Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Sep 7.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2010 Aug;22(3):635–653. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000337

Developmental cascades: Externalizing, internalizing, and academic competence from middle childhood to early adolescence

Kristin L Moilanen a, Daniel S Shaw a, Kari L Maxwell b
PMCID: PMC3168570  NIHMSID: NIHMS316640  PMID: 20576184

Abstract

The current study was initiated to increase understanding of developmental cascades in childhood in a sample of at-risk boys (N = 291; 52% White). Mothers, teachers, and boys reported on boys’ externalizing problems, internalizing difficulties, and academic competence. Consistent with hypotheses regarding school-related transitions, high levels of externalizing problems were associated with both low levels of academic competence and high levels of internalizing problems during the early school-age period, and with elevations in internalizing problems during the transition to adolescence. Low levels of academic competence were associated with high levels of internalizing problems in middle childhood, and with high levels of externalizing problems during the transition from elementary school to middle school. Shared risk factors played a minimal role in these developmental cascades. Results suggest that there are cascading effects of externalizing problems and academic competence in childhood and early adolescence, and that some cascading effects are more likely to occur during periods of school-related transitions. Implications of developmental cascade effects for research and intervention are discussed.


It is not uncommon for children and adolescents to experience comorbid externalizing and internalizing problems (e.g., Capaldi, 1992; McConaughy & Achenbach, 1994; Oland & Shaw, 2005; Reitz, Deković, & Meijer, 2005), often in conjunction with academic problems (Flook, Repetti, & Ullman, 2005; Hinshaw, 1992; Maughan, Rowe, Loeber, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 2003). Although it has been firmly established that externalizing and internalizing problems are negatively associated with children’s academic competence concurrently, less is known about their associations over time and whether problem behavior in any of these domains is likely to increase the likelihood of increases in problem behavior in another area of functioning. Multiple transactional models of developmental psychopathology have been postulated to explain how functioning within individual domains of children’s adaptation may affect the course of adjustment across domains, with most of these suggesting snowballing or cascading effects (e.g., Capaldi, 1992; Patterson & Stoolmiller, 1991). However, other than a limited number of exceptions in the substance use literature (e.g., Locke & Newcomb, 2001), relatively few researchers have actually tested the validity of such models empirically (Burt, Obradović, Long, & Masten, 2008; Masten et al., 2005). The current study seeks to add to the existing literature on cascading effects by examining the interplay of boys’ externalizing and internalizing problems, as well as academic functioning, across two key developmental periods: the transition to formal schooling and the transition from elementary to middle school. Such data are important not only for basic research but also for prevention efforts, because targeting domains of adjustment that will likely lead to problems in other areas may be cost effective in preventing more pervasive psychopathology.

In models that emphasize cascading processes, symptoms of problem behavior in one domain (e.g., externalizing problems) are hypothesized to undermine functioning in one or more other domains (e.g., academic competence), which may exacerbate the risk of the initial type of behavior and possibly other problem behaviors (e.g., internalizing problems; Masten, Burt, & Coatsworth, 2006). To test the validity of cascading processes, it is therefore optimal to have three or more domains of behavior measured at three or more time points (Cole & Maxwell, 2003). Each construct’s stability is controlled at each time point, and contemporaneous correlations across domains are also modeled. In essence, this means that predicting behavior across domains is conducted while controlling for within-time covariance across domains and for change in each separate domain over time (Masten et al., 2006). This provides a more conservative test of transactional hypotheses than that provided by less sophisticated models that do not account for autoregressive effects.

Although several researchers have used cross-lagged analyses to examine interrelationships among different domains of child and adolescent functioning over time (e.g., Beyers & Loeber, 2003; Chen, Li, Li, Li, & Liu, 2000; Cole, Martin, Powers, & Truglio, 1996; Kiesner, 2002; Locke & Newcomb, 2001; Mason & Windle, 2002), few longitudinal studies have formally tested the validity of cascading models on multiple dimensions of psychopathology and competence over three or more points in time. Following up and extending recent research by Masten and colleagues (2005), we examined cascading processes among externalizing and internalizing problems and academic competence over five time points from the transition to formal school entry through the transition to middle school with a cohort of boys at risk for problems in all three domains of functioning. Below we review two theoretical perspectives that suggest hypotheses for cascading effects, as discussed by Masten and colleagues (2005). We then discuss how transition points in children’s schooling might influence the timing and manifestation of cascading effects at specific ages. We conclude by reviewing evidence for a shared risk model that could also potentially account for the appearance of cascading effects.

Models of Cascading Effects: The Adjustment Erosion Hypothesis

The adjustment erosion hypothesis posits that initial externalizing or internalizing symptoms reduce later academic competence and increase future vulnerability to symptoms from other domains. From this perspective, prior externalizing or internalizing symptoms would lead to later academic difficulties. In the case of a child with initial externalizing problems, such behaviors commonly associated with externalizing problems (e.g., inattention, impulsivity, aggression, or hostility) would limit the opportunities for learning to occur in the classroom, thereby impeding academic competence directly (Chen, Rubin, & Li, 1997; Dishion, Patterson, Stoolmiller, & Skinner, 1991), and because of the greater probability of social rejection by healthy peers and acceptance by more deviant peers (Dishion, 1990), lead to disinterest in school and indirectly reduce academic achievement (Chen, Rubin, & Li, 1997; Schwartz, Gorman, Nakamoto, & McKay, 2006). At extreme levels, aggression could also increase children’s likelihood of school expulsion (Risi, Gerhardstein, & Kistner, 2003), which would remove them from the context in which academic competence is fostered.

Mechanisms also have been proposed for why initial externalizing may lead to subsequent internalizing symptoms. For example, difficulties associated with externalizing problems (e.g., frustration tolerance, attentional difficulties) may increase the likelihood of poor family or peer relationships, both of which may contribute directly to heightened anxiety and lower self worth (Burks, Dodge, & Price, 1995; Coie, Terry, Lenox, Lochman, & Hyman, 1995), and leave children more vulnerable to depression (Reinherz et al., 1993).

Cascading effects between initial externalizing and later academic or internalizing problems have been found in several previous investigations. For example, boys ages 7–12 years who were diagnosed with externalizing types of disorders such as oppositional defiant disorder or conduct disorder were at increased risk for depression by age 18 (Burke, Loeber, Lahey, & Rathouz, 2005). Capaldi (1992) found that twice as many boys with conduct problems in sixth grade reported increases in depressive symptoms at eighth grade, compared to boys with internalizing problems in sixth grade who reported increases in conduct problems by eighth grade, a general pattern of results that has been replicated by other researchers (Lahey, Loeber, Burke, Rathouz, & McBurnett, 2002). In another investigation, boys’ alcohol use in late adolescence subsequently predicted elevated symptoms of depression in early adulthood, which in turn predicted problematic alcohol use in middle adulthood (Locke & Newcomb, 2001). Early externalizing symptoms have been repeatedly implicated in the establishment of later, poor academic functioning and achievement (Ansary & Luthar, 2009; Campbell, Spieker, Burchinal, Poe, & The NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2006; Chen et al., 1997; Maguin & Loeber, 1996; Stipek & Miles, 2008). Notwithstanding, a recent analysis of six large national data sets indicated that, controlling for children’s prior academic achievement and attentional problems, high levels of children’s externalizing behavior problems around the time of kindergarten entry were weakly associated with low levels of subsequent reading and/or math achievement in only two of the six data sets (Duncan et al., 2007). Specifically, their analysis of data drawn from the Children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth—79 indicated that high levels of externalizing problems at kindergarten entry were associated with low levels of reading achievement at age 13–14. The analysis of data drawn from the NICHD Study of Child Care and Youth Development also indicated that low levels of externalizing and attention problems at kindergarten entry were associated with high achievement scores in the fifth grade. Although some inconsistencies are present in the literature, there is a growing body of evidence in support of the notion that children’s early behavioral problems impact their subsequent academic competence.

There are also models for suggesting that initial internalizing problems might lead to later academic difficulties or externalizing problems. Internalizing symptoms may undermine academic competence by eroding cognitive functioning (Maughan et al., 2003), or by interfering with attentional focus and participation during classroom learning activities (Roeser, van der Wolf, & Strobel, 2001). Internalizing symptoms, such as being withdrawn or passive, may also discourage the use of adaptive learning strategies, resulting in poorer academic self-efficacy and performance (Roeser et al., 2001). Initial internalizing problems also have been linked to increases in externalizing problems through two processes. First, children may “mask” depression by acting out and engaging in antisocial activities (Carlson & Cantwell, 1980). Second, because of high rates of peer rejection and isolation, children with initial internalizing problems may be more likely to develop relationships with deviant peers, who would then model and reinforce them for engaging in antisocial activities (Oland & Shaw, 2005).

Although more consistent relations have been established between initial externalizing problems leading to later academic and/or internalizing difficulties, a few studies have found support for cascading processes in relation to initial internalizing problems. For example, internalizing difficulties have been linked to poor future academic performance (Chen, Rubin, & Li, 1995; Flook et al., 2005), particularly during the transition to middle school (McGee, Feehan, Williams, & Anderson, 1992). For example, in a study by Roeser, Eccles, and Sameroff (2000), seventh-grade students experiencing high levels of emotional distress demonstrated poorer academic performance in eighth grade and poorer perceived academic confidence in the ninth grade. In another investigation with a primarily African American sample, highly anxious first-grade students were disproportionately likely to show very low levels of reading and mathematics achievement in the eighth grade (Grover, Ginsburg, & Ialongo, 2007).

Cascading Effects: The Academic Incompetence Hypothesis

The second hypothesis to account for cascading effects suggests that initial failures in academic functioning instigate the development of externalizing or internalizing symptoms, or exacerbate current symptoms of problem behavior (Chen et al., 1997; Dishion et al., 1991; Maughan et al., 2003; Williams & McGee, 1994). Deviant peer affiliation is one such mechanism by which academic incompetence has been postulated to lead to externalizing problems (Deater-Deckard, 2001), with poor school performance often resulting in children being placed in classes with other low-achieving children (Hallinan & Kubitschek, 1999) and increasing their probability of associating with deviant peers (Kelly, 1975) and demonstrating subsequent antisocial behavior (Dishion, McCord, & Poulin, 1999).

For externalizing problems, empirical validation for the Academic Incompetence hypothesis has been found in some, but not all instances. An analysis of data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study—Kindergarten Class revealed that children who struggled with reading during the 1st grade were more likely to experience externalizing problems in the 3rd grade (Morgan, Farkas, Tufis, & Sperling, 2008). Miles and Stipek (2006) revealed similar associations between reading achievement and aggressive behavior in the 3rd and 5th grades. In one recent study, affluent, low-achieving 10th grade students reported particularly high levels of externalizing problems during the 10th, 11th, and 12th grades (Ansary & Luthar, 2009). Low academic competence has been found to be associated with elevations in externalizing problems (Dishion et al., 1991; Williams & McGee, 1994), and improvements in school performance have been observed to lead to desistence from delinquent behavior (Maguin & Loeber, 1996; Thornberry, Lizotte, Krohn, Smith, & Porter, 2003). However, Chen and colleagues (1997) did not corroborate this link when studying academic competence among a Chinese sample of school-age children.

Academic problems also have been linked to future internalizing problems. Theoretically, poor academic functioning would be associated with increases in negative affect (Patterson & Stoolmiller, 1991) and decreases in positive self-representations, as well as increases in self-deprecation and decreases in perceived control, all of which would be posited to lead to increases in internalizing problems (Herman, Lambert, Reinke, & Ialongo, 2008; Maughan et al., 2003; Waniel, Priel, & Besser, 2006). Finally, doing very poorly in school might also reduce children’s positive social status in some cases, which might also lead to negative self-perceptions and subsequent socioemotional problems (Chen et al., 1997).

Despite strong theoretical premises, validation for academic incompetence leading to later internalizing difficulties ismixed. One longitudinal study indicated that students transitioning to middle school (i.e., seventh graders) who perceived themselves as academically competent and earning good grades were less emotionally distressed by the end of eighth grade than children who perceived themselves to be less academically competent (Roeser et al., 2000). Another analysis of data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study—Kindergarten Class revealed that children who struggled with reading during the first grade were more likely to display internalizing problems in the third grade (Morgan et al., 2008). A study by Herman and colleagues (2008) demonstrated that academic competence in the first grade was associated with children’s depression in the seventh grade via their perceived control assessed during the sixth grade. Conversely, academic competence was not associated with elevations in internalizing problems in three independent samples of older children and young adolescents (e.g., ages 9 and 12: Cole et al., 1996; ages 10 and 9–12: Patterson & Stoolmiller, 1991; ages 16–18: Ansary & Luthar, 2009).

The Timing and Appearance of Cascading Effects: School Transitions

Although cascading effects from one domain of child functioning to another could occur during any developmental period, there is reason to believe that they would more likely be manifest during developmental transitions. Recently, several preventive interventions have been designed to capitalize on the social and maturational challenges associated with such developmental periods of change, such as the birth of a child (e.g., Olds, 2002), the onset of the terrible twos (Shaw, Dishion, Supplee, Gardner, & Arnds, 2006), and the transition to adolescence (Dishion & Kavanagh, 2003). As a large part of school-age children’s daily life involves meeting the challenges of school, there is reason to believe that changes in the demands of school might increase the probability that cascading effects from one domain of child functioning to another might be evident. Two of the most pronounced transitional periods associated with the “school-age” period are the transition to formal school entry and the movement from elementary to middle school.

The formal school entry period, which occurs around age 6 for most children, is a period when many children transition from spending most of their day with adults to spending an increasing amount of time with other children (Gross, Shaw, & Moilanen, 2008; Rimm-Kaufman & Pianta, 2000). Even children who have received care from a nonparental caregiver prior to this transition (e.g., preschool, daycare) are vulnerable to experiencing problems (Rimm-Kaufman, 2004). Beginning in kindergarten and continuing in first grade, there is a significant increase in the demands made on children’s social skills, work-related skills (including compliance to instructions and the ability to work independently), self-regulation, and academic readiness. One study using a large, national sample (N = 3,595) found that based on teachers’ reports, almost half of children entering school experienced some difficulty in the transition to formal schooling (Rimm-Kaufman, Pianta, & Cox, 2000). This reality is troubling, because children’s school readiness at the entry to kindergarten is a powerful predictor of their subsequent academic achievement (Duncan et al., 2007).

The transition from elementary school to middle school, which coincides with the transition to adolescence, can also be a time of stress from an academic perspective, as children must adjust to the demands of more teachers and classes than they experienced in elementary school (i.e., 7–10 different classes per week). At the same time, from the perspectives of both physical and social maturation, the transition to middle school is a time where hormonal changes and social expectations are high, as youth face multiple challenges in social domains (e.g., social roles, vocational decisions, peer influences) and neurobehavioral changes associated with puberty (Dahl, 2004). Thus, in contrast to the relatively tranquil period between ages 7 to 10, there is reason to suspect that if cascading effects are to be found, they would more likely occur during the transition to formal schooling (following age 6) and the transition to middle school (ages 10–12).

An Alternative Explanation for Cascading Effects: The Shared Risk Hypothesis

Cascading effects may also be a product of “third variable” factors that may place individuals on a path that underlies adjustment in multiple domains of functioning. Such shared risk factors, including low intelligence, low socioeconomic status, or poor parenting may place individuals on a path that underlies low academic competence and/or high levels of symptoms at school entry, which in turn may promote cascading processes among academic functioning and child problem behaviors. Several risk factors have been jointly associated with academic incompetence, and internalizing and externalizing problems, including temperament (Rothbart, Derryberry, & Hershey, 2000), insecure attachment (Moss et al., 2006), neighborhood adversity (Ingoldsby & Shaw, 2002; McLeod & Shanahan, 1996), ineffective parenting (Barber & Olsen, 1997; Best, Hauser, & Allen, 1997), and low intelligence (Masten et al., 1999). Thus, before attributing increases in maladjustment across domains to prior academic incompetence or problem behavior, it is important to account for how such common risk factors might increase vulnerability to reduced levels of functioning across domains. In the current study, we focus on three factors, all of which have been linked adjustment problems in both socioemotional and academic functioning: intelligence (Fergusson, Horwood, & Ridder, 2005; Rapport, Denney, Chung, & Hustace, 2001; Schonfeld, Shaffer, O’Connor, & Portnoy, 1988), responsive and supportive parenting (Bayer, Sanson, & Hemphill, 2006; Dodici, Draper, & Peterson, 2003; Joussemet, Koestner, Lekes, & Landry, 2005; Supplee, Unikel, & Shaw, 2007), and neighborhood adversity (Chase-Lansdale, Gordon, Brooks-Gunn, & Klebanov, 1997; Connell & Halpern-Felsher, 1997; Ingoldsby et al., 2006; Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn, 2000).

True Studies of Cascade Models

As noted above, although examples of cross-lagged effects among different domains of child adjustment exist (e.g., Beyers & Loeber, 2003; Chen et al., 2000; Cole et al., 1996; Kiesner, 2002), true studies of developmental cascade processes that track cross-domain associations among three domains of functioning across three or more time points are few in number. Here we discuss two particularly relevant examples, including one study by Masten and colleagues (2005) and one by Ansary and Luthar (2009).

Masten and colleagues (2005) studied developmental cascades over a 20-year period in a sample of normally developing participants. They found evidence that externalizing problems, assessed between the ages of 8–12, were associated with poorer academic competence (ages 15–19), which in turn were related to increased risk of later internalizing problems. In addition, high levels of academic competence in late adolescence and early adulthood (ages 18–22) predicted better emotional adjustment as participants approached age 30 (ages 28–32). Overall, the authors concluded that externalizing problems in middle childhood appeared to foreshadow problems in all three domains, from an initial pathway to academic problems in adolescence, and from academic to internalizing problems in early adulthood. These associations remained or were strengthened when other potential causes were included in structural models. These findings lend support to the adjustment erosion and academic competence hypotheses tested in the current study.

Ansary and Luthar (2009) studied cross-domain effects over a 3-year period in a sample of affluent youth in Grades 10, 11, and 12. They found that youths who engaged in more serious types of externalizing behaviors (i.e., cluster groups defined by marijuana use and multiple behavioral/emotional problems) during the 10th grade had the lowest academic achievement and poorest classroom adjustment at all three assessments. Furthermore, low-achieving youth during the 10th grade reported the highest levels of drug use and delinquency at each study wave. There were no observed mean differences in internalizing problems by achievement group. Ansary and Luthar (2009) concluded that the cross-domain effects of youth behavioral maladjustment and academic achievement form a cycle that is likely initiated early in development and perpetuated throughout adolescence. Primarily for externalizing problems, these findings with this sample of affluent youth lend additional support to the adjustment erosion and academic competence hypotheses evaluated in this investigation.

The Role of Gender

The sample’s design of only including boys, although limiting our ability to examine sex differences, also provided an opportunity to focus more intensively on how cascading processes might develop among males at risk for poor sociodevelopmental adjustment. It is reasonable to expect that the timing, types, and long-term consequences of developmental cascades vary by gender. The early and middle childhood years may be vital periods for the emergence of boys’ developmental cascades, as boys show high rates of problem behavior prior to adolescence relative to girls, particularly externalizing problems (Crick & Zahn-Waxler, 2003; Hartung & Widiger, 1998). Boys’ early conduct problems may undermine academic competence and achievement as early as kindergarten by limiting boys’ academic engagement or by adversely affecting teacher–child relationships (Dishion et al., 1991; Stipek & Miles, 2008). Whereas associations between academic achievement and internalizing problems tend to be stronger for girls, in contrast to associations between academic achievement and externalizing problems being stronger for boys (Duncan et al., 2007; McCarty et al., 2008; Pomerantz, Altermatt, & Saxon, 2002), we expected to find fewer cascades originating from internalizing problems and more consistent cascading effects originating from externalizing or academic problems in the current sample of boys. Boys’ tendency toward younger initiation into problem behaviors probably also increases the chance that they will experience spillover effects, such as school drop-out and limited academic achievement by early adulthood (Fergusson & Lynskey, 1998; Vitaro, Laroque, Janosz, & Tremblay, 2001). Thus, it is important to examine childhood-era developmental cascades to see if they are particularly detrimental to boys’ behavioral development and academic success. Understanding these processes is vital for the development and implementation of effective prevention and intervention efforts.

The Current Study

The goals of the current study were to both replicate and extend the findings of Masten and colleagues (2005), testing for cascading effects among externalizing, internalizing, and academic problems, and examining the possibility that risk factors common to these three domains of child functioning would account for cascading effects. In contrast to the work by Masten and colleagues (2005), which covered a 20-year period spanning from the school-age period to adulthood, the current study included a more intensive focus on the period between formal school entry and the transition to middle childhood and adolescence. This design of the school-age period allowed us to extend Masten’s prior work by investigating whether cascading effects would be more pronounced during times of school transition. Other novel features of the study included the use of a cohort of boys at high risk for demonstrating problem behavior in the three domains of functioning being studied based on their low socioeconomic status and the use of parent, youth, and teacher reports, as well as observations to provide assessments of youth functioning and shared risk factors. We followed Masten and colleagues’ (2005) analytic strategy, which involved evaluating a series of nested path models.

Based on prior research (i.e., Masten et al., 2005), we expected to find evidence of cascading effects, specifically that externalizing problems would adversely impact academic competence. Our predictions for academic problems to lead to externalizing or internalizing problems were more tentative as such effects were not found during comparable age periods in the Masten study (but have been suggested in others; e.g., Williams & McGee, 1994). Furthermore, we expected to see evidence of such cascading processes more so during the earliest and later assessments of the study period, which coincided with the transition to elementary school (ages 6–8) and the transition to middle school (ages 10–12), respectively. Finally, we expected shared risk factors, such as parenting and IQ, to attenuate but not fully account for cascading effects of early problem behavior and academic functioning.

Method

Participants and procedures

This study used data from a larger project on vulnerability and resiliency in boys growing up in low socioeconomic status families (Shaw, Gilliom, Ingoldsby, & Nagin, 2003). The sample was restricted to boys because the original intent of the study was to examine precursors of antisocial behavior. Funding did not permit recruitment of a sufficiently large sample of girls who were expected to show serious levels of antisocial behavior. Recruitment took place at Women, Infants, and Children Nutritional Supplement Program clinics throughout the metropolitan Pittsburgh area. Mothers with male infants 6 to 17 months of age were asked to take part in a longitudinal study on child development. Of 421 mothers approached at the Women, Infants, and Children sites, 311 participated in the first assessment when boys were at an average age of 18 months. At the time of the first assessment, mothers ranged in age from 17 to 43 years (M = 27.82, SD = 5.33).

Two-hundred ninety-four families participated in at least one of the assessments (95% of the original sample); of those families, 291 boys, mothers, and teachers provided sufficient data to be included in analyses (i.e., they had data at one or more waves for each construct; 94% of the original sample). The resulting analytic sample was composed primarily of European American (51.9%) and African American boys (38.8%), with a small number of biracial (8.9%) and Hispanic (0.3%) participants. As the vast majority of non-European American boys were part or fully African American, we refer to non-European American boys as African American for the duration of this manuscript. Mean monthly family income at Time 1 (i.e., when the boys were age 6) was $1,662 (SD = $1,167), corresponding to a per capita average of $371 (SD = $293). At Time 1, 69% of the primary caregivers identified themselves as being married or living with a partner; 19% identified themselves as single (never married); and the remaining 12% were separated, divorced, or widowed. Analyses were conducted to compare retained and attrited families on all study characteristics. No significant differences were revealed in terms of demographic characteristics, cascade variables, or common risk factors.

Measures

Study constructs were measured using a combination of self-and observer-reported instruments. Data were collected during home and lab visits, during which research assistants interviewed the participating boys and their primary caregiver (at least 90% of the primary caretakers were mothers at each study time point). Teachers were recruited separately with the permission of the primary caretaker, and they completed questionnaires by mail.

Externalizing problems

The externalizing problems factors were constructed using two or four scale scores at each study time point. Two maternal-report subscales from the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983) were used at all study waves, specifically the 20-item aggressive behavior subscale (average α = 0.90; sample item: “cruelty, bulling, or meanness to others”) and the 13-item delinquent behavior subscale (average α = 0.64; sample item: “runs away from home”). Parents rated each item on a 3-point response scale, ranging from 0 (not true [as far as you know]) to 2 (very true or often true). At ages 10, 11, and 12, boys also reported on their antisocial behavior during the last year using the Self-Reported Delinquency Measure (Elliott, Huizinga, & Ageton, 1985). Boys responded to a total of 33 items on a 3-point response scale, ranging from 0 (never) to 2 (more often). Twenty-two of these items were used to form two subscales, including a 9-item aggression subscale (average α = 0.57; sample item: “In the past year, have you on purpose broken or damaged or destroyed something belonging to your parents or other people in the family?”) and a 13-item delinquency subscale (average α = 0.66; sample item: “Since [1 year ago], have you taken something from a car that did not belong to you?”). All subscale scores were calculated by summing the responses to all items, such that high scores correspond to high levels of externalizing problems. All available subscale scores within each time point were standardized before performing principal component analyses and calculating Cronbach alpha values (see Table 1). As these preliminary analyses indicated that these subscales formed consistent factors, improper factor scores were calculated by summing the standardized subscale scores within each time point. Using z scores to compute such factor scores outside of principal components analysis is beneficial when the means and/or standard deviations of the indicator variables vary, as it will ensure that variables with smaller values are not overshadowed by the variables with larger values (C. Garbin, personal communication, June 2, 2009). Descriptive statistics for the standardized externalizing variables and all other study variables are provided in Table 2.

Table 1.

Principal component analysis loadings

Construct Age 6 Age 8 Age 10 Age 11 Age 12
Externalizing Problems
CBCL delinquency .90 .92 .83 .83 .83
CBCL aggressive behavior .90 .92 .78 .82 .82
Elliott delinquency .77 .54 .69
Elliott aggression .64 .70 .76
Eigenvalue (% variance) 1.63 (89%) 1.69 (84%) 2.30 (57%) 2.13 (53%) 2.42 (60%)
Cronbach α 0.77 0.81 0.75 0.70 0.78

Academic Competence
SSRS academic competence .91 .93 .94 .96 .96
TRF academic competence .91 .93 .94 .96 .96
Eigenvalue (% variance) 1.65 (83%) 1.71 (86%) 1.75 (87%) 1.83 (92%) 1.84 (92%)
Cronbach α 0.79 0.84 0.86 0.91 0.92

Internalizing Problems
CBCL withdrawn .84 .85 .85 .87 .87
CBCL somatic problems .60 .63 .63 .69 .44
CBCL anxious/depressed .85 .86 .85 .85 .88
CDI .46 .28 .48
Eigenvalue (% variance) 1.78 (59%) 1.86 (62%) 2.04 (51%) 2.03 (51%) 1.96 (49%)
Cronbach α 0.65 0.68 0.66 0.63 0.61

Note: All factor loadings were generated in principal component analysis. The Cronbach alpha values were calculated using standardized scale scores. CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; SSRS, Social Skills Rating Scale; TRF, Teacher Report Form; CDI, Child Depression Inventory.

Table 2.

Study variable descriptive statistics

Wave Variable N M (SD) Range
Age 6 Externalizing problems 262 0.00 (1.80) −2.88–6.93
Academic competence 206 0.00 (1.79) −4.48–2.08
Internalizing problems 262 0.00 (2.29) −2.64–9.77
Age 8 Externalizing problems 247 0.00 (1.84) −2.41–8.31
Academic competence 155 0.00 (1.84) −5.07–2.13
Internalizing problems 247 0.00 (2.35) −2.48–12.12
Age 10 Externalizing problems 246 0.00 (2.93) −3.93–13.88
Academic competence 168 0.00 (1.86) −4.78–2.84
Internalizing problems 246 0.00 (2.68) −3.12–11.50
Age 11 Externalizing problems 242 0.00 (2.87) −3.70–12.77
Academic competence 152 0.00 (1.63) −3.79–2.55
Internalizing problems 242 0.00 (2.72) −3.15–13.27
Age 12 Externalizing problems 238 0.00 (2.70) −3.57–12.56
Academic competence 157 0.00 (1.91) −4.42–2.77
Internalizing problems 238 0.00 (2.70) −2.94–12.34
Age 2 Parenting quality 275 33.07 (6.20) 14.00–45.00
Age 5.5 Intelligence 227 96.05 (15.05) 47.00–132.00
Age 1.5–5 Neighborhood adversity 288 0.34 (1.02) −1.52–3.10

Academic competence

The academic competence factors were based on two sources at each time point. The first source was a single factor created using six items from the Social Skills Rating Scale (Gresham & Elliott, 1990; average α = 0.88). Items included “uses free time in an acceptable way,” “finishes class assignments within time limits,” “follows your directions,” “puts work materials or school property away,” “attends to your instructions,” and “easily makes transitions from one classroom activity to another.” Teachers responded to these items on a 3-point response scale, with possible responses ranging from 0 (never) to 2 (very often). The second source was a scale score constructed using six items from the Teacher Report Form (TRF; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001; average α = 0.91). Items included “fails to finish things he starts,” “difficulty following directions,” “has difficulty learning,” “poor school work,” “underachieving, not working up to potential,” and “fails to carry out assigned tasks.” Teachers responded to the TRF items on a 3-point response scale, and responses ranged from 0 (not true [as far as you know]) to 2 (very true or often true). Each subscale score was computed by summing the items, and high scores correspond to better academic competence. The internal consistency and factor structure of the academic competence variables were examined using the same procedure employed for the externalizing variables. Likewise, these preliminary analyses confirmed that these two indices of academic competence shared significant common variance (see Table 1).

Internalizing problems

At all study waves, the internalizing problems factors were defined by three factors from the CBCL (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983). These included the 14-item anxious/depressed subscale (average α = 0.80; sample item: “cries a lot”), the 9-item withdrawn subscale (average α = 0.70; sample item: “refuses to talk”), and the 9-item somatic complaints subscale (average α = 0.62; sample item: “overtired”). Parents rated each item on a 3-point response scale, ranging from 0 (not true [as far as you know]) to 2 (very true or often true).At ages 10, 11, and 12, boys also reported on their symptoms of depression during the last 2 weeks using an abbreviated form of the Child Depression Inventory (Kovacs, 1985; average α = 0.61). Boys were presented with 10 sets of statements corresponding to symptoms of depression, and selected the statement that was most true for them (sample set: “I am sad once in a while,” “I am sad many times,” and “I am sad all the time”). The scale anchors range from zero to two, with zero corresponding to the lowest levels and two corresponding to highest levels of depression. All subscale scores were calculated by summing the responses to all items, such that high scores correspond to high levels of internalizing problems. The internal consistency and factor structure of the internalizing problems variables were examined using the same procedure employed for the externalizing and academic competence variables. Likewise, these preliminary analyses confirmed that these indices of internalizing problems shared significant common variance (see Table 1).

Parenting quality (age 2)

Study examiners completed the 36-item Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) Inventory for Families of Infants and Toddlers (Caldwell & Bradley, 1978) during home-based observations and an interview with the mother at age 2 years. This version of the HOME taps dimensions of emotional and verbal responsivity, acceptance of the child’s behavior, organization of the environment, provision of play materials, parental involvement with the child, and opportunities for variety. The standard total HOME scale score is the sum of these six factors, and was used in analyses (overall α = 0.82, sample responsivity item: “parent spontaneously praises child at least twice”; sample organization of environment item: “child is taken regularly to doctor’s office or clinic”; parental involvement item: “parent keeps child in visual range, looks at often”).

Intelligence (age 5.5)

Child intellectual skills were evaluated at the age 5.5 laboratory assessment using a four-subtest short form of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence—Revised (Wechsler, 1989), a commonly used measure of children’s cognitive abilities. The block design, geometric design, information, and vocabulary subtests from the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence were selected because of their high average correlation with overall full-scale IQs and the high test–retest reliability and internal consistency coefficients of these subtests (Sattler, 1990, 1992). Full-scale IQ scores were derived according to prorating procedures described in Sattler (1990, 1992).

Neighborhood adversity (composite)

Neighborhood adversity was constructed based on US Census data. Families’ addresses were geocoded at the block group level, the smallest unit for which all census data are available. Addresses were collected from 1991 to 2003, and were matched to census block groups at each study wave. Based on methods devised by Wikström and Loeber (2000) and adapted by Winslow and Shaw (2007) and Schonberg, Shaw, Beck, Vanderbilt, and McTeague (2005), a composite variable of neighborhood adversity was generated using the following census block group level variables: (a) median family income, (b) percentage of families below poverty level, (c) percentage of households on public assistance, (d) percentage unemployed, (e) percentage of single-mother households, (f) percentage African American, and (g) percentage with a Bachelor’s degree or higher. Wikström and Loeber (2000) selected these variables based on previous research investigating associations between neighborhood-level structural characteristics and boys’ antisocial behavior (for more details, see Wikström & Loeber, 2000). For data from assessments collected between 1990 and 1995, the 1990 census data were used. These individual variables were standardized, summed, and then averaged (after reverse scoring median family income and percent with a bachelor’s degree) to create an overall neighborhood adversity factor score for each block group. Past research demonstrates that these variables correlate highly (Ingoldsby, Shaw, Schonberg, & Flanagan, 2003; Wikström & Loeber, 2000). Preliminary analyses indicated that the neighborhood adversity indices were highly correlated over time (r range = .75–.95). Consequently, neighborhood adversity scores were averaged across time points (i.e., at boys’ ages 1.5, 2, 3.5, and 5 years) to generate a composite index of cumulative childhood neighborhood risk.

Analysis plan

Preliminary analyses

Prior to calculating improper factor scores, all variables used to form the three cascade constructs and the observed shared risk factors were screened for normality. Descriptive statistics indicated that almost all variables were normally distributed and within acceptable limits of skewness and kurtosis. The single exception was somatic problems at age 8, which was moderately skewed and kurtotic (skew = 3.70, kurtosis = 22.09). A square-root transformation reduced skew and kurtosis to acceptable levels (skew = 1.06, kurtosis = 0.52), prior to standardizing this subscale and conducting any further analyses.

Missing data

Explorations of missing data patterns were completed with SPSS Missing Values Analysis. The results of this analysis suggested that data were missing completely at random, Little’s missing completely at random χ2 (1,926) = 2002.42, p >.05 (Little, 1988). On average, 45% of teacher-reported academic competence data were missing at each time point (range = 30–57% missing), compared to an average of 17% missing parent- and child-reported internalizing or externalizing scores (range = 10–22% missing). We further examined missing data patterns by exploring mean-level differences between missing and present cases for all study variables. This exploration revealed only one instance in which missing data were associated with levels of that construct at another point in time (i.e., nonrandom missingness): specifically, boys missing academic competence data at age 8 (n = 77; M = −0.34, SD = 1.80) had lower levels of academic competence at age 10 than boys who had teacher-reported academic competence data at age 8 (n = 91; M = 0.28, SD = 1.87), F (1, 166) = 4.72, p < .05, η2 = 0.03. Additional analyses indicated that there were other instances in which boys missing and present differed. Boys missing academic competence data at age 8 had higher levels of externalizing problems at age 8 (n = 103; M = 0.34, SD = 1.85) and internalizing problems (M = 0.41, SD = 2.81) at age 8 compared to boys with teacher-reported academic competence data at age 8 (n = 144; externalizing M = −0.24, SD = 1.79; internalizing M = −0.29, SD = 1.91), externalizing F (1, 245) = 6.05, p < .05, η2 = 0.02, internalizing F (1, 245) = 5.40, p < .05, η2 = 0.02. Boys missing academic competence data at age 8 also had higher levels of parent-reported externalizing problems at age 11 (M = 0.47, SD = 3.18) and poorer home environments at age 2 (n = 129; M = 32.00, SD = 6.63) than those with academic competence data at age 8 (age 11 externalizing n = 144; M = −0.24, SD = 1.79; age 2 parenting n = 146; M = 34.02, SD = 5.64), age11 externalizing F (1, 240) = 4.83, p < .05, η2 = 0.02, age 2 parenting F (1, 273) = 7.45, p < .01, η2 = 0.03. The same pattern was evidenced for academic competence at age 12, such that boys missing these data (n = 100; M = 0.52, SD = 3.53) had higher levels of parent- and self-reported externalizing problems than boys who had academic competence data (n = 146; M = −0.36, SD = 2.38), F (1, 244) = 5.47, p < .05, η2 = 0.02. Likewise, boys missing academic competence data at age 12 also had higher internalizing problems at age 10 (n = 99; M = 0.54, SD = 3.11) than boys with academic competence data at age 12 (n = 147; M = −0.37, SD = 2.28), F (1, 244) = 7.16, p < .01, η2 = 0.03. Finally, boys missing CBCL data(parent-reported externalizing and internalizing) at age 8 (n = 37; M = 29.84, SD = 7.16) had poorer home environments at age 2 than boys whose parents completed the CBCL at age 8 (n = 238; M = 33.58, SD = 5.89), F (1, 273) = 12.12, p < .001, η2 = 0.04. These analyses suggested that data were missing at random: In only one instance was missingness associated with subsequent levels of the same construct (i.e., academic competence at age 8 with age 10 scores), and in a handful of other instances, missingness was associated with previous or subsequent levels of other study constructs. Of the 289 tests performed, just 9 revealed significant associations (3%). As these few associations were small in magnitude, we interpreted them as largely being due to chance. Consequently, we employed full information maximum likelihood when fitting models, which estimates missing values using all available data and can even be used with data that are not missing at random (for more information, see Enders, 2001). Full information maximum likelihood based parameter estimates provide less biased information than ad hoc procedures such as listwise deletion, pairwise deletion, or imputation (Schafer, 1997).

Cascade analyses

We followed Masten and colleagues’ (2005) overall analytic strategy. We initially evaluated a series of nested path models to determine whether the adjustment erosion or competence hypotheses were supported (see Table 3). The first model evaluated each construct’s stability and contemporaneous correlations across the three factors. This model provided the basis for all other nested models. Ten cross-domain cascade paths were added in the second model, which modeled the hypothesized developmental cascade. In the third model, 2 additional cross-domain paths were added, corresponding to early academic competence effects. This was followed by the inclusion of paths between the latent externalizing and internalizing dimensions, which required 8 additional paths. Finally, the fifth and final model included paths from internalizing to subsequent academic competence, which required 4 additional paths.

Table 3.

Hierarchically nested cascade path models

Age 6–8 Paths Age 8–10 Paths Age 10–11 Paths Age 11–12 Paths
Model 1: Continuity Modela
Ext6 → Ext8 Ext8 → Ext10 Ext10 → Ext11 Ext11 → Ext12
AC6 → AC8 AC8 → AC10 AC10 → AC11 AC11 → AC12
Int6 → Int8 Int8 → Int10 Int10 → Int11 Int11 → Int12

Model 2: Basic Cascades Modelb
Ext6 → AC8 Ext8 → AC10 Ext10 → AC11 Ext11 → AC12
AC8 → Ext10 AC10 → Ext11 AC11 → Ext12
AC8 → Int10 AC10 → Int11 AC11 → Int12

Model 3: Academic Cascades Beginning Earlierc
Ext6 → AC8 Ext8 → AC10 Ext10 → AC11 Ext11 → AC12
AC6 → Ext8 AC8 → Ext10 AC10 → Ext11 AC11 → Ext12
AC6 → Int8 AC8 → Int10 AC10 → Int11 AC11 → Int12

Model 4: Adding Externalizing–Internalizing Linksd
Ext6 → AC8 Ext8 → AC10 Ext10 → AC11 Ext11 → AC12
Ext6 → Int8 Ext8 → Int10 Ext10 → Int11 Ext11 → Int12
AC6 → Ext8 AC8 → Ext10 AC10 → Ext11 AC11 → Ext12
AC6 → Int8 AC8 → Int10 AC10 → Int11 AC11 → Int12
Int6 → Ext8 Int8 → Ext10 Int10 → Ext11 Int11 → Ext12

Model 5: Full Cascade Modele
Ext6 → AC8 Ext8 → AC10 Ext10 → AC11 Ext11 → AC12
Ext6 → Int8 Ext8 → Int10 Ext10 → Int11 Ext11 → Int12
AC6 → Ext8 AC8 → Ext10 AC10 → Ext11 AC11 → Ext12
AC6 → Int8 AC8 → Int10 AC10 → Int11 AC11 → Int12
Int6 → Ext8 Int8 → Ext10 Int10 → Ext11 Int11 → Ext12
Int6 → AC8 Int8 → AC10 Int10 → AC11 Int11 → AC12

Note: Contemporaneous correlations were also included in all models. Ext, externalizing problems; AC, academic competence; Int, internalizing problems.

a

Included in all subsequent models.

b

Total of 10 cross-domain paths.

c

Total of 12 cross-domain paths.

d

Total of 20 cross-domain paths.

e

Total of 24 cross-domain paths.

Shared risk analyses

Once the best-fitting cascade model was selected, a separate model evaluating the shared risk factors hypothesis was estimated. In this model, the three common risk factor variables were simultaneously regressed upon age 6 externalizing, internalizing, and academic competence, as were their indirect effects via any cascade pathways revealed in the previous stage of modeling. The presence of significant indirect effects indicated that a particular developmental cascade was at least partially attributable to parenting quality, intelligence, or neighborhood adversity. Bootstrapping was employed to estimate the standard errors and 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals of these coefficients (McCartney, Burchinal, & Bub, 2006).

All analyses were conducted in Mplus version 4.0 (Muthén & Muthén, 2004). Model fit was evaluated through the examination of several indices. The fit of a path model was considered acceptable if it had a nonsignificant chi-square fit statistic (χ2), a χ2/df ratio smaller than 3.00, a comparative fit index larger than 0.90, a root mean square error of approximation smaller than 0.05, and a standardized root mean square residual close to 0 (Kline, 1998). Changes in chi-square fit values were examined when comparing nested models.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Bivariate correlations between all study variables are presented in Table 4. These suggested moderate to high stability of each longitudinal study construct. The correlations between adjacent scores of externalizing (r range = .56–.80) and internalizing problems (r range = .57–.71) were higher than the adjacent indices of academic competence (r range = .47–.58). There was also evidence for potential cross-lagged pathways. Externalizing and internalizing scores were moderately and negatively correlated with subsequent academic competence values (externalizing r range = −.11 to −.30; internalizing r range = −.13 to −.30). Correlations between contemporaneous internalizing and externalizing scores were also moderate (r range = .30–.46).

Table 4.

Study variable correlations

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
  1. Ext6
  2. AC6 −.18*
  3. Int6 .52*** −.07a
  4. Ext8 .66*** −.11 .30***
  5. AC8 −.28** .47*** −.13 −.30***
  6. Int8 .44*** −.14 .59*** .49*** −.18*
  7. Ext10 .48*** −.18* .17* .56*** −.17* .24***
  8. AC10 −.31*** .40*** −.17* −.30*** .56*** −.23** −.32***
  9. Int10 .27*** −.23** .47*** .33*** −.29** .62*** .39*** −.22**
10. Ext11 .42*** −.13 .34*** .43*** −.22** .31*** .68*** −.43***
11. AC11 −.24** .42*** −.18* −.20* .48*** −.16 −.25** .58***
12. Int11 .30*** −.24** .42*** .34*** −.36*** .50*** .31*** −.30***
13. Ext12 .45*** −.10 .32*** .50*** −.25** .33*** .69*** −.42***
14. AC12 −.24** .28** −.21* −.20* .35** −.16* −.21* .56***
15. Int12 .35*** −.11 .40*** .40*** −.24** .48*** .36** −.26**
16. IQ −.11 .36*** .01 −.06 .25** .00 −.09 .25**
17. Parenting −.21** .24** −.05 −.14* .14 .02 −.23*** .15
18. Neigh Adv .27*** −.19** .12 .17** −.18* .00 .25*** −.14

9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17

  1. Ext6
  2. AC6
  3. Int6
  4. Ext8
  5. AC8
  6. Int8
  7. Ext10
  8. AC10
  9. Int10
10. Ext11 .35***
11. AC11 −.22** −.27**
12. Int11 .57*** .54*** −.25**
13. Ext12 .42*** .80*** −.30*** .50***
14. AC12 −.19* −.27** .53*** −.20* −.32***
15. Int12 .60*** .46*** −.29** .71*** .60*** −.24**
16. IQ −.01 −.10 .21* −.12 −.15* .16 −.13
17. Parenting .01 −.22** .16 −.11 −.23** .08 −.01 .37***
18. Neigh Adv .05 .18** −.07 .02 .19** −.18* −.01 −.26*** .41***

Note: Ext, externalizing problems; AC, academic competence; Int, internalizing problems; IQ, intelligence; Neigh Adv, neighborhood adversity.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

The shared risk factors were also modestly correlated with some cascade variables. At all time points, externalizing problems were negatively associated with parenting (r range = −.14 to .23) and positively associated with neighborhood adversity (r range = .17–.27). Intelligence was negatively associated with externalizing problems at age 12 only (r = −.15), and was positively associated with academic competence from ages 6 to 11 (r range = .21–.36). Academic competence was also negatively correlated with neighborhood adversity at ages 6, 8, and 12 (r range = −.18 to −.19), and positively correlated with parenting at age 6 only (r = .24). There were no significant correlations between the shared risk factors and internalizing problems.

Cascade analyses

Table 5 shows the model fitting results for the cascade analyses. Models 2 through 5 all fit the data significantly better than Model 1. As evidenced by the significant chi-square change statistic, Model 4 provided a better fit to the data than Models 2 or 3. Consequently, Model 4 was retained for subsequent analyses.

Table 5.

Model fit statistics

Model CFI RMSEA SRMR χ2 df χ2/df Δχ2 (df)
1 0.907 0.079 (0.067–0.092) 0.142 220.02 78 2.82
2 0.936 0.071 (0.057–0.084) 0.092 166.47 68 2.45 53.55 (10)***
3 0.936 0.071 (0.058–0.085) 0.088 163.66 66 2.48 2.81 (2)
4 0.948 0.069 (0.054–0.084) 0.071 137.61 58 2.37 26.05 (8)**
5 0.947 0.072 (0.057–0.087) 0.068 135.23 54 2.50 2.38 (4)

Note: CFI, comparative fit index; RMSEA, root mean square error of approximation; SRMR, standardized root mean square residual.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

Figure 1 shows the standardized path coefficients for all paths of Model 4. Without exception, all of the within-construct continuity paths were positive and significant (p < .001). On average, the stabilities for externalizing problems were the highest, ranging from 0.59 to 0.73, followed by those for internalizing problems, which ranged from 0.50 to 0.63. Academic competence was also stable, ranging from 0.44 to 0.57. Eight of the directed cascade paths were significant and in the expected direction. Externalizing problems at age 6 and at age 8 were negatively associated with academic competence at age 8 and age 10, respectively, such that high levels of age 6 and 8 externalizing problems were indicative of poorer academic competence at age 8 and 10, controlling for prior academic competence. Externalizing problems at age 6 were positively associated with internalizing at age 8, such that high levels of externalizing problems at age 6 were linked to high levels of internalizing problems at age 8, controlling for prior levels of internalizing problems at age 6. In turn, academic competence at ages 10 and 11 were negatively associated with externalizing at ages 11 and 12: high levels of academic competence were associated with low levels of externalizing problems, controlling for prior problem levels. Likewise, high levels of academic competence at age 10 were linked to low levels of internalizing problems at age 11. Finally, high levels of externalizing problems at age 11 were associated with high levels of internalizing problems at age 12.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The developmental cascade model with externalizing–internalizing links. The inset table contains the model’s within-time point correlations/covariances. Dashed arrows indicate nonsignificant pathways. Parenthetical values refer to the total portion of the variance explained by the cross-lagged pathways (ΔR2 values). Ext, externalizing problems; AC, academic competence; Int, internalizing problems.p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Shared risk analyses

Following Masten and colleagues’ (2005) analytic plan, we also evaluated the shared risk factors hypothesis by including three control variables in the model. The goal of these analyses was to see if the shared risk factors had indirect effects on study cascades. This model fit the data reasonably well, χ2 fit (94) = 181.96, p < .001, comparative fit index = 0.94, root mean square error of approximation = 0.057 (95% confidence interval = 0.044–0.069), standardized root mean square residual = 0.071 (see Figure 2). Neighborhood adversity predicted externalizing problems at age 6, such that high levels of early neighborhood risk were indicative of greater externalizing problems (R2 = .08). High levels of intelligence were associated with higher academic competence at age 6 (R2 = .15). No shared risk factors were associated with internalizing problems at age 6. Intelligence was indirectly related to externalizing problems at age 11 via academic competence at ages 6 through 10, such that high levels of intelligence at age 6.5 years were associated with lower levels of externalizing problems at age 11 (B = −0.003, SE = 0.001, p < .05). Neighborhood adversity also was indirectly associated academic competence at age 8 via externalizing problems at age 6, such that high levels of neighborhood risk were indicative of low levels of academic competence at age 8 (B = −0.079, SE = 0.039, p < .05). There were no other indirect effects of early risk factors. The addition of shared risk factors had a small effect on three cascade paths. Two weaker pathways present in Model 4 were no longer significant contributors once the shared risk factors were included in the model. These included the cascade pathways from academic competence at age 10 internalizing problems at age 11 (p < .06) and the pathway from age 11 externalizing problems to age 12 internalizing problems (p < .08). The marginally significant pathway from age 10 externalizing problems at age 10 to internalizing problems at age 11 became significant (p < .05).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

The shared risk model. The inset table contains the model’s additional within-time point correlations/covariances. Dashed arrows indicate nonsignificant paths between shared risk factors and age 6 cascade constructs or marginally significant cascade paths. All other nonsignificant cascade paths included in Figure 1 were also modeled but are omitted for the sake of simplicity. NA, neighborhood adversity; Ext, externalizing problems; IQ, intelligence; AC, academic competence; Par, parenting; Int, internalizing problems. p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Discussion

In the current study, we tested a series of hypotheses to increase understanding of developmental cascade effects in childhood and early adolescence. Consistent with both adjustment erosion and academic incompetence models, there was some evidence to suggest that developmental cascade effects were evident. Externalizing problems were associated with low levels of academic competence and high levels of internalizing psychopathology throughout the study period. Path models suggested that high levels of externalizing problems at ages 6 and 8 were respectively associated with poorer academic competence at ages 8 and 10. In turn, poor academic competence at ages 10 and 11 was related to high levels of externalizing problems at ages 11 and 12. Poor academic competence at age 10 was also linked to high levels of internalizing problems at age 11. High levels of externalizing problems around the time of initial school entry (age 6) and during the transition to middle school and adolescence (age 11) were predictive of high levels of internalizing problems at ages 8 and 12. Finally, shared risk factors measured in early childhood did not markedly alter the developmental cascades evidenced in this study, but intelligence was indirectly related to externalizing problems at age 11 via prior academic competence, and neighborhood adversity was indirectly associated with academic competence at age 8 via age 6 externalizing problems.

Contributions

This is the one of the first known studies to fully test a cascade model and the first we are aware of to evaluate such issues intensively during the school-age period. Additional investigations into developmental cascades appear to be warranted, as this study’s findings demonstrated partial convergence with Masten and colleagues’ (2005) previous study and other transactional studies (e.g., Ansary & Luthar, 2009). In particular, cascading effects first became evident during the transition to primary school when high levels of externalizing problems were associated with poorer subsequent academic competence (consistent with Chen et al., 1997; Dishion et al., 1991; Masten et al., 2005) and higher levels of internalizing problems at age 8 (Capaldi, 1992). Subsequent cascading effects occurred throughout the study period, with these first cascades followed by significant effects between low academic competence at age 10 and subsequent internalizing problems at ages 10 and 12 (consistent with Masten et al., 2005; Morgan et al., 2008; Roeser et al., 2000) and externalizing problems at ages 11 and 12 (consistent with Dishion et al., 1991; Maguin & Loeber, 1996; Morgan et al., 2008; Williams & McGee, 1994). Notably, these cascades were revealed in an earlier developmental period than the cascades found in Masten and colleagues’ (2005) study. They also found direct effects of internalizing difficulties on externalizing problems, but we revealed direct cascading effects from externalizing to internalizing problems. All in all, these discrepancies are likely attributable to the more intensive measurement employed over our shorter study’s time span during an earlier phase of development.

The current pattern of results is relatively unique in linking multiple paths from early childhood to adolescence (Bosquet & Egeland, 2006); however, individual paths between domains of functioning are consistent with previous research. For example, an initial path links externalizing problems during middle childhood (ages 6 and 8) to low levels of subsequent academic competence (ages 8 and 10), corroborating a previously found relationship (e.g., Campbell et al., 2006; Capaldi, 1992; Chen et al., 1997; Morgan et al., 2008). Likewise, the direct links between externalizing to internalizing symptoms have also been demonstrated elsewhere (e.g., Capaldi, 1992; Locke & Newcomb, 2001; Reinherz et al., 1993). Also consistent with prior work, low levels of academic competence during the transition to adolescence were associated with high levels of subsequent internalizing and externalizing problems (Dishion et al., 1991; Maguin & Loeber, 1996; Roeser et al., 2000). Finally, the shared risk model revealed associations between the early childhood neighborhood environment with externalizing symptoms at school age, as well as links between intelligence and academic competence, both of which have been found in previous research (Campbell, Shaw, & Gilliom, 2000; Fergusson et al., 2005; Rapport et al., 2001; Supplee et al., 2007; Winslow & Shaw, 2007).

Note that all of these significant paths emerged after accounting for autoregressive effects of individual domains, which were moderately high in magnitude in all three domains of child adjustment (i.e., ranging from 0.44 to 0.73 for individual paths). It is noteworthy that despite the use of a conservative analytical approach, 8 of 20 cross-domain paths emerged as significant and a ninth cross-domain path was marginally significant. Furthermore, cascading effects were detectable in ways that were consistent with prior research examining individual paths involving one or two of domains of child functioning (e.g., externalizing problems predicting poor later academic competence).

The current study provided support for the adjustment erosion hypothesis regarding externalizing but not internalizing problems. Externalizing symptoms were linked to poorer academic competence during middle childhood, first from ages 6 to 8, and then again from ages 8 to 10. Behaviors that commonly accompany externalizing problems (e.g., impulsivity or hostility) may directly limit children’s opportunities for productive classroom engagement (Chen et al., 1997; Dishion et al., 1991). For example, boys who struggle with self-regulation may spend much of their time in the classroom showing hostility toward peers instead of acquiring basic academic skills. Their disruptive behavior may lead to their removal from the classroom altogether, thus further reducing children’s opportunities to learn or their motivation to achieve. Over time, children with externalizing problems may experience heightened levels of negative arousal while in the school setting, to the point that their negative moods interfere with their school performance (Roeser et al., 2001). The shared risk analyses suggested that neighborhood adversity played a small role in the link between early externalizing problems and academic competence in middle childhood. From an early age, boys who live in adverse neighborhoods may be inundated with socialization messages that simultaneously promote antisocial activities and devalue academic success (Ceballo, McLoyd, & Toyokawa, 2004; Huesmann & Guerra, 1997), and these values become entrenched as socialization experiences accumulate. At the same time, academic expectations increase, shifting from basic academic competencies in kindergarten to being required to complete homework on a regular basis by the second grade. These changes in the school environment can make academic experiences even more negative for boys who struggle to control their behavior. Overall, possessing limited academic skills in conjunction with holding antischool beliefs and highly negative feelings about school may interact to make it increasingly difficult for children with behavioral problems to make meaningful gains in academic competence.

These findings are somewhat contradictory to the results revealed in Duncan and colleagues’ (2007) analysis of six national longitudinal data sets. We urge readers to carefully consider important methodological similarities and differences when interpreting the current study in light of Duncan and colleagues’ (2007) findings. In that investigation, Duncan and colleagues (2007) found small-sized effects of children’s externalizing difficulties at school entry on their subsequent academic achievement, and these links were observed in just two of the six data sets analyzed. Most notably, Duncan and colleagues (2007) focused on children’s academic achievement, whereas the current investigation focused on children’s adaptive classroom behaviors that support academic achievement. Children’s externalizing problems might impact academic achievement via classroom behaviors, a possibility that was not examined in the Duncan and colleagues (2007) study. This seems likely, particularly as a recent inquiry suggested that aggressive children are more likely to have conflict with their teachers, which reduces their engagement in the classroom, thus undermining their achievement (Stipek & Miles, 2008). Furthermore, the selected national data sets differ from each other and from the current study in terms of sample and design characteristics, which also might contribute to the inconsistent pattern of findings. It is intriguing that the two analyses that revealed negative associations between externalizing problems and academic achievement employed measures identical or similar to those used in the current study: in the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth, mothers reported on their young children’s problems with attention and externalizing behavior using the Behavior Problems Index (Peterson & Zill, 1986), a measure that strongly resembles the CBCL (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1983). In the NICHD Study of Child Care and Youth Development data set, teachers reported on children’s attention and externalizing problems using the TRF (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). Additional research is required to determine whether observed links between externalizing problems and academic competence/achievement are attributable to the use of the CBCL, TRF, and similar measures.

Externalizing problems were also directly associated with internalizing problems during the early school years (age 6–8) and the transition to adolescence (ages 11–12), both times of school transitions and observed changes in children’s self-worth (e.g., Wigfield et al., 1997). To the best of our knowledge, the vast majority of this sample of boys was enrolled in the Pittsburgh public school system, and unless they were retained, they transitioned from elementary to middle school between the fifth and sixth grades. Aggressive boys may establish peer relationships with other children displaying externalizing symptoms, or be rejected by peers when they enter new school settings, when other children may view their acting-out behaviors as objectionable or unpleasant. Peer rejection may lead to reductions in boys’ self-worth, contributing to symptoms of depression or anxiety (Burks et al., 1995; Coie et al., 1995; Morrow, Hubbard, Rubin, & McAuliffe, 2008). This is consistent with the results of other studies (e.g., Capaldi, 1992; Maguin & Loeber, 1996).

We found little support for the adjustment erosion hypothesis for internalizing problems. The current investigation revealed no pathways between internalizing problems and subsequent externalizing behaviors or academic competence. This was somewhat in contrast to the findings of Masten and colleagues (2005), who, in their investigation of developmental cascades between ages 8 and 32 years, revealed that high levels of internalizing symptoms at ages 15–19 were indicative of fewer externalizing problems at ages 18–22. This discrepancy is possibly due to two differences between the two studies. The current investigation focused on cascades during the period of middle childhood to early adolescence in a sample of boys purposely recruited because they were at risk for externalizing difficulties. In contrast, Masten et al. (2005) analyzed data provided by a relatively low-risk community sample followed between middle childhood and early adulthood. Either the focal age range or sample variations could explain these differences. Neither the current study nor Masten and colleagues (2005) supported the notion that internalizing symptoms affected subsequent academic competence. A limited number of investigations have provided tentative evidence for this link (e.g., Flook et al., 2005; Roeser et al., 2000), but in these studies prior levels of academic competence have largely not been controlled when exploring relations with internalizing difficulties. It is possible that emotional problems have small impacts on academic competence, but these small effects do not emerge when longitudinal stability in academic competence is controlled.

The academic incompetence hypothesis was supported for externalizing problems, such that low levels of academic competence at ages 10 and 11 predicted high levels of externalizing problems at ages 11 and 12. The shared risk analysis indicated that a small portion of age 11 externalizing problems was attributable to intelligence via academic competence from ages 6 to 10. There are a few possible mechanisms to explain this pattern of results and their timing. As discussed earlier, having low intelligence or doing poorly in school work typically leads to having children placed in classrooms with children who display high levels of externalizing problem behavior, an issue that might become more important during the transition to middle school and adolescence, when children’s rates of covert antisocial behavior also begin to increase (Patterson, Shaw, Snyder, & Yoerger, 2005). Thus, one could imagine modeling of antisocial activities increasing as a result of increased exposure to less academically oriented peers becoming more salient around the age of 10 versus in earlier periods of childhood (Dishion et al., 1999). Boys may increase their rates of disruptive behavior, particularly covert antisocial acts, to gain social acceptance with these peers (Bagwell, Coie, Terry, & Lochman, 2000).

The academic incompetence hypothesis was also supported for internalizing problems, such that low levels of academic competence at ages 8 and 10 were predictive of high levels of internalizing symptoms at ages 10 and 11. As a result of low academic attainment in the classroom, boys may experience increases in negative feedback from teachers and parents, which may serve as stressors inducing negative moods (Patterson & Stoolmiller, 1991), or to perpetuate the formation or maintenance of poor self-representations or low perceived control (e.g., Herman et al., 2008; Maughan et al., 2003; Waniel et al., 2006). This feedback from others may take on greater importance as boys transition from elementary to middle school and with boys’ greater appreciation for their social standing during the latter part of the school-age period. In turn, any or all of these reactions may thus lead to symptoms of anxiety and depression (e.g., Accordino, Accordino, & Slaney, 2000; Richardson, Bergen, Martin, Roeger, & Allison, 2005).

Study Limitations and Future Directions

Despite advancing our understanding of developmental cascades during the middle childhood and early adolescence, the study has several notable methodological limitations. First, one limitation involved the measurement of study constructs. With the exception of the three shared risk factors (interviewer-based observations of early parenting, a performance-based standardized test of intelligence, and the census-based neighborhood adversity composite), study data were based on questionnaires, which could have resulted in inflated correlations between constructs because of common method variance (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003). This limitation can be addressed by including additional objective measurement techniques whenever possible. This should be taken into consideration particularly for academic competence, which did not include any index of academic performance such as standardized achievement test scores in the present study. Likewise, it would also have been useful to have more conclusive information about the timing of boys’ school transitions, including whether boys had been academically retained. However, we were able to limit reporter bias by obtaining reports from multiple informants (i.e., teachers provided reports of academic competence, and mothers and sons reported on the son’s symptoms of psychopathology). The shared risk factors were assessed at different points of development, which is also a limitation of the current study, as is the relatively low internal consistency of the measures of internalizing problems. Researchers are urged to take deliberate care when planning future studies on developmental cascades (e.g., include multiple reporters, use alternate and reliable methodologies, and measure potential antecedents prior to school entry).

Second, these results should be interpreted with caution, because the path model fit did not fully attain commonly accepted standards. Model modification indices suggested that estimating correlations between concurrent variables’ residual variances would improve model fit. The presence of these correlations indicates that variables’ measurement errors are correlated (Loehlin, 2004) or that an underlying common cause is not represented in the model (Klein, 1998). These covariances are to be expected in these types of models, as participants completed the same measures repeatedly over time (Little, Preacher, Selig, & Card, 2007). Without a specific conceptual reason to estimate such correlations, we chose not to include them in the path models presented here. Researchers interested in studying developmental cascades in other samples may also wish to adopt a hybrid structural equation modeling strategy (i.e., modeling growth in latent variables versus observed factors) versus the path modeling approach used in the current study. Hybrid structural equation modeling permits the estimation of correlations between the residuals of the repeated-measures indicators used to construct the latent factors, which permits the isolation of measurement error. However, parameter estimates from such complex models are not necessarily trustworthy or stable if the study sample is too small or if the latent constructs are modeled with too few indicators (Kline, 1998). As both of these were limitations of the current study, we employed a more conservative path modeling approach, which allowed us to fit a relatively complex model despite these practical constraints.

Third, as alluded to briefly above, the study sample was comprised entirely of low-income boys from an urban context who were primarily of European American or African American ethnicity; thus, the current results may be less generalizable to girls, children from nonurban settings, and boys and girls from other ethnicities. This study revealed cascade effects earlier in development (Ansary & Luthar, 2009) and over a shorter period of time than has been studied previously (Masten et al., 2005). It is possible that developmental cascades occur more or less rapidly than observed in this and other existing samples. Likewise, cascade processes could function entirely differently for girls during childhood and adolescence, as was suggested in a recent investigation on longitudinal cross-lagged associations between delinquency and substance use, a construct that is moderately correlated with antisocial behavior (Mason & Windle, 2002). Cascades could also occur on individual timetables, which may be obscured in variable-focused investigations like the current study. Alternatively and as noted earlier, these earlier cascading effects could be attributable to the timing of measurement, which coincided with important developmental transitions involving school, although cascade effects were not limited to these transition points in the current study. Although the findings are consistent with the hypothesis that transitions at school are likely to be associated with increases in cascade effects, the current study was unfortunately limited in that we did not have precise data on when children made such school transitions. Thus, a practical direction for future research includes determining whether developmental cascades operate in shorter periods and/or are more likely to occur during points of development transition (e.g., the end of high school or the “terrible twos”). This further knowledge may also assist in the construction of appropriate preventive interventions for boys and girls.

Implications for intervention

Overall, the results of this study reiterate the importance of considering cascade effects when intervening to prevent problem behavior or to promote healthy adjustment. If replicated across child gender, ethnicity, socioeconomic background, and similar developmental periods, the current findings could have important implications for early identification and prevention of problem behavior and promotion of academic success. These findings suggest that it may be possible to prevent declines in academic competence and escalations in internalizing and externalizing problems during the transitions to formal schooling and the transition to middle school (and adolescence), as well as during the school-age period (e.g., effects of academic competence on emerging internalizing problems between ages 8 and 10) by intervening early in the developmental cascade. As the shared risk factors considered in the current study and other preexisting qualities appeared to help place youth on stable behavioral and academic trajectories prior to the early elementary school years (e.g., Masten et al., 1999), it appears that it may be possible to prevent detrimental cascades by intervening early in the home or neighborhood. Thus, interventions targeted at improving early cognitive/intellectual development and/or promoting financial investments in struggling urban neighborhoods might be beneficial for promoting academic competence and/or preventing early psychopathology (e.g., the Family Check-Up intervention; Dishion et al., 2008).

Acknowledgments

The research reported in this paper was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants MH 50907 and MH 01666 (to D.S.S.). We are grateful to the work of the staff of the Pitt Mother & Child Project for their years of service and to our study families for making the research possible. Our thanks also go to Heather Gross, Luke Hyde, Christopher Trentacosta, and Ella Vanderbilt-Adriance for their assistance during the preparation of this manuscript.

Footnotes

Portions of this study were previously presented at the 2007 meeting of the Society for Research on Child Development.

References

  1. Accordino DB, Accordino MP, Slaney RB. An investigation of perfectionism, mental health, achievement, and achievement motivation in adolescents. Psychology in the Schools. 2000;37:535–545. [Google Scholar]
  2. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and revised Child Behavior Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  3. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  4. Ansary NS, Luthar SS. Distress and academic achievement among adolescents of affluence: A study of externalizing and internalizing problem behaviors and school performance. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21:319–341. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bagwell CL, Coie JD, Terry RA, Lochman JE. Peer clique participation and social status in preadolescence. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly. 2000;46:280–305. [Google Scholar]
  6. Barber BK, Olsen JA. Socialization in context: Connection, regulation, and autonomy in the family, school, and neighborhood, and with peers. Journal of Adolescent Research. 1997;12:287–315. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bayer JK, Sanson AV, Hemphill SA. Parent influences on early childhood internalizing difficulties. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2006;27:542–559. [Google Scholar]
  8. Best KM, Hauser ST, Allen JP. Predicting young adult competencies: Adolescent era parent and individual influences. Journal of Adolescent Research. 1997;12:90–112. [Google Scholar]
  9. Beyers JM, Loeber R. Untangling developmental relations between depressed mood and delinquency in male adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2003;31:247–266. doi: 10.1023/a:1023225428957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bosquet M, Egeland B. The development and maintenance of anxiety symptoms from infancy through adolescence in a longitudinal sample. Development and Psychopathology. 2006;18:517–550. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Burke JD, Loeber R, Lahey BB, Rathouz PJ. Developmental transitions among affective and behavioral disorders in adolescent boys. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005;46:1200–1210. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.00422.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Burks VS, Dodge KA, Price JM. Models of internalizing outcomes of early rejection. Development and Psychopathology. 1995;7:683–695. [Google Scholar]
  13. Burt KB, Obradović J, Long JD, Masten AS. The interplay of social competence and psychopathology over 20 years: Testing transactional and cascade models. Child Development. 2008;79:359–373. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01130.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. Manual for the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment. Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  15. Campbell SB, Shaw DS, Gilliom M. Early externalizing behavior problems: Toddlers and preschoolers at risk for later maladjustment. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12:467–488. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400003114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Campbell SB, Spieker S, Burchinal M, Poe MD The NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Trajectories of aggression from toddlerhood to age 9 predict academic and social functioning through age 12. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2006;47:791–800. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01636.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Capaldi DM. Co-occurrence of conduct problems and depressive symptoms in early adolescent boys: A 2-year follow-up at grade 8. Development and Psychopathology. 1992;4:125–144. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499001959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Carlson GA, Cantwell DP. Unmasking masked depression in children and adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1980;137:445–449. doi: 10.1176/ajp.137.4.445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ceballo R, McLoyd VC, Toyokawa T. The influence of neighborhood quality on adolescents’ educational values and school effort. Journal of Adolescent Research. 2004;19:716–739. [Google Scholar]
  20. Chase-Lansdale PL, Gordon RA, Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov PK. Neighborhood and family influences on the intellectual and behavioral competence of preschool and early school-age children. In: Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ, Aber JL, editors. Neighborhood poverty: Vol. 1. Context and consequences for children. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 1997. pp. 79–118. [Google Scholar]
  21. Chen X, Li D, Li Z-Y, Li B-S, Liu M. Sociable and prosocial dimensions of social competence in Chinese children: Common and unique contributions to social, academic, and psychological adjustment. Developmental Psychology. 2000;36:302–314. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.36.3.302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Chen X, Rubin KH, Li B. Depressed mood in Chinese children: Relations with school performance and family environment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1995;63:938–947. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.63.6.938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Chen X, Rubin KH, Li D. Relation between academic achievement and social adjustment: Evidence from Chinese children. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33:518–525. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.3.518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Coie J, Terry R, Lenox K, Lochman J, Hyman C. Childhood peer rejection and aggression as predictors of stable patterns of adolescent disorder. Development and Psychopathology. 1995;7:697–713. [Google Scholar]
  25. Cole DA, Martin JM, Powers B, Truglio R. Modeling causal relations between academic and social competence and depression: A multitrait–multimethod longitudinal study of children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1996;105:258–270. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.105.2.258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Cole DA, Maxwell SE. Testing mediational models with longitudinal data: Questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112:558–577. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Connell JP, Halpern-Felsher B. How neighborhoods affect educational outcomes in middle childhood and adolescence: Conceptual issues and an empirical example. In: Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ, Aber JL, editors. Neighborhood poverty: Vol. 1. Context and consequences for children. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 1997. pp. 174–199. [Google Scholar]
  28. Crick NR, Zahn-Waxler C. The development of psychopathol-ogy in females and males: Current progress and future challenges. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:719–742. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Dahl RE. Adolescent brain development: A period of vulnerabilities and opportunities. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2004;1021:1–22. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Deater-Deckard K. Annotation: Recent research examining the role of peer relationships in the development of psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2001;42:565–579. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Dishion TJ. The family ecology of boys’ peer relations in middle childhood. Child Development. 1990;61:874–892. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02829.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Dishion TJ, Kavanagh K. Intervening in adolescent problem behavior: A family-centered approach. New York: Guilford Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  33. Dishion TJ, McCord J, Poulin F. When interventions harm: Peer groups and problem behavior. American Psychologist. 1999;54:755–764. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.54.9.755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Dishion TJ, Patterson GR, Stoolmiller M, Skinner ML. Family, school, and behavioral antecedents to early adolescent involvement with antisocial peers. Developmental Psychology. 1991;27:172–180. [Google Scholar]
  35. Dishion TJ, Shaw DS, Connell AM, Gardner F, Weaver C, Wilson M. The Family Check-Up with high-risk indigent families: Preventing problem behavior by increasing parents’ positive behavior support in early childhood. Child Development. 2008;79:1395–1414. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01195.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Dodici BJ, Draper DC, Peterson CA. Early parent–child interactions and early literacy development. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education. 2003;23:124–136. [Google Scholar]
  37. Duncan GJ, Dowsett CJ, Claessens A, Magnuson K, Huston AC, Klebanov P, et al. School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43:1428–1446. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Elliott DS, Huizinga D, Ageton SS. Explaining delinquency and drug use. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  39. Enders CK. A primer on maximum likelihood algorithms available for use with missing data. Structural Equation Modeling. 2001;8:128–141. [Google Scholar]
  40. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Ridder EM. Show me the child at seven II: Childhood intelligence and later outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005;46:850–858. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01472.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Fergusson DM, Lynskey MT. Conduct problems in childhood and psychosocial outcomes in young adulthood: A prospective study. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders. 1998;6:2–18. [Google Scholar]
  42. Flook L, Repetti RL, Ullman JB. Classroom social experiences as predictors of academic performance. Developmental Psychology. 2005;41:319–327. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.2.319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gresham FM, Elliott SN. Social skills rating system manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  44. Gross HE, Shaw DS, Moilanen KL. Reciprocal associations between boys’ externalizing problems and mothers’ depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2008;36:693–709. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9224-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Grover RL, Ginsburg GS, Ialongo N. Psychosocial outcomes of anxious first graders: A seven-year follow-up. Depression and Anxiety. 2007;24:410–420. doi: 10.1002/da.20241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hallinan MT, Kubitschek WN. Curriculum differentiation and high school achievement. Social Psychology of Education. 1999;3:41–62. [Google Scholar]
  47. Hartung CM, Widiger TA. Gender differences in the diagnosis of mental disorders: Conclusions and controversies of the DSM-IV. Psychological Bulletin. 1998;123:260–278. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.123.3.260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Herman KC, Lambert SF, Reinke WM, Ialongo NS. Low academic competence in first grade as a risk factor for depressive cognitions and symptoms in middle school. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2008;55:400–410. doi: 10.1037/a0012654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hinshaw SP. Externalizing behavior problems and academic under-achievement in childhood and adolescence: Causal relationships and underlying mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;111:127–155. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Huesmann LR, Guerra NG. Children’s normative beliefs about aggression and aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997;72:408–419. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.72.2.408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Ingoldsby E, Shaw DS. Neighborhood contextual factors and the onset and progression of early-starting antisocial pathways. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2002;5:21–55. doi: 10.1023/a:1014521724498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Ingoldsby E, Shaw DS, Winslow E, Schonberg M, Gilliom M, Criss M. Neighborhood disadvantage, parent–child conflict, neighborhood peer relationships, and early antisocial behavior problem trajectories. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2006;34:303–319. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9026-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Ingoldsby EM, Shaw DS, Schonberg MA, Flanagan C. Neighborhood context, neighborhood peers, and young boy’s trajectories of overt and covert problems. Tampa, FL: Poster presented at the Biennial Meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development; Apr, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  54. Joussemet M, Koestner R, Lekes N, Landry R. A longitudinal study of the relationship of maternal autonomy support to children’s adjustment and achievement in school. Journal of Personality. 2005;73:1215–1235. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00347.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kelly DH. Status origins, track position, and delinquent involvement: A self-report analysis. Sociological Quarterly. 1975;16:264–271. [Google Scholar]
  56. Kiesner J. Depressive symptoms in early adolescence: Their relations with classroom problem behavior and peer status. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2002;12:463–478. [Google Scholar]
  57. Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  58. Kovacs M. The children’s depression inventory. Psychopharmacological Bulletin. 1985;21:995–998. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Lahey BB, Loeber R, Burke J, Rathouz PJ, McBurnett K. Waxing and waning in concert: Dynamic comorbidity of conduct disorder with other disruptive and emotional problems over 7 years among clinic-referred boys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111:556–567. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.111.4.556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J. The neighborhoods they live in: The effects of neighborhood residence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126:309–337. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Little RJA. A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1988;83:1198–1202. [Google Scholar]
  62. Little TD, Preacher KJ, Selig JP, Card NA. New developments in latent variable panel analyses of longitudinal data. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2007;31:357–365. [Google Scholar]
  63. Locke TF, Newcomb MD. Alcohol involvement and dys-phoria: A longitudinal examination of gender differences from late adolescence to adulthood. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2001;15:227–236. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Loehlin JC. Latent variable models. 4th ed. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  65. Maguin E, Loeber R. Academic performance and delinquency. Crime and Justice: A Review of Research. 1996;20:1–41. [Google Scholar]
  66. Mason WA, Windle M. Reciprocal relations between adolescent substance use and delinquency: A longitudinal latent variable analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111:63–76. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.111.1.63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Masten AS, Burt KB, Coatsworth JD. Competence and psy–chopathology in development. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Developmental psychopathology: Vol 3. Risk, disorder and adaptation. 2nd ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2006. pp. 696–738. [Google Scholar]
  68. Masten AS, Hubbard JJ, Gest SD, Tellegen A, Garmezy N, Ramirez M. Competence in the context of adversity: Pathways to resilience and maladaptation from childhood to late adolescence. Development and Psychopathology. 1999;11:143–169. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499001996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Masten AS, Roisman GI, Long JD, Burt KB, Obradović J, Riley JR, et al. Developmental cascades: Linking academic achievement and externalizing and internalizing symptoms over 20 years. Developmental Psychology. 2005;41:733–749. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.5.733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Maughan B, Rowe R, Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M. Reading problems and depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2003;31:219–229. doi: 10.1023/a:1022534527021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. McCartney K, Burchinal MR, Bub KL. Best practices in quantitative methods for developmentalists. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2006;71 doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2006.07103001.x. (3, Serial No. 285) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. McCarty CA, Mason WA, Kosterman R, Hawkins JD, Lengua LJ, McCauley E. Adolescent school failure predicts later depression among girls. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2008;43:180–187. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.01.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. McConaughy S, Achenbach T. Comorbidity of empirically based syndromes in matched general populations and clinical samples. Journal of Child Psychiatry and Psychology. 1994;35:1141–1157. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01814.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. McGee R, Feehan M, Williams S, Anderson J. DSM-III disorders from age 11 to age 15 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1992;31:50–59. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199201000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. McLeod J, Shanahan M. Trajectories of poverty and children’s mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1996;37:207–220. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Miles SB, Stipek D. Contemporaneous and longitudinal associations between social behavior and literacy achievement in low-income elementary school children. Child Development. 2006;77:103–117. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00859.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Morgan PL, Farkas G, Tufis PA, Sperling RA. Are reading and behavior problems risk factors for each other? Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2008;41:417–436. doi: 10.1177/0022219408321123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Morrow MT, Hubbard JA, Rubin RM, McAuliffe MD. The relation between childhood aggression and depressive symptoms: The unique and joint mediating roles of peer rejection and peer victimization. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly. 2008;54:316–340. [Google Scholar]
  79. Moss E, Smolla N, Cyr C, Dubois-Comtois K, Mazzarello T, Berthiaume C. Attachment and behavior problems in middle childhood as reported by adult and child informants. Development and Psychopathology. 2006;18:425–444. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. 3rd ed. Los Angeles: Author; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  81. Oland A, Shaw DS. Pure vs. co-occurring externalizing and internalizing symptomatology in children: The potential role of socio-developmental milestones. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2005;8:247–270. doi: 10.1007/s10567-005-8808-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Olds D. Prenatal and infancy home visiting by nurses: From randomized trials to community replication. Prevention Science. 2002;3:153–172. doi: 10.1023/a:1019990432161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Patterson GR, Shaw DS, Snyder JJ, Yoerger K. Changes in maternal ratings of children’s overt and covert antisocial behavior. Aggressive Behavior. 2005;31:473–484. [Google Scholar]
  84. Patterson GR, Stoolmiller M. Replications of a dual failure model for boy’s depressed mood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1991;59:491–498. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.59.4.491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Peterson JL, Zill N. Marital disruption, parent–child relationships, and behavioral problems in children. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 1986;48:295–307. [Google Scholar]
  86. Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Lee J-Y, Podsakoff NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology. 2003;88:879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Pomerantz EM, Altermatt ER, Saxon JL. Making the grade but feeling distressed: Gender differences in academic performance and internal distress. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2002;94:396–404. [Google Scholar]
  88. Rapport MD, Denney CB, Chung KM, Hustace K. Internalizing behavior problems and scholastic achievement in children: Cognitive and behavioral pathways as mediators of outcome. Journal of Child Clinical Psychology. 2001;30:536–551. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3004_10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Reinherz HZ, Giaconia RM, Pakiz B, Silverman AB, Farst AK, Lefkowitz ES. Psychosocial risk for major depression in late adolescence. Journal of American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1993;32:1155–1163. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199311000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Reitz E, Deković M, Meijer AM. The structure and stability of externalizing and internalizing problem behavior during early adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2005;34:577–588. [Google Scholar]
  91. Richardson AS, Bergen HA, Martin G, Roeger L, Allison S. Perceived academic performance as an indicator of risk of attempted suicide in young adolescents. Archives of Suicide Research. 2005;9:163–176. doi: 10.1080/13811110590904016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Rimm-Kaufman S. School transition and school readiness: An outcome of early childhood development. In: Tremblay RE, Barr RG, Peters RD, editors. Encyclopedia on early childhood development. Montreal: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  93. Rimm-Kaufman SE, Pianta RC. An ecological perspective on the transition to kindergarten: A theoretical framework to guide empirical research. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2000;21:491–511. [Google Scholar]
  94. Rimm-Kaufman SE, Pianta RC, Cox MJ. Teachers’ judgments of problems in the transition to kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2000;15:147–166. [Google Scholar]
  95. Risi S, Gerhardstein R, Kistner J. Children’s classroom peer relationships and subsequent educational outcomes. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2003;32:351–361. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3203_04. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Roeser RW, Eccles JS, Sameroff AJ. School as a context of early adolescents’ academic and social–emotional development: A summary of research findings. Elementary School Journal. 2000;100:443–471. [Google Scholar]
  97. Roeser RW, van der Wolf K, Strobel KR. On the relation between social–emotional and school functioning during early adolescence: Preliminary findings from Dutch and American samples. Journal of School Psychology. 2001;39:111–139. [Google Scholar]
  98. Rothbart MK, Derryberry D, Hershey K. Stability of temperament in childhood: Laboratory infant assessment to parent report at seven years. In: Molfese VJ, Molfese DL, editors. Temperament and personality development across the life span. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 2000. pp. 85–119. [Google Scholar]
  99. Sattler JM. Assessment of children. 3rd ed. San Diego, CA: Jerome M. Sattler; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  100. Sattler JM. Assessment of children. 3rd ed. San Diego, CA: Jerome M. Sattler; 1992. rev. [Google Scholar]
  101. Schafer JL. Analysis of incomplete multivariate data. London: Chapman & Hall; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  102. Schonberg MA, Shaw DS, Beck JE, Vanderbilt E, McTeague EE. Risk factors for boys’ antisocial behavior in the context of prolonged neighborhood poverty and prosperity. Poster presented as the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research on Child Development; Atlanta, GA. Apr, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  103. Schonfeld IS, Shaffer D, O’Connor P, Portnoy S. Conduct disorder and cognitive functioning: Testing three causal hypotheses. Child Development. 1988;59:933–1007. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1988.tb03252.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Schwartz D, Gorman AH, Nakamoto J, McKay T. Popularity, social acceptance, and aggression in adolescent peer groups: Links with academic performance and school attendance. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42:1116–1127. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Shaw DS, Dishion TJ, Supplee L, Gardner F, Arnds K. A family-centered approach to the prevention of early-onset antisocial behavior: Two-year effects of the Family Check-Up in early childhood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2006;74:1–9. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Shaw DS, Gilliom M, Ingoldsby EM, Nagin D. Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Developmental Psychology. 2003;39:189–200. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.39.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Stipek D, Miles S. Effects of aggression on achievement: Does conflict with the teacher make it worse? Child Development. 2008;79:1721–1735. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01221.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Supplee LH, Unikel E, Shaw DS. Physical environmental adversity and the protective role of maternal monitoring in relation to early child conduct problems. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2007;28:166–183. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2006.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Thornberry TP, Lizotte AJ, Krohn MD, Smith CA, Porter PK. Causes and consequences of delinquency: Findings from the Rochester Youth Development study. In: Thornberry TP, Krohn MD, editors. Taking stock of delinquency: An overview of findings from contemporary longitudinal studies. New York: Plenum Press; 2003. pp. 11–46. [Google Scholar]
  110. Vitaro F, Laroque D, Janosz M, Tremblay RE. Negative social experiences and dropping out of school. Educational Psychology. 2001;21:401–415. [Google Scholar]
  111. Waniel A, Priel B, Besser A. Mother and self-representations: Investigating associations with symptomatic behavior and academic competence in middle childhood. Journal of Personality. 2006;74:223–265. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00374.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio TX: Psychological Corporation; 1989. (rev.) [Google Scholar]
  113. Wigfield A, Eccles JS, Yoon KS, Harold RD, Arbreton AJ, Freedman-Doan C, et al. Changes in children’s competence beliefs and subjective task values across the elementary school years: A three-year study. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1997;89:451–469. [Google Scholar]
  114. Wikström PO, Loeber R. Do disadvantaged neighborhoods cause well-adjusted children to become adolescent delinquents? A study of male juvenile serious offending, individual risk and protective factors, and neighborhood context. Criminology. 2000;38:1109–1142. [Google Scholar]
  115. Williams S, McGee R. Reading attainment and juvenile delinquency. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1994;35:441–459. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01733.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Winslow EB, Shaw DS. The impact of neighborhood context on boys’ early conduct problem trajectories in a low-income sample. Aggressive Behavior. 2007;33:207–219. doi: 10.1002/ab.20178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES