1995;2:56–78. [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ, Frith CD. Schizophrenia: a disconnection syndrome? Clin.Neurosci. 1995;3(2):89–97. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frith CD, Friston KJ, Herold S, Silbersweig D, Fletcher P, Cahill C, Dolan RJ, Frackowiak RS, Liddle PF. Regional brain activity in chronic schizophrenic patients during the performance of a verbal fluency task. Br.J.Psychiatry. 1995;167(3):343–349. doi: 10.1192/bjp.167.3.343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ganzevles PG, Haenen MA. A preliminary study of externally and self-ordered task performance in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1995;16(1):67–71. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(94)00061-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garrity A, Pearlson GD, McKiernan K, Lloyd D, Kiehl KA, Calhoun VD. Aberrant 'default mode' functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Am.J.Psychiatry. 2007;164(3):450–457. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.3.450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaser C, Nenadic I, Volz HP, Buchel C, Sauer H. Neuroanatomy of "hearing voices": a frontotemporal brain structural abnormality associated with auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex. 2004;14(1):91–96. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Genovese CR, Lazar NA, Nichols T. Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage. 2002;15(4):870–878. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greicius MD, Srivastava G, Reiss AL, Menon V. Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer's disease from healthy aging: evidence from functional MRI. Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A. 2004;101(13):4637–4642. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308627101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinrichs RW, Zakzanis KK. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology. 1998;12(3):426–445. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.12.3.426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herbster AN, Nichols T, Wiseman MB, Mintun MA, DeKosky ST, Becker JT. Functional connectivity in auditory-verbal short-term memory in Alzheimer's disease. Neuroimage. 1996;4(2):67–77. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honey GD, Pomarol-Clotet E, Corlett PR, Honey RA, McKenna PJ, Bullmore ET. Functional dysconnectivity in schizophrenia associated with attentional modulation of motor function. Brain. 2005;128:2597–2611. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jorm AF, Mackinnon A, Christensen H, Griffiths KM. Structure of beliefs about the helpfulness of interventions for depression and schizophrenia. Results from a national survey of the Australian public. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2005;40(11):877–883. doi: 10.1007/s00127-005-0991-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Josin GM, Liddle PF. Neural network analysis of the pattern of functional connectivity between cerebral areas in schizophrenia. Biol.Cybern. 2001;84(2):117–122. doi: 10.1007/s004220000197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim D, Zemon V, Saperstein A, Butler PD, Javitt DC. Dysfunction of early-stage visual processing in schizophrenia: harmonic analysis. Schizophr Res. 2005;76(1):55–65. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kivinienri V, Kantola JH, Biswal BB, Jauhiainen J, Hyvarinen A, Tervonen O. Localization of the Resting State Vasomotor Fluctuation with FFT, Cross Correlation. Principal Component and Independent Component Analysis of fMRI Data. 2001:1708. [Google Scholar]
- Lawrie SM, Buechel C, Whalley HC, Frith CD, Friston KJ, Johnstone EC. Reduced frontotemporal functional connectivity in schizophrenia associated with auditory hallucinations. Biological Psychiatry. 2002;51(12):1008–1011. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01316-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy DL, Lajonchere CM, Dorogusker B, Min D, Lee S, Tartaglini A, Lieberman JA, Mendell NR. Quantitative characterization of eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2000;42(3):171–185. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(99)00122-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li Y, Adali T, Calhoun VD. A Novel Method to Improve Order Selection in Multivariate fMRI Data. Hum Brain Mapp. 2006 [Google Scholar]
- Liang M, Zhou Y, Jiang T, Liu Z, Tian L, Liu H, Hao Y. Widespread functional disconnectivity in schizophrenia with resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroreport. 2006;17(2):209–213. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000198434.06518.b8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liddle PF. Syndromes of schizophrenia on factor analysis. Br.J.Psychiatry. 1992;161:861. doi: 10.1192/bjp.161.6.861b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liddle PF, Friston KJ, Frith CD, Hirsch SR, Jones T, Frackowiak RS. Patterns of cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia. Br.J.Psychiatry. 1992;160:179–186. doi: 10.1192/bjp.160.2.179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu J, Calhoun VD. parallel independent component analysis for multimodal analysis: Application to fMRI and EEG DATA. 2007 [Google Scholar]
- Lowe MJ, Mock BJ, Sorenson JA. Functional connectivity in single and multislice echoplanar imaging using resting-state fluctuations. NeuroImage. 1998;7(2):119–132. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makeig S, Delorme A, Westerfield M, Jung TP, Townsend J, Courchesne E, Sejnowski TJ. Electroencephalographic brain dynamics following manually responded visual targets. PLoS Biol. 2004;2(6):e176. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKeown MJ, Makeig S, Brown GG, Jung TP, Kindermann SS, Bell AJ, Sejnowski TJ. Analysis of fMRI Data by Blind Separation Into Independent Spatial Components. Hum.Brain Map. 1998;6:160–188. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1998)6:3<160::AID-HBM5>3.0.CO;2-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menon V, Anagnoson RT, Glover GH, Pfefferbaum A. Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for disrupted basal ganglia function in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(4):646–649. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.4.646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer-Lindenberg A, Poline JB, Kohn PD, Holt JL, Egan MF, Weinberger DR, Berman KF. Evidence for abnormal cortical functional connectivity during working memory in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(11):1809–1817. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mikula S, Niebur E. A novel method for visualizing functional connectivity using principal component analysis. Int J Neurosci. 2006;116(4):419–429. doi: 10.1080/00207450500505761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulus MP, Hozack NE, Zauscher BE, Frank L, Brown GG, McDowell J, Braff DL. Parietal dysfunction is associated with increased outcome-related decision-making in schizophrenia patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51(12):995–1004. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01358-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearlson GD. Neurobiology of schizophrenia. Ann Neurol. 2000;48(4):556–566. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc.Natl.Acad.Sci.U.S.A. 2001;98(2):676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rajapakse JC, Tan CL, Zheng X, Mukhopadhyay S, Yang K. Exploratory analysis of brain connectivity with ICA. IEEE Eng Med.Biol.Mag. 2006;25(2):102–111. doi: 10.1109/memb.2006.1607674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spitzer RL, Williams JB, Gibbon M. Structured Clinical interview for DSM-IV: Non-patient edition (SCID-NP) New York: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Stevens MC, Kiehl KA, Pearlson G, Calhoun VD. Functional neural circuits for mental timekeeping. Hum Brain Mapp. 2006 doi: 10.1002/hbm.20285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Talairach J, Tournoux P. A co-planar sterotaxic atlas of a human brain. Thieme, Stuttgart: Georg Thieme Verlag; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Turner GH, Twieg DB. Study of temporal stationarity and spatial consistency of fMRI noise using independent component analysis. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2005;24(6):712–718. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2005.846852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van de Ven VG, Formisano E, Prvulovic D, Roeder CH, Linden DE. Functional connectivity as revealed by spatial independent component analysis of fMRI measurements during rest. Hum Brain Mapp. 2004;22(3):165–178. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]