Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Sep 9.
Published in final edited form as: Annu Rev Sociol. 2010 Aug;36:349–370. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.012809.102529

Socioeconomic Disparities in Health Behaviors

Fred C Pampel 1, Patrick M Krueger 2, Justin T Denney 3
PMCID: PMC3169799  NIHMSID: NIHMS317106  PMID: 21909182

Abstract

The inverse relationships between socioeconomic status (SES) and unhealthy behaviors such as tobacco use, physical inactivity, and poor nutrition have been well demonstrated empirically but encompass diverse underlying causal mechanisms. These mechanisms have special theoretical importance because disparities in health behaviors, unlike disparities in many other components of health, involve something more than the ability to use income to purchase good health. Based on a review of broad literatures in sociology, economics, and public health, we classify explanations of higher smoking, lower exercise, poorer diet, and excess weight among low-SES persons into nine broad groups that specify related but conceptually distinct mechanisms. The lack of clear support for any one explanation suggests that the literature on SES disparities in health and health behaviors can do more to design studies that better test for the importance of the varied mechanisms.

Keywords: smoking, exercise, diet, obesity, education, socioeconomic status

INTRODUCTION

Why do low–socioeconomic status (SES) groups more often act in ways that harm their health than high-SES groups? Health behaviors such as use of tobacco, lack of exercise, and poor diet contribute importantly to—though by no means completely explain (Lantz et al. 1998)—SES differences in health and mortality (Rogers et al. 2000). In addition, these behaviors have a characteristic that makes them of special interest: They involve more than the inability to purchase goods and services that promote good health. Smoking involves expenditure of money to purchase an unhealthy product, and some forms of exercise such as walking cost little. The tendency of low-SES groups to adopt unhealthy behaviors despite the monetary and health costs is a puzzle that many studies have examined but, with one very recent exception (Cutler & Lleras-Muney 2010), not addressed in a comprehensive way.

We review explanations of the relationship between low SES and unhealthy behaviors and the empirical support they have received. In focusing on the mechanisms that account for the relationships between SES and health behaviors, we review studies from sociology, economics, and public health that go beyond description and that offer insight into the sources of health inequality. These studies recognize that SES disparities in health behavior involve more than freely chosen lifestyles. To the contrary, the explanations reviewed below suggest that unhealthy behaviors result from the vast differences in the social circumstances of low- and high-SES groups. Our attention to the social origins of health behavior runs counter to perspectives that ignore how SES structures social life.

For several reasons, we limit the health behaviors we examine. Avoidance of tobacco, participation in physical activity, and maintenance of proper weight and diet involve actions that, certainly for smoking and by most accounts for the others, promote health and extend longevity (Rogers et al. 2000). These behaviors also differ from others that depend more directly on having the financial resources to purchase health. It makes sense that the less well-off have fewer opportunities to undergo regular preventive medical checkups and screenings, to work at jobs with low physical danger or contact with hazardous materials, to live in well-built housing in safe neighborhoods with low pollution, and to drive safe cars. Although finances relate in some ways to tobacco cessation (paying for counseling), exercise (joining gyms and clubs), and good diet (buying fresh fruits and vegetables or lean meats), money nonetheless is not a requirement as it is for other health behaviors. Tobacco use, to the contrary, involves significant monetary costs—on average about $1638 per year for a pack-a-day smoker (Smith 2008).

Other behaviors involving use of illegal drugs and participation in criminal violence likewise create risks to health but raise issues different from legal behaviors. Reviews of these behaviors and of research on crime and deviance warrant separate study (although some theories posit a link between participation in legal but unhealthy behavior and participation in illegal behavior). Alcohol consumption has similarities to use of illegal substances (e.g., driving under the influence) and, except at extremely high levels that are relatively rare in the U.S. adult population, has an ambiguous relationship with health and mortality outcomes (Rogers et al. 2000, Thun et al. 1997). Another health behavior, sleep durations of shorter or longer than seven hours per night, is associated with increased mortality, but little research has systematically examined the relationship between SES and sleep duration (Krueger & Friedman 2009, Moore et al. 2002). Still further, health care consumption, adherence to treatment regimens, interaction with physicians, and acceptance of new medical technologies greatly influence health and mortality and are the subject of large literatures (Cutler & Lleras-Muney 2010, Glied & Lleras-Muney 2008, Hadley 2003, Lutfey & Freese 2005, Phelan et al. 2004, Ross & Mirowsky 2000). Because of space constraints, we give only minimal attention to these behaviors.

SES (or sometimes socioeconomic position) refers to standing in the stratification system and is usually measured by education, occupation, employment, income, and wealth. These components of SES are not interchangeable and have different kinds of influences on health behavior. SES can reflect diverse underlying theoretical concerns such as material well-being, human capital, prestige, and productive relations. Even so, it is convenient to refer to SES as a summary term (without assuming that it represents a unidimensional construct) when the particular measure does not have key importance.

IMPORTANCE OF HEALTH BEHAVIOR FOR SES HEALTH DISPARITIES

The focus on SES disparities in health behaviors has generated some dispute. Lantz et al. (1998) reject claims that elevated mortality risks among disadvantaged SES groups come primarily from the higher prevalence of risky behaviors in these groups. In comparing mortality over a 7.5-year period, Lantz and colleagues find that the odds ratio for the lowest to the highest education group falls by only 14% with controls for smoking, drinking, sedentary lifestyle, and relative body weight. Similarly, a study of British civil servants over a 25-year period shows that smoking and other coronary risk factors account for 27% of the inverse social gradient in coronary heart disease (Marmot 2006). Studies that use different methods find stronger effects. Using indirect estimation techniques to attribute mortality to either smoking-related causes or other causes among men ages 36–69 in four nations, Jha et al. (2006) calculate that smoking accounts for nearly half of the excess mortality among the lowest stratum in each nation.

Regardless of the method used or conclusion reached, SES disparities clearly involve more than health behaviors such as smoking, exercise, and eating. Some suggest that even if differences in health behaviors across socioeconomic strata disappeared, the relationship between SES and health would change little, as other sources of disparities would grow in importance (Link & Phelan 1995). At the same time, health behaviors account for, on average, roughly one-quarter of SES disparities in health—an amount of some importance.

To present some numbers, Table 1 describes the disparities for several components of SES and smoking, exercise, and body mass using the 2006 National Health Interview Survey for persons ages 25–64 (National Center for Health Statistics 2008). The dichotomous outcomes assign values of one to survey participants who currently smoke, do not participate in vigorous or moderate physical activity, or have a body mass index (BMI) of 30 or more (obese). The SES predictors are treated categorically, with the highest category serving as the referent so that odds ratios compare lower SES groups to higher SES groups. The logistic regression estimates for SES control for gender, race-ethnicity, age, age squared, and foreign birth.

Table 1.

Odds ratios from logistic regression of health behaviors on SES variables (Ns range from 14,129 to 14,608)a

No controls for other SES variables Controls for other SES variables
SES Variables Currently
smokes
No exercise BMI obese Currently
smokes
No exercise BMI obese
Education (years)
    0–11 3.7* 4.9* 1.8* 2.9* 2.8* 1.5*
    12 2.7* 3.2* 1.8* 2.4* 2.1* 1.5*
    13–15 2.3* 1.8* 1.6* 2.1* 1.4* 1.5*
    16+ 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
Occupation
    Labor-Farm 2.2* 3.1* 1.5* 1.2 1.7* 1.2
    Protect-Service 1.9* 2.0* 1.7* 1.1 1.2 1.4*
    Admin-Sales 1.6* 1.7* 1.4* 1.1 1.2 1.2
    Prof-Manager 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
Income
    Low 2.6* 3.4* 1.5* 1.5* 1.9* 1.2
    Middle low 1.5* 2.3* 1.2 1.1 1.6* 1.0
    Middle high 1.4* 1.6* 1.3* 1.0 1.2 1.1
    High 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
Unemployed
    Yes 1.6* 0.7* 0.2* 1.6* 0.7* 1.0
    No 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
Housing
    Rent 1.9* 1.5* 1.1 1.5* 1.1 0.9
    Own 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
a

Controlling for age, age squared, gender, race, and foreign birth.

*

p < 0.001.

The results in Table 1 show large disparities for smoking and no exercise and more modest disparities for obesity. Of the SES variables, education generally has the strongest influence.1 For example, without controls for other SES variables, high school dropouts have odds of smoking and not exercising that are, respectively, 3.7 and 4.9 times larger than for college graduates. The three columns on the right of the table show that controls for the other SES variables reduce the odds ratios to 2.9 and 2.8, respectively, but they remain substantial. The lowest occupation and income groups have net odds ratios for smoking and not exercising ranging from 1.2 to 1.9. Renting rather than owning and unemployment increase the odds of smoking but do not increase the odds of not exercising. Not surprisingly given evidence suggesting that SES disparities in obesity have lessened over the past three decades (Zhang & Wang 2004), the results for obesity show smaller SES differences. The odds ratios reach 1.8 in the gross models and 1.5 in the net models.

Socioeconomic conditions may produce an underlying health lifestyle that similarly affects smoking, exercise, and diet (Cockerham 2005). Vigorous exercise, for example, promotes weight control and nonsmoking. This clustering encourages prevention efforts on unhealthy behaviors in general and warrants drawing generalizations across multiple behaviors. However, health behaviors differ in important ways that may affect the potential for disparities. Smoking requires action to purchase cigarettes, whereas lack of exercise involves inaction; quitting smoking often produces unpleasant symptoms of withdrawal and increases weight, whereas starting to exercise often increases feelings of well-being and reduces weight. Given these differences, we treat literatures on smoking, exercise, and diet as separate but also attempt to draw some generalizations.

EXPLANATIONS

Link & Phelan (1995) point out that resources favoring high-SES groups are so extensive and wide-ranging as to make SES a fundamental cause of health. Because the underlying relationship between SES and health persists through historical changes in causes of death, advances in medical treatments, and new public health efforts, no single mechanism accounts for the observed relationship (Lutfey & Freese 2005). Rather, some diminish in importance in particular situations while others increase. To define the sources of enduring SES advantages more clearly, we consider nine broad mechanisms that underlie the relationship between SES and health behavior.

Deprivation, Inequality, and Stress

In the stress paradigm, disadvantaged social position is both a source of adversity and a drain on the capacity to cope (e.g., Pearlin 1989). Given these circumstances, smoking, overeating, and inactivity represent forms of pleasure and relaxation that help regulate mood among the disadvantaged (Lantz et al. 2005, Layte & Whelan 2009, Wilkinson 1996). The coping or self-medicating functions of these behaviors make the costs of giving them up particularly salient and limit the ability of low-SES individuals to adopt healthy but challenging behaviors (Lutfey & Freese 2005).

Those deprived economically and living in disadvantaged neighborhoods face a variety of chronic stressors in daily living: They struggle to make ends meet; have few opportunities to achieve positive goals; experience more negative life events such as unemployment, marital disruption, and financial loss; and must deal with discrimination, marginality, isolation, and powerlessness (Baum et al. 1999, Lantz et al. 2005, McEwen 1998). These stresses trigger a host of compulsive behaviors such as overeating, drinking, and smoking (Björntorp 2001, Marmot 2004). Studies give indirect support to stress arguments by showing higher smoking among persons in positions of high stress, including unemployed workers (Fagan et al. 2007), poor single women with childrearing duties (Graham 1995, Marsh & McKay 1994), those from disadvantaged backgrounds (Lynch et al. 1997), and residents of deprived neighborhoods (Duncan et al. 1999). In terms of diet, Miech et al. (2006) find that family poverty status is associated with increasing overweight prevalence for 15- to 17-year-olds.

Other studies do more than describe the association between stress and unhealthy behaviors. Johnson & Hoffman (2000) find that smoking increases among poor performing students in more competitive schools, a proxy for pressures faced by students. Colby et al. (1994) find that a stress index based on divorce, business failures, and natural disasters for U.S. states relates to state smoking prevalence. Several studies measuring perceived stress or biological stress markers find relationships with higher fat consumption and lower levels of mild, moderate, and strenuous physical activity (Burdette & Hill 2008, Dallman et al. 2003, Ng & Jeffery 2003). Grunberg et al. (1999) find evidence that workers who report higher job stress also report problem drinking and heavy drinking more frequently, but only if they also endorse the notion that drinking is an effective strategy for coping with stress.

The research on the relationship between SES, stress, and health behaviors faces at least two limitations, however. First, although low-SES individuals may experience more stressors, they also report lower levels of perceived stress than their high-SES counterparts (Krueger & Chang 2008, Schieman et al. 2006). Further, low-SES individuals report fewer but more severe daily stressors (Grzywacz et al. 2004). The inconsistent relationship between SES and various measures of stress (e.g., perceived stress, acute or chronic stressors, daily hassles) suggests the need for research on whether some dimensions of stress are especially important mediators of the relationship between SES and health behaviors.

Second, prior research often assumes that stress precedes rather than follows unhealthy behavior, although the evidence on smoking and physical activity is less clear. Parrott (1999) argues that smoking worsens stress by creating nicotine dependency. Although cigarettes temporarily relieve withdrawal symptoms, greater exposure to nicotine withdrawal increases stress among smokers. Lang et al. (2007) similarly find that levels of pleasure differ little among low-SES smokers and nonsmokers. Exercise and stress may also have a bidirectional relationship. Although most individuals may respond to stress by becoming more sedentary, Salmon (2001) suggests that exercise may produce neurochemical changes in the body that reduce depression and anxiety and moderate sensitivity to stress. Although smoking or inactivity may alleviate stress in the short term, they might increase stress levels in the long term.

A related version of the stress argument focuses less on absolute than on relative deprivation. Because the SES gradient in health behavior is continuous, with each level showing quantitatively higher prevalence of health behaviors, something more than a threshold of economic and social deprivation must be involved (Adler et al. 1994, Marmot 2004). Rather, disadvantageous social comparisons among nearly all SES groups with those at higher levels weaken social cohesion across society in ways that motivate unhealthy behaviors to deal with the stress (Marmot 2004). High degrees of societal inequality thus worsen feelings of relative deprivation and contribute to disparities in health behavior (Wilkinson 1996).

Support for arguments about relative deprivation comes from studies showing a positive relationship between income in equality and mortality across nations, states, or metropolitan areas. However, a huge literature that has grown on the topic has led to negative evidence (e.g., Beckfield 2004) or at least failed to reach a consensus on whether the relationship is real (Schnittker & McLeod 2005). Although health behaviors have a prominent role in the relative deprivation arguments, few studies examine behaviors directly. Using individual-level data on smoking, Siahpush et al. (2006a) find that high perceived income inequality is associated with higher smoking, and Eibner & Evans (2005) find that deprivation measured relative to those in the same state, race, education, and age group increases the probability of taking health risks. In an aggregate-level study, however, Pampel (2002) finds that relative deprivation does not explain strong SES smoking disparities across egalitarian member nations of the European Union. Similarly, Chang & Christakis (2005) find no positive association between income inequality and the odds of being overweight or obese.

Fewer Benefits of Health Behaviors for Longevity

Claims that low-SES groups have less to gain from healthy behavior come from economics, epidemiology, and sociology. Economists argue that the lower lifetime earnings and wealth of low-SES groups give them less reason to invest in future longevity and more reason to focus on the present in making decisions about health behaviors (Cutler & Lleras-Muney 2008). In the language of economics, their risk or time preferences more heavily discount the future. For example, the declining cost of food in recent decades should increase the utility of all individuals, but some economists suggest that a subset of individuals have hyperbolic discount functions and benefit intensely from eating immediately, regardless of the medium- or long-term health costs (Cutler et al. 2003).

With regard to smoking, the rational addiction model of Gary Becker (Becker & Murphy 1988, Becker et al. 1994) predicts that, given their future orientation, the highly educated are little influenced by cigarette prices but that less educated persons make smoking decisions based more on current cigarette prices. Evidence that cigarette prices reduce smoking is strong—a 10% increase in cigarette prices leads to a 4% decline in smoking (Gallet & List 2003). Consistent with the model, some argue further that prices more strongly reduce smoking among low-SES groups and therefore reduce disparities (Farrelly & Bray 1998, Thomas et al. 2008). However, skeptics claim that youth are more influenced by peers than by prices (DeCicca et al. 2002) and note that SES disparities have failed to decline as expected in the United States with increasing prices (Pampel 2009).

Physical activity, eating patterns, and sleep, although different from smoking in their requirement for substantial time commitments, also reflect different incentives by SES. Economists note that individuals with higher incomes face greater opportunity costs in exercising, preparing nutritious meals, or sleeping because their time is valued more highly in the labor force. Simultaneously, however, individuals with greater human capital or higher earnings gain more financially from exercise, nutrition, and sleep through improved productivity and longer lives (Biddle & Hamermesh 1990). High-SES groups therefore exercise more on weekends and holidays when the opportunity costs are lowest (Mullahy & Robert 2008), report shorter sleep hours on average (Biddle & Hamermesh 1990, Mullahy & Robert 2008), and avoid very short sleep hours (Krueger & Friedman 2009).

Technological advances in food processing and preparation lower the price and time costs of eating but may increase obesity, although those changes likely affect all SES groups equally (Cutler et al. 2003). Chou et al. (2004) find that the growth in fast-food restaurants and the simultaneous decline in the relative cost of a meal over time in the United States coincide with weight gain. The authors suggest that this may result from the “increasing scarcity and increasing value of household or nonmarket time” (Chou et al. 2004, p. 584). Because both high- and low-SES groups value their increasingly limited time outside of work and come to rely on fast food, such explanations may partially account for the declining SES gradient in obesity in the United States (Zhang & Wang 2004).

Epidemiological and sociological arguments also suggest that increased risks of premature death brought on by worse social conditions among low-SES persons make health behaviors less beneficial. Low-SES groups may believe they gain little in terms of longevity from healthy behavior (Lawlor et al. 2003) and feel fatalistic about their ability to act in ways that extend their lives (Niederdeppe et al. 2008). For example, smoking is more common among blue-collar workers who are exposed at work to dangerous dust, fumes, and toxic substances (Sterling & Weinkam 1990). Adams & White (2009) find that a strong concern with the future consequences of health decisions partially mediates the relationship between SES and body weight. Lynch et al. (1997) find that those having experienced socioeconomic disadvantage early in their lives feel a heightened sense of hopelessness throughout the life course that affects health behaviors, and Vangeli & West (2008) find that high-SES groups attempt to quit smoking because of future health concerns, whereas low-SES groups are more often motivated by cost and current health problems. Niederdeppe & Levy (2007) find that the less educated are more likely to agree with fatalistic statements about their ability to reduce their risks of cancer. Those agreeing with fatalistic statements are more likely to smoke and are less likely to exercise or eat fruits and vegetables.

Indulging in enjoyable but unhealthy behaviors may make sense given a shorter life expectancy and a limited payoff from healthier behavior. Interestingly, Blaxter (1990) offers empirical support for these beliefs. She finds that healthy lifestyles do less to lower mortality among low-SES groups than among high-SES groups. But recent evidence contradicts Blaxter’s hypothesis (Krueger & Chang 2008, Pampel & Rogers 2004). Even so, belief in the limited benefits of healthy behavior may obstruct action among low-SES groups.

Latent Traits

Some arguments suggest that latent traits determine both SES and health behavior. If traits determined early enough in life affect educational and occupational attainment as well as adult health behaviors, then SES has a spurious relationship with health rather than a direct or indirect causal effect (Fuchs 1982). Studies generally find a causal impact of education on health more generally (de Walque 2007, Mirowsky & Ross 2003). However, some arguments about self-control and intelligence fit a latent trait perspective.

One stream of literature notes that both crime and unhealthy behaviors such as cigarette use come from the same family, peer, and community influences (Jacobson et al. 2001). According to Gottfredson & Hirschi (1990), both crime and smoking involve low self-control, attraction to risk, and the propensity to choose short-term gain even in the face of long-term harm. The attraction to short-term gain emerges early in life, in large part through poor parental socialization, and in turn leads to poor performance in school, limited career options, and unhealthy behavior (Hirschi & Gottfredson 1994). Gottfredson (2004) suggests that general intelligence rather than self-control and attraction to risk is crucial to the relationship between SES and health. This latent trait not only affects educational and occupational attainment but also physical fitness, nonsmoking, preference for low-sugar, low-fat diets, and adherence to regimens to change unhealthy behaviors.

Few direct tests of the latent trait arguments appear in the literature, nearly all focused on smoking. Farrell & Fuchs (1982) show that the relationship between schooling and smoking at age 24 is accounted for by smoking at age 17— before education is completed. They do not measure the factor underlying the early emergence of the relationship between schooling and smoking, but their results are consistent with the latent trait argument. Other studies demonstrate that health lifestyles are related to crime and deviance among teens (Paternoster & Brame 1998) and that college students who smoke are involved in various risky activities (Emmons et al. 1998). Again, the findings provide only indirect evidence. Gottfredson (2004) cites much literature on the relationship between intelligence and health, but Link et al. (2008) find that controls for intelligence do little to change the relationship between SES and health. Also rejecting the latent trait argument, Cutler & Lleras-Muney (2008) cite findings of little relationship between risk preferences and smoking.

Class Distinctions

High-SES groups may use the adoption of healthy behaviors and lifestyles to set themselves apart from other SES groups (Cockerham 2005). Tobacco avoidance (Pampel 2006), exercise (Stempel 2005), and thinness (Hesse-Biber 2007, McLaren 2007) represent forms of SES-based social distinction as well as means to a longer life. Although applied to health behavior rather than consumer goods, such arguments stem from classical and modern theorists (Bourdieu 1984, Veblen 1992 [1899]) who emphasize lifestyle as a source of social differentiation and the adoption of innovative fashion as a way to reinforce those differences.

Smoking represented an innovative behavior early in the twentieth century that, despite early worries about its harm to health, was first adopted by high-SES groups, perhaps as a form of distinction, and later diffused to lower SES groups (Pampel 2005). In more recent decades, class distinction shows in the early adoption of the innovative idea that changing individual behavior can extend life (Link 2008). Today, smoking is stigmatized more among highly educated than among less educated groups (Stuber et al. 2008). If class distinction gives motives for high-SES groups to act in healthy ways, it may also motivate lower SES groups to set themselves apart with behavior like smoking that, in some contexts, symbolizes independence, toughness, and freedom from convention.

High-status groups also distinguish themselves from low-status groups by participating in activities such as strenuous aerobic sports, moderate levels of weight training, competitive sports that limit physical domination, or activities that require extensive training or other hidden entry requirements aside from simple economic costs (Bourdieu 1984, Stempel 2005). In contrast, low-status groups participate in activities that emphasize strength or the visible appearance of strength, physical domination, and direct physical violence. Wilson (2002) finds that higher levels of economic capital (as indicated by income) and cultural capital (as indicated by education) predict greater participation in sports in general, but attendance at automobile or motorcycle races is most common among those with low levels of cultural capital and is insensitive to levels of economic capital. Scheerder et al. (2002) show that high occupational status groups are more likely to play golf, low-status groups are more likely to participate in boxing or wrestling, and both high- and low-status groups participate in soccer.

The tastes for different sports across socioeconomic groups may result in SES differences in lifelong activity levels. First, high-status groups often embrace activities that can be maintained throughout middle and older age such as tennis or jogging. Second, Howell & McKenzie (1987) find that high-status individuals cultivate participation in any sport: Compared to students in vocational tracks in high school, students in college preparatory tracks have higher levels of sports participation in later life. Further, watching television is a sedentary activity that often displaces time for exercise and is stigmatized in higher SES households. College-educated parents are less likely than others to permit their children to have televisions in their bedrooms or to allow television viewing during meals (Berry 2007). Finally, high-status groups exhibit the characteristics of cultural omnivores—they have wide-ranging participation in both elite and middle-brow sports but avoid the low-brow sports favored by low-status individuals (Stempel 2005). Participation in a wide variety of sports among high-status groups facilitates continued activity even if injury, time constraints, or other barriers limit participating in any single activity.

Although the effects have possibly lessened over time, there is also evidence of class distinctions in diet and weight (McLaren 2007, Sobal & Stunkard 1989). Research suggests a socioeconomic gradient in diet for more developed countries that results in lower weights for the most well-off (McLaren 2007). The gradient is especially pronounced for women (McLaren & Kuh 2004), where thinness is viewed as a sign of beauty, fashion, and prestige. In less developed countries, a positive association between SES and weight predominates, where excess weight may signify success and well-being. Especially for men, larger body size is often viewed as a sign of physical dominance and prowess (McLaren 2007). Consistent with class-based norms in higher-income nations, however, an inverse gradient in obesity is emerging in lesser developed countries (Monteiro et al. 2004).

Lack of Knowledge and Access to Information about Health Risks

Less educated persons with jobs that offer few opportunities for learning may have limited knowledge of the harm of unhealthy behavior and therefore less motivation to adopt healthy behaviors. They are exposed less often to warnings about smoking, poor diet, and lack of exercise and may not grasp the potential long-term harm of these activities (Siahpush et al. 2006b). They instead may be exposed more to advertising that promotes the enjoyment of tobacco and unhealthy food and associates smoking, drinking, and eating with a glamorous lifestyle.

However, evidence suggests that knowledge of the risks of smoking is widespread and does little to account for SES disparities. Although differences in knowledge of risks of smoking played a more important role in the past (Link 2008), more recent antitobacco campaigns, public education, the nonsmokers rights movement, and comprehensive state programs to raise prices, pass clean indoor air laws, and fund media campaigns have successfully publicized risks and reduced smoking (Warner 2005). In 1999, 92% of Americans linked smoking with cancer, and in 2006 84% agreed that smoking is very harmful for adults (Saad 2006). Although they may rationalize their habit by minimizing the harm, smokers in all SES groups likely know of the majority opinion about the harm of smoking. In support of this claim, the desire to quit differs little by SES (Barbeau et al. 2004, Link 2008). In fact, Viscusi& Hakes (2008) find that better educated persons perceive smoking as less dangerous than less educated persons. Layte & Whelan (2009) find that measures of knowledge about the risks do little to explain SES differences in smoking. Interestingly, many physicians in developing nations smoke, despite medical education and knowledge of the harm of smoking (Smith & Leggat 2007).

Knowledge about the importance of adequate exercise and sleep for good health is also widespread. In 2005, 86% of adults agreed with the statement that a lack of sleep is bad for their health and 63% said that regular exercise is a highly important activity for a healthy lifestyle (Lyons 2005).

But knowledge about the risks of obesity is less widespread and differs by SES. Only 36% of U.S. adults rate obesity as a very serious health problem—far behind AIDS, even though obesity contributes to more deaths each year (Bleich et al. 2007). Compared to those with less than a high school degree, those with more than a college degree are 3.6 times as likely to report that they pay a lot or a fair amount of attention to nutritional information from scientific experts. Similarly, higher educational attainment is related to an awareness of whether one is overweight (Paeratakul et al. 2002), and knowledge about the risks of obesity can contribute to attempts to control weight (Kan & Tsai 2004). Indeed, the effectiveness of educational programs for promoting proper diet among low-income adults (Howard-Pitney et al. 1997) suggests that poor knowledge about nutrition may partially account for SES differences in weight.

Efficacy and Agency

Schooling increases the efficacy, problem-solving skills, ability to process information, and locus of control needed to overcome obstacles to good health such as nicotine addiction, the inertia of inactivity, the discomfort of exercise, and the desire for unhealthy foods and excess calories. Mirowsky & Ross (2003, p. 1) make the case for the causal benefits of education:

Education creates desirable outcomes because it trains individuals to acquire, evaluate, and use information. It teaches individuals to tap the power of knowledge. Education develops learned effectiveness that enables self-direction toward any and all values sought, including health.

The increase in human capital, effective agency, and a sense of personal control that comes with greater education (Mirowsky & Ross 2007) proves particularly important in dealing with the difficulties of adopting and maintaining healthy lifestyles.2 In fact, highly educated persons may induce short-term stress as a means for long-term gain (Thoits 2006)— a key to overcoming initial feelings of discomfort and deprivation that come from adopting healthy behaviors. Conversely, less educated persons in positions of powerlessness have more trouble overcoming the obstacles to healthy behavior. Thus, the ability to act on health knowledge rather than the knowledge itself affects health behavior.3

Efficacy and agency include the search for innovative means to help change behavior and relate to a long tradition of research on diffusion of innovations that identifies education as a key to early adoption (Rogers 2003). Consistent with diffusion research, high-SES groups are quickest to use new medical technologies, such as Pap smears and mammography for screening or coronary stents and statins for treatment (Glied & Lleras-Muney 2008, Link et al. 1998). High-SES groups not only seek out new health-promoting technologies, but they also are better able to overcome obstacles to using those new technologies effectively to promote health behavior. In relation to health behaviors, then, high-SES groups are open to new smoking cessation methods, diets, and exercise regimens.

Consistent with these arguments, studies of smoking find that education increases both the use of aids to help quit (Honjo et al. 2006) and the responsiveness to antismoking ad campaigns (Niederdeppe et al. 2008). The higher educated also learn more from negative health events. Wray et al. (1998) find that educated persons are more likely to quit smoking after a heart attack. Droomers et al. (1998) find that locus of control and active problem solving explain about half the relationship between education and physical activity. Cutler & Lleras-Muney (2010) attribute roughly 30% of the education gradient across a large number of health behaviors to cognitive ability but note that the influence of cognitive ability stems ultimately from higher education rather than from a latent trait determined earlier in life.

A weakness in the efficacy argument comes from SES disparities in health behaviors other than smoking, exercise, and weight control that require little in the way of effort, problem solving, or efficacy (Freese & Lutfey 2010). For example, seat belts require less than a second to buckle, but despite decades of publicity about the benefit, laws making seatbelts mandatory in many states, and dashboard warning lights, a SES disparity persists (Harper & Lynch 2007). This persistence suggests that disparities involve something more than problem solving or cognitive ability.

Aids for Healthy Behavior

Adopting many healthy behaviors does not require money, but paying for tobacco cessation aids, joining fitness clubs and weight loss programs, and buying more expensive fruits, vegetables, and lean meats can help realize desires for healthy behavior. Income and the ability to pay for these kinds of aids can help overcome low education, efficacy, and agency and thus represent an independent means to healthy behavior. Cutler&Lleras-Muney (2010) attribute roughly 20% of the education gradient in health behaviors to economic resources.

For smoking, individual counseling and medications to ease withdrawal symptoms can be costly, and low-SES groups tend to use low-cost and often less effective methods (Lillard et al. 2007). For exercise, since the 1960s the prices of sports equipment, bicycles, and sports club memberships have increased more quickly than the prices of televisions or movie tickets, whereas the income per hour of leisure time has fallen most among those with lower incomes (Berry 2007). Droomers et al. (1998) find that income accounts for up to 40% of the increased odds of physical inactivity among less educated individuals, even after adjusting for psychosocial factors. And for weight control, some researchers contend that the obesity epidemic is a relatively simple matter of changing economics—the drastic increase in obese persons in developed countries coincides with dropping prices of refined grains and added sugars and fats, making these cheap, high-calorie foods accessible to low-SES groups (Drewnowski 2004).

Occupational resources overlap with financial ones. Those with good jobs and benefit packages gain access to aids for healthy behavior without having to purchase them. In regard to smoking, workers with better jobs may have employer-provided health insurance and better access to health care, which increasingly emphasizes the treatment of tobacco dependency (Manley et al. 2003). In addition, the worksites of higher prestige professional, managerial, and administrative employees more often have clean indoor air rules that make smoking more difficult, and they sometimes offer smoking cessation programs that help smokers quit (Bauer et al. 2005). Blue-collar and factory workers have less access to these benefits (Sorensen et al. 2004).

Working in some occupations can directly impact physical activity. Since the 1950s, many workers have moved into occupations that are traditionally sedentary, whereas strenuous occupations have become less so due to technological changes, leading to lower levels of physical activity and higher levels of obesity (Brownson et al. 2005, Ladkawalla & Philipson 2007). And although some have found that blue-collar workers who have more physically demanding jobs are more likely to undertake more vigorous activity in their spare time (Wu & Porell 2000), others have found that white-collar workers with the least strenuous jobs are more likely to participate in vigorous activity outside of the workplace and, as such, are more likely to retain their higher levels of activity even after retirement (Berger et al. 2005). Consistent with that perspective, leisure time has increased in recent decades, and low-status groups allocate a greater share of their time to sedentary activities than do high-status groups (Berry 2007).

Community Opportunities

Communities shape opportunities to adopt and maintain healthy behaviors. Low-income neighborhoods have more than their share of fast-food restaurants, liquor stores, and places to buy cigarettes and have less than their share of large grocery stores with a wide selection of healthy fresh foods. Some research finds that low-SES neighborhoods have greater or equal access to gyms, parks, or recreation centers than high-SES neighborhoods, although others find that high-SES neighborhoods have more attractive open spaces and free recreation facilities, and greater access to beaches, rivers, golf courses, tennis courts, and bike trails (Giles-Corti & Donovan 2002, Powell et al. 2006). Even when residents in low-SES neighborhoods have access to more recreational resources than residents in high-SES neighborhoods, they tend to report lower perceived access to recreational facilities (Giles-Corti & Donovan 2002). Poor communities and neighborhoods are targeted by tobacco companies for outdoor advertising (Barbeau et al. 2004) and have weaker enforcement of restrictions on sales of cigarettes to minors (Gemson et al. 1998). In contrast, affluent communities are more likely to pass clean air laws that tend to lower smoking among high-SES groups (Skeer et al. 2004).

Research from the United States and Canada (see Cummins & Macintyre 2006 for a review) finds associations between obesity and food quality, prices, and availability in a community. Persons in disadvantaged areas with less access to healthier foods also consume fewer fruits and vegetables and have higher body weight (Cummins & Macintyre 2006). Inagami et al. (2006) find a positive association between distance traveled to nearest grocery store and weight. Similarly, Morland et al. (2006) report a lower prevalence of obesity and overweight in neighborhoods with greater access to supermarkets.

But the effects of supermarket availability on SES differences in overweight and obesity are inconsistent across nations. Studies from the United Kingdom and Australia find no socioeconomic differences in food and supermarket availability, nor in fruit and vegetable consumption (Cummins & Macintyre 2002, Pearson et al. 2005, Winkler et al. 2006). The persistent relationship in the United States may result from high residential segregation in cities and neighborhoods (Cummins & Macintyre 2006) and more pronounced inequalities in the availability of high-quality foods. Evidence for fast-food outlet density and increased weight is more consistent across countries. Studies from the United States, United Kingdom, and Australia show increased numbers of fast-food outlets in poorer areas and propose a link with the increased levels of obesity in disadvantaged neighborhoods (Cummins et al. 2005, Maddock 2004, Reidpath et al. 2002).

Nevertheless, selection effects may operate. To some extent, grocery stores and restaurants sell the food in most demand, cigarette companies market in neighborhoods with high smoking, and physically active people locate closer to parks and amenities for exercise than less active people. The literature has done more to demonstrate an association than to establish causal direction (Freese & Lutfey 2010).

Social Support, Social Cohesion, and Peer Influence

Group membership—and the characteristics of individuals within communities—can affect health behavior through two forms of social capital (Kawachi et al. 2008). First, networks of health-oriented family members, relatives, friends, and neighbors support healthy behavior, sanction unhealthy behavior, and exchange information on ways to change (Smith & Christakis 2008). Given that high-SES persons adopt healthy behaviors and associate with other high-SES persons, their networks of social support, influence, and engagement promote health and widen disparities. Freese & Lutfey (2010) refer to spillover effects as the influence that high-SES persons who care much about healthy behavior have on other high-SES individuals who otherwise would care little about healthy behavior.

The effects of network social support on SES disparities in smoking emerge in several studies. Cutler & Glaeser (2007) find a social multiplier impact of workplace smoking bans where by workers who change their smoking behavior influence family and friends outside the workplace to do the same. Such processes tend to occur more strongly among the highly educated. Christakis & Fowler (2008) find that among friends who both had at least one year of college, a decision by one friend to quit smoking decreased the chance of the other smoking by 61%. Among friends with a high school education or less, no such influence appeared. Interestingly, highly educated smokers who continue to smoke become less central to their network than do less educated smokers.

Using national data from the United States, Boardman et al. (2005) show that persons living in economically depressed areas and neighborhoods with high rates of obesity are more likely to be obese, regardless of their individual characteristics. This suggests a type of social contagion. Indeed, Christakis & Fowler’s (2007) analyses show that obesity can follow social networking paths that influence persons and cement inequalities in obesity by SES.

Although not couched in terms of social capital, a huge literature on teen smoking highlights the importance of peer influences and group membership. The smoking of friends is perhaps the strongest predictor of smoking among adolescents (Jacobson et al. 2001, p. 85). Similarly, support from peers, parents, and siblings are important predictors of adolescents’ physical activity and drinking behaviors, as are opportunities and access to recreation facilities or places to acquire alcohol ( Jessor et al. 2006, Sallis et al. 2000). In terms of disparities, parental SES affects smoking among teens (Lindstrom 2008). However, teen disparities based on parental SES are weaker than adult disparities based on own SES ( Jacobson et al. 2001, p. 93), suggesting that SES disparities in smoking crystallize and strengthen after adolescence when disparities in social capital widen (Glendinning et al. 1994).

Second, social capital based on social cohesion helps explain community differences in health behavior. Kawachi et al. (1999) define this form of social capital as encompassing trust between citizens, norms of reciprocity, and group membership that facilitate cooperation for mutual benefit. Social cohesion tends to be greater in high-SES neighborhoods and to promote healthy behavior (Lindstrom 2008).

Studies offer mixed evidence of neighborhood and community effects on smoking. Ross (2000) finds that neighborhood disadvantage increases the smoking of men but not of women. Miles (2006) finds in a study of seven European cities that indicators of neighborhood disorder such as litter, graffiti, and the lack of plants and flowers increase smoking, again more for men than for women. Brown et al. (2006) find that community social capital from religious groups reduces the number of cigarettes consumed, although not the overall prevalence.

The importance of social capital for understanding the relationship between neighborhood SES and exercise is ambiguous. Residents of disadvantaged neighborhoods are more likely to walk to shops or work but are less likely to walk, bicycle, or participate in other sports for leisure, even after adjusting for individual SES, in part because low-SES neighborhoods are less safe and attractive (Giles-Corti & Donovan 2002, Ross 2000). Wen et al. (2007a) find that an index of social capital is associated with regular exercise among neighborhood residents in Chicago until adjusting for neighborhood SES, which suggests that the relationship between neighborhood social capital and physical activity is either spurious or mediated by neighborhood SES. But Wen et al. (2007b) find that neighborhood SES in California is not associated with physical activity, although higher levels of neighborhood social cohesion persistently predict increased physical activity.

Social cohesion is also linked to neighborhood obesity levels. For example, examining adolescents in Los Angeles County, California, Cohen et al. (2006) find that neighborhood SES is not significant in models of body mass and overweight after adjusting for neighborhood collective efficacy. Burdette & Hill (2008) find in Texas that the relationship between neighborhood disorder and obesity is entirely mediated by the psychological distress associated with living in a disadvantaged neighborhood.

Although arguments about social capital and peer influence receive much attention, they are incomplete. They do not explain the origins of SES differences in norms that support healthy behavior or why social capital supports healthy behavior. If cohesive groups of high-SES family members, friends, and neighbors who adopt healthy behaviors help others do the same, then additional arguments are needed to account for the initial adoption of healthy behaviors by high-SES groups. Other theories relating to motives and problem solving thus remain crucial to arguments about social influence.

DISCUSSION

The literature has done little to systematically compare and contrast the mechanisms reviewed here. This makes it difficult to offer an overarching framework that integrates or adjudicates between the various approaches. We instead aimed more modestly to present interrelated arguments in a way that helps organize previous work on health behaviors and sets the stage for future research.

The very recent study by Cutler & Lleras-Muney (2010) represents a notable exception to the lack of comparative research on mechanisms and evaluates numerous explanations of the relationship of education and a variety of health behaviors using several data sets. Cutler & Lleras-Muney (2010, p. 1) conclude that

income, health insurance, and family background can account for about 30 percent of the gradient. Knowledge and measures of cognitive ability explain an additional 30 percent. Social networks account for another 10 percent. Our proxies for discounting, risk aversion, or the value of future do not account for any of the education gradient, and neither do personality factors such as a sense of control of oneself or over one’s life.

Although their findings need replication with other data sets and measures, these conclusions help organize and evaluate the diversity of causes and represent a valuable starting point for future empirical research on SES disparities in health behaviors.

In addition, we can offer some thoughts on improvements in theory and method that go beyond the current state of the field. In terms of theory, it may help to organize these mechanisms by distinguishing between motives and means for health behavior (Freese & Lutfey 2010). First, SES can affect the incentives or motivations for healthy behavior. Low-SES groups may have less reason than high-SES groups to want to forego the short-term pleasures of unhealthy behavior for long-term gain in longevity. Arguments related to stress, limited benefits, class distinctions, and knowledge of risk each emphasize how SES shapes motives for healthy behavior. High-SES groups face less stress that might encourage coping through unhealthy behavior; they gain more longevity benefits from healthy behavior; they accrue prestige by setting themselves apart with healthy behavior; and, although less clearly, they have greater knowledge of risks to motivate healthy behavior.

Second, SES can affect the means to reach health goals. All SES groups may have similar desires for healthy behavior, but low-SES groups have more difficulty in realizing their goals. Arguments about efficacy in reaching goals for healthy behavior, access to aids for healthy behavior, and community opportunities for healthy behavior focus more on SES differences in resources for goal attainment. The distinction between motives and means tends to blur at the edges, as strong motives increase efforts to find effective means, and possessing scant means tends to sap motivation to change. Indeed, arguments about social support and social capital tend to mix motives rather than emphasize one over the other. However, motives and means are analytically distinct, and distinguishing among them may aid in the study of disparities in health behaviors.

In terms of methods, the literature has done better in describing associations than in testing specific causes. Designs that distinguish among the various mechanisms are not straightforward. Creative efforts to measure stress, control for latent traits, and examine the psychosocial benefits of schooling move in this direction, but studies can do more to explicitly match mechanisms with measures. Future research could also do more to help isolate the causal influence of education and related components of SES. Progress toward these goals requires longitudinal studies with adequate controls for underlying heterogeneity, well-measured mediators, and attention to the emergence of health behaviors that begin early in life and accumulate over the life course. For example, with detailed data on genetic, family, and school characteristics, the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (http://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth) offers opportunities to evaluate the mechanisms that link education to health behaviors early in life. Alternatively, qualitative designs such as used by Lutfey & Freese (2005) in their study of diabetes clinics can help us understand how the motives and means for healthy behavior differ by education and SES.

Some insights may come from designs that compare health behaviors as well as the determinants. Our review emphasizes the similarities among health behaviors, but each behavior also has unique dimensions in terms of the time and effort required, the pleasure it provides, and its social meanings. Whereas physical activity, sleep, and preparing nutritious meals take a considerable amount of time, smoking and drinking practices seem less constrained by temporal concerns. Modeling the different dimensions of health behaviors has received the most sustained attention in the literature on physical activity—SES appears to have different relationships with walking out of necessity (to work or to shops) than walking for leisure. But the method of tobacco consumption (e.g., cigarettes, pipes, cigars), the kinds of alcohol consumed (e.g., wine, beer, or spirits), the ingredients and food preparation methods employed, or the contexts where food, tobacco, or alcohol is consumed may also provide important insight into how the behaviors are linked to SES. Research that compares measures from multiple theoretical perspectives across multiple health behaviors could clarify when specific mechanisms are most important for shaping SES disparities in health behaviors.

The approach of comparing health behaviors also applies to health care consumption. High-SES groups may see adoption of healthy behaviors and effective use of medical care as closely linked in the pursuit of health (Ross & Mirowsky 2000). Medical care, which depends greatly on access to insurance and relates directly to income and affluence, has some similarities to lifestyle behaviors. Much as they affect smoking, exercise, and diet, SES differences in knowledge, efficacy, and opportunities affect the willingness to use new medical technologies (Glied & Lleras-Muney 2008) and follow treatment regimens laid out by medical personnel (Lutfey & Freese 2005). Commitment to a healthy lifestyle could encompass efforts to take full advantage of medical care resources and to avoid unhealthy behaviors, and comparisons of the determinants of medical care usage and other behaviors may offer new insights.

Focusing on historical trends in health behaviors may also illuminate the mechanisms that link SES to health behaviors. First, the salience of mechanisms may change over time. For example, high-SES individuals accumulate the most benefit from their knowledge, efficacy, and resources in adopting innovative health-related behaviors and using emerging medical technologies (Glied & Lleras-Muney 2008, Link 2008). In contrast, more widespread publicity about and agreement on established risks (e.g., smoking) or common medical treatments (e.g., cholesterol tests) tend to reduce the importance of SES differences in knowledge. Second, the direction of the relationship between SES and health behaviors can change over time. For example, high-status groups once had higher rates of tobacco use (Pampel 2005), cocaine use (Miech 2008), and cholesterol (Chang & Lauderdale 2009). Changes in the social meanings and circumstances affected the motives and means of high-SES groups to reject these behaviors. However, changes can occur in the opposite direction. Goldman & Lakdawalla (2005) find that the introduction of beta-blockers to treat hypertension led to reduced disparities in hypertensive heart disease. In these cases, other influences such as the ease of using new medicines and the willingness of clinicians to prescribe them may overwhelm forces of stratification in health behaviors. Historical trends can thus offer leverage in understanding changes in the mechanisms that link SES to health behaviors and reversals in the relationship between SES and some health behaviors.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We received funding and administrative support for this work from the University of Colorado Population Center (NICHD R21 HD051146), and thank Stefanie Mollborn, Richard Miech, and Bruce Link for helpful comments.

Footnotes

1

Although measures are seldom available and rarely studied, subjective status may influence health independent of objective status (Schnittker & McLeod 2005).

2

Mirowsky & Ross (2003) in particular make causal arguments. They reject claims that latent traits or advantaged backgrounds produce a spurious relationship between educational attainment and health. Rather, those attaining advanced education learn new skills and gain new confidence for problem solving that make it easier to adopt healthy, though difficult, behaviors.

3

This form of means-based knowledge differs conceptually from knowledge of the risks of unhealthy behavior discussed above. Although knowledge of both risks and ways to reduce risks overlaps, the more widespread access to knowledge of risk across SES groups makes it different from the less widespread knowledge and skills to adopt healthy behaviors.

DISCLOSURE STATEMENT

The authors are not aware of any affiliations, memberships, funding, or financial holdings that might be perceived as affecting the objectivity of this review.

LITERATURE CITED

  1. Adams J, White M. Time perspective in socioeconomic inequalities in smoking and body mass index. Health Psychol. 2009;28:83–90. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.28.1.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adler NE, Boyce T, Chesney MA, Cohen S, Folkman S, et al. Socioeconomic status and health: the challenge of the gradient. Am. Psychol. 1994;49:15–24. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.49.1.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barbeau EM, Krieger N, Soobader M-J. Working class matters: socioeconomic disadvantage, race/ethnicity, gender, and smoking in NHIS 2000. Am. J. Public Health. 2004;94:269–278. doi: 10.2105/ajph.94.2.269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bauer JE, Hyland A, Li Q, Steger C, Cummings KM. A longitudinal assessment of the impact of smoke-free worksite policies on tobacco use. Am. J. Public Health. 2005;95:1024–1029. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.048678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baum A, Garofalo JP, Yali AM. Socioeconomic status and chronic stress: Does stress account for SES effects on health? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1999;896:131–144. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08111.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Becker GS, Grossman M, Murphy KM. An empirical analysis of cigarette addiction. Am. Econ. Rev. 1994;84:396–418. [Google Scholar]
  7. Becker GS, Murphy KM. A theory of rational addiction. J. Polit. Econ. 1988;96:675–700. [Google Scholar]
  8. Beckfield J. Does income inequality harm health? New cross-national evidence. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2004;45 doi: 10.1177/002214650404500301. 251-48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Berger U, Der G, Mutrie N, Hannah MK. The impact of retirement on physical activity. Ageing Soc. 2005;25:181–195. [Google Scholar]
  10. Berry B. Disparities in free time inactivity in the United States: trends and explanations. Sociol. Perspect. 2007;50:177–208. [Google Scholar]
  11. Biddle JE, Hamermesh DS. Sleep and the allocation of time. J. Polit. Econ. 1990;98:922–943. [Google Scholar]
  12. Björntorp P. Do stress reactions cause abdominal obesity and comorbidities? Obesity Rev. 2001;2:73–86. doi: 10.1046/j.1467-789x.2001.00027.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Blaxter M. Health and Lifestyles. London: Tavistock; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bleich S, Blendon R, Adams A. Trust in scientific experts on obesity: implications for awareness and behavior change. Obesity. 2007;15:2145–2156. doi: 10.1038/oby.2007.255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Boardman J, Saint Onge J, Rogers R, Denney J. Race differentials in obesity: the impact of place. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2005;46:229–243. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bourdieu P. Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgment of Taste. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  17. Brown TT, Scheffler RM, Seo S, Reed M. The empirical relationship between community social capital and the demand for cigarettes. Health Econ. 2006;15:1159–1172. doi: 10.1002/hec.1119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brownson RC, Boehmer TK, Like DA. Declining rates of physical activity in the United States: What are the contributors? Annu. Rev. Public Health. 2005;26:421–443. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Burdette A, Hill T. An examination of processes linking perceived neighborhood disorder and obesity. Soc. Sci. Med. 2008;67:38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.03.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Chang VW, Christakis NA. Income inequality and weight status in US metropolitan areas. Soc. Sci. Med. 2005;61:83–96. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.11.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Chang VW, Lauderdale DS. Fundamental cause theory, technological innovation, and health disparities: the case of cholesterol in the era of statins. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2009;50:245–261. doi: 10.1177/002214650905000301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Chou S-Y, Grossman M, Saffer H. An economic analysis of adult obesity: results from the Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System. J. Health Econ. 2004;23:565–587. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2003.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Christakis N, Fowler J. The spread of obesity in a large social network over 32 years. New Engl. J. Med. 2007;357:370–379. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa066082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Christakis NA, Fowler JH. The collective dynamics of smoking in a large social network. New Engl. J. Med. 2008;358:2249–2258. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsa0706154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Cockerham WC. Health lifestyle theory and the convergence of agency and structure. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2005;46:51–67. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Cohen D, Finch B, Bower A, Sastry N. Collective efficacy and obesity: the potential influence of social factors on health. Soc. Sci. Med. 2006;62:769–778. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.06.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Colby JP, Jr, Linsky AS, Straus MA. Social stress and state-to-state differences in smoking and smoking-related mortality in the United States. Soc. Sci. Med. 1994;38:373–381. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)90407-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cummins S, Macintyre S. A systematic study of an urban foodscape: the price and availability of food in Greater Glasgow. Urban Stud. 2002;39:2115–2130. [Google Scholar]
  29. Cummins S, Macintyre S. Food environments and obesity—neighborhood or nation? Int. J. Epidemiol. 2006;35:100–104. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyi276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Cummins S, McKay L, Macintyre S. McDonald’s restaurants and neighborhood deprivation in Scotland & England. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2005;4:308–310. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2005.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Cutler DM, Glaeser EL. Social interactions and smoking. Harvard Inst. Econ. Res. Disc. Pap. No. 2153, Harvard Univ.; 2007. http://www.economics.harvard.edu/pub/hier/2008/HIER2153.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  32. Cutler DM, Glaeser EL, Shapiro JM. Why have Americans become more obese? J. Econ. Perspect. 2003;17:93–118. [Google Scholar]
  33. Cutler DM, Lleras-Muney A. Education and health: evaluating theories and evidence. In: Schoeni RF, House JS, Kaplan GA, Pollack H, editors. Making Americans Healthier: Social and Economic Policy as Health Policy. New York: Russell Sage Found; 2008. pp. 29–60. [Google Scholar]
  34. Cutler DM, Lleras-Muney A. Understanding differences in health behaviors by education. J. Health Econ. 2010;29(1):1–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2009.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Dallman M, Pecoraro N, Akana S, la Fleur S, Gomez F, et al. Chronic stress and obesity: a new view of comfort food. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2003;100:11696–11701. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1934666100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. DeCicca P, Kenkel DS, Mathios AD. Putting out the fires: Will higher taxes reduce the onset of youth smoking? J. Polit. Econ. 2002;110:144–169. [Google Scholar]
  37. de Walque D. Does education affect smoking behaviors? Evidence using the Vietnam draft as an instrument for college education. J. Health Econ. 2007;26:877–895. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2006.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Drewnowski A. Obesity and the food environment: dietary energy density and diet costs. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2004;27:154–162. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2004.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Droomers M, Schrijvers CTM, Van De Mheen H, Mackenbach JP. Educational differences in leisure-time physical inactivity: a descriptive and explanatory study. Soc. Sci. Med. 1998;47:1665–1676. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(98)00272-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Duncan C, Jones K, Moon G. Smoking and deprivation: Are there neighbourhood effects? Soc. Sci. Med. 1999;48:497–505. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(98)00360-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Eibner C, Evans WN. Relative deprivation, poor health habits, and mortality. J. Hum. Resour. 2005;40:591–620. [Google Scholar]
  42. Emmons KM, Wechsler H, Dowdall G, Abraham M. Predictors of smoking among U.S. college students. Am. J. Public Health. 1998;88:104–107. doi: 10.2105/ajph.88.1.104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Fagan P, Shavers V, Lawrence D, Gibson JT, Ponder P. Cigarette smoking and quitting behaviors among unemployed adults in the United States. Nicotine Tob. Res. 2007;9:241–248. doi: 10.1080/14622200601080331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Farrell P, Fuchs VR. Schooling and health: the cigarette connection. J. Health Econ. 1982;1:217–230. doi: 10.1016/0167-6296(82)90001-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Farrelly MC, Bray J. Response to increases in cigarette prices by race/ethnicity, income, and age groups—United States, 1976–1993. MMWR. 1998;47:605–609. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Freese J, Lutfey K. Fundamental causality: challenges of an animating concept for medical sociology. In: Pescosolido B, Martin J, McLeod J, Rogers A, editors. Handbook of the Sociology of Health, Illness, and Healing. New York: Springer; 2010. In press. [Google Scholar]
  47. Fuchs VR. Time preference and health: an exploratory study. In: Fuchs VR, editor. Economic Aspects of Health. Chicago: Univ. Chicago Press; 1982. pp. 93–120. [Google Scholar]
  48. Gallet CA, List JA. Cigarette demand: a metaanalysis of elasticities. Health Econ. 2003;12:821–835. doi: 10.1002/hec.765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Gemson D, Moats H, Watkins B, Ganz M, Robinson S, Healton E. Laying down the law: reducing illegal tobacco sales to minors in central Harlem. Am. J. Public Health. 1998;88:936–939. doi: 10.2105/ajph.88.6.936. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Giles-Corti B, Donovan RJ. Socioeconomic status differences in recreational physical activity levels and real and perceived access to a supportive physical environment. Prev. Med. 2002;35:601–611. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2002.1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Glendinning A, Shucksmith J, Hendry L. Social class and adolescent smoking behavior. Soc. Sci. Med. 1994;38:1449–1460. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)90283-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Glied S, Lleras-Muney A. Technological innovation and inequality in health. Demography. 2008;45:741–761. doi: 10.1353/dem.0.0017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Goldman DP, Lakdawalla DN. A theory of health disparities and medical technology. Contrib. Econ. Anal. Policy. 2005;4:1–30. [Google Scholar]
  54. Gottfredson LS. Intelligence: Is it the epidemiologists’ elusive “fundamental cause” of social class inequalities in health? J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2004;86:174–199. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Gottfredson MR, Hirschi T. A General Theory of Crime. Stanford, CA: Stanford Univ. Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  56. Graham H. Cigarette smoking: a light on gender and class inequality in Britain? J. Soc. Policy. 1995;24:509–527. [Google Scholar]
  57. Grunberg L, Moore S, Greenberg ES. Work stress and self-reported alcohol use: the moderating role of escapist reasons for drinking. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 1999;4:29–36. doi: 10.1037//1076-8998.4.1.29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Grzywacz JG, Almeida DM, Neupert SD, Ettner SL. Socioeconomic status and health: a microlevel analysis of exposure and vulnerability to daily stressors. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2004;45:1–16. doi: 10.1177/002214650404500101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Hadley J. Sicker and poorer—the consequences of being uninsured: a review of the research on the relationship between health insurance, medical care use, health, work, and income. Med. Care Res. Rev. 2003;60:3S–75S. doi: 10.1177/1077558703254101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Harper S, Lynch J. Trends in socioeconomic inequalities in adult health behaviors among U.S. states, 1990–2004. Public Health Rep. 2007;122:177–189. doi: 10.1177/003335490712200207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Hesse-Biber S. The Cult of Thinness. 2nd ed New York: Oxford Univ. Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  62. Hirschi T, Gottfredson MR, editors. The Generality of Deviance. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  63. Honjo K, Tsutsumi A, Kawachi I, Kawakami N. What accounts for the relationship between social class and smoking cessation? Results of a path analysis. Soc. Sci. Med. 2006;62:317–328. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Howard-Pitney B, Winkleby M, Albright C, Bruce B, Fortmann S. The Stanford Nutrition Action Program: a dietary fat intervention for low-literacy adults. Am. J. Public Health. 1997;87:1971–1976. doi: 10.2105/ajph.87.12.1971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Howell FM, McKenzie JA. High school athletics and adult sport-leisure activity: gender variations across the life cycle. Sociol. Sport J. 1987;4:329–346. [Google Scholar]
  66. Inagami S, Cohen D, Finch B, Asch S. You are where you shop: grocery store locations, weight, and neighborhoods. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2006;31:10–17. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2006.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Jacobson PD, Lantz P, Warner K, Wasserman J, Pollack H, Ahlstrom A. Combating Teen Smoking: Research and Policy Strategies. Ann Arbor: Univ. Mich. Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  68. Jessor R, Costa FM, Krueger PM, Turbin MS. A developmental study of high-volume drinking among college students: the role of psychosocial and behavioral protective and risk factors. J. Stud. Alcohol. 2006;67:86–94. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Jha P, Peto R, Zatonski W, Boreham J, Jarvis MJ, Lopez AD. Social inequalities in male mortality, and in male mortality from smoking: indirect estimation from national death rates in England and Wales, Poland, and North America. Lancet. 2006;368:367–370. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68975-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Johnson RA, Hoffman JP. Adolescent cigarette smoking in U.S. racial/ethnic subgroups: findings from the National Education Longitudinal Study. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2000;41:392–407. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Kan K, Tsai W. Obesity and risk knowledge. J. Health Econ. 2004;23:907–934. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2003.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Kawachi I, Kennedy BP, Glass R. Social capital and self-rated health: a contextual analysis. Am. J. Public Health. 1999;89:1187–1193. doi: 10.2105/ajph.89.8.1187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Kawachi I, Subramanian SV, Kim D, editors. Social Capital and Health. New York: Springer; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  74. Krueger PM, Chang V. Being poor and coping with stress: health behaviors and the risk of death. Am. J. Public Health. 2008;98:889–896. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2007.114454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Krueger PM, Friedman EM. Sleep duration in the United States: a cross-sectional population based study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2009;169:134–141. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Ladkawalla D, Philipson TJ. Labor supply and weight. J. Hum. Resour. 2007;42:85–116. [Google Scholar]
  77. Lang I, Gardener E, Huppert FA, Melzer D. Was John Reid right? Smoking, class, and pleasure: a population-based cohort study in England. Public Health. 2007;121:518–524. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2007.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Lantz PM, House JS, Lepkowski JM, Williams DR, Mero RP, Chen J. Socioeconomic factors, health behaviors, and mortality: results from a nationally representative prospective study of U.S. adults. JAMA. 1998;279:1703–1708. doi: 10.1001/jama.279.21.1703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Lantz PM, House JS, Mero RP, Williams DR. Stress, life events, and socioeconomic disparities in health: results from the Americans’ Changing Lives Study. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2005;46:274–288. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Lawlor DA, Frankel S, Shaw M, Ebrahim S, Smith GD. Smoking and ill health: Does lay epidemiology explain the failure of smoking cessation programs among deprived populations? Am. J. Public Health. 2003;93:266–270. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.2.266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Layte R, Whelan CT. Explaining social class inequalities in smoking: the role of education, self-efficacy, and deprivation. Eur. Sociol. Rev. 2009;25:399–410. [Google Scholar]
  82. Lillard DR, Plassmann V, Kenkel D, Mathios A. Who kicks the habit and how they do it: socioeconomic differences across methods of quitting smoking in the USA. Soc. Sci. Med. 2007;64:2504–2519. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.02.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Lindstrom M. Social capital and health-related behaviors. In: Kawachi I, Subramanian SV, Kim D, editors. Social Capital and Health. New York: Springer; 2008. pp. 215–238. [Google Scholar]
  84. Link BG. Epidemiological sociology and the social shaping of population health. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2008;49:367–384. doi: 10.1177/002214650804900401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Link BG, Northridge ME, Phelan JC, Ganz ML. Social epidemiology and the fundamental cause concept: on the structuring of effective cancer screens by socioeconomic status. Milbank Q. 1998;76:375–402. doi: 10.1111/1468-0009.00096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Link BG, Phelan J. Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1995:80–94. Extra Issue. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Link BG, Phelan J, Miech R, Westin EL. The resources that matter: fundamental social causes of health disparities and the challenge of intelligence. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2008;49:72–91. doi: 10.1177/002214650804900106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Lutfey K, Freese J. Toward some fundamentals of fundamental causality: socioeconomic status and health in the routine clinic visit for diabetes. Am. J. Sociol. 2005;110:1326–1372. [Google Scholar]
  89. Lynch JW, Kaplan GA, Salonen JT. Why do poor people behave poorly? Variation in adult health behaviours and psychosocial characteristics by stages of the socioeconomic lifecourse. Soc. Sci. Med. 1997;44:809–819. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(96)00191-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Lyons L. Older Americans: sleep “doze” a body good. Gallup. 2005 Dec 13; http://www.gallup.com/poll/20434/Older-Americans-Sleep-Doze-Body-Good.aspx.
  91. Maddock J. The relationship between obesity and the prevalence of fast-food restaurants: state-level analysis. Am. J. Health Promot. 2004;19:137–143. doi: 10.4278/0890-1171-19.2.137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Manley MW, Griffin T, Foldes SS, Link CC, Sechrist RAJ. The role of health plans in tobacco control. Annu. Rev. Public Health. 2003;23:237–266. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.24.100901.140838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Marmot M. The Status Syndrome: How Social Standing Affects Our Health and Longevity. New York: Times Books/Henry Holt; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  94. Marmot M. Smoking and inequalities. Lancet. 2006;368:341–342. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68976-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Marsh A, McKay S. Poor Smokers. London: Policy Stud. Inst; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  96. McEwen B. Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. New Engl. J. Med. 1998;338:171–179. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199801153380307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. McLaren L. Socioeconomic status and obesity. Epidemiol. Rev. 2007;29:29–48. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxm001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. McLaren L, Kuh D. Women’s body dissatisfaction, social class, and social mobility. Soc. Sci. Med. 2004;58:1575–1584. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00209-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Miech R. The formation of a socioeconomic health disparity: the case of cocaine use during the 1980s and 1990s. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2008;49:352–366. doi: 10.1177/002214650804900308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Miech R, Kumanyika S, Stettler N, Link B, Phelan J, Chang V. Trends in the association of poverty with overweight among US adolescents, 1971–2004. JAMA. 2006;295:2385–2393. doi: 10.1001/jama.295.20.2385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Miles R. Neighborhood disorder and smoking: findings of a European urban survey. Soc. Sci. Med. 2006;63:2464–2475. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Mirowsky J, Ross CE. Education, Social Status, and Health. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine De Gruyter; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  103. Mirowsky J, Ross CE. Life course trajectories of perceived control and their relationship to education. Am. J. Sociol. 2007;112:1339–1382. [Google Scholar]
  104. Monteiro C, Moura E, Conde W, Popkin B. Socioeconomic status and obesity in adult populations of developing countries: a review. Bull. World Health Organ. 2004;82:940–946. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Moore PJ, Adler NE, Williams DR, Jackson JS. Socioeconomic status and health: the role of sleep. Psychosomat. Med. 2002;64:337–344. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200203000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Morland K, Diez Roux A, Wing S. Supermarkets, other food stores, and obesity: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2006;30:333–339. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2005.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Mullahy J, Robert SA. No time to lose? Time constraints and physical activity. Cambridge, MA: NBER Work. Pap. 14513, Natl. Bur. Econ. Res.; 2008. http://www.nber.org/papers/w14513.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  108. National Center for Health Statistics. National Health Interview Survey. Atlanta, GA: Cent. Dis. Cont. Prev; 2008. http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhis.htm. [Google Scholar]
  109. Ng DM, Jeffery RW. Relationships between perceived stress and health behaviors in a sample of working adults. Health Psychol. 2003;22:638–642. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.22.6.638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Niederdeppe J, Fiore MC, Baker TB, Smith SS. Smoking-cessation media campaigns and their effectiveness among socioeconomically advantaged and disadvantaged populations. Am. J. Public Health. 2008;98:916–924. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2007.117499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Niederdeppe J, Levy AG. Fatalistic beliefs about cancer prevention and three prevention behaviors. Cancer Epidemiol. Biomark. Prev. 2007;16:998–1003. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-06-0608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Paeratakul S, White M, Williamson D, Ryan D, Bray G. Sex, race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and BMI in relation to self-perception of overweight. Obesity Res. 2002;10:345–350. doi: 10.1038/oby.2002.48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Pampel FC. Inequality, diffusion, and the status gradient in smoking. Soc. Probl. 2002;49:35–57. [Google Scholar]
  114. Pampel FC. Diffusion, cohort change, and social patterns of smoking. Soc. Sci. Res. 2005;34:117–139. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2003.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Pampel FC. Socioeconomic distinction, cultural tastes, and cigarette smoking. Soc. Sci. Q. 2006;87:19–35. doi: 10.1111/j.0038-4941.2006.00366.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Pampel FC. The persistence of educational disparities in smoking. Soc. Probl. 2009;56:526–542. [Google Scholar]
  117. Pampel FC, Rogers RG. Socioeconomic status, smoking, and health: a test of competing theories of cumulative advantage. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2004;45:306–321. doi: 10.1177/002214650404500305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Parrott AC. Does cigarette smoking cause stress? Am. Psychol. 1999;54:817–820. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.54.10.817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Paternoster R, Brame R. The structural similarity of processes generating criminal and analogous behaviors. Criminology. 1998;36:633–669. [Google Scholar]
  120. Pearlin LI. The sociological study of stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1989;30:241–256. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Pearson T, Russell J, Campbell M, Barker M. Do “food-deserts” influence fruit and vegetable consumption—a cross-sectional study. Appetite. 2005;45:195–197. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2005.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Phelan JC, Link BG, Diez-Roux A, Kawachi I, Levin B. “Fundamental causes” of social inequalities in mortality: a test of the theory. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2004;45:265–285. doi: 10.1177/002214650404500303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Powell LM, Slater S, Chaloupka FJ, Harper D. Availability of physical activity-related facilities and neighborhood demographic and socioeconomic characteristics: a national study. Am. J. Public Health. 2006;96:1676–1680. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2005.065573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Reidpath D, Burns C, Garrard J, Mahoney M, Townsend M. An ecological study of the relationship between social and environmental determinants of obesity. Health Place. 2002;8:141–145. doi: 10.1016/s1353-8292(01)00028-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Rogers EM. Diffusion of Innovations. 5th ed New York: Free Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  126. Rogers RG, Hummer RA, Nam CB. Living and Dying in the USA: Behavioral, Health, and Social Differentials of Adult Mortality. San Diego, CA: Academic; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  127. Ross CE. Walking, exercise, and smoking: Does neighborhood matter? Soc. Sci. Med. 2000;51:265–274. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(99)00451-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Ross CE, Mirowsky J. Does medical insurance contribute to socioeconomic differentials in health? Milbank Q. 2000;78:291–321. doi: 10.1111/1468-0009.00171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Saad L. Many Americans still downplay risk of passive smoking. Gallup. 2006 July 21; http://www.gallup.com/poll/23851/Many-Americans-Still-Downplay-Risk-Passive-Smoking.aspx.
  130. Sallis JF, Prochaska JJ, Taylor WC. A review of correlates of physical activity ofchildren and adolescents. Med. Sci. Sport Exerc. 2000;32:963–975. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200005000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Salmon P. Effects of physical exercise on anxiety, depression, and sensitivity to stress: a unifying theory. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2001;21:33–61. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(99)00032-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Scheerder J, Vanreusel B, Taks M, Renson R. Social sports stratification in Flanders 1969–1999: inter-generational reproduction of social inequalities. Int. J. Sociol. Sport. 2002;37:219–245. [Google Scholar]
  133. Schieman S, Kurashina Whitestone Y, Van Gundy K. The nature of work and the stress of higher status. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2006;47:242–257. doi: 10.1177/002214650604700304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Schnittker J, McLeod JD. The social psychology of health disparities. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2005;31:75–103. [Google Scholar]
  135. Siahpush M, Borland R, Taylor J, Singh GK, Ansari Z, Serraglio A. The association of smoking with perception of income inequality, relative material well-being, and social capital. Soc. Sci. Med. 2006a;63:2801–2812. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Siahpush M, McNeil A, Hammond D, Fong GT. Socioeconomic and country variations in knowledge of health risks of tobacco smoking and toxic constituents of smoke: results from the 2002 International Tobacco Control (ITC) Four Country Survey. Tob. Control. 2006b;15 doi: 10.1136/tc.2005.013276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Skeer M, George S, Hamilton WL, Cheng DM, Siegel M. Town-level characteristics and smoking policy adoption in Massachusetts: Are local restaurant smoking regulations fostering disparities in health protection? Am. J. Public Health. 2004;94:286–292. doi: 10.2105/ajph.94.2.286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Smith DR, Leggat PA. An international review of tobacco smoking in the medical profession: 1974–2004. BMC Public Health. 2007;7:1–12. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-7-115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Smith H. The high cost of smoking. MSN Money. 2008 Sept 3; http://articles.moneycentral.msn.com/Insurance/InsureYourHealth/HighCostOfSmoking.aspx.
  140. Smith KP, Christakis NA. Social networks and health. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2008;34:405–429. [Google Scholar]
  141. Sobal J, Stunkard A. Socioeconomic status and obesity: a review of the literature. Psychol. Bull. 1989;105:260–275. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.105.2.260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Sorensen G, Barbeau E, Hunt MK, Emmons K. Reducing social disparities in tobacco use: a social-contextual model for reducing tobacco use among blue-collar workers. Am. J. Public Health. 2004;94:230–239. doi: 10.2105/ajph.94.2.230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Stempel C. Adult participation in sports as cultural capital: a test of Bourdieu’s theory of the field of sports. Int. Rev. Sociol. Sport. 2005;40:411–432. [Google Scholar]
  144. Sterling T, Weinkam J. The confounding of occupation and smoking and its consequences. Soc. Sci. Med. 1990;30:457–467. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(90)90348-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Stuber J, Galea S, Link BG. Smoking and the emergence of a stigmatized social status. Soc. Sci. Med. 2008;67:420–430. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.03.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Thoits PA. Personal agency in the stress process. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2006;47:309–324. doi: 10.1177/002214650604700401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Thomas S, Fayter D, Misso K, Ogilvie D, Petticrew M, et al. Population tobacco control interventions and their effects on social inequalities in smoking: systematic review. Tob. Control. 2008;17:230–237. doi: 10.1136/tc.2007.023911. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Thun MJ, Peto R, Lopez AD, Monaco JH, Henley SJ, et al. Alcohol consumption and mortality among middle-aged and elderly U.S. adults. New Engl. J. Med. 1997;337:1705–1714. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199712113372401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Vangeli E, West R. Sociodemographic differences in triggers to quit smoking: findings from a national survey. Tob. Control. 2008;17:410–415. doi: 10.1136/tc.2008.025650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Veblen T. Theory of the Leisure Class. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction; 1992 [1899]. [Google Scholar]
  151. Viscusi WK, Hakes JK. Risk beliefs and smoking behavior. Econ. Inq. 2008;46:45–59. [Google Scholar]
  152. Warner K. Tobacco policy in the United States: lessons for the obesity epidemic. In: Mechanic D, Rogut LB, Colby DC, editors. Policy Challenges in Modern Health Care. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers Univ. Press; 2005. pp. 99–114. [Google Scholar]
  153. Wen M, Browning CR, Cagney KA. Neighborhood deprivation, social capital and regular exercise during adulthood: a multilevel study in Chicago. Urban Stud. 2007a;44:2651–2671. [Google Scholar]
  154. Wen M, Kandula NR, Lauderdale DS. Walking for transportation or leisure: What difference does the neighborhood make? J. Gen. Intern. Med. 2007b;22:1674–1680. doi: 10.1007/s11606-007-0400-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Wilkinson RG. Unhealthy Societies: The Afflictions of Inequality. London: Routledge; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  156. Wilson TC. The paradox of social class and sports involvement: the roles of cultural and economic capital. Int. Rev. Sociol. Sport. 2002;37:5–16. [Google Scholar]
  157. Winkler E, Turrell G, Paterson C. Does living in a disadvantaged area mean fewer opportunities to purchase fresh fruit and vegetables in the area? Findings from the Brisbane Food Study. Health Place. 2006;12:306–319. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2004.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Wray LA, Herzog AR, Willis RJ, Wallace RB. The impact of education and heart attack on smoking cessation in middle-aged adults. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1998;39:271–294. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Wu B, Porell F. Job characteristics and leisure physical activity. J. Aging Health. 2000;12:538–559. doi: 10.1177/089826430001200405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Zhang Q, Wang Y. Trends in the association between obesity and socioeconomic status in U.S. adults: 1971 to 2000. Obesity Res. 2004;12:1622–1632. doi: 10.1038/oby.2004.202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES