Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Mov Disord. 2011 May 24;26(11):2051–2057. doi: 10.1002/mds.23792

Olfactory dysfunction is associated with neuropsychiatric manifestations in Parkinson’s disease

James F Morley 1,2, Daniel Weintraub 1,2,3,4, Eugenia Mamikonyan 3, Paul J Moberg 1,2,3, Andrew D Siderowf 1,2, John E Duda 1,2
PMCID: PMC3168697  NIHMSID: NIHMS292016  PMID: 21611985

Abstract

Background

Hyposmia, psychiatric disorders and cognitive problems are common non-motor manifestations in Parkinson's Disease but how they are related remains unclear.

Methods

To investigate the relationship between olfactory dysfunction and neuropsychiatric manifestations we performed a cross-sectional study of 248 patients at two movement disorders clinics at academic medical centers. Psychiatric measures were the Geriatric Depression Scale-15, Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology, State Anxiety Inventory, Apathy Scale and Parkinson's Psychosis Rating Scale. Cognitive measures were the Mini Mental State Examination, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised, Digit Span, Tower of London-Drexel and the Stroop Color Word Test. Olfaction was tested with the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification test.

Results

There was no significant association between olfaction and mood measures, but psychotic symptoms were more common in patients with olfaction scores below the median (30% vs. 12%, p<0.001). Worse olfaction was associated with poorer memory (Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised delayed recall items: mean(standard deviation) 6.2(3.2) vs. 8.4(2.8), p<0.001) and executive performance (Tower of London total moves, 52(38) vs. 34(21), p<0.001). Odor-identification score was a significant predictor of abnormal performance on these cognitive tests after adjustment for age, sex and disease characteristics in logistic regression models.

Conclusions

The relationship between hyposmia, psychosis, and specific cognitive impairments may reflect the anatomic distribution of Lewy pathology and suggests that olfactory dysfunction could be a biomarker of additional extranigral disease. Future prospective studies are warranted to assess whether hyposmia, a very early feature of Parkinson's disease, might be used to predict the appearance of other common non-motor symptoms.

Keywords: Parkinson's Disease, olfaction, non-motor symptoms, psychiatric symptoms, cognitive symptoms

INTRODUCTION

Olfactory dysfunction (hyposmia) is a common non-motor symptom in Parkinson’s disease (PD), with estimates of prevalence up to 80–90% (1, 2). Hyposmia is present early in disease and, in fact, may be one of the first manifestations of synucleinopathy, appearing years before the onset of motor symptoms (36). The high prevalence, persistence throughout disease, and ease of olfactory testing has fostered interest in the use of olfaction as a biomarker in PD for early diagnostic strategies, differential diagnosis, prediction of clinical outcomes and as a potential therapy-independent marker of disease progression (7, 8).

In some studies, olfactory dysfunction does not demonstrate a clear relationship with commonly used clinical measures such as Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) scores, Hoehn and Yahr stage or disease duration (9), at least when examined throughout the disease course. In contrast, other investigators have observed an inverse relationship between clinical variables and olfaction when odor discrimination or electrophysiologic variables were measured (1012). Additionally, olfactory function does not appear to decline in a linear fashion with clinical progression of disease in individual patients, though some investigators have documented longitudinal worsening of olfactory performance at a group level (1315). Taken together, these observations suggest a complex relationship between markers of olfaction and motor disability and whether hyposmia will ultimately prove a useful predictor of these features in PD remains unclear.

However, there is increasing interest in the relationship between hyposmia and other common non-motor symptoms in PD. For example, a recent report describes that clinical and physiologic markers of autonomic dysfunction were significantly worse in anosmic PD patients compared to those with only mild-moderate hyposmia (16) and hyposmia has been linked to decreased cardiac MIBG uptake in a Japanese population (17, 18). One recent report described associations between olfactory dysfunction, cognitive decline and visual hallucinations while another documented worse olfactory identification in apathetic versus non-apathetic patients (19, 20). However, these associations were established using retrospective chart review (20) or relatively small samples(19). Therefore, the goal of this study was to investigate the relationship between olfactory function, mood disorders, psychotic symptoms and cognitive function using a battery of established neuropsychiatric and cognitive instruments in a large cohort of PD patients.

METHODS

We performed a cross-sectional study of 248 PD patients at the Parkinson’s Disease Research, Education and Clinical Center of the Philadelphia Veterans' Affairs Medical Center and the Parkinson’s Disease and Movement Disorder Center of the University of Pennsylvania. This study represents a secondary analysis of data collected initially for a cross-sectional study of the epidemiology of neuropsychiatric aspects of PD. PD was determined clinically by a movement disorder neurologist consistent with a diagnosis of possible or probable PD by the Gelb criteria (21). Disease severity was measured using the Hohen and Yahr stage (22) and Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) Part III (23). Subjects were not instructed to alter their medication dosing and, therefore, were tested in their typical functional state. Olfactory function was tested by administration of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test (UPSIT) (24). A modified levodopa equivalent daily dosage (LEDD) including immediate and controlled release levodopa formulations and catechol-O-methyltransferase inhibitors was calculated as quantitative data were not available for all medications. All tests for each patient were performed at a single visit unless prohibited by fatigue or scheduling conflict.

Psychiatric assessments

The following psychiatric measures were used: 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale(GDS-15; scores 0–15, higher scores corresponding to greater depression) (25), Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (26) (IDS; scores 0–84, higher scores indicating greater depression severity); the State Form of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (27) (SAI; scores 20–80, higher scores indicating greater anxiety severity); and the Apathy Scale (28) (AS; scores 0 to 42, higher scores indicating greater apathy severity). Psychosis was assessed with a modified version of the Parkinson’s Psychosis Rating Scale (29) (PPRS). A positive response to either the illusions or hallucinations item was coded as psychosis and analyzed as a dichotomous variable.

Cognitive assessments

Global cognitive function was assessed using the Mini Mental State Examination (30) (MMSE). The score on the delayed free recall component of the Hopkins Verbal Learning Task-Revised (31) (HVLT-R; scores 0–12, higher scores indicating better performance) was used as an indication of short-term memory. Executive functioning was assessed with the Tower of London-Drexel test (TOL-DX)(32), recording total moves required to complete the task, more moves indicating worse performance. An additional test of executive function was the inhibition condition of the Stroop Color Word Test (33) (SWCT). Attention was assessed with the backward score on the Digit Span subtest from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition (34), which is thought to be more specific to attentional abilities and working memory compared with the forward Digit Span (35).

Statistical methods

Median UPSIT score (19) was used to divide the cohort into an upper and lower half based on olfactory identification performance (lower scores indicating worse olfaction). Differences in demographics (age, sex, smoking, education), disease characteristics (Hoehn and Yahr stage, UPDRS score, duration and LEDD) and neuropsychiatric test scores between the olfactory groups were determined using independent samples t-tests for scale variables or chi-squared tests for proportions. Non-parametric Mann-Whitney tests were also used for non-normally distributed variables (Hoehn and Yahr stage) with similar results (not shown). Backward logistic regression models were analyzed using an abnormal score on each of the neuropsychiatric tests (or the presence of illusions or visual hallucinations for psychosis) as the dependent variable with olfactory performance, age, sex, disease duration, Hoehn and Yahr stage and LEDD as independent variables for determination of odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals. Age-normalized scores (T<35, Z<−1.5 abnormal) were used for SCWT, HVLT-R and TOL-DX, therefore age was not included as a covariate in these models. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS for Windows version 17.0.

RESULTS

Demographic and clinical characteristics of the cohort-relationship with olfactory function

The mean (SD) age of patients in this cohort was 64 (10) years and 186 (75%) subjects were male. Mean total UPSIT score was 20(7.4), and the median score was 19. Two-hundred thirteen (85%) subjects scored below the 25th percentile, adjusted for age and gender (36), in keeping with prior descriptions of the high prevalence of hyposmia in PD. Subjects below the median of olfactory function in our sample were older and more commonly male (Table 1). There was no significant difference in the proportion of smokers or years of education between olfactory groups (Table 1). Mean UPDRS-III scores and PD duration for the entire sample were 22(10) and 6.6(5.4) years, respectively. The median Hoehn and Yahr stage was 2 (interquartile range 2–2.5). UPDRS scores and Hoehn and Yahr stage were higher in patients with worse olfactory function whereas there was no significant difference in disease duration between the groups (Table 1).

Table 1. Group differences in demographics, disease characteristics and psychiatric or cognitive measures based on odor identification performance.

The cohort was divided in half based on the median UPSIT score. Lower UPSIT score implies worse performance. Data are expressed as mean(SD) for continuous measures or percentage of subjects (sex, smokers, psychosis). P-values for differences between groups are reported from independent samples t-tests for continuous measures or χ2 tests for percentages. UPSIT, University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test. UPDRS, Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale.

UPSIT
bottom
(N=123)
UPSIT
top
(N=125)
p
UPSIT score 13(2.8) 26 (5.2) <0.001
Demographic
Age (years) 67(9.5) 63(10.3) <0.001
Men(%) 84 66 0.001
Education (years) 16(2.9) 16(3.3) 0.20
Smokers(%) 4 6 0.41
Disease Characteristics
Hoehn and Yahr Stage 2.3(0.71) 2.1(0.66) 0.001
UPDRS Part III 24(12) 20(8) 0.001
PD duration (years) 7.3(5.2) 6.0(5.4) 0.07
levodopa dose (mg) 580 (330) 450 (430) 0.01
Psychiatric
Geriatric Depression Scale 3.8(4.0) 4.2(3.7) 0.45
Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology 19(12) 18(13) 0.30
State Anxiety Inventory 41(14) 39(15) 0.18
Apathy Scale 13(6.6) 12(7.1) 0.24
Psychosis (%) 30 12 <0.001
Cognitive
Mini Mental State Examination 28(2.1) 29(1.6) 0.01
Digit Span 6.4(2.4) 7.2(2.6) 0.008
Stroop Color Word Test 29(12) 34(11) 0.001
Tower of London-DX 52(38) 34(21) <0.001
Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised 6.3(3.2) 8.4(2.8) <0.001

Psychiatric correlates of olfactory dysfunction in PD

We compared UPSIT performance with several well-characterized scales measuring depression, anxiety and apathy. In this cohort, mean score on the GDS-15 was 4.0(3.9) and 80 patients (33%) scored ≥5, suggesting clinically significant depressive symptoms. Mean score on the IDS was 18 (13) with 132 patients (52%) scoring in the "depressed" range (≥14). Mean score on the SAI was 40 (14), with 47 patients (16%) scoring>55, suggesting clinically relevant anxiety. The mean Apathy Scale score was 12(6.9), and 88 patients (37%) scored in the abnormal range (≥14). Mean scores on these mood scales did not differ significantly between patients with better or worse olfactory function (Table 1). With respect to items on the PPRS, patients with UPSIT scores below the median were more likely to report psychotic symptoms of visual hallucinations or illusions (Table 1), whereas auditory hallucinations (AH) were not significantly associated with UPSIT performance (11% of subjects in the bottom olfactory group reported AH vs. 7% in the top olfactory group, p=0.34)). UPSIT score was also a significant independent predictor of these psychotic symptoms in a logistic regression model that adjusted for age, sex and other clinical variables (Table 2).

Table 2. Poorer olfactory identification is associated with higher odds of psychotic symptoms and abnormal performance on tests of verbal memory or executive function.

Odds ratios and p-values of having an abnormal score on the indicated tests after adjustment for age, sex, disease severity, duration and medication status. Backward logistic regression was performed with an abnormal score on the indicated test as the dependent variable and UPSIT performance (top or bottom half) together with the indicated covariates as independent variables. Age-specific standardized scores were available for the Stroop, Tower of London and Hopkins Verbal Learning Test, so age was not included as a covariate in the regression models for those tests. Covariates remaining significant at the p=0.05 level in the last iteration of the regression model are listed in the far-right column. UPSIT, University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test. LEDD, levodopa equivalent daily dose.

Adjusted OR (95% CI)
for UPSIT performance
p Significant covariates at
final iteration
Psychiatric
Geriatric Depression Scalea 1.2 (0.70–2.3) 0.42 Hoehn/Yahr
Inventory of Depressive Symptomatologya 1.5 (0.86–2.7) 0.15 Hoehn/Yahr, sex
State Anxiety Inventorya 0.97 (0.47–2.0) 0.94 Hoehn/Yahr, age
Apathy Scale a 1.1 (0.61–1.9) 0.77 Hoehn/Yahr
Psychosisa 2.1 (1.0–4.3) 0.05 UPSIT, age, LEDD
Cognitive
Mini Mental State Examinationa 1.0 (0.47–2.1) 0.99 Hoehn/Yahr, age, sex
Digit Spana 1.0 (0.52–2.1) 0.90 Hoehn/Yahr, age
Stroop Color Word Testb 1.3 (0.50–3.2) 0.61 Hoehn/Yahr, sex, PD duration
Tower of London-DXb 3.1 (1.5–6.2) 0.001 UPSIT, Hoehn/Yahr
Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revisedb 1.8 (1.1–3.7) 0.04 UPSIT, Hoehn/Yahr, duration
a

Initial covariates in the regression model: UPSIT performance, age, sex, PD duration, Hoehn and Yahr stage, LEDD

b

Initial covariates in the regression model: UPSIT performance, sex, PD duration, Hoehn and Yahr stage, LEDD.

Cognitive correlates of olfactory dysfunction in PD

We administered a variety of tests assessing global cognition (MMSE) and specific domains including attention (Digit Span), memory (HVLT-R) and executive function (SCWT and TOL-DX). In this cohort, mean score on the MMSE was 28(1.9) and 40(15%) of subjects scored below 27/30. SCWT mean score was 31(12) and 30(13% scored in the abnormal range. Mean reverse digit span was 6.8(2.5) with 50(21%) scoring <5. For MMSE, DS and SCWT, there were small (e.g. 28.0 vs. 28.6 for MMSE, Table 1) but statistically significant differences in mean score between olfactory groups ; however, olfactory performance was not a significant predictor of abnormal performance on these tests in logistic regression models adjusting for age, sex and disease characteristics. Mean score for the HVLT-R was 7.3(3.1) with 60(27%) of subjects scoring in the abnormal range. The mean total moves for TOL-DX was 43(32) and 54(22%) performed in the abnormal range. Mean scores on HVLT-R and TOL-DX were significantly worse in patients in patients with worse olfaction (Table 1), and UPSIT score below the median was associated with significantly increased odds of abnormal performance on these verbal memory (HVLT-R) and fronto-executive tasks (TOL-DX, Table 2).

DISCUSSION

Olfactory dysfunction is one of the earliest recognized signs of synucleinopathy in PD, and has garnered interest as a potential marker for other clinical manifestations that typically appear later in the disease course. In this study, we have examined the relationship of hyposmia to neuropsychiatric and cognitive outcomes in PD patients. Consistent with prior observations (9, 37), we found a high prevalence of olfactory dysfunction and a substantial fraction of patients in our cohort experienced clinically significant psychiatric symptoms(38). Global cognitive function, as measured by the MMSE, was relatively unaffected in our sample (mean 28), whereas tests of verbal memory and executive functions were abnormal in a significant proportion of our subjects. A small number of subjects had MMSE scores <24, however, excluding these patients from the analysis did not affect our results (not shown). These findings are consistent with prior descriptions of domain-specific cognitive dysfunction in non-demented PD patients(39, 40), and also reflect the relative insensitivity of the MMSE for detection of mild cognitive impairment in PD (41, 42).

While the value of hyposmia as a biomarker of motor symptoms is currently unclear, our results, and those of others, increasingly support a link between olfactory dysfunction and non-motor symptoms. In particular, our results suggest an association between olfactory dysfunction and neuropsychiatric manifestations. Cramer and colleagues have described that apathetic PD patients exhibited worse olfactory performance than non-apathetic patients (19). While we observed trends toward higher levels of apathy and anxiety in patients with worse olfactory function, these did not reach statistical significance (Table 1); however, our studies used different instruments to measure both olfactory function and apathy. A recent retrospective cohort analysis by Stephenson et al. found that worse baseline olfactory function increased the risk of developing visual hallucinations (20). We also observed that psychotic symptoms, such as illusions or visual hallucinations, were significantly more common in patients with the worst olfactory function, and given the association between hallucinations and subsequent dementia, it seems possible that very poor olfaction may herald cognitive decline in PD.

Consistent with this idea, the report from Stephenson et al. also demonstrated an increased risk of incident cognitive dysfunction in patients with the worst baseline olfactory function (20). Cognitive problems were identified by a score of 2 (or 1 with corroborating chart documentation of the problem) on the corresponding UPDRS item and, therefore could not further differentiate the types of problems described or cognitive domains involved. Our data demonstrating an association of UPSIT score with performance on the HVLT-R and TOL-DX suggest the link may be specific to verbal learning as well as executive functions. In addition to these clinical findings, Bohnen and colleagues recently demonstrated that olfactory dysfunction was correlated with radiologic markers of cholinergic denervation in hippocampus and other cortical areas (43). Thus, evidence linking olfactory and cognitive dysfunction, along with potential neurochemical substrates, is developing rapidly.

The early onset of olfactory dysfunction in PD is subserved by the appearance of Lewy pathology in the olfactory system. Braak’s detailed neuropathological analyses (44, 45) suggest that the olfactory bulb and lower brainstem may be induction sites from which Lewy pathology spreads through the midbrain and ultimately to cortical areas. An alternative "top-down" hypothesis has been advanced suggesting that olfactory connections could allow parallel spread both "up" to higher cortical areas and "down" to other recognized induction sites, such as, the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus nerve (46). Variability in the pace and pattern of progression observed among different individuals with PD supports the idea that both mechanisms may coexist, and it is possible that early events in this process influence the route of pathological spread giving rise to clinical phenotypes including hallucinations or cognitive impairment (47). One interpretation of these results is that early, severe olfactory dysfunction may be a biomarker of additional extranigral disease, such as higher cortical Lewy pathology, leading to the development of hallucinations or cognitive dysfunction. Non-motor symptoms are increasingly recognized as common, treatment-refractory and disabling (4850). Understanding early events that lead to non-motor features and, potentially the ability to predict their onset, could have tremendous clinical impact.

The results of our study must be interpreted in the context of several limitations. Using the UPSIT, we only interrogated odor identification whereas odor discrimination and threshold are also affected in PD, though there is some debate whether these tests all measure a common source of variance(51, 52). While our use of ten different neuropsychiatric instruments represents the first study to broadly investigate the relationship of hyposmia with specific psychiatric and cognitive domains, such an analysis necessarily introduces concerns about the influence of multiple statistical testing. However, for those relationships that persisted after adjustment for covariates (psychosis, HVLT-R, TOL-DX), the group differences were most highly significant in the bivariate comparisons (p<0.001, Table 1). Additionally, a recent preliminary analysis in a small cohort (published as a letter to the Editor commenting on Bohnen’s paper(43)) described a correlation between olfaction and verbal memory but not the MMSE (TOL-DX was not tested), supporting the association of hyposmia with specific cognitive domains as we observed.

The magnitudes of effect size we observed were relatively modest (adjusted ORs: 1.8–3.1). Verbaan et al. (53) found small but significant differences in scores between PD patients with and without olfactory impairment on the Scale for Outcomes in Parkinson's disease cognitive and psychiatric measures (SCOPA-COG, SCOPA-PC) in the PROPARK cohort but reported "no significant moderate or strong (>0.4) correlations" between olfactory impairment and other clinical domains, consistent with a small effect size. In their study, a relationship between individual cognitive domains and olfactory impairment, as we have observed, would potentially be minimized by use of an aggregate measure cognitive of function, such as the SCOPA-COG. While these results indicate that olfactory performance, along with other clinical variables, provides some information about neuropsychiatric manifestations (and vice versa), the strength of this relationship is presently unclear.

It is tempting to speculate that the association between olfactory dysfunction (a very early event) and typically later-occurring neuropsychiatric or cognitive phenotypes implies that hyposmia might be used predicatively. The data presented here are cross-sectional and cannot directly support such a claim. However, further prospective studies to determine whether early characteristics of olfactory impairment may predict future disease course are certainly warranted.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This work was supported by NIH grant K23 MH067894 (D.W.).

DISCLOSURES

Dr. Morley has received travel funding from Teva Pharmaceutical Industries Ltd. Dr. Weintraub has served on a scientific advisory board for Boehringer Ingelheim; serves on the editorial board of Movement Disorders; has received speaker honoraria from Boehringer Ingelheim, ACADIA Pharmaceuticals, Novartis, Osmotica Pharmaceutical Corp., BrainCells Inc., Merck Serono, Sanofi-aventis, and Pfizer Inc; and has received/receives research support from Avid Radiopharmaceuticals, Inc., Boehringer Ingelheim, NIH (NIMH K23 MH067894 [PI], NINDS P50 NS053488-01 [Co-Investigator], NIA RO1AG031348 [Site PI], and NINDS R01NS065087 [Co-Investigator]), and from the Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson's Research. Ms. Mamikonyan reports no disclosures. Dr. Siderowf serves on a scientific advisory board for and has received speaker honoraria from Teva Pharmaceutical Industries Ltd.; serves as a consultant for Supernus Pharmaceuticals, Inc.; and receives research support from Avid Radiopharmaceuticals, Inc., the NIH (NINDS U10 NS044451-023 [Site PI], NINDS P50 NS053488-01 [Co-Core Leader and Project Leader], NINDS R43NS0636071 [Site PI], and NINDS R01NS065087 [Co-Investigator]), and from the Institute for Neurodegenerative Disorders. Dr. Duda serves on a grant review panel for the Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson's Research; receives research support from the U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs (Merit Award [PI]), the Michael J. Fox Foundation, and the Samueli Institute; and holds stock in C.R. Bard, Inc., Celgene, Clarient, Inc., and Johnson & Johnson.

Footnotes

AUTHOR ROLES

Morley: 2A, 2B, 3A. Weintraub: 1, 2A, 2C, 3B. Mamikonyan: 1C, 3B. Moberg: 1A, 3B. Siderowf: 1A, 1C, 2C, 3B. Duda: 1, 2C, 3B.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Ansari KA, Johnson A. Olfactory function in patients with parkinson's disease. J Chronic Dis. 1975 Oct;28(9):493–497. doi: 10.1016/0021-9681(75)90058-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kranick SM, Duda JE. Olfactory dysfunction in parkinson's disease. Neurosignals. 2008;16(1):35–40. doi: 10.1159/000109757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Ponsen MM, Stoffers D, Booij J, van Eck-Smit BL, Wolters EC, Berendse HW. Idiopathic hyposmia as a preclinical sign of parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol. 2004 Aug;56(2):173–181. doi: 10.1002/ana.20160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Ross GW, Petrovitch H, Abbott RD, Tanner CM, Popper J, Masaki K, et al. Association of olfactory dysfunction with risk for future parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol. 2008 Feb;63(2):167–173. doi: 10.1002/ana.21291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Stephenson R, Siderowf A, Stern MB. Premotor parkinson's disease: Clinical features and detection strategies. Mov Disord. 2009;24 Suppl 2:S665–S670. doi: 10.1002/mds.22403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Haehner A, Hummel T, Hummel C, Sommer U, Junghanns S, Reichmann H. Olfactory loss may be a first sign of idiopathic parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2007 Apr 30;22(6):839–842. doi: 10.1002/mds.21413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Morley JF, Duda JE. Olfaction as a biomarker in parkinson's disease. Biomark Med. 2010 Oct;4(5):661–670. doi: 10.2217/bmm.10.95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Stern MB, Siderowf A. Parkinson's at risk syndrome: Can parkinson's disease be predicted? Mov Disord. 2010;25 Suppl 1:S89–S93. doi: 10.1002/mds.22719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Doty RL, Deems DA, Stellar S. Olfactory dysfunction in parkinsonism: A general deficit unrelated to neurologic signs, disease stage, or disease duration. Neurology. 1988 Aug;38(8):1237–1244. doi: 10.1212/wnl.38.8.1237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Barz S, Hummel T, Pauli E, Majer M, Lang CJ, Kobal G. Chemosensory event-related potentials in response to trigeminal and olfactory stimulation in idiopathic parkinson's disease. Neurology. 1997 Nov;49(5):1424–1431. doi: 10.1212/wnl.49.5.1424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Boesveldt S, Verbaan D, Knol DL, Visser M, van Rooden SM, van Hilten JJ, et al. A comparative study of odor identification and odor discrimination deficits in parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2008 Oct 30;23(14):1984–1990. doi: 10.1002/mds.22155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Tissingh G, Berendse HW, Bergmans P, DeWaard R, Drukarch B, Stoof JC, et al. Loss of olfaction in de novo and treated parkinson's disease: Possible implications for early diagnosis. Mov Disord. 2001 Jan;16(1):41–46. doi: 10.1002/1531-8257(200101)16:1<41::aid-mds1017>3.0.co;2-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Muller A, Reichmann H, Livermore A, Hummel T. Olfactory function in idiopathic parkinson's disease (IPD): Results from cross-sectional studies in IPD patients and long-term follow-up of de-novo IPD patients. J Neural Transm. 2002 May;109(5–6):805–811. doi: 10.1007/s007020200067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Herting B, Schulze S, Reichmann H, Haehner A, Hummel T. A longitudinal study of olfactory function in patients with idiopathic parkinson's disease. J Neurol. 2008 Mar;255(3):367–370. doi: 10.1007/s00415-008-0665-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Meusel T, Westermann B, Fuhr P, Hummel T, Welge-Lussen A. The course of olfactory deficits in patients with parkinson's disease--a study based on psychophysical and electrophysiological measures. Neurosci Lett. 2010 Dec 17;486(3):166–170. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.09.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Goldstein DS, Sewell L, Holmes C. Association of anosmia with autonomic failure in parkinson disease. Neurology. 2010 Jan 19;74(3):245–251. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181ca014c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Iijima M, Osawa M, Momose M, Kobayakawa T, Saito S, Iwata M, et al. Cardiac sympathetic degeneration correlates with olfactory function in parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2010 Jul 15;25(9):1143–1149. doi: 10.1002/mds.23001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Oka H, Toyoda C, Yogo M, Mochio S. Olfactory dysfunction and cardiovascular dysautonomia in parkinson's disease. J Neurol. 2010 Jun;257(6):969–976. doi: 10.1007/s00415-009-5447-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cramer CK, Friedman JH, Amick MM. Olfaction and apathy in parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2010 Feb;16(2):124–126. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2009.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Stephenson R, Houghton D, Sundarararjan S, Doty RL, Stern M, Xie SX, et al. Odor identification deficits are associated with increased risk of neuropsychiatric complications in patients with parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2010 Oct 15;25(13):2099–2104. doi: 10.1002/mds.23234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gelb DJ, Oliver E, Gilman S. Diagnostic criteria for parkinson disease. Arch Neurol. 1999 Jan;56(1):33–39. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.1.33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hoehn MM, Yahr MD. Parkinsonism: Onset, progression and mortality. Neurology. 1967 May;17(5):427–442. doi: 10.1212/wnl.17.5.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Fahn S, Elton RL. Unified parkinson's disease rating scale. In: Fahn S, Marsden CD, Caine BD, Lieberman A, editors. Recent developments in Parkinson's Disease. Florham Park: Macmillan Health Care Information; 1987. pp. 153–163. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Doty RL, Shaman P, Dann M. Development of the university of pennsylvania smell identification test: A standardized microencapsulated test of olfactory function. Physiol Behav. 1984 Mar;32(3):489–502. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(84)90269-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA. Clinical Gerontology : A Guide to Assessment and Intervention. New York: The Haworth Press; 1986. Geriatric depression scale (GDS): Recent evidence and development of a shorter version; pp. 165–173. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Rush AJ, Giles DE, Schlesser MA, Fulton CL, Weissenburger J, Burns C. The inventory for depressive symptomatology (IDS): Preliminary findings. Psychiatry Res. 1986 May;18(1):65–87. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(86)90060-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory (form Y) 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Starkstein SE, Mayberg HS, Preziosi TJ, Andrezejewski P, Leiguarda R, Robinson RG. Reliability, validity, and clinical correlates of apathy in parkinson's disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1992 Spring;4(2):134–139. doi: 10.1176/jnp.4.2.134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Friedberg G, Zoldan J, Weizman A, Melamed E. Parkinson psychosis rating scale: A practical instrument for grading psychosis in parkinson's disease. Clin Neuropharmacol. 1998 Sep–Oct;21(5):280–284. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975 Nov;12(3):189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Brandt J, Benedict RHB. The hopkins verbal learning test-revised. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Culbertson WC, Zillmer EA. Tower of london-drexel (TOL-DX) technical manual. North Tonawanda: Multi-Health Syatems; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Golden C. The stroop color and word test. Wood Dale: Stoelting Company; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Comprehensive norms for an expanded halstead-reitan battery: Demographic corrections, research findings, and clinical applications. Lutz, Fl: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological assessment. 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Doty RL. The smell identification test administration manual. 3rd edition ed. Haddon Hts NJ: Sensonics, Inc; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Doty RL. Olfaction in parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2007;13 Suppl 3:S225–S228. doi: 10.1016/S1353-8020(08)70006-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Aarsland D, Marsh L, Schrag A. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2009 Nov 15;24(15):2175–2186. doi: 10.1002/mds.22589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Aarsland D, Bronnick K, Larsen JP, Tysnes OB, Alves G Norwegian ParkWest Study Group. Cognitive impairment in incident, untreated parkinson disease: The norwegian ParkWest study. Neurology. 2009 Mar 31;72(13):1121–1126. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000338632.00552.cb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Aarsland D, Bronnick K, Williams-Gray C, Weintraub D, Marder K, Kulisevsky J, et al. Mild cognitive impairment in parkinson disease: A multicenter pooled analysis. Neurology. 2010 Sep 21;75(12):1062–1069. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f39d0e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Mamikonyan E, Moberg PJ, Siderowf A, Duda JE, Have TT, Hurtig HI, et al. Mild cognitive impairment is common in parkinson's disease patients with normal mini-mental state examination (MMSE) scores. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2009 Mar;15(3):226–231. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2008.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Nazem S, Siderowf AD, Duda JE, Have TT, Colcher A, Horn SS, et al. Montreal cognitive assessment performance in patients with parkinson's disease with "normal" global cognition according to mini-mental state examination score. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009 Feb;57(2):304–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.02096.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bohnen NI, Muller ML, Kotagal V, Koeppe RA, Kilbourn MA, Albin RL, et al. Olfactory dysfunction, central cholinergic integrity and cognitive impairment in parkinson's disease. Brain. 2010 Jun;133(Pt 6):1747–1754. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Braak H, Del Tredici K, Rub U, de Vos RA, Jansen Steur EN, Braak E. Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic parkinson's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2003 Mar–Apr;24(2):197–211. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(02)00065-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Braak H, Bohl JR, Muller CM, Rub U, de Vos RA, Del Tredici K. Stanley fahn lecture 2005: The staging procedure for the inclusion body pathology associated with sporadic parkinson's disease reconsidered. Mov Disord. 2006 Dec;21(12):2042–2051. doi: 10.1002/mds.21065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Lerner A, Bagic A. Olfactory pathogenesis of idiopathic parkinson disease revisited. Mov Disord. 2008 Jun 15;23(8):1076–1084. doi: 10.1002/mds.22066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hurtig HI, Trojanowski JQ, Galvin J, Ewbank D, Schmidt ML, Lee VM, et al. Alpha-synuclein cortical lewy bodies correlate with dementia in parkinson's disease. Neurology. 2000 May 23;54(10):1916–1921. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.10.1916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Lim SY, Lang AE. The nonmotor symptoms of parkinson's disease--an overview. Mov Disord. 2010;25 Suppl 1:S123–S130. doi: 10.1002/mds.22786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Barone P, Antonini A, Colosimo C, Marconi R, Morgante L, Avarello TP, et al. The PRIAMO study: A multicenter assessment of nonmotor symptoms and their impact on quality of life in parkinson's disease. Mov Disord. 2009 Aug 15;24(11):1641–1649. doi: 10.1002/mds.22643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Zesiewicz TA, Sullivan KL, Arnulf I, Chaudhuri KR, Morgan JC, Gronseth GS, et al. Practice parameter: Treatment of nonmotor symptoms of parkinson disease: Report of the quality standards subcommittee of the american academy of neurology. Neurology. 2010 Mar 16;74(11):924–931. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181d55f24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Lotsch J, Reichmann H, Hummel T. Different odor tests contribute differently to the evaluation of olfactory loss. Chem Senses. 2008 Jan;33(1):17–21. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjm058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Doty RL, Smith R, McKeown DA, Raj J. Tests of human olfactory function: Principal components analysis suggests that most measure a common source of variance. Percept Psychophys. 1994 Dec;56(6):701–707. doi: 10.3758/bf03208363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Verbaan D, Boesveldt S, van Rooden SM, Visser M, Marinus J, Macedo MG, et al. Is olfactory impairment in parkinson disease related to phenotypic or genotypic characteristics? Neurology. 2008 Dec 2;71(23):1877–1882. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000336651.48596.c7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES