Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychosom Med. 2011 Aug 26;73(7):604–611. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318229e1e0

Physical activity moderates stressor-induced rumination on cortisol reactivity

Eli Puterman 1, Aoife O’Donovan 2, Nancy E Adler 3, A Janet Tomiyama 4, Margaret Kemeny 5, Owen M Wolkowitz 6, Elissa Epel 7
PMCID: PMC3167008  NIHMSID: NIHMS312717  PMID: 21873586

Abstract

Objective

Physically active individuals have lower rates of morbidity and mortality, and recent evidence indicates that physical activity may be particularly beneficial to those experiencing chronic stress. The tendency to ruminate increases and prolongs physiological stress responses, including hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis responses as indexed by cortisol reactivity to stressful experiences. We examined the association between ruminating in response to a laboratory stressor task and HPA axis reactivity and recovery, and whether a physically active lifestyle moderates the associations between rumination and cortisol output trajectories.

Methods

Forty-six post-menopausal women underwent the Trier Social Stress Test while salivary cortisol was repeatedly measured. Twenty-five minutes after the end of the stressor, participants reported level of rumination in response to the stress.

Results

Findings indicate that physical activity moderated the initial rate (B = −.10, SE = .04, p < .05) and curvature (B = −.03, SE = .01, p = .06) of the relationship between rumination and log-transformed cortisol trajectory. Among sedentary participants, those who responded to the stressor with higher levels of rumination had a more rapid initial increase in cortisol (0.26 vs 0.21, p < .001), a later peak (56 vs. 39 minutes), and a delayed recovery (curvature −0.07 vs. −0.08, p < .001) compared to those with lower levels of rumination. In active participants, cortisol trajectories were equivalent, regardless of level of rumination.

Conclusions

In sum, individuals who maintain a physically active lifestyle may be protected against the effects of rumination on HPA axis reactivity to and recovery from acute stress.

Keywords: acute stress, rumination, physical activity, mixed modeling, cortisol reactivity


Numerous longitudinal and intervention studies have shown the direct benefits of exercise on health. For example, exercise and increased fitness reduce risk for health problems, including depression (15), cognitive impairment and dementia (610), cardiovascular disease (1113), diabetes (14; 15), and mortality (1618).

A growing body of evidence suggests that remaining physically active is particularly beneficial for those undergoing chronic stress (1923). Sources of chronic stress, such as caregiving and low socioeconomic status, are associated with increased risk for cardiovascular disease, metabolic syndrome, and other markers of disease (2427). These relationships are likely mediated at least partially through repeated and prolonged activation of the stress-responsive hypothalamic-pituitary adrenocortical (HPA) axis. Exposure to even a brief stressor can stimulate the HPA axis and produce an elevated cortisol level that can persist for approximately one hour after exposure (28). While adaptive in the short term, repeated and chronic stimulation of the HPA axis and release of cortisol can lead to allostatic load (i.e. the physiological effects of repeated or chronic exposure to stress that lead to pathophysiological state) and accelerated biological aging (2931).

Perseverative cognitions can prolong affective and physiological stress responses (3236). A hallmark of perseverative cognition is rumination, the tendency to perseverate on self-relevant negative content and emotion often directed at one’s past experiences (3740). A disposition to ruminate, as measured by trait rumination measures, appears to be higher in women (41), predicts the severity and number of major depressive episodes (42; 43), and is also considered a feature of some psychiatric disorders (42; 44). Trait rumination may unfold daily by increasing ruminative responses to daily stressors (45). Such responses may in turn lead to increases in daily negative affect (4547) and related increases in daily cortisol output (48). There is also evidence that laboratory-induced rumination affects physiological systems, prolonging stress-related stimulation of the HPA axis as evidenced by increased cortisol reactivity to the stressor (49). Therefore in this study, we examined the relationship between state rumination (i.e. rumination after a stressor) on concurrent cortisol responses.

Previous studies of exercise and fitness effects on the stress response have compared fit individuals (i.e. trained athletes and physically fit individuals) and unfit individuals and how they respond physiologically to laboratory induced acute mental stress. These have shown that fit versus unfit individuals have reduced HPA (5052), inflammatory (53), and cardiovascular reactivity and faster recovery (5457; 50). For example, Rimmele and colleagues (51) demonstrated that elite young adult male athletes had significantly lower cortisol and autonomic responses to the Trier Social Stress Task (TSST) compared to untrained men, and they additionally maintained positive mood and calmness in the face of stress. In another study, Traustadottir and colleagues (52) examined physiological stress reactivity in response to a laboratory stressor in unfit young as well as fit and unfit postmenopausal women. Fit post-menopausal women had similar cortisol responses to a laboratory stressor as the unfit younger women. On the other hand, unfit post-menopausal women had significantly greater output compared to their matched-age cohort and younger unfit equivalents. The findings from these studies suggest that being physically active appears to promote a healthier physiological response to stress in postmenopausal women.

In the present study, we examined the association between laboratory-induced rumination and cortisol reactivity and recovery in post-menopausal women. We additionally examined whether the relations between stress-induced rumination and cortisol varied as a function of activity level. We hypothesized that higher levels of rumination would be associated with faster initial cortisol reactivity and slower rate of recovery in response to the laboratory challenge tasks. In addition, we examined whether these associations would be more pronounced in sedentary compared to physically active participants.

Growth curve modeling was employed to capture reactivity and recovery from stress (58). Measuring cortisol output repeatedly permits the examination of the trajectory of cortisol in response to stress across time. Growth curve modeling can successfully attend to our outcomes of interest, such as whether rumination is related to participants’ baseline cortisol levels, initial rate of cortisol increase (i.e. reactivity) in response to stress induction, and recovery over time. From these models we can then extrapolate number of minutes the average person takes to peak in cortisol levels and return to baseline. Modeling the overall pattern of HPA responses to stress, including initial rise, time to peak and time to recovery from stress to basal state, addresses increasing calls in the literature for a more nuanced understanding of HPA axis response to stress (5961).

Methods

Post-menopausal women aged between 54 and 82 years were recruited through flyers and posters in the community and from service providers serving the elderly in the San Francisco Bay Area. Participants were part of a prospective study on caregiving and its effects on physical and psychological well-being that began in May 2005. All data presented are from participants’ baseline visit. Women included both healthy women who were providing at least 4 hours of care to a relative with dementia per day and who reported high levels of perceived stress and age-matched non-caregiver controls reporting low levels of daily stress. Exclusion criteria included the presence of major medical conditions such as heart disease, cancer, or diabetes, use of medications containing agents known to affect stress hormone levels, and regular smoking. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of California, San Francisco. Written, informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Procedures

Women who called or emailed indicating interest were screened for eligibility criteria by telephone. They had a physical exam, fasting blood draw, and provided written informed consent at the UCSF Clinical and Translational Science Institute’s Clinical Research Center (CCRC). They were scheduled to return on a separate afternoon one week later to undergo the TSST. During the week between visits, participants completed three consecutive days of daily diary assessment on mood, daily events, coping, and physical activity. On their return to the laboratory, they ate a standardized lunch provided by the CCRC metabolic kitchen, and had an intravenous forearm catheter inserted around 1300 hr. Participants had a one-hour resting baseline period while listening to relaxing music using headphones after catheter insertion. At the end of this baseline period (Time 0), the first saliva sample was collected. A modified form of the TSST (62) including performance of a speech and math task was administered. The phases of the stressor included four 5-min stressful periods (20 min total), including introduction to two trained evaluators who described the task, a preparatory period for the speech, a speech (about strengths and weaknesses, instead of a job interview, to fit the age group which includes many retirees), and lastly, a math task (serial subtraction of consecutive prime numbers). In line with the TSST, evaluators maintained neutral expressions throughout the tasks and followed a script to provide neutral feedback throughout the tasks.

Materials

Sociodemographics

Participants’ age was calculated from date of birth. Ethnicity was assessed with a list from which participants selected one option (i.e. White, Black, Hispanic, Asian, Pacific Islander, Native-American). Participants selected from the following choices for education: ‘Less than 12 years,’ ‘high school graduate,’ ‘some college or technical degree,’ ‘Associate in Arts degree,’ ‘Bachelor’s degree,’ and ‘advanced degree.’ Income was assessed by providing 22 categories of income ranges (lowest category, $0–$3,000 - highest category, $250,000 and above).

Saliva sample collections

Saliva samples were collected via passive drool method, using polypropylene saliva tubes at six time points throughout for the assessment of cortisol. Samples were collected at the following times in minutes: 0 (baseline), 15 (after the speech task), 20 (after the stressor ended), 30 (to capture cortisol peak), 50 (short term recovery), and 90 min (long term recovery, which is 70 min after the stressor ended, when cortisol is typically back to baseline levels) (63). Saliva samples were kept on ice and frozen at the end of each session, and sent for batch assay to Dresden, Germany (laboratory of Clemens Kirschbaum). Salivary cortisol was assayed using a chemiluminescence immunoassay (CLIA). Intra-assay CV was 2.9% for high levels and 7.7% for low levels. The inter-assay CV was 5.7% for high levels and 9.1% for low levels. The sensitivity lower limit was 0.16 ng/ml. Cortisol values ranged from 0.58 ng/ml to 33.75 ng/ml, well within the range of detection of the assays. Slight skewness (skewness statistic = 2.15) existed in the data and thus all values were log transformed for correction. Log-transformed values were normally distributed, and no outliers existed in the data.

Psychological Measures

Stress-induced rumination

Rumination items were adapted from the Ruminative Responses Scale (RRS) (37; 63). Twenty-five minutes after completion of the TSST, approximately when we sampled their 50-minute cortisol, participants completed the following rumination items based on the RRS and in response to completing the tasks: “I thought ‘Why do I always react this way?’” “I thought about the tasks, wishing they would have gone better,” and “I thought ‘Why can’t I handle things better?’ “Participants responded on a 4-point Likert scale from 0 (never) to 3 (always). Average scores were calculated for the three questions for each participant. Due to concerns about item overlap between rumination and depression (64), we used items from the brooding subscale of the RRS without the items that included references to depressive symptomatology. Examples of excluded statements include “I thought about how hard it is to concentrate” and “I thought about all my shortcomings, failings, faults, mistakes?” The TSST-induced rumination subscale had good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha=.75).

Physical activity

Participants reported at the end of each day, on three consecutive days, the number of minutes they engaged in vigorous exercise. They were asked, “Did you exercise today?” and were given the definition of vigorous activity that produced “increased heart rate and/or sweating.” If participants reported exercising, they were then asked, ‘How long did you exercise today?” For those participants who did not exercise on a particular day, minutes of exercise for that day were recoded to 0. All participants who participated in the daily component of the study answered these questions on all three days. Higher levels of self-reported physical activity are associated with greater fitness (64). Retrospective reporting is plagued by recall bias (65), and thus daily reporting of behaviors is considered a stronger measure of behavior (66; 67). Daily reports of physical activity were extremely skewed (range 0 to 300 minutes; 24 participants reported no exercise throughout the 3 day period) and therefore we split participants based on reported required amounts of physical activity for good health per week. The CDC(68), based on previous work (69), recommends an average of 75 minutes of vigorous activity per week (an average of 33 minutes for a three day period, which is the number of days participants recorded their activities). We have previously split participants based on meeting these recommended guidelines (less than 33 minutes over a 3 day period = 0, equal or greater than 33 minutes = 1), labeled here, for brevity, as “sedentary” vs. “active”(22).

Covariates

Body Mass Index (BMI; kg/m2) was included based on associations between BMI and cortisol in previous work (70). Depressive symptomatology was measured with the Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology and was included as a covariate given its strong associations with rumination (71), physical activity (72), and cortisol reactivity to stress (73).

Statistical Approach

Descriptive statistics and figure 1 for cortisol trajectories are presented with raw values. Log transformed values of cortisol were included in all analyses. Successive measurement of cortisol during and following a stressor permits the examination of cortisol trajectory over time with multilevel growth curve modeling (58). Multilevel growth curve modeling requires the employment of mixed modeling statistical designs. Given our small sample size and skewed data, we fitted a restricted maximum likelihood (REML) to our mixed models. We also fitted an unstructured covariance structure. Two strengths of mixed modeling with REML are that it can accommodate missing and skewed data and that it computes unbiased estimators (58; 74). Analyses were conducted with SPSS 18.0, using the MIXED syntax as recommended by Singer and Willett (58). All analyses were replicated with BMI, age, depressive symptomatology and control versus caregiver group as covariates, and the findings were consistent with reported results. In addition, the results did not vary as a function of hormone replacement therapy (HRT) (either when the 3 on HRT were excluded or HRT was included as a covariate).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Cortisol trajectory over the course of the TSST (90 minutes) as a function rumination and activity level.

Note. Values on the Y axis are solved Y values for salivary cortisol (ng/ml) from the quadratic equations for sedentary participants at low and high rumination and for all active participants. X-axis corresponds to time elapse over 90 minutes from beginning to the end of the TSST. The vertical black arrow corresponds to the approximate time the stressor ended. All active participants had similar trajectories, regardless of level of rumination, and thus are graphed as one group.

We first examined the unconditional means model (i.e. with no predictors) to determine amount of variation occurring at the within person level. Next, we examined the unconditional multilevel growth curve model to determine the cortisol reactivity and recovery trajectory across the whole sample. Multilevel growth curve modeling of cortisol over time (set at 30 minute intervals) permits the delineation of a series of parameters that increase our understanding of reactivity and recovery from laboratory stressors. Growth curve modeling estimates the intercept (B0), initial rate of change (i.e. time at each sampling of cortisol, Brise), and curvature of the cortisol trajectory (i.e. time-squared, Bcurvature). Stressor reactivity is captured by the intercept, initial rate of change, and number of minutes to peak, all of which is estimated by the growth curve model. Minutes for cortisol to peak is calculated with equation: peak = (−Brise/(2*Bcurvature) (58). Recovery was captured by the curvature in the trajectory and the minutes estimated to return to baseline.

Next, we conducted two analyses, first examining the interaction between rumination and time and, second, the interaction between physical activity and time. We used Cohen, Cohen, West, and Aiken’s approach (75) to regression analyses to analyze a three-way interaction between two continuous variables (time and rumination) and a dichotomous variable (activity level). In line with Cohen and colleagues recommendations, significant three-way interactions would suggest that cortisol trajectory varies differentially as a function of rumination at the two different activity levels. To follow-up on a significant three-way interaction, we examined simple interactions between rumination and time at activity= 0 and activity = 1. Significant simple interactions suggest that cortisol trajectories are significantly different at varying levels of rumination. As a result, simple slopes are examined at one standard deviation above and below the mean rumination score to determine the intercept, rise and curvature at these two levels of rumination. If the simple interactions are not significant at a particular activity level, simple slopes are not examined since this suggests that rumination does not predict trajectory at that activity level.

Results

Univariate and bivariate results

Forty-six postmenopausal women completed cortisol measurement during the TSST. Table 1 presents means and standard deviations of age, BMI, rumination, depression, and raw cortisol values (ng/ml) at each time point during the TSST for the entire sample of participants, as well as for active versus sedentary participants. The range of time spent exercising ranged from 0 to 300 minutes over the 3 days of reporting, with a mean of 43 minutes (SD = 66.23). When categorized by activity level, eighteen participants were in the active group and 28 were in the sedentary group. Of those categorized as sedentary, the majority (N=24, 86%) reported no activity over the three days, and the remaining 4 participants reported less than 20 minutes total time spent exercising over the three days. While there were no significant differences between caregivers and controls in activity levels (p = .23), the majority of controls (61%) were active and the majority of caregivers (61%) were sedentary. Furthermore, caregivers and controls did not significantly differ in rumination (p = .72). Bivariate analyses indicated that only depression scores were different between the groups, such that those who were more active had fewer depressive symptoms than those who were sedentary (p = .05). All cortisol measures (log-transformed) were moderately to strongly significantly correlated with one another (correlations range: .42 to .85) with the exception of baseline cortisol to 50 and 90 minutes post baseline (p = .09).

Table 1.

Means and standard deviations for the overall sample and the active and sedentary sub-groups of participants,

Total (N = 46) Sedentary (N=28) Active (N=18)
Group (Control N, %) 24 (52%) 11 (39%) 11 (61%)
BMI 26.40 (5.82) 27.16 (6.16) 25.22 (5.18)
Age 65.33 (5.81) 65.04 (5.98) 65.78 (5.67)
Depressive Symptoms 14.31 (10.73) 16.62 (11.99) 10.83 (7.54)
Rumination 1.81 (0.70) 1.89 (0.81) 1.72 (0.50)
2. Cortisol (ng/ml) 0min 5.04 (2.49) 5.08 (2.84) 4.98 (1.92)
3. Cortisol (ng/ml) 10min 5.88 (3.23) 6.32 (3.70) 5.14 (2.15)
4. Cortisol (ng/ml) 20min 8.04 (5.90) 8.88 (7.12) 6.72 (2.86)
5. Cortisol (ng/ml) 30min 7.90 (4.34) 7.61 (4.78) 8.32 (3.74)
6. Cortisol (ng/ml) 50min 7.36 (4.50) 7.49 (4.71) 7.16 (4.29)
7. Cortisol (ng/ml) 90min 5.06 (2.36) 5.33 (2.66) 4.62 (1.78)

Unconditional Means model

We partitioned the between and within person variation in cortisol output in response to the TSST among our participants. The estimates of the residual and intercept covariance parameters were 10.07 (SE = 0.96) and 7.61 (SE = 2.01) respectively. The intraclass correlation was .43 – in other words, 57% of the variation in cortisol response occurred within person.

Growth Models

We examined model fit for time in log-transformed cortisol, and results indicated that across all participants, log-transformed cortisol output followed a curvilinear relationship where intercept was 0.65 (SE = .04, p = .00), initial rate (time) of increase was 0.23 (SE = 0.04, p < .01) with a curvature (time-squared) −0.08 (SE = 0.01, p < .01). The moment when the quadratic trajectory curve reached its peak and turned downward was at 44 minutes after onset of stressor for the entire sample.

Next, we examined cortisol trajectory as a function of rumination alone, activity level alone, and the interaction between activity and rumination. In analyses with rumination alone, rumination did not significantly predict the trajectory of cortisol in response to stress (interaction Bslope = .04, SE = 0.05, p = .48, interaction Bcurvature = −0.01, SE = 0.01, p = .68). Similarly, in analyses with activity level alone, activity did not significantly predict the trajectory of cortisol in response to stress (interaction Bslope = −.01, SE = 0.11, p = .95, interaction Bcurvature = −0.00, SE = 0.03, p = .98). However, as hypothesized, the interaction term between rumination and exercise was significant in relation to the initial rate of cortisol increase (interaction Bslope = −0.10, SE = 0.04, p = .01) and was marginally significant in relation to curvature (interaction Bcurvature = −0.02, SE = 0.01, p = .06). The nature of the three-way interaction (i.e. rumination, exercise, time) was further examined for sedentary versus active participants. Results indicated that the association between rumination and cortisol trajectory (slope and curvature), but not baseline levels, significantly varied as a function of activity.

Rumination and cortisol trajectory in sedentary participants

For sedentary participants, initial increase and curvature in cortisol trajectory varied significantly as a function of levels of rumination (Bslope = .23, SE = .04, p < .001 and Bcurvature =− 08, SE = .01, p < .001, respectively). For sedentary participants at one standard deviation below the mean of rumination, baseline levels of log-transformed cortisol were 0.70 (SE = 0.06, p < .001), cortisol initially increased at a rate of 0.21 (SE = 0.04, p <.01) and had a significant curvature of −0.08 (SE = 0.01, p < .001), corresponding to a peak in cortisol reactivity at 39 minutes. At one standard deviation above the mean of rumination, baseline levels of log-transformed cortisol were 0.63 (SE = 0.05, p < .001), cortisol initially increased at a rate of 0.26 (SE = 0.04, p <.001) and had a significant curvature of −0.07 (SE = 0.01, p < .001), corresponding to a peak in cortisol reactivity at 56 minutes, 17 minutes after the peak of sedentary low ruminators. Relatedly, while sedentary low ruminators were on average likely to have returned to their baseline levels by the end of the study (extrapolated time from the graph was 49 minutes after end of stressor), those high ruminators who were sedentary had on average not yet returned to their own baseline. Recovery to baseline did not occur during our measurement period in sedentary high ruminators, and extrapolation showed average recovery to baseline at approximately 115 minutes - nearly 90 minutes after the end of the TSST, and 36 minutes after the sedentary low ruminators. In summary, compared with sedentary low ruminators, sedentary high ruminators had a more pronounced initial rate of cortisol increase, a later cortisol peak, and a slower return to baseline cortisol levels.

Rumination and cortisol trajectory in active participants

For all active participants, there was a significant cortisol intercept (B = 0.64, SE = 0.05, p < .001), significant rise (Bslope = 0.24, SE = 0.06, p < .001) and significant curvature (Bcurvature = −0.08, SE = 0.02, p < .001) to the stressor corresponding to a peak of 41 minutes and return to baseline 60 minutes post-stressor. However, rumination was unrelated to cortisol intercept (p = .53), initial rate of change (p = .10), or curvature (p = .18). In other words, all active participants had similar trajectories, regardless of level of rumination.

Figure 1 summarizes these interaction effects. As illustrated, sedentary high ruminators had a more marked stress response and slower recovery than both sedentary low ruminators and all active participants. Furthermore, it seems that sedentary, low ruminating participants had, on average, similar trajectories to those who were active.1

Discussion

The present study examined whether being physically active moderates the effects of rumination on the trajectory of cortisol responses to acute stress. We hypothesized that one pathway through which physical activity could lower the stress response is by mitigating the effects of stress-related cognitions on the body’s stress arousal systems. Our findings support this notion by demonstrating that the cortisol trajectory was a function of stress-induced rumination only in those who were sedentary, and unrelated to rumination in those who were active. Specifically, sedentary participants who had higher self-reported levels of rumination in response to the stressor had faster and prolonged reactivity and delayed recovery to stress, evidenced by a more rapid initial increase, a later peak, and a delayed return to baseline cortisol compared to the sedentary lower ruminating participants. For active participants, cortisol trajectory was not a function of rumination, regardless of level of stressor-induced rumination.

It is interesting to note that we may be detecting a trait-like effect rather than just the immediate effect of state rumination on cortisol. Rumination was determined after the acute stressor had terminated. Sedentary ruminators were not just high in cortisol during the recovery phase when we measured their levels of rumination, but also during the initial onset of the stressor. Therefore, our measure of state rumination may be differentiating people who are different in their initial acute stress reactivity, possibly due to differential appraisal of the stressor. Furthermore, activity alone may not shape cortisol responses. Rather, activity may be especially beneficial to those responding to stress with rumination, given the findings that activity moderates the relationship between rumination and cortisol reactivity and recovery, but does not directly predict cortisol reactivity to and recovery from stress.

We have recently demonstrated that being physically active, as defined here, moderates the relationship between chronic stress and short leukocyte telomere length, a marker increasingly understood to capture the accumulated burden of genetics, life stressors, and health behaviors (22). Understanding the psychological and physiological mechanisms through which physical activity confers such effects to those most vulnerable to stress is critical for the development of interventions to enhance health, and for identifying those individuals for whom new interventions would be beneficial. The present findings address these issues and suggest that one pathway that may explain the physiological benefits of physical activity is reduced activation and enhanced recovery of stress arousal systems tied to psychological responses to stress. Overall, these findings lend support to the idea that being physically active may confer stress resistance, at least in post-menopausal women. Importantly, these findings appear to extend to elderly women.

Chronic stress is documented to produce different profiles of HPA axis dysregulation, including either hyperactive or blunted cortisol responses to stressors (29; 74). In this study, we examined HPA axis reactions to acute stress in the context of rumination, a potential psychological mechanism of chronic stress. A recent meta-analysis by Denson and colleagues (36) provides evidence that rumination may induce increased cortisol levels in order to maintain vigilance toward unresolved stressors. Repeatedly ruminating in response to stress in the real world may repeatedly stimulate the HPA axis resulting in chronically elevated levels of cortisol. Chronic exposure to elevated cortisol is in turn linked with insulin resistance, accumulation of abdominal fat, and increased risk for cardiovascular disease (29; 31). Healthy levels of physical activity may differentially affect high ruminating individuals by creating a physiological eustress state (76), and attenuating biological stress responses including cortisol responses (5052). A caveat of our study is that the heightened cortisol reactivity and delayed recovery observed in high ruminators was defined as such based on comparisons with low ruminators. At present, to our knowledge, there are no standard trajectories that are understood to represent excessively high cortisol reactivity or delayed cortisol recovery.

Our study adds to the current literature on the benefits of physical activity in that it tests if being physically active moderates the physiological effects of commonly experienced stress-related cognitions. There are a number of neurobiological and physiological mechanisms by which exercise may confer benefits, including increases in the expression of genes that encode brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) leading to increased cognitive functioning and neural plasticity (7779), a heightened anti-inflammatory environment in the body (8082), enhanced insulin sensitivity (8386), and enhanced oxidative buffering capacity (76; 87). Physical activity reduces depressive symptoms among those genetically predisposed to lower BDNF levels (20), and reduces symptom reporting and doctor visits in those with increased numbers of stressful life events (19; 21). Here, we demonstrate that physical activity may attenuate the acute physiological response to stress, as indexed by HPA axis reactivity, in those particularly vulnerable to stress – ruminators.

Limitations of the study included our short-term and self-reported measure of physical activity. Physical activity was reported on a daily basis across three days, and participants reported the number of minutes they engaged in activities in which their heart rate was increased and/or they perspired. We assume that these three days of reporting represent typical exercise behaviors, and thus, a long-term lifestyle factor. However, we do not know if the short-term activity specifically occurring in the days before the TSST was important in the results here, or whether this was a proxy measure for general fitness. Although our measure represents an important advance over the usual retrospective report of physical activity, future studies should measure participants’ baseline fitness levels and objectively capture daily physical activity with the use of accelerometers or assessments of current physical fitness. Our measurement across three days may be an over or under representation of actual engagement in physical activity per week, and thus extending measurement to longer than 3 days to a week or two would benefit future studies that aim to use daily measurement of vigorous activity as a measure of high activity level. These limitations of our measure of physical activity may explain why we did not detect a main effect of physical activity on cortisol trajectory, in contrast with other studies on fitness and cortisol output (5254). Self-report of physical activity is not as strong a predictor of health as measurements of actual fitness levels (82). Finally, only a randomized controlled aerobic exercise intervention study can truly test the buffering effects of physical activity on HPA axis activation as a result of stress-induction. Our results are thus limited to between-group differences that can also be a function of personality and other between group differences.

Findings of the present study are also limited to post-menopausal women, and should be replicated in larger samples, including men as well as individuals with diverse ages and ethnic backgrounds. Of particular importance, our sample size is limited to 18 participants in the active group, and 28 in the sedentary one. While mixed models with REML estimation are considered unbiased and robust, it may be possible that our findings are limited to our specific sample. We thus consider these findings preliminary, and suggest that follow up studies with larger sample sizes and more diverse groups and ages may be worthwhile. Given the sample size, we were unable to stratify our results by caregiver group, age and BMI. Exposure to ongoing chronic stress may change psychological responses including ruminative responses to acute stressors. However, in our sample, caregivers and controls showed similar levels of rumination in response to the acute laboratory stress, and our findings were significant over and above the effects of caregiver status. Exposure to chronic stress may also influence physical activity levels. Although not statistically significant, the caregivers in our study were disproportionately sedentary compared with controls. While significant differences between caregivers and controls in our study were not apparent, possibly due to sample size limitations, the current findings may be particularly relevant to caregivers, given their lower rates of physical activity. Caregivers who ruminate about everyday stressors may benefit from physical exercise interventions.

Further, there may be sex differences, which we could not test in this all-female sample. It may be that rumination is not only more common in women but has different physiological effects in women. Thus including men in future studies is important to further our understanding of the moderating potential of being active on physiological consequences of rumination. Comparing these trajectories to younger, fit and unfit women who are high and low on rumination will also deepen our understanding of the interplay of age with psychological and physiological response to stress.

In summary, the findings reported in the present study are the first, to our knowledge, that demonstrate the moderating effects of being physically active on the physiological responses associated with rumination following acute laboratory stressors. We applied a statistical model, namely growth curve modeling, which better captures the nature of physiological responses by modeling initial increases, minutes to peak responses, and speed of recovery. The effects found in the present study were apparent, even after covarying age, BMI, caregiving group, and depressive symptomatology. Heightened increases in cortisol responses and delayed recoveries to repeated stressors across the day would possibly lead to sustained exposure to elevated cortisol levels, ultimately affecting physical health outcomes(60). Our study demonstrates the potential for physical activity to allow rapid recovery of the HPA axis after the induction of stress, especially in those who are ruminating, thus reducing the heightened cortisol response earlier and potentially protecting individuals from continued, prolonged exposure. It has been increasingly clear that it is important to understand factors that enhance physiological recovery from stressors in the service of promoting health. Exercise appears to be one promising way to promote physiological stress resistance, particularly in stressed ruminators who are prone to affective and physiological disorders.

Acknowledgments

We thank the study participants for their support and generous contribution of time. We thank Dr. Clemens Kirschbaum for his laboratory services for cortisol measurement. The research study was supported by The Division of Behavioral and Social Research at The National Institute of Aging/National Institute of Health R56 grant (Epel). The contents of this publication are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH.

Abbreviations

HPA

hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal

TSST

Trier Social Stress Task

RRS

The Ruminative Responses Scale

BMI

body mass index

REML

restricted maximum likelihood

HRT

hormone replacement therapy

Footnotes

1

We examined interactive effects of rumination and activity on the trajectories of heart rate (HR), diastolic blood pressure (DBP), and systolic blood pressure (SBP). Our data suggests no direct or interaction effects on BP, DBP or SBP trajectories. However, for DBP, physical activity and rumination significantly interacted to predict DBP across all sampling times (matched to times of cortisol sampling). For inactive participants, higher rumination was significantly related to increased DBP across the entire recording during and after the TSST. However in active participants, rumination was unrelated to DBP.

Contributor Information

Eli Puterman, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco.

Aoife O’Donovan, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco Veteran’s Affairs Medical Center.

Nancy E. Adler, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco.

A. Janet Tomiyama, Departments of Psychology & Nutrition, Rutgers University.

Margaret Kemeny, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco.

Owen M. Wolkowitz, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco.

Elissa Epel, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco.

References

  • 1.Babyak M, Blumenthal JA, Herman S, Khatri P, Doraiswamy M, Moore K, Craighead WE, Baldewicz TT, Krishnan KR. Exercise treatment for major depression: maintenance of therapeutic benefit at 10 months. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2000;62(5):633. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200009000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Blumenthal JA, Babyak MA, Doraiswamy PM, Watkins L, Hoffman BM, Barbour KA, Herman S, Craighead WE, Brosse AL, Waugh R, et al. Exercise and pharmacotherapy in the treatment of major depressive disorder. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2007;69(7):587. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318148c19a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Blumenthal J, Babyak M, Moore K, Craighead W, Herman S, Khatri P, Waugh R, Napolitano M, Forman L, Appelbaum M, Doraiswamy P, Krishnan K. Effects of exercise training on older patients with major depression. Archives of Internal Medicine. 1999;159(19):2349–2356. doi: 10.1001/archinte.159.19.2349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Dunn AL, Trivedi MH, Kampert JB, Clark CG, Chambliss HO. Exercise treatment for depression: Efficacy and dose response. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2005 Jan;28(1):1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2004.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hamer M, Molloy GJ, de Oliveira C, Demakakos P. Leisure time physical activity, risk of depressive symptoms, and inflammatory mediators: The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(7):1050–1055. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Etgen T, Sander D, Huntgeburth U, Poppert H, Forstl H, Bickel H. Physical activity and incident cognitive impairment in elderly persons: the INVADE study. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2010;170(2):186. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2009.498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Larson EB, Wang L, Bowen JD, McCormick WC, Teri L, Crane P, Kukull W. Exercise is associated with reduced risk for incident dementia among persons 65 years of age and older. Annals of Internal Medicine. 2006;144(2):73. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-144-2-200601170-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rolland Y, Pillard F, Klapouszczak A, Reynish E, Thomas D, Andrieu S, Riviere D, Vellas B. Exercise program for nursing home residents with Alzheimer’s disease: a 1-year randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2007;55(2):158–165. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01035.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Rovio S, K\aareholt I, Helkala EL, Viitanen M, Winblad B, Tuomilehto J, Soininen H, Nissinen A, Kivipelto M. Leisure-time physical activity at midlife and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. The Lancet Neurology. 2005;4(11):705–711. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70198-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Teri L, Gibbons LE, McCurry SM, Logsdon RG, Buchner DM, Barlow WE, Kukull WA, LaCroix AZ, McCormick W, Larson EB. Exercise Plus Behavioral Management in Patients With Alzheimer Disease: A Randomized Controlled Trial. JAMA. 2003 Oct;290(15):2015–2022. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.15.2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Smith GD, Shipley M, Batty G, Morris J, Marmot M. Physical activity and cause-specific mortality in the Whitehall study. Public Health. 2000 Sep;114(5):308–315. doi: 10.1038/sj.ph.1900675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Thompson PD. Exercise and Physical Activity in the Prevention and Treatment of Atherosclerotic Cardiovascular Disease: A Statement From the Council on Clinical Cardiology (Subcommittee on Exercise, Rehabilitation, and Prevention) and the Council on Nutrition, Physical Activity, and Metabolism (Subcommittee on Physical Activity) Arteriosclerosis, Thrombosis, and Vascular Biology. 2003;23(8):42e–49. doi: 10.1161/01.ATV.0000087143.33998.F2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Twisk JWR, Kemper HC, van MECHELEN W. Tracking of activity and fitness and the relationship with cardiovascular disease risk factors. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 2000;32(8):1455–1461. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200008000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Tuomilehto J, Lindstrom J, Eriksson JG, Valle TT, Hamalainen H, Ilanne-Parikka P, Keinanen-Kiukaanniemi S, Laakso M, Louheranta A, Rastas M, Salminen V, Aunola S, Cepaitis Z, Moltchanov V, Hakumaki M, Mannelin M, Martikkala V, Sundvall J, Uusitupa M the Finnish Diabetes Prevention Study Group. Prevention of Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus by Changes in Lifestyle among Subjects with Impaired Glucose Tolerance. N Engl J Med. 2001 May;344(18):1343–1350. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200105033441801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Manson JE, Nathan DM, Krolewski AS, Stampfer MJ, Willett WC, Hennekens CH. A Prospective Study of Exercise and Incidence of Diabetes Among US Male Physicians. JAMA. 1992 Jul;268(1):63–67. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Blair SN, Kohl HW, Barlow CE, Paffenbarger RS, Gibbons LW, Macera CA. Changes in physical fitness and all-cause mortality. JAMA. 1995;273(14):1093–1098. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Blair SN, Kampert JB, Kohl HW, Barlow CE, Macera CA, Paffenbarger RS, Gibbons LW. Influences of Cardiorespiratory Fitness and Other Precursors on Cardiovascular Disease and All-Cause Mortality in Men and Women. JAMA. 1996 Jul;276(3):205–210. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Cole CR, Blackstone EH, Pashkow FJ, Snader CE, Lauer MS. Heart-Rate Recovery Immediately after Exercise as a Predictor of Mortality. N Engl J Med. 1999 Oct;341(18):1351–1357. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199910283411804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Kobasa SC, Maddi SR, Puccetti MC. Personality and exercise as buffers in the stress-illness relationship. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1982 Dec;5(4):391–404. doi: 10.1007/BF00845369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Mata J, Thompson RJ, Gotlib IH. BDNF Genotype Moderates the Relation Between Physical Activity and Depressive Symptoms. Health psychology. 2010;29(2):130. doi: 10.1037/a0017261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ouellette Kobasa SC, Maddi SR, Puccetti MC, Zola MA. Effectiveness of hardiness, exercise and social support as resources against illness. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1985;29(5):525–533. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(85)90086-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Puterman E, Lin J, Blackburn E, O’Donovan A, Adler N, Epel E. The Power of Exercise: Buffering the Effect of Chronic Stress on Telomere Length. PLoS ONE. 2010 May;5(5):e10837. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010837. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Roth D, Holmes D. Influence of physical fitness in determining the impact of stressful life events on physical and psychologic health. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1985 Mar;47(2):164–173. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198503000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Janicki-Deverts D, Cohen S, Matthews KA, Cullen MR. History of unemployment predicts future elevations in C-reactive protein among male participants in the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2008;36(2):176–185. doi: 10.1007/s12160-008-9056-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Glaser R, Shuttleworth EC, Dyer CS, Ogrocki P, Speicher CE. Chronic stress and immunity in family caregivers of Alzheimer’s disease victims. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1987;49(5):523. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198709000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Mooy JM, De Vries H, Grootenhuis PA, Bouter LM, Heine RJ. Major stressful life events in relation to prevalence of undetected type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2000;23(2):197–201. doi: 10.2337/diacare.23.2.197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Steptoe A, Brydon L, Kunz-Ebrecht S. Changes in Financial Strain Over Three Years, Ambulatory Blood Pressure, and Cortisol Responses to Awakening. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2005 Mar;67(2):281–287. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000156932.96261.d2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH. Salivary cortisol in psychoneuroendocrine research: Recent developments and applications. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1994;19(4):313–333. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(94)90013-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.McEwen BS. Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. N Engl J Med. 1998;338(3):171. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199801153380307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.McEwen BS. Protection and damage from acute and chronic stress. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2004;1032:1–7. doi: 10.1196/annals.1314.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.McEwen BS. Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and allostatic load. Annals NY Acad Sci. 1998;840:33–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Brosschot JF, Pieper S, Thayer JF. Expanding stress theory: Prolonged activation and perseverative cognition. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005 Nov;30(10):1043–1049. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Brosschot JF, Gerin W, Thayer JF. The perseverative cognition hypothesis: A review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2006 Feb;60(2):113–124. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.06.074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kubzansky LD, Davidson KW, Rozanski A. The Clinical Impact of Negative Psychological States: Expanding the Spectrum of Risk for Coronary Artery Disease. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2005 May;67(Supplement_1):S10–14. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000164012.88829.41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Thayer JF, Lane RD. Perseverative Thinking and Health: Neurovisceral Concomitants. Psychology & Health. 2002;17(5):685. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Denson TF, Spanovic M, Miller N. Cognitive appraisals and emotions predict cortisol and immune responses: A meta-analysis of acute laboratory social stressors and emotion inductions. Psychological Bulletin. 2009;135(6):823–853. doi: 10.1037/a0016909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Nolen-Hoeksema S, Morrow J. A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: the 1989 Loma Prieta Earthquake. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991 Jul;61(1):115–121. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.61.1.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco BE, Lyubomirsky S. Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2008;3(5):400–424. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Trapnell PD, Campbell JD. Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1999;76(2):284–304. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.76.2.284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Wyer RS, Martin LL, Tesser A. Ruminative Thoughts: Advances in Social Cognition. IX. Lawrence Erlbaum; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Nolen-Hoeksema S. Sex differences in unipolar depression: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin. 1987;101(2):259–282. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Nolen-Hoeksema S, Morrow J. A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;61(1):115–121. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.61.1.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Nolen-Hoeksema S, Morrow J, Fredrickson BL. Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1993;102(1):20–28. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.102.1.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Muris P, Roelofs J, Rassin E, Franken I, Mayer B. Mediating effects of rumination and worry on the links between neuroticism, anxiety and depression. Personality and Individual Differences. 2005;39(6):1105–1111. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Puterman E, DeLongis A, Pomaki G. Protecting Us from Ourselves: Social Support as a Buffer of Trait and State Rumination. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2010;29(7):797–820. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Wood JV, Saltzberg JA, Neale JM, Stone AA, Rachmiel TB. Self-focused attention, coping responses, and distressed mood in everyday life. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1990;58(6):1027–1036. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.58.6.1027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Lavallee LF, Campbell JD. Impact of personal goals on self-regulation processes elicited by daily negative events. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69(2):341–352. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.McCullough ME, Orsulak P, Brandon A, Akers L. Rumination, fear, and cortisol: An in vivo study of interpersonal transgressions. Health Psychology. 2007;26(1):126–132. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.1.126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Zoccola PM, Dickerson SS, Zaldivar FP. Rumination and cortisol responses to laboratory stressors. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2008;70(6):661. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31817bbc77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Rimmele U, Seiler R, Marti B, Wirtz PH, Ehlert U, Heinrichs M. The level of physical activity affects adrenal and cardiovascular reactivity to psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009 Feb;34(2):190–198. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.08.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Rimmele U, Zellweger BC, Marti B, Seiler R, Mohiyeddini C, Ehlert U, Heinrichs M. Trained men show lower cortisol, heart rate and psychological responses to psychosocial stress compared with untrained men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007 Jul;32(6):627–635. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Traustadottir T, Bosch PR, Matt KS. The HPA axis response to stress in women: effects of aging and fitness. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005 May;30(4):392–402. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ford ES. Does exercise reduce inflammation? Physical activity and C-reactive protein among U.S. adults. Epidemiology. 2002 Sep;13(5):561–568. doi: 10.1097/00001648-200209000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Blumenthal JA, Fredrikson M, Kuhn CM, Ulmer RL, Walsh-Riddle M, Appelbaum M. Aerobic exercise reduces levels of cardiovascular and sympathoadrenal responses to mental stress in subjects without prior evidence of myocardial ischemia. The American Journal of Cardiology. 1990;65(1):93–98. doi: 10.1016/0002-9149(90)90032-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Crews DJ, Landers DM. A meta-analytic review of aerobic fitness and reactivity to psychosocial stressors. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 1987;19(5):S121. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Dishman RK, Jackson EM, Nakamura Y. Influence of Fitness and Gender on Blood Pressure Responses During Active or Passive Stress. Psychophysiology. 2002;39(05):568–576. doi: 10.1017.S0048577202394071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Jackson EM, Dishman RK. Cardiorespiratory fitness and laboratory stress: A meta-regression analysis. Psychophysiology. 2006;43(1):57–72. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00373.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Singer JD, Willett JB. Applied longitudinal data analysis: modeling change and event occurrence. Oxford University Press; US: 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Dienstbier RA. Behavioral correlates of sympathoadrenal reactivity: the toughness model. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 1991;23(7):846. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Brosschot JF, Pieper S, Thayer JF. Expanding stress theory: Prolonged activation and perseverative cognition. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;30(10):1043–1049. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Linden W, Earle TL, Gerin W, Christenfeld N. Physiological stress reactivity and recovery: Conceptual siblings separated at birth? Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1997 Feb;42(2):117–135. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(96)00240-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Kirschbaum C, Pirke KM, Hellhammer DH. The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’ a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology. 1993;28(1–2):76–81. doi: 10.1159/000119004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Treynor W, Gonzalez R, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2003;27(3):247–259. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Church TS, Cheng YJ, Earnest CP, Barlow CE, Gibbons LW, Priest EL, Blair SN. Exercise Capacity and Body Composition as Predictors of Mortality Among Men With Diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2004 Jan;27(1):83–88. doi: 10.2337/diacare.27.1.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Fransson E, Knutsson A, Westerholm P, Alfredsson L. Indications of recall bias found in a retrospective study of physical activity and myocardial infarction. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2008 Aug;61(8):840–847. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2007.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Verbrugge LM. Health diaries. Medical Care. 1980;18(1):73–95. doi: 10.1097/00005650-198001000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Gillmore MR, Gaylord J, Hartway J, Hoppe MJ, Morrison DM, Leigh BC, Rainey DT. Daily Data Collection of Sexual and Other Health-Related Behaviors. The Journal of Sex Research. 2001 Feb;38(1):35–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Physical Activity for Everyone: Guidelines: Adults | DNPAO | CDC [Internet] [date unknown];[cited 2009 Oct 26] Available from: http://www.cdc.gov/physicalactivity/everyone/guidelines/adults.html.
  • 69.Nelson ME, Rejeski WJ, Blair SN, Duncan PW, Judge JO, King AC, Macera CA, Castaneda-Sceppa C. Physical activity and public health in older adults. Recommendation from the American College of Sports Medicine and the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2007 doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.107.185650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Epel E, Lapidus R, McEwen B, Brownell K. Stress may add bite to appetite in women: a laboratory study of stress-induced cortisol and eating behavior. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2001;26(1):37–50. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(00)00035-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Nolen-Hoeksema S. The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2000;109(3):504–511. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Camacho TC, Roberts RE, Lazarus NB, Kaplan GA, Cohen RD. Physical activity and depression: Evidence from the Alameda County Study. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1991 Jul;134(2):220–231. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a116074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Burke HM, Davis MC, Otte C, Mohr DC. Depression and cortisol responses to psychological stress: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;30(9):846–856. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS. Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. Sage Publications, Inc; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Cohen P, Cohen J, West SG, Aiken LS. Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 3. Lawrence Erlbaum; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Li G, He H. Hormesis, allostatic buffering capacity and physiological mechanism of physical activity: A new theoretic framework. Medical Hypotheses. 2009 May;72(5):527–532. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2008.12.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Dishman RK, Berthoud HR, Booth FW, Cotman CW, Edgerton VR, Fleshner MR, Gandevia SC, Gomez-Pinilla F, Greenwood BN, Hillman CH, et al. Neurobiology of exercise. Obesity Research. 2006;14(3):345–356. doi: 10.1038/oby.2006.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Vaynman S, Ying Z, Gomez-Pinilla F. Interplay between brain-derived neurotrophic factor and signal transduction modulators in the regulation of the effects of exercise on synaptic-plasticity. Neuroscience. 2003;122(3):647–657. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Vaynman S, Ying Z, Gomez-Pinilla F. Hippocampal BDNF mediates the efficacy of exercise on synaptic plasticity and cognition. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;20(10):2580–2590. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03720.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Gielen S, Adams V, Mobius-Winkler S, Linke A, Erbs S, Yu J, Kempf W, Schubert A, Schuler G, Hambrecht R. Anti-inflammatory effects of exercise training in the skeletal muscle of patients with chronic heart failure. Journal of the American College of Cardiology. 2003;42(5):861. doi: 10.1016/s0735-1097(03)00848-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Goldhammer E, Tanchilevitch A, Maor I, Beniamini Y, Rosenschein U, Sagiv M. Exercise training modulates cytokines activity in coronary heart disease patients. International Journal of Cardiology. 2005;100(1):93–99. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2004.08.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Petersen AM, Pedersen BK. The anti-inflammatory effect of exercise. Journal of Applied Physiology. 2005;98(4):1154. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00164.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Perseghin G, Price TB, Petersen KF, Roden M, Cline GW, Gerow K, Rothman DL, Shulman GI. Increased glucose transport-phosphorylation and muscle glycogen synthesis after exercise training in insulin-resistant subjects. N Engl J Med. 1996 Oct;335(18):1357–1362. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199610313351804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Koivisto VA, Yki-Jarvinen H, DeFronzo RA. Physical training and insulin sensitivity. Diabetes Metab Rev. 1986;1(4):445–481. doi: 10.1002/dmr.5610010407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Kang J, Robertson RJ, Hagberg JM, Kelley DE, Goss FL, DaSilva SG, Suminski RR, Utter AC. Effect of exercise intensity on glucose and insulin metabolism in obese individuals and obese NIDDM patients. Diabetes Care. 1996;19(4):341. doi: 10.2337/diacare.19.4.341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Perseghin G, Price TB, Petersen KF, Roden M, Cline GW, Gerow K, Rothman DL, Shulman GI. Increased glucose transport-phosphorylation and muscle glycogen synthesis after exercise training in insulin-resistant subjects. N Engl J Med. 1996;335(18):1357. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199610313351804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Jackson MJ. Reactive oxygen species and redox-regulation of skeletal muscle adaptations to exercise. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2005;360(1464):2285. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES