Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011 Apr 19;118(2-3):166–172. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.03.014

Psychometric modeling of cannabis initiation and use and the symptoms of cannabis abuse, dependence and withdrawal in a sample of male and female twins

Nathan A Gillespie 1,2, Kenneth S Kendler 1,3, Michael C Neale 1,3
PMCID: PMC3172349  NIHMSID: NIHMS290221  PMID: 21507586

Abstract

Background

Despite an emerging consensus that the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for cannabis abuse and dependence are best represented by a single underlying liability, it remains unknown if latent class or hybrid models can better explain the data.

Method

Using structured interviews, 7,316 adult male and female twins provided complete data on DSM-IV symptoms of cannabis abuse and dependence. Our aim was to derive a parsimonious, best-fitting cannabis use disorder (CUD) phenotype based on DSM-III-R/IV criteria by comparing an array of psychometric models (latent factor analysis, latent class analysis and factor mixture modeling) using full information maximum likelihood ordinal data methods in Mx.

Results

We found little evidence to support population heterogeneity since neither latent class nor hybrid factor mixture models provided a consistently good fit to the data. When conditioned on initiation and cannabis use, the endorsement patterns of the abuse, dependence and withdrawal criteria were best explained by two latent factors for males and females. The first was a general CUD factor for which genetic effects explained 53–54% of the variance. A less interpretable second factor included a mix of cross-loading dependence and withdrawal symptoms.

Conclusions

This is the first study to compare competing measurement models to derive an empirically determined CUD phenotype. Commensurate with proposed changes to substance use disorders in the DSM-V, our results support an emerging consensus that a single CUD latent factor can more optimally assess the risk or liability underpinning correlated measures of use, abuse, dependence and withdrawal criterion.

Keywords: cannabis, abuse, dependence, withdrawal, heterogeneity, latent class, latent factor, factor mixture

1. Introduction

Cannabis is the most widely used illicit drug in developed countries including the United States (Dennis et al., 2002; Hall et al., 1999). Population based estimates of lifetime cannabis use in the USA between 1990 and 2004 range from 41.2% to 55.9% (Agrawal et al., 2004; Degenhardt et al., 2008; Kendler et al., 2003; Kendler et al., 2000; Samhsa, 1998; von Sydow et al., 2001). Estimates of the lifetime prevalence of abuse range from 5.5% to 8.4% whereas cannabis dependence ranges from 1.3% to 2.2% (Agrawal and Lynskey, 2007; Stinson et al., 2005; von Sydow et al., 2001). A central issue in interpreting the available epidemiologic data on cannabis use has been the assumption that the DSM-IV symptoms of cannabis abuse and dependence captured two distinct, categorical outcomes (American Psychiatric Association, 1980, 1987, 1994; Edwards et al., 1981).

Recent attempts to understand why covariation between the symptoms of cannabis abuse and dependence exist have steadily challenged this assumption and the DSM categorical model. It is now commonly accepted that the covariance arises because of a general factor that influences all cannabis symptoms (Feingold and Rounsaville, 1995a, b; Gillespie et al., 2007; Hartman et al., 2008; Langenbucher et al., 2004; Lynskey and Agrawal, 2007; Nelson et al., 1999; Teesson et al., 2002). By relying on clinical and population-based samples these reports have shown how the physiological, behavioral and cognitive components of cannabis abuse and dependence, including withdrawal, are consistent with a single psychometric factor model.

Overall, the factor model approach (Spearman, 1904) has been useful for developing and validating constructs when investigating the use and abuse of either a single drug or variety of substances (Harford and Muthen, 2001; Hasin et al., 1997; Lennox et al., 1996; Nelson et al., 1999). Indeed, similar results have been reported for alcohol, cocaine, opiates and sedatives (Feingold and Rounsaville, 1995a, b; Langenbucher et al., 2004; Nelson et al., 1999). Within the framework of item response theory, it can also provide a powerful means of calibrating correlated criteria in terms of item difficulty and discrimination, and for developing phenotypic measures with the most informative and robust items (Kirisci et al., 2009). However, in the current context of cannabis users, the approach depends critically on the population being homogeneous, i.e., the same model applies to all individuals.

In the absence of model comparisons we cannot a priori rule out alternative, better fitting approaches particularly when plausibly, there exist population subgroups of cannabis users. For instance, there have been significant cohort differences in the prevalence of cannabis use in the US between 1934 and 1974 (Kendler et al., 2005), with higher rates of use, abuse and dependence reported among males (Agrawal and Lynskey, 2007; Agrawal et al., 2004; Kendler et al., 2005; von Sydow et al., 2001). These cohort differences may be qualitative, rather than quantitative in origin (Degenhardt et al., 2000; Johnson and Gerstein, 1998; Muller and Gmel, 2002). The trajectory from cannabis initiation to use, abuse and dependence (CUD) can also vary between subjects (phenotypic heterogeneity) or the same disorder may result from different sets of risk factors (etiological heterogeneity) (Ellickson et al., 2004; Kandel and Chen, 2000; Windle and Wiesner, 2004).

Latent class analyses (LCA) are appropriate for modeling heterogeneous data because they assume that correlations between symptoms arise not because of some unmeasured, latent factor but because populations consist of subgroups differing in their means or variances. Although LCA is relatively unstructured, insofar as entirely different response probability sets can exist for different classes, it has been used to identify severity spectrums (Lazarsfeld and Henry, 1968; Moustaki, 1996; Pickles and Angold, 2003; Waller and Meehl, 1997) for alcohol (Bucholz et al., 1996; Lynskey et al., 2005), cannabis use (Grant et al., 2006), and to study treatment response and evaluation (Grant et al., 2006). It is arguably a useful method for defining and validating psychiatric phenotypes (Leoutsakos et al., 2010). However, a limitation of LCA is that very minor differences between classes can make it difficult to distinguish one from the next and because of the local independence assumption (Lazarsfeld and Henry, 1968) it is impossible, unlike LFA, to distinguish individuals within a class from one another, e.g., individual within-class differences in severity (Muthen, 2006).

Historically, the latent factor and class approaches have been applied independently and for different purposes when in reality each represents a competing hypothesis regarding the covariation of cannabis diagnostic criteria. An alternative is a factor mixture model (FMM) that circumvents the limitations of LFA and LCA by providing a useful bridge between the two methods (Dolan and Maas, 1998; Everitt, 1988; Jedidi et al., 1997; McLachlan and Peel, 2000; Muthen, 2006; Muthen and Shedden, 1999; Yung, 1997). FMMs can be used to identify latent classes on the one hand, while assessing whether there exist individual differences along continuous dimensions within each of the classes. In principle, this hybrid modeling strategy may fit the data better and if so, provide a superior measurement and classification for complex phenotypes. To date FMMs have been applied to symptoms of alcohol and nicotine consumption and arguably provide a richer represenation of the data when compared to the fit of LFA or LCA models (Muthen, 2006; Muthen and Asparouhov, 2006). FMMs can also be applied to longitudinal data to identify subgroups with different growth trajectories. For example, a growth FMM was applied to antisocial behavior and heavy drinking (Muthen and Muthen, 2000) as well as to assess intervention effects of reducing aggressive behavior (Muthen et al., 2002).

We argue that supposition ought to be replaced with empirical evidence wherever possible. In this case, it has been supposed that the factor model is correct, but it has not been put to empirical test. While one would not want to expend effort rejecting models that are implausible, this not the case for LCA or FMMs of cannabis use patterns because if population heterogeneity exists, then it could prove very useful in the context of prevention or treatment. Since we know of no published reports that have compared LFAs, LCAs and FMMs fitted to cannabis use disorder (CUD) symptoms our aim is to run omnibus comparisons between these models using DSM-III-R and DSM-IV cannabis diagnostic criteria. This will enable us to derive a best fitting and parsimonious CUD phenotype from existing, widely used assessment items which can potentially improve our understanding of the etiology and the prevention and treatment of CUDs.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Described in detail elsewhere (Kendler and Prescott, 2006), male data came from a 2nd wave of interviews between 1994 and 1998. Subjects were eligible for participation in the 1st wave if they were successfully matched to birth records, or their cotwin was; a member of a multiple birth with at least one male; Caucasian; and born between 1940 and 1974. Of 9,417 eligible individuals for the 1st wave (1993–1996), 6,814 (72.4%) completed the interview. At least one year later, we contacted male-male and opposite-sex male-female twin pairs who had completed the initial interview to schedule a second interview. The 2nd interview was completed by 5,629 individuals (82.6%).

As part of another ongoing study using the Mid Atlantic Twin Registry, the female data came from a 4th wave of interviews (1995–1997) of same-sex female-female twin pairs. These twins were eligible for participation if one or both twins were successfully matched to birth records; a member of a multiple birth with at least one female; Caucasian, and born between 1933 and 1972. At the 4th wave, ~2400 twins were potentially eligible from whom complete data were collected from 1928 individuals (~80%). These data were augmented with females from the 2nd interview (1994–1998) described above.

Complete cannabis initiation data were available from 4013 male subjects ranging in age from 20 to 58 years (⎧ = 37.0 yrs, σ2 = 9.1yrs). Unlike previous analyses, these data (Gillespie et al., 2007) included an additional 1602 males from opposite-sex and incomplete twin pairs. Complete cannabis initiation data were available from 3303 female subjects ranging in age from 21 to 63 years (⎧ = 36.5yrs, σ2 = 8.5yrs).

2.2. Measures and reliability

All data sets included identical measures of cannabis lifetime abuse and dependence which were assessed using an adaptation of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R-Patient Version (Spitzer et al., 1987) which was structured to allow assignment of DSM-IV drug disorder diagnoses. The eleven symptoms shown in Table 1 assessed four abuse and seven dependence criteria on a three-point scale [Definite / Probable / No]. ‘Definite’ and ‘Probable’ were combined in order to analyse each symptom as a dichotomous outcome.

Table 1.

Item endorsements for the stem and diagnostic criteria.

Males Females

1. Stem item 1 36.4% (N=4013) 16.9% (N=3245)
2. Used cannabis in dangerous situations 39.9% (N=1460) 21.4% (N=757)
3. Use caused legal problems or traffic accidents 4.4% (N=1460) 1.7% (N=757)
4. Use caused problems with family, friends, or people at work 15.2% (N=1458) 11.3% (N=755)
5. Use caused physical problems, made depressed or very nervous 13.0% (N=1459) 10.9% (N=755)
6. Failure to fulfill major role obligation at work school or home2 28.2% (N=1459) 18.6% (N=757)
7. When started use ended up taking more than planned 26.0% (N=976) 31.9% (N=389)
8. Tried to cut down or stop using it 43.9% (N=976) 44.4% (N=390)
9. Spent time taking or using it, recovering from it, or doing whatever to get it 22.5% (N=976) 18.3% (N=389)
10. Used so often instead of work, hobbies or spending time with family or friends 12.7% (N=975) 9.5% (N=389)
11. Needed to use a lot more to get high or feel effects than when first used 31.3% (N=976) 28.7% (N=390)
12. Withdrawal symptoms3 6.5% (N=976) 5.7% (N=389)

1 Stem coding: 0 = never tried or tried but never ≥ 11 times per month or ≥ 6 times lifetime, 1 = tried & used ≥ 6 times lifetime but never ≥ 11 times per month or, 2 = tried & used ≥ 11 times per month & ≥ 6 times lifetime. Represents proportion of subjects with stem ≥ 1

3 Aggregation of used often when doing something important OR stayed away from school or missed appointments because of use.

4 Aggregation of felt sick when cutting down or stopped use OR after not using cannabis, used to prevent sickness.

In order to correspond to the first DSM-IV abuse criterion, item six is an aggregate of “used often when doing something important” or “stayed away from school or missed appointments because of use.” Similarly, item 12 is an aggregate of “felt sick when cutting down or stopped use” or “after not using cannabis, used to prevent sickness” to correspond with the DSM-III-R withdrawal.

Following screening for having ever taken cannabis (yes/no), administration of the drug abuse and dependence symptoms was contingent upon responses to two binary (yes/no) items measuring cannabis use. Subjects who endorsed using cannabis ‘6 or more times in a lifetime’ but not ‘ 11 times in a month’ were only asked abuse symptoms. However, if any abuse symptoms were endorsed then the dependence symptoms were also asked. Subjects who endorsed more frequent cannabis use ‘11 times in a month’ were automatically asked all symptoms. To determine whether they provided effective screening, the screening items were summed and recoded onto a three-point ordinal scale ‘stem item’ and included in the analyses. Our rationale for including the stem was that joint analysis of the stem and substance-use symptoms should produce asymptotically unbiased estimates of (i) the proportion of people in general the population who would develop symptoms if they were to initiate cannabis (i.e., the probes’ thresholds) and (ii) the correlation between liability to initiate and the liability to endorse abuse and dependence (represented by the factor loadings).

Interviewers had a master’s degree in a mental health-related field or a bachelor’s degree in this area, plus 2 years of clinical experience. We have reported previously on the high test-retest reliability of our assessment of drug abuse and dependence and the similarity of our prevalence estimates to those reported from other large U.S. samples (Kendler et al., 2000).

2.3. Statistical Analyses

We used marginal maximum likelihood (MML) raw ordinal data analysis in Mx (Neale et al., 2006b) to model the cannabis diagnostic criteria. MML (Bock and Aitken, 1981) provides a fast means of calculating model parameters by integrating the latent factor distribution using 10-point quadrature (Neale et al., 2006a). In MML the likelihood function is calculated as the product of the conditional probability for each item at a series of abscissae along the latent trait distribution. The product of these conditional single item responses provides the overall likelihood for an observed vector of item scores. This product of one-dimensional integrals can be used because when conditioned on the factor, the items are independent.

2.4. Choice of best fitting and most parsimonious model

Latent factor, latent class, and factor mixture models were fitted to the ordinal data and compared using omnibus fit indices. In addition to inspecting the 'minus twice log likelihood' (−2LL) and the traditional Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) (Akaike, 1987) we used the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) and sample-size adjusted BIC (Schwarz, 1978) indices. The lowest, most negative −2LL value is indicative of the best fitting model, whereas the lowest, most negative AIC, BIC and sBIC values are indicative of model parsimony. This distinction is important because in Maximum Likelihood estimation, log likelihoods can be improved by simply 'overfitting' or increasing the number of parameters. Indices of parsimony penalize models with increasing numbers of parameters thereby providing an index of each model's efficiency in terms of explaining observed data patterns.

Simulations have shown that BIC indices can correctly discriminate between LCA and LFA models (Markon and Krueger, 2004); the difference between any 2 BICs can be directly interpreted as having corrected for expected effects of sampling variation, and is exponentially related to the posterior odds of one model versus another (Markon and Krueger, 2005). Moreover, the BIC should correctly identify the best approximating model in large samples, even among nonnested alternatives (Barron and Cover, 1991; Markon and Krueger, 2005; Vereshchagin and Vitanyi, 2004) thus providing a good omnibus comparison which is central to this paper’s aim.

Our decision to include the BIC and sBIC was also based on simulations (Nylund et al., 2007) that have shown how the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) and sample size adjusted BIC (sBIC) (Schwarz, 1978) can outperform the AIC in complex structures in which symptoms have different endorsement probabilities for more than one latent class. Although parametric bootstrapping may provide a better discrimination between LCA and FMM models with different numbers of latent classes (Nylund et al., 2007) it was not used because of its heavy computational burden.

3. Results

3.1. Item endorsements

Table 1 includes the item endorsements for each of the stem and diagnostic criteria. We formally modeled age at interview on item thresholds to adjust for potential age-related cohort changes in symptom endorsement. Among users, the two most commonly endorsed items for males and females are using cannabis in dangerous or hazardous situations and trying to cut down or stop using. The two least endorsed items are cannabis use resulting in legal problems or traffic accidents and withdrawal symptoms which include either feeling sick when cutting down or stopping use or using cannabis to prevent sickness.

3.2. Phenotypic correlations and eigenvalues

Polychoric correlations for the stem and diagnostic criteria appear in Table 2. Except for item 8 ‘Tried to cut down…stop using it’ most of the correlations were moderate to high. The first three eigenvalues for males and females respectively were 7.19, 1.18, 0.75 and 7.32, 1.08, 0.98, suggesting either a one or two factor dimensional structure to the data.

Table 2.

Age adjusted phenotypic polychoric1 correlations for the male (below diagonal) and female (above diagonal) stem and diagnostic criteria.

1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12.

1. Stem item 1 0.62 0.28 0.71 0.41 0.85 0.67 0.23 0.82 0.77 0.79 0.68
2. Used cannabis in dangerous situations 0.68 1 0.52 0.59 0.44 0.69 0.56 0.26 0.65 0.64 0.60 0.56
3. Use caused legal problems or traffic accidents 0.71 0.60 1 0.36 0.13 0.36 0.22 0.19 0.49 0.39 0.33 0.19
4. Use caused problems with family, friends, or people at work 0.73 0.61 0.67 1 0.54 0.76 0.67 0.43 0.73 0.82 0.65 0.62
5. Use caused physical problems, made depressed or very nervous 0.36 0.47 0.44 0.50 1 0.44 0.50 0.34 0.59 0.50 0.42 0.40
6. Failure to fulfill major role obligation at work school or home 2 0.86 0.67 0.62 0.71 0.41 1 0.71 0.32 0.84 0.87 0.70 0.70
7. When started use ended up taking more than planned 0.56 0.58 0.42 0.53 0.47 0.57 1 0.50 0.74 0.74 0.72 0.70
8. Tried to cut down or stop using it −0.02 −0.01 0.08 0.15 0.13 0.01 0.19 1 0.24 0.30 0.39 0.36
9. Spent time taking or using it, recovering from it, or doing whatever to get it 0.77 0.64 0.59 0.66 0.44 0.78 0.65 0.09 1 0.89 0.79 0.75
10. Used so often instead of work, hobbies or spending time with family or friends 0.74 0.66 0.62 0.72 0.55 0.80 0.67 0.14 0.80 1 0.72 0.78
11. Needed to use a lot more to get high or feel effects than when first used 0.68 0.55 0.57 0.59 0.42 0.63 0.68 0.11 0.74 0.69 1 0.73
12. Withdrawal symptoms 3 0.66 0.52 0.55 0.53 0.45 0.58 0.65 0.26 0.72 0.66 0.70 1

1 Based on the Central Limit Theorem polychoric correlations assume that underlying each ordinal response there is continuously and normally distributed scale of liability which is multifactorial and that the joint distribution of this scale with liability scales underlying other ordinal items is bivariate normal.

2 Aggregation of used often when doing something important OR stayed away from school or missed appointments because of use.

3 Aggregation of felt sick when cutting down or stopped use OR after not using cannabis, used to prevent sickness.

3.3. Model comparisons

Fit statistics for males and females with the two best models highlighted under each fit index appear in Table 3. For both sexes, a 1-factor 3-class FMM solution provided a good fit as judged by the AIC. However, it was the 2-factor LFA solution that consistently performed better across all three fit indices. Varimax-rotated factor loadings based on 2-factor LFA appear in Table 4. For males and females this included a general use, abuse and dependence dimension. Only one item “Tried to cut down or stop using it” did not load onto this factor. All of the dependence items cross-loaded onto a second factor for males. However, for females, the second factor was less interpretable; it included two dependence “Tried to cut down or stop using it” and “When started use ended up taking more than planned” and one abuse item “Use caused problems with family, friends, or people at work” cross-loaded onto this dimension.

Table 3.

Comparison of the empirical, latent factor (LFA), latent class (LCA) and factor mixture models (FMM) by way of Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) and sample size adjusted Bayesian Information Criterion (sBIC). LFA analyses also include oblique (r) solutions. Best fitting model is based on consistent performance across three fit indices. Bolding indicates top two under each index.

Males −2LL df AIC BIC sBIC
LFA – 1 dimension 18097.07 17127 −16156.93 −62005.35 −34794.35
LFA – 2 dimensions 17942.46 17116 16289.54 62037.02 34843.49
LFA – 2 dimensions r 17942.46 17115 −16287.54 62032.87 34840.94
LFA – 3 dimensions
LCA – 1 class 19671.17 17140 −14608.83 −61272.23 −34040.58
LCA – 2 class 18147.99 17113 −16078.01 −61921.81 −34733.05
LCA – 3 class 17964.42 17087 −16209.58 −61905.73 −34758.28
FMM – 1 factor 2 class 17976.43 17089 −16201.57 −61908.02 −34757.39
FMM – 1 factor 3 class 17762.09 17051 16339.91 −61857.54 −34767.29

Females −2LL df AIC BIC sBIC

LFA - 1 dimension 8705.13 9325 −9944.87 33343.15 18528.34
LFA - 2 dimension 8659.90 9314 9968.10 33321.29 18523.97
LFA - 2 dimensions r 8659.90 9313 −9966.10 −33317.25 −18521.51
LFA - 3 dimension
LCA - 1 class 9382.87 9338 −9293.13 33056.83 −18221.37
LCA - 2 class 8731.30 9311 −9890.70 −33273.46 −18480.90
LCA - 3 class 8638.88 9285 −9931.12 −33214.57 −18463.32
FMM - 1 factor 2 class 8677.89 9287 −9896.11 −33203.15 −18448.72
FMM - 1 factor 3 class 8517.28 9249 9980.73 −33129.85 −18435.79

−2LL = −2 × Log Likelihood

Table 4.

Varimax rotated factor loadings for the best fitting and parsimonious two factor solution for males and females.

Males
Females
η1 η2 η1 η2
1. Stem item 0.97 −0.01 0.87 0.08
2. Used cannabis in dangerous situations 0.66 0.31 0.64 0.22
3. Use caused legal problems or traffic accidents 0.68 0.21 0.50 0.11
4. Use caused problems with family, friends, or people at work 0.72 0.30 0.69 0.42
5. Use caused physical problems, made depressed or very nervous 0.35 0.51 0.43 0.39
6. Failure to fulfill major role obligation at work school or homea 0.84 0.21 0.86 0.24
7. When started use ended up taking more than planned 0.52 0.59 0.62 0.56
8. Tried to cut down or stop using it 0.00 0.32 0.07 0.76
9. Spent time taking or using it, recovering from it, or doing whatever to get it 0.79 0.40 0.89 0.24
10. Used so often instead of work, hobbies or spending time with family or friends 0.77 0.45 0.88 0.30
11. Needed to use a lot more to get high or feel effects than when first used 0.66 0.45 0.67 0.36
12. Withdrawal symptomsb 0.60 0.52 0.72 0.35

η1 and η2 denote latent factors 1 and 2. Factor loadings >0.40 are bolder for ease of interpretation.

a

Aggregation of used often when doing something important OR stayed away from school or missed appointments because of use.

b

Aggregation of felt sick when cutting down or stopped use OR after not using cannabis, used to prevent sickness.

4. Discussion

We found little evidence for population heterogeneity among cannabis users. Instead, our findings were commensurate with the consensus for a single factor that captures most of the association between the cannabis symptoms (Feingold and Rounsaville, 1995a, b; Hartman et al., 2008; Langenbucher et al., 2004; Lynskey and Agrawal, 2007; Nelson et al., 1999; Teesson et al., 2002). This consensus is anticipated in the forthcoming DSM-V that proposes to remove the current abuse-dependence distinction as well as revive the withdrawal criteria. With the exception of one item, all of the abuse and dependence symptoms as well as the stem loaded onto a single, general cannabis use disorder (CUD) factor. Although any incremental contribution made by the second factor is likely to be minimal, the small but significant second factor nevertheless does suggest that the physiological, behavioral and cognitive problems relating to cannabis are perhaps more complex than recent findings indicate.

A closer reading of population (Compton et al., 2009; Hartman et al., 2008; Lynskey and Agrawal, 2007; Teesson et al., 2002) and clinical (Feingold and Rounsaville, 1995a; Hartman et al., 2008; Langenbucher et al., 2004) studies that have reported a single cannabis factor model based on either 12-month or lifetime criteria revealed that a two factor solution provided a comparable (Hartman et al., 2008; Teesson et al., 2002) or, in two instances, a better (Feingold and Rounsaville, 1995a; Lynskey and Agrawal, 2007) fit to the data. Of the two studies that reported eigen values (Langenbucher et al., 2004; Nelson et al., 1999), in both cases two eigens exceeded one. However, it is important to note that the ratios of the first to second eigens were very large (Langenbucher et al., 2004; Nelson et al., 1999). Moreover, the factor correlations seen in the larger population based studies were extremely high (Lynskey and Agrawal, 2007; Teesson et al., 2002). Lynskey (Lynskey and Agrawal, 2007), in his analysis of the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions data, justified the single factor solution by arguing that improvements in model fit associated with two-factor models can be attributable in part to large samples (Bentler, 1990) that can lead to trivial rejections of more parsimonious models.

There are notable differences between the current and previous findings. Previously, we reported (Gillespie et al., 2007) a single factor solution for cannabis use, abuse and dependence. This result was based on a smaller subset of the male data whereas the current analyses were augmented with males from opposite-sex and unmatched twin pairs. We also aggregated “used often when doing something important” and “stayed away from school or missed appointments because of use” into a single measure to correspond to the first DSM-IV abuse criterion. Likewise, two withdrawal items “felt sick when cutting down or stopped use” and “after not using cannabis, used to prevent sickness” were aggregated to correspond to DSM-III-R withdrawal.

Another difference with previous findings concerns the size of the factor loading particularly for the ‘Use caused legal problems or traffic accidents’ item. This raises questions about the item’s usefulness as discriminating marker for CUD. Although four papers based on varying sample size and measurement criteria (12-month versus lifetime) have reported moderate to high loadings (Agrawal and Lynskey, 2007; Feingold and Rounsaville, 1995a; Lynskey and Agrawal, 2007; Nelson et al., 1999), two papers based on a small Australian sample of cannabis users (Teesson et al., 2002) and the US population based sample (National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC)) (Compton et al., 2009) found that legal problems loaded poorly onto the single factor solutions. This discrepancy in factor loadings indicates that the item’s utility in terms of discrimination (see Takane and Leeuw, 1987) varies considerably. However, if we disregard the small or clinical samples (Feingold and Rounsaville, 1995a; Nelson et al., 1999; Teesson et al., 2002) and focus on the two large population based NESARC studies, then it is possible that the discrepancy is attributable in part to the 12-month (Compton et al., 2009) versus lifetime criteria (Lynskey and Agrawal, 2007). Argrawal (Agrawal and Lynskey, 2007) in a re-analysis of the NESARC data using a life-time criteria found that although the item was endorsed at higher levels of risk among female users it was less discriminating by virtue of its relatively lower factor loading. In the absence of formal tests of sex invariance it is unclear if there was a significant sex difference in terms of item discrimination. In our sample, the lower factor loadings for females (0.50 versus 0.68) does suggest that legal problems may in fact discriminate better among male users.

Contrary to the uncertainty surrounding the clinical significance of withdrawal symptoms (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), there is converging evidence from laboratory and clinical studies (Budney et al., 2004; Budney et al., 2003; Lichtman and Martin, 2002; Lichtman et al., 2005) that withdrawal diagnostic criteria form part of a valid syndrome. Our pattern of factor loadings strongly indicates that withdrawal as measured either by the symptom of feeling sick when cutting down or stopping use or using cannabis to prevent sickness is likely part of a larger, single cannabis use disorder comprising withdrawal, use, abuse and dependence criteria.

Finally, although not part of the central focus of this paper we capitalized on the twin data to model the genetic etiology of this CUD factor. And consistent with previous findings showing that familial aggregation in cannabis use, abuse and dependence is best explained by a varying combinations (depending on the measure) of additive genetic and shared environmental variance (Agrawal and Lynskey, 2006; Gillespie et al., 2009) our results shown in Table 5 demonstrate that over half the total variance in the first CUD factor was explained by additive genetic effects. Based on the 95% confidence intervals we found marginal evidence of shared environmental variance with the remainder of the variance accounted for by non-shared or unique environmental variance. Similarly large genetic variance has been reported elsewhere (Kendler and Prescott, 1998; Lynskey et al., 2002; Rhee et al., 2003; van den Bree et al., 1998). The less interpretable second factor for females and the ‘dependence’ factor for males was largely explained by non-shared environmental.

4.1. Limitations

Our findings must be interpreted in the context of at least four potential limitations. First, our data were restricted to a white Virginian sample. Previous analyses based on the Mid Atlantic Twin Registry (Kendler et al., 2000) have suggested that it does not differ from the general population in rates of psychopathologic conditions, including illicit substance-use, and although the prevalence rates cannot be generalized to ethnic minorities and African Americans, it is likely to be broadly representative of white North American men and women. Two, assessments were based on a single interview which necessarily included measurement error. Three, endorsement rates for one item ‘loss of control, unable to stop…tried to cut down’ was extremely low which may have resulted in unstable parameter estimates. Finally, because the current analyses assumed that members of twin pairs were independent, there is the limitation of non-independent data. Failure to take account of statistical non-independence of the twins is not expected to change parameter estimates but may alter their confidence intervals. However, non-independence of observations is rarely a problem when the group size is small; in the case of twin data, group size is at most two.

4.2. Conclusion

Results from exhaustive model fitting revealed that the abuse and dependence symptoms as well as the stem loaded onto a single, general factor. Although a second but less interpretable factor that partly captured the dependence and withdrawal symptoms was observed in males, any incremental contribution made by this second factor was likely to be minimal. These conclusions are commensurate with an emerging consensus for a single factor to explain the covariance between use and diagnostic criteria. In addition, these support the proposed changes in the forthcoming DSM-V with regard to substance-related disorders1.

Before attempting to replicate these findings using other nationally representative samples, our next aim will be to investigate sex differences in the observed factors within a measurement invariance framework (Meredith, 1993; Vandenberg, 2000). This will enable quantification and significance testing of possible measurement differences in the factor means, factor loadings or item thresholds, as well as correction for these effects if found. Following this, we will further leverage this genetically informative data to estimate maximally heritable factor scores for each individual in order to provide increased power to detect quantitative trait loci signals for CUD and correlated drug use phenotypes as part of anticipated genome wide tests of genomic association.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Ms Indrani Ray for database assistance and Dr Linda Corey for assistance with the ascertainment of twins from the Virginia Twin Registry, now part of the Mid-Atlantic Twin Registry (MATR), directed by Dr. Judy Silberg. The MATR has received support from NIH, the Carman Trust, and the W.M. Keck, John Templeton, and Robert Wood Johnson Foundations.

Role of Funding Source

Funding was received from the US National Institute on Drug Abuse (R00DA023549, 1K99DA023549-01A2, DA-18673) and the US National Institutes of Health (DA-11287, MH/AA/DA-49492,MH-01458, AA-00236). The US NIH and NIDA had no role in the design of the study, data collection, data analysis and interpretation of data, in the writing of this manuscript, or decision to submit this manuscript for publication.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributors

Dr Gillespie designed the study, undertook the statistical analyses, reviewed the literature, and wrote the first draft manuscript. Dr Neale assisted in the design and implementation of the statistical analyses. All authors contributed to and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

There are no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Agrawal A, Lynskey MT. The genetic epidemiology of cannabis use, abuse and dependence. Addiction. 2006;101:801–812. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01399.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Agrawal A, Lynskey MT. Does gender contribute to heterogeneity in criteria for cannabis abuse and dependence? Results from the National Epidemiological Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;88:300–307. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Agrawal A, Neale MC, Prescott CA, Kendler KS. A twin study of early cannabis use and subsequent use and abuse/dependence of other illicit drugs. Psychol. Med. 2004;34:1227–1237. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704002545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  5. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-III-R. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  6. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  7. Barron AR, Cover TM. Minimum complexity density estimation. IEEE Trans. Info. Theory. 1991;1:1034–1054. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol. Bull. 1990;107:238–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bock RD, Aitken M. Marginal maximum likelihood estimation of item parameters: an application of the EM algorithm. Psychometrika. 1981;46:443–460. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bucholz KK, Heath AC, Reich T, Hesselbrock VM, Kramer JR, Nurnberger JI, Jr, Schuckit MA. Can we subtype alcoholism? A latent class analysis of data from relatives of alcoholics in a multicenter family study of alcoholism. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 1996;20:1462–1471. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb01150.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Budney AJ, Hughes JR, Moore BA, Vandrey R. Review of the validity and significance of cannabis withdrawal syndrome. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2004;161:1967–1977. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.11.1967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Budney AJ, Moore BA, Vandrey RG, Hughes JR. The time course and significance of cannabis withdrawal. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2003;112:393–402. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.112.3.393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Compton WM, Saha TD, Conway KP, Grant BF. The role of cannabis use within a dimensional approach to cannabis use disorders. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;100:221–227. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.10.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Degenhardt L, Chiu WT, Sampson N, Kessler RC, Anthony JC, Angermeyer M, Bruffaerts R, de Girolamo G, Gureje O, Huang Y, Karam A, Kostyuchenko S, Lepine JP, Mora ME, Neumark Y, Ormel JH, Pinto-Meza A, Posada-Villa J, Stein DJ, Takeshima T, Wells JE. Toward a global view of alcohol, tobacco, cannabis, and cocaine use: findings from the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. PLoS Med. 2008;5:e141. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0050141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Degenhardt L, Lynskey M, Hall W. Cohort trends in the age of initiation of drug use in Australia. Aust. N. Z. J. Public Health. 2000;24:421–426. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-842x.2000.tb01605.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dennis M, Babor TF, Roebuck MC, Donaldson J. Changing the focus: the case for recognizing and treating cannabis use disorders. Addiction. 2002;97 Suppl. 1:4–15. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.97.s01.10.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dolan CV, Maas HLJvd. Fitting multivariate normal finite mixtures subject to structural equation modeling. Psychometrika. 1998;63:227–253. [Google Scholar]
  18. Edwards G, Arif A, Hadgson R. Nomenclature and classification of drug- and alcohol-related problems: a WHO Memorandum. Bull. World Health Organ. 1981;59:225–242. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ellickson PL, Martino SC, Collins RL. Marijuana use from adolescence to young adulthood: multiple developmental trajectories and their associated outcomes. Health Psychol. 2004;23:299–307. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.23.3.299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Everitt BS. A Finite Mixture Model For The Clustering Of Mixed-Mode Data. Statistics and Probability Letters. 1988;6:305–309. [Google Scholar]
  21. Feingold A, Rounsaville B. Construct validity of the abuse-dependence distinction as measured by DSM-IV criteria for different psychoactive substances. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1995a;39:99–109. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(95)01142-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Feingold A, Rounsaville B. Construct validity of the dependence syndrome as measure by DSM-IV for different psychoactive substances. Addiction. 1995b;90:1661–1669. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1995.901216618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gillespie NA, Neale MC, Jacobson K, Kendler KS. Modeling the genetic and environmental association between peer group deviance cannabis use in male twins. Addiction. 2009;104:420–429. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02457.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gillespie NA, Neale MC, Prescott CA, Aggen SH, Kendler KS. Factor and item-response analysis of DSM-IV criteria for abuse of and dependence on cannabis, cocaine, hallucinogens, sedatives, stimulants and opioids. Addiction. 2007;102:920–930. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01804.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Grant JD, Scherrer JF, Neuman RJ, Todorov AA, Price RK, Bucholz KK. A comparison of the latent class structure of cannabis problems among adult men and women who have used cannabis repeatedly. Addiction. 2006;101:1133–1142. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01463.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hall W, Johnston L, Donnelly N. The epidemiology of cannabis use and its consequences. In: Kalant H, Corrigal W, Hall W, Smart R, editors. The Health Effects of Cannabis. Toronto: Addiction Research Foundation; 1999. pp. 69–125. [Google Scholar]
  27. Harford TC, Muthen BO. The dimensionality of alcohol abuse and dependence: a multivariate analysis of DSM-IV symptom items in the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs. 2001;62:150–157. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2001.62.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hartman CA, Gelhorn H, Crowley TJ, Sakai JT, Stallings M, Young SE, Rhee SH, Corley R, Hewitt JK, Hopfer CJ. Item response theory analysis of DSM-IV cannabis abuse and dependence criteria in adolescents. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2008;47:165–173. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31815cd9f2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hasin D, Grant BF, Cottier L, Blaine J, Towle L, Ustun B, Sartonus N. Nosological comparisons of alcohol and drug diagnoses: a multisite, multi-instrument international study. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1997;25:217–226. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(97)00092-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Jedidi K, Jagpal HS, Desarbo WS. Finite-mixture structural equation models for response-based segmentation and unobserved heterogeneity. Mark. Sci. 1997;16:39–59. [Google Scholar]
  31. Johnson RA, Gerstein DR. Initiation of use of alcohol, cigarettes, marijuana, cocaine, and other substances in US birth cohorts since 1919. Am. J. Public Health. 1998;88:27–33. doi: 10.2105/ajph.88.1.27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kandel DB, Chen K. Types of marijuana users by longitudinal course. J. Stud. Alcohol. 2000;61:367–378. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2000.61.367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kendler KS, Gardner C, Jacobson KC, Neale MC, Prescott CA. Genetic and environmental influences on illicit drug use and tobacco use across birth cohorts. Psychol. Med. 2005;35:1349–1356. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705004964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kendler KS, Jacobson KC, Prescott CA, Neale MC. Specificity of genetic and environmental risk factors for use and abuse/dependence of cannabis, cocaine, hallucinogens, sedatives, stimulants, and opiates in male twins. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2003;160 doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.687. 687-495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, Neale MC, Prescott CA. Illicit psychoactive substance use, heavy use, abuse, and dependence in a US population-based sample of male twins. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2000;57:261–269. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.3.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kendler KS, Prescott CA. Cannabis use, abuse, and dependence in a population-based sample of female twins. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1998;155:1016–1022. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.8.1016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kendler KS, Prescott CA. Genes, Environment, and Psychopathology: Understanding the Causes of Psychiatric and Substance Use Disorders. New York: The Guilford Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  38. Kirisci L, Tarter R, Mezzich A, Ridenour T, Reynolds M, Vanyukov M. Prediction of cannabis use disorder between boyhood and young adulthood: clarifying the phenotype and environtype. Am. J. Addict. 2009;18:36–47. doi: 10.1080/10550490802408829. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Langenbucher JW, Labouvie E, Martin CS, Sanjuan PM, Bavly L, Kirisci L, Chung T. An application of item response theory analysis to alcohol, cannabis, and cocaine criteria in DSM-IV. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2004;113:72–80. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lazarsfeld PF, Henry NW. Latent structure analysis. Boston: Houghton Mifflin; 1968. [Google Scholar]
  41. Lennox RD, Zarkin GA, Bray JW. Latent variable models of alcohol-related constructs. J. Subst. Abuse. 1996;8:241–250. doi: 10.1016/s0899-3289(96)90282-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Leoutsakos JM, Zandi PP, Bandeen-Roche K, Lyketsos CG. Searching for valid psychiatric phenotypes: discrete latent variable models. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 2010;19:63–73. doi: 10.1002/mpr.301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lichtman AH, Martin BR. Marijuana withdrawal syndrome in the animal model. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 2002;42:20S–27S. doi: 10.1002/j.1552-4604.2002.tb05999.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lichtman AH, Martin BR, Budney AJ, Hughes JR, Moore BA, Vandrey R. Cannabinoid tolerance and dependence: review of the validity and significance of cannabis withdrawal syndrome. Handb. Exp. Pharmacol. 2005;161:691–717. doi: 10.1007/3-540-26573-2_24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lynskey MT, Agrawal A. Psychometric properties of DSM assessments of illicit drug abuse and dependence: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC) Psychol. Med. 2007;37:1345–1355. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lynskey MT, Heath AC, Nelson EC, Bucholz KK, Madden PA, Slutske WS, Statham DJ, Martin NG. Genetic and environmental contributions to cannabis dependence in a national young adult twin sample. Psychol. Med. 2002;32:195–207. doi: 10.1017/s0033291701005062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lynskey MT, Nelson EC, Neuman RJ, Bucholz KK, Madden PA, Knopik VS, Slutske W, Whitfield JB, Martin NG, Heath AC. Limitations of DSM-IV operationalizations of alcohol abuse and dependence in a sample of Australian twins. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 2005;8:574–584. doi: 10.1375/183242705774860178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Markon KE, Krueger RF. A modeling approach to distinguishing between discrete and continuous forms of psychopathology. Paper presented at: 19th Annual Meeting of the Society for Research in Psychopathology; October 10, 2004; St Louis, Mo. 2004. [Google Scholar]
  49. Markon KE, Krueger RF. Categorical and continuous models of liability to externalizing disorders: a direct comparison in NESARC. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2005;62:1352–1359. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.12.1352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. McLachlan GJ, Peel D. Finite mixture models. New York: John Wiley and Sons; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  51. Meredith W. Measurement invariance, factor analysis, and factorial invariance. Psychometrika. 1993;58:525–543. [Google Scholar]
  52. Moustaki I. A latent trait and latent class model for mixed observed variables. Br. J. Math. Stat. Psychol. 1996;49:313–334. [Google Scholar]
  53. Muller S, Gmel G. Changes in the age of onset of cannabis use: results of the 2nd Swiss Health Survey 1997. Soz. Praventivmed. 2002;47:14–23. doi: 10.1007/BF01318401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Muthen B. Should substance use disorders be considered as categorical or dimensional? Addiction. 2006;101 Suppl. 1:6–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01583.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Muthen B, Asparouhov T. Item response mixture modeling: application to tobacco dependence criteria. Addict. Behav. 2006;31:1050–1066. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.03.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Muthen B, Brown CH, Masyn K, Jo B, Khoo ST, Yang CC, Wang CP, Kellam SG, Carlin JB, Liao J. General growth mixture modeling for randomized preventive interventions. Biostatistics. 2002;3:459–475. doi: 10.1093/biostatistics/3.4.459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Muthen B, Muthen LK. Integrating person-centered and variable-centered analyses: growth mixture modeling with latent trajectory classes. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2000;24:882–891. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Muthen B, Shedden K. Finite mixture modeling with mixture outcomes using the EM algorithm. Biometrics. 1999;55:463–469. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341x.1999.00463.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Neale MC, Aggen SH, Maes HH, Kubarych TS, Schmitt JE. Methodological issues in the assessment of substance use phenotypes. Addict. Behav. 2006a;31 doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.03.047. 1010-1011-1034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Neale MC, Boker SM, Xie G, Maes HH. Mx: Statistical Modeling. 7th Edition. Richmond: Medical College of Virginia; 2006b. [Google Scholar]
  61. Nelson CB, Rehm J, Ustun TB, Grant B, Chatterji S. Factor structures for DSM-IV substance disorder criteria endorsed by alcohol, cannabis, cocaine and opiate users: results from the WHO reliability and validity study. Addiction. 1999;94:843–855. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.9468438.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Nylund KL, Asparouhov T, Muthén B. Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling. A Monte Carlo simulation study. Struct. Equ. Modeling. 2007;14:535–569. [Google Scholar]
  63. Pickles A, Angold A. Natural categories or fundamental dimensions: on carving nature at the joints and the rearticulation of psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 2003;15:529–551. doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Rhee SH, Hewitt JK, Young SE, Corley RP, Crowley TJ, Stallings MC. Genetic and environmental influences on substance initiation, use, and problem use in adolescents. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2003;60:1256–1264. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.12.1256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Samhsa . National Household Survey on Drug Abuse Main Findings 1996. Rockville, MD: National Clearinghouse for Alcohol & Drug Info (NCADI); 1998. [Google Scholar]
  66. Schwarz G. Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat. 1978;6:461–464. [Google Scholar]
  67. Spearman C. General intelligence, objectively determined and measured. Am. J. Psychol. 1904;4:201–293. [Google Scholar]
  68. Spitzer RL, Williams JB, Gibbon J. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R -Patient Version (SCID-P, 4/1/87) New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  69. Stinson FS, Grant BF, Dawson DA, Ruan WJ, Huang B, Saha T. Comorbidity between DSM-IV alcohol and specific drug use disorders in the United States: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005;80:105–116. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Takane Y, Leeuw Jd. On the relationship between item response theory and factor analysis of discretized variables. Psychometrika. 1987;52:393–408. [Google Scholar]
  71. Teesson M, Lynskey M, Manor B, Baillie A. The structure of cannabis dependence in the community. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2002;68:255–262. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(02)00223-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. van den Bree MB, Johnson EO, Neale MC, Pickens RW. Genetic and environmental influences on drug use and abuse/dependence in male and female twins. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1998;52:231–241. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(98)00101-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Vandenberg RJ. Toward a further understanding of and improvement in measurement invariance methods and procedures. Org. Res. Methods. 2000;5:139–158. [Google Scholar]
  74. Vereshchagin NK, Vitanyi PMB. Kolmogorov’s structure functions and model selection. IEEE Trans. Info. Theory. 2004;50:3265–3290. [Google Scholar]
  75. von Sydow K, Lieb R, Pfister H, Hofler M, Sonntag H, Wittchen HU. The natural course of cannabis use, abuse and dependence over four years: a longitudinal community study of adolescents and young adults. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2001;64:347–361. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(01)00137-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Waller NG, Meehl PE. Multivariate taxometric procedures: Distinguishing types from continua (Advanced Quantitative Techniques in the Social Sciences) Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  77. Windle M, Wiesner M. Trajectories of marijuana use from adolescence to young adulthood: predictors and outcomes. Dev. Psychopathol. 2004;16:1007–1027. doi: 10.1017/s0954579404040118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Yung YF. Finite mixtures in confirmatory factor-analysis models. Psychometrika. 1997;62:297–330. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES