Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2011 Aug 31;31(35):12613–12628. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0152-11.2011

Dissociable Effects of Top-Down and Bottom-Up Attention during Episodic Encoding

Melina R Uncapher 1,, J Benjamin Hutchinson 1, Anthony D Wagner 1,2
PMCID: PMC3172893  NIHMSID: NIHMS321959  PMID: 21880922

Abstract

It is well established that the formation of memories for life's experiences—episodic memory—is influenced by how we attend to those experiences, yet the neural mechanisms by which attention shapes episodic encoding are still unclear. We investigated how top-down and bottom-up attention contribute to memory encoding of visual objects in humans by manipulating both types of attention during fMRI of episodic memory formation. We show that dorsal parietal cortex—specifically, intraparietal sulcus (IPS)—was engaged during top-down attention and was also recruited during the successful formation of episodic memories. By contrast, bottom-up attention engaged ventral parietal cortex—specifically, temporoparietal junction (TPJ)—and was also more active during encoding failure. Functional connectivity analyses revealed further dissociations in how top-down and bottom-up attention influenced encoding: while both IPS and TPJ influenced activity in perceptual cortices thought to represent the information being encoded (fusiform/lateral occipital cortex), they each exerted opposite effects on memory encoding. Specifically, during a preparatory period preceding stimulus presentation, a stronger drive from IPS was associated with a higher likelihood that the subsequently attended stimulus would be encoded. By contrast, during stimulus processing, stronger connectivity with TPJ was associated with a lower likelihood the stimulus would be successfully encoded. These findings suggest that during encoding of visual objects into episodic memory, top-down and bottom-up attention can have opposite influences on perceptual areas that subserve visual object representation, suggesting that one manner in which attention modulates memory is by altering the perceptual processing of to-be-encoded stimuli.

Introduction

The creation of new memories for life's experiences is intimately linked to how we attend (Craik and Lockhart, 1972). During an event, we may willfully direct attention to information relevant to ongoing goals (“top-down” attention) or our attention may be captured by salient information (“bottom-up” attention) (Kastner and Ungerleider, 2000; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002). How such attentive acts shape event encoding is unclear.

The neurobiology of top-down and bottom-up attention is posited to consist of separable yet interacting frontoparietal networks (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002), and recent work has begun to consider how the parietal components of these networks are engaged during event encoding (Uncapher and Wagner, 2009). During attention paradigms, functional imaging data indicate that dorsal posterior parietal cortex (dPPC) activity tracks demands on top-down attention, and ventral posterior parietal cortex (vPPC) activity tracks recruitment of bottom-up attention (for review, see Corbetta et al., 2008). During event encoding, the fMRI signal in frontal and parietal regions frequently predicts whether the event will be subsequently remembered or forgotten (“subsequent memory effects”) (for review, see Paller and Wagner, 2002; Rugg et al., 2002; Blumenfeld and Ranganath, 2006; Uncapher and Wagner, 2009). A recent across-study analysis of the encoding literature raised the possibility that PPC subsequent memory effects anatomically overlap with PPC correlates of attention (Uncapher and Wagner, 2009). Moreover, the majority of PPC foci demonstrating enhanced activity for events later remembered versus forgotten (“positive” subsequent memory effects) localized to dPPC, whereas foci demonstrating enhanced activity for events later forgotten versus remembered (“negative” subsequent memory effects) localized exclusively to vPPC.

These data motivated the “dual-attention encoding hypothesis,” which posits that top-down and bottom-up attention may differentially foster encoding success and failure, respectively (Uncapher and Wagner, 2009). This hypothesis builds on evidence that top-down attention enhances cortical representations of attended information (Corbetta et al., 1990; Noudoost et al., 2010) (for review, see Beck and Kastner, 2009) and thus may increase the probability that attended information ultimately projects to the medial temporal lobe (MTL) for encoding (e.g., Moscovitch and Umilta, 1990; Uncapher and Rugg, 2009). From this perspective, positive subsequent memory effects emerge when dPPC-mediated top-down attention is directed toward information that will be relevant to later remembering. By contrast, engagement of vPPC-mediated bottom-up attention may lead to later memory failure when attention is captured by information not relevant or helpful to later remembering (Otten and Rugg, 2001a; Wagner and Davachi, 2001; Cabeza et al., 2008; Uncapher and Wagner, 2009).

Because consideration of PPC attention mechanisms on event encoding has largely come from across-study comparisons, it remains unclear whether parietal fluctuations that predict the mnemonic fate of events reflect the influence of top-down and bottom-up attention on episodic encoding. Here we implemented a paradigm that provides independent assays of attention and encoding, within subjects, to determine (1) whether dPPC and vPPC show attention-sensitive activity that is also predictive of subsequent memory success or failure, and (2) whether the manner in which these attention-sensitive regions dynamically interact with the rest of the brain predicts the mnemonic fate of an event.

Materials and Methods

Participants.

Data are reported from 18 right-handed, native English-speaking volunteers (9 female; age range: 18–27 years; mean age: 20.4 years; SD: 2.9 years), each of whom gave written informed consent in accordance with procedures approved by the Institutional Review Board of Stanford University. All participants were recruited from the Stanford University community, reported no history of neurological trauma or disease, and were remunerated at a rate of $20/h. Data from two additional participants were acquired but excluded from analyses, due to their failure to exhibit a behavioral reorienting effect (see Behavioral task, below).

Stimulus materials.

Stimuli consisted of 660 black-and-white line drawings of common objects and 80 artificial “greeble” objects (Gauthier and Tarr, 1997). Common objects were drawn from the International Picture Naming Project (Szekely et al., 2004) and from a set previously used by Uncapher and Rugg (2009). Greebles and common objects were transformed into black-and-white line drawings in Photoshop CS, version 8.0 (Adobe Systems). For the purpose of counterbalancing, common objects were divided into 33 sets of 20 objects each, and greebles into 5 sets of 16 objects each. Object sets were rotated across study and test lists across subjects, and greebles were rotated across study lists. For each subject, 10 study lists were created from these sets, each containing 40 objects and 16 greebles. In each study list, 46 stimuli were presented in the cued location (“valid” trials, see Behavioral task, below; 32 objects and 14 greebles), and the remaining 10 stimuli were presented in the noncued location (“invalid” trials; 8 objects and 2 greebles), yielding an 18% probability that a stimulus would be invalidly cued.

Of the 400 common objects encountered during the study phase, 350 were carried forward into the test phase (to minimize test list length). Thus, in each of 10 sessions, 35 common objects served as the critical study items (27 valid and 8 invalid). In the later memory test, 180 common objects served as foils. A separate set of 10 common objects and 3 greebles was used to create a practice study list.

Behavioral task.

Within the present task context, “top-down attention” refers to the goal-directed allocation and maintenance of visuospatial attention evoked by a preparatory cue (described below), and “bottom-up attention” refers to a shift of visuospatial attention evoked by an object occurring outside the focus of attention. We are agnostic as to whether bottom-up shifts of attention occur by means of automatic, involuntary, or “exogenous” processes versus goal-directed, voluntary, or “endogenous” processes (for review, see Corbetta et al., 2008; Posner and Cohen, 1984; Downar et al., 2001; de Fockert et al., 2004; Kincade et al., 2005; Serences et al., 2005; Indovina and Macaluso, 2007; Burrows and Moore, 2009).

The experiment consisted of 10 scanned incidental study sessions followed by one nonscanned memory test. Each study session lasted ∼6.5 min, and the memory test lasted ∼40 min. The interval between the end of the study phase and the start of test phase was ∼10–15 min. Participants received instructions and practiced the study task before entering the scan suite; study task performance was analyzed during this training session to ensure that participants understood and complied with the task instructions (see below). Moreover, eye movements were visually monitored by two investigators, and feedback was given to the participant to ensure that participants could perform the study task while maintaining fixation; participants were trained to ceiling level, continuing until no visually detectable saccades occurred at any point during the training session. We acknowledge that this training does not ensure that eye movements did not occur during scanning. However, given recent studies of overt versus covert attentional shifts that demonstrate that the same frontoparietal regions are engaged when eye movements occur and when they do not (Ikkai and Curtis, 2008), we believe the possible presence of eye movements in the present study does not alter the interpretation of the findings. We also note that eye movements were not recorded during scanning in many prior studies of Posner attention cueing, including the study to which we compare our attention-related effects (Corbetta et al., 2000).

During each study session, participants viewed a black screen containing two white boxes (subtending 5° horizontal and vertical angles) appearing to the right and left of a white central fixation crosshair (Fig. 1). The nearest edges of the boxes subtended a 1° horizontal angle from central fixation. The beginning of each trial was indicated by the appearance of a green arrow cue, which replaced the central fixation for 1 s. The arrow pointed to the left or right, cuing subjects to covertly shift their attention (without moving their eyes) to the corresponding box. After a variable cue-to-stimulus interval (CSI)—1, 3, or 5 s—an object appeared for 500 ms in one of the two boxes. On 82% of trials, the object appeared in the cued box (valid trials), while on the remaining trials it appeared in the noncued box (invalid trials). Upon appearance of the object, participants were to indicate whether the stimulus represented a real object or belonged to the artificial object class of greebles (index and middle finger key press, respectively); the response hand was counterbalanced across subjects. Speed and accuracy were given equal emphasis in the task instructions. A variable intertrial interval (ITI) of 1.5, 3.5, or 5.5 s separated the offset of the object stimulus from the onset of the following cue. The variable CSIs and ITIs were pseudorandomly distributed across trials such that the regressors in the general linear model (GLM) (see fMRI data analysis) that estimated the neural responses to cues and objects were minimally correlated (<0.13), thus allowing activity elicited by each phase in the trial (cue and object) to be independently assessed. Stimuli were presented in pseudorandom order, with no more than three trials of one item type (objects/greebles) occurring consecutively. Stimuli were projected onto a mirror mounted on the MRI headcoil.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Experimental design. Left, During the study phase, subjects were scanned while incidentally encoding a series of visually presented objects and greebles in a variant of the Posner cueing paradigm. Items were preceded by an arrow cue, pointing to a box on the left or right side of the screen. Subjects were to covertly shift attention to the cued box, in anticipation of an upcoming item. Items appeared after a variable duration (see Materials and Methods, Behavioral task) in either the cued location (validly cued trial) or the uncued location (invalidly cued trial). Subjects made a button press to indicate they had identified a real object or a greeble, regardless of whether the item appeared in the validly or invalidly cued location. Right, Following the study phase, subjects were administered a surprise memory test for the real objects. Subjects made one of four memory responses indicating whether they recognized the item from the previous study phase (old) or whether the object was unstudied (new), as well as their level of confidence in their decision (sure or unsure). If an old response was given, subjects were cued to make a source judgment for the location in which the item was studied (left or right).

Immediately following the final study session, participants were transferred from the scanner to a neighboring testing suite and received instructions for the surprise memory test. The test list comprised 350 studied (old) and 180 unstudied (new) common objects, centrally presented, individually, in pseudorandom order. For each test object, participants were to indicate whether or not they had encountered the item in any of the study sessions, and to indicate their level of confidence. One of four responses (sure old, unsure old, unsure new, sure new) was made with the index or middle fingers of each hand. Old and new responses were made using separate hands, with high and low confidence indicated by middle and index fingers, respectively. The mapping of hands to old or new responses was counterbalanced across participants.

Each test trial began when the white fixation crosshair changed to red for 500 ms (Fig. 1), after which the test object was presented and remained onscreen until a response was made or until 4 s elapsed (if no response was made). If the object was judged to be new, the test advanced to the next trial. If the object was judged to be old, it remained onscreen for up to 4 more seconds, during which time participants attempted to recollect the location at which the object had appeared during the study (cued with the appearance of a “Left?”, “Right?” prompt) (Fig. 1). Participants made a left or right index finger button press to indicate memory for the object appearing on the left or right side of the screen, respectively, and made their best guess when uncertain. A 1.5 s ITI (displaying a white fixation crosshair) separated test trials. To mitigate fatigue, participants were given a self-paced break after each third of the test.

fMRI data acquisition.

Whole-brain imaging was performed on a 3 T Signa MR scanner (GE Medical Systems). Anatomical images were collected using a high-resolution T1-weighted spoiled gradient recalled (SPGR) pulse sequence (130 slices; 1.5 mm thick; 256 × 256 matrix; 0.86 mm2 in-plane resolution). Functional images were obtained from 30 4-mm-thick axial slices, aligned to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure plane and positioned to give full coverage of the cerebrum and most of the cerebellum, using a T2*-weighted, two-dimensional gradient-echo spiral-in/out pulse sequence (TR = 2 s; TE = 30 ms; flip angle = 75°; 64 × 64 matrix; 3.44 mm2 in-plane resolution). Data were acquired in 10 sessions, comprised of 190 volumes each. Volumes within sessions were acquired continuously in a descending sequential order. The first four volumes were discarded to allow tissue magnetization to achieve a steady state.

fMRI data analysis.

Data preprocessing and statistical analyses were performed with Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM5, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK: http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm5/), implemented in MATLAB 7.7 (Mathworks). For each participant, all volumes were realigned spatially to the first volume, and then to the across-run mean volume. All volumes were corrected for differences in acquisition times between slices (temporally realigned to the acquisition of the middle slice). The anatomical volume was coregistered to the mean functional volume, and then a unified segmentation procedure (Ashburner and Friston, 2005) was applied to segment the anatomical volume into gray matter, white matter, and CSF. The segmented images were deformed to probabilistic maps of each tissue type in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space, and the resulting deformation parameters were applied to the functional images for normalization. Functional images were resampled into 3 mm3 voxels using nonlinear basis functions (Ashburner and Friston, 1999). Functional images were concatenated across sessions. Normalized functional images were smoothed with an isotropic 8 mm full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel.

Univariate analyses.

Statistical analyses were performed in two stages of a mixed-effects model. In the first stage, neural activity was modeled by a delta function (impulse event) at the onset of each cue and each stimulus. These functions were convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF) and its temporal and dispersion derivatives (Friston et al., 1998) to yield regressors in a GLM that modeled the BOLD response to each event type. The two derivatives modeled variance in latency and duration, respectively. Analyses of the parameter estimates pertaining to the dispersion derivative of cue-related effects are reported below (parameter estimates pertaining to the derivatives of other items contributed no theoretically meaningful information beyond that contributed by the canonical HRF, and thus are not reported).

The time series were high-pass filtered to 1/128 Hz to remove low-frequency noise and scaled to a grand mean of 100 across both voxels and scans. As described below, parameter estimates for events of interest were estimated using one of two GLMs. Nonsphericity of the error covariance was accommodated by an AR(1) model, in which the temporal autocorrelation was estimated by pooling over suprathreshold voxels (Friston et al., 2002). The parameters for each covariate and the hyperparameters governing the error covariance were estimated using restricted maximum likelihood. Effects of interest were tested using linear contrasts of the parameter estimates. These contrasts were carried forward to a second stage in which subjects were treated as a random effect. Unless otherwise specified, whole-brain analyses were used when no strong regional a priori hypothesis was possible; in these cases, only effects surviving an uncorrected threshold of p < 0.001 and including four or more contiguous voxels were interpreted. When we held an a priori hypothesis about the localization of a predicted effect, we corrected for multiple comparisons by using a small-volume correction employing an FWE rate based on the theory of random Gaussian fields (Worsley et al., 1996). The peak voxels of clusters exhibiting reliable effects are reported in MNI coordinates.

Regions of overlap between the outcomes of two contrasts were identified by inclusively (or “conjunctively”) masking the relevant SPMs. When two contrasts are independent, the statistical significance of the resulting SPM can be computed using Fisher's method of estimating the conjoint significance of independent tests (Fisher, 1950; Lazar et al., 2002). For each inclusive masking procedure, the constituent contrasts were determined to be independent with tests of orthogonality for linear contrasts. This test concludes that two linear contrasts are statistically independent if the sum of the products of the coefficients is equal to zero (i.e., for contrasts A and B: if a1b1 + a2b2 + a3b3 + a4b4 = 0, then contrasts are orthogonal). The goal of inclusive masking in the present study was to search within regions that exhibited one pattern of activity to identify whether any voxels also showed a second pattern. As such, we maintained the original threshold of the contrast identifying the first pattern (p < 0.001), using a threshold of p < 0.05 for the masking contrast, to give a conjoint significance of p < 0.0005. Exclusive (or “disjunctive”) masking was used to identify voxels where effects were not shared between two contrasts. The SPM constituting the exclusive mask was thresholded at p < 0.10, whereas the contrast to be masked was thresholded at p < 0.001. Note that the more liberal the threshold of an exclusive mask, the more conservative is the masking procedure.

To identify the relationship between attention effects and encoding effects, we orthogonalized the factors in our design: (1) top-down attention effects were identified by interrogating cue-related activity for all items, collapsing over subsequent memory status, and bottom-up attention effects were identified by comparing validly cued versus invalidly cued items; and (2) positive and negative subsequent memory effects were identified by comparing subsequently remembered versus forgotten items. To accommodate these analyses, two GLMs were estimated. The first was optimized to model stimulus-related effects (i.e., subsequent memory effects and bottom-up attention effects). Study trials were segregated according to subsequent memory [high confidence hits (HCHs), low confidence hits, and misses (Ms)] and cue validity (valid, invalid), resulting in six object-related regressors. Two additional regressors modeled greebles (validly and invalidly cued). Objects for which memory was not later tested were modeled as events of no interest, as were objects for which a response was omitted at test. Nine additional regressors modeled the cue-related activity associated with each of the aforementioned stimulus types. Finally, six regressors modeled movement-related variance (three rigid-body translations and three rotations determined from the realignment stage), and session-specific constant terms modeled the mean over scans in each session.

The second GLM implemented a parametric modulation analysis designed to detect top-down attention effects. Previous Posner cueing studies (Corbetta et al., 2000) identified top-down attention effects by isolating activity elicited by cues to shift covert attention. To separately estimate neural responses associated with cues versus stimuli, prior studies used “catch trials” in which no stimulus appeared postcue (Corbetta et al., 2000). Here we opted to decorrelate cue- and stimulus-related activity by parametrically varying the CSI (1, 3, or 5 s). Furthermore, because cue-related activity could reflect not only top-down attention effects but also low-level visual responses to the cue itself, we used a parametric modulation analysis to distinguish activity transiently responding to the cue from that responding across the duration the variable CSIs. In other words, by isolating BOLD responses that were elicited by the cue to shift attention and that were also sustained throughout the variable interval over which attention was to be maintained, we could rule out the possibility that any cue-related effects were simply a reflection of low-level visual responses. To accomplish this, we included an orthogonal variable in the GLM to isolate top-down effects, identifying cue-related activity that increased with increasing CSI duration. It should be noted that because the HRF effectively integrates the total activity over a period of a few seconds, parametric modulators identify activity that is modulated in duration or magnitude (or both). As we discuss below, the obtained data suggest that the parametric modulator captured variance in duration of the cue-related HRF. In sum, for this second GLM, one regressor modeled all cue-related activity, and a second regressor parametrically modulated the first according to the CSI following each cue. Thus, the second regressor for each subject in this GLM was of equal length to the first regressor and comprised a vector of values where each value represented the duration of the CSI for the corresponding cue. This regressor identified voxels in which cue-related activity varied as a linear function of the duration over which attention was to be maintained. To accommodate the remainder of the known variance, a third regressor modeled all stimulus-related activity (not segregated according to subsequent memory or validity), and movement and session effects were modeled as described above.

An alternative method of interrogating sustained BOLD responses across the CSI is to analyze effects associated with the dispersion derivative of the cue-related HRF. In the present design, cue-related attention effects (but not cue-related visual effects) might show a more sustained response, which would be captured by significant loading on the dispersion derivative. Accordingly, below we report the cue-related dispersion derivative outcome, which confirmed the parametric modulator outcome. However, we note that a dispersion derivative analysis is less optimal than a parametric modulation analysis for our design for two reasons. First, a dispersion derivative analysis can only accommodate variance in the duration of the HRF up to ∼1 s, and here the CSI varied from 1, 3, or 5 s. Second, the differing durations of the CSI are suboptimally modeled by a dispersion derivative, which is fit across all trial types (i.e., 1, 3, and 5 s CSIs). This trial-by-trial variability is better modeled by a parametric modulation analysis (Henson, 2007).

Response profiles of regions of interest (ROIs) identified in mapwise analyses were investigated using the deconvolution algorithm implemented in MarsBar (marsbar.sourceforge.net). This algorithm deconvolves the BOLD signal in the ROI using a finite impulse response function, which assumes no shape for the hemodynamic response. From these analyses, the clusterwise integrated percentage signal change was extracted for further characterization of the functional response (see Results for details).

Connectivity analyses.

The second main goal of the present study was to determine whether the functional dynamics of the putative dorsal and ventral attention networks changed when an event memory was effectively formed relative to when one was not. An understanding of how these attention networks dynamically interact with other neural structures during the encoding of event information may inform how episodic memories are created. We therefore sought to determine whether neural components of the dorsal and ventral attention networks showed connectivity profiles that predicted later memory success or failure.

Multivariate connectivity analyses were conducted by submitting seed regions—ROIs identified in the univariate analyses as exhibiting top-down or bottom-up attention effects (see next paragraph)—to psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analyses to determine whether they showed memory-related functional connectivity with other regions in the brain (Friston et al., 1997). In this manner, we investigated whether the connectivity between attention-related regions and other brain regions differed as a function of encoding success or failure.

Seed regions were components of PPC that exhibited relevant attention effects: a left dPPC [medial intraparietal sulcus (mIPS), extending into superior parietal lobule (SPL)] region that displayed top-down attention effects, and bilateral vPPC (TPJ) regions that displayed bottom-up attention effects (for details, see Results, Neural correlates of top-down and bottom-up attention subsection). Using standard PPI analysis techniques, seed clusters were individually defined for each subject on the basis of the random-effects group analyses. For each subject, the data for each seed region was the principal eigenvariate of all significant (p < 0.05) voxels within a 4 mm sphere, centered on the local peak maximum that fell within 2× FWHM of the smoothing kernel (i.e., 16 mm) and was within the anatomical region of interest, identified from each subject's normalized structural scan. These subject-specific timeseries represented the physiological component of the PPI. All but one of the 18 subjects met all criteria for every seed region; PPI analyses were performed on these 17 subjects. The timeseries were adjusted for variance associated with effects of no interest, and then a deconvolution with the hemodynamic responses was performed. The resultant vector was weighted by a contrast vector representing the relevant psychological factor (in this case, a subsequent memory contrast: HCH vs M), and then reconvolved with the hemodynamic responses. The outcome of this process formed the “psychophysiological interaction,” or PPI regressor. This regressor models the between-condition difference in regression slopes between each voxel in the brain and the seed region. This PPI regressor was entered into a GLM, along with regressors modeling the main effects of the psychological and physiological factors (i.e., the condition contrast vector and the timeseries, respectively). In line with the univariate GLMs, we additionally modeled movement-related effects as well as session-specific constant terms. Because PPI analyses are inherently less powered than their univariate counterparts (as only the unshared variance among the three regressors is attributed to the interaction term, or the PPI regressor), we adopted a whole-brain uncorrected threshold of p < 0.005, four voxel extent. Voxels that surpassed threshold in these PPI analyses can be interpreted as showing a significant difference in connectivity with the attention-related seed region as a function of later memory outcome.

Results

Behavioral performance

Study task

Participants performed at ceiling on the object/greeble discrimination task for items that were cued validly and invalidly (0.98, SE = 0.0002/0.99, SE = 0.0002, respectively). All but two of 20 participants showed evidence of attentional reorienting, as indexed by longer response times (RTs) to items appearing in the invalidly cued relative to the validly cued location; as noted above, data from the two subjects failing to show this behavioral reorienting effect were omitted from all analyses. Data from the 18 participants submitted to analysis revealed that the reorienting effect (all items: F(1,17) = 21.00, p < 0.001) (Table 1) was significant for both common objects (t(17) = 5.97, p < 0.001) and greebles (t(17) = 2.91, p < 0.01). The CSI between the orienting cue and the onset of the object did not impact the reorienting effect, as evidenced by the absence of an interaction between CSI and validity (F < 1).

Table 1.

Response times to critical study items (objects that were later tested for memory) as a function of study CSI and subsequent memory

According to study CSI
According to later memory
1 s CSI 3 s CSI 5 s CSI Across CSIs HCH LCH M
Validly cued items (ms) 734 (43) 714 (41) 708 (42) 719 (42) 715 (40) 713 (44) 721 (42)
Invalidly cued items (ms) 881 (49) 851 (47) 842 (40) 859 (46) 873 (55) 830 (52) 864 (45)

Values are given as the means of the median response times (SEs) to critical study items. LCH, Low-confidence hits.

Memory task

Overall memory performance.

Recognition memory was estimated by calculating the difference in the probabilities of an old response to an old vs a new item [discrimination measure, Pr = (pOld | Old) − (pOld | New)]. Recognition memory performance was superior when participants were highly confident in their old/new decision (Prhi conf = 0.30, SE = 0.03) relative to when a low-confidence response was given (Prlow conf = 0.07, SE = 0.01) (Prhi conf vs Prlow conf, t(17) = 5.75, p < 0.001). Analyses of d′, excluding three participants who had zero high-confidence false alarms, resulted in the same pattern as the Pr analyses: high-confidence d′ = 1.25, SE = 0.11 vs low-confidence d′ = 0.29, SE = 0.08.

For studied items that were confidently endorsed as old, participants were well above chance (0.5) when indicating the location in which the object was studied (“source memory” accuracy: mean = 0.71, SE = 0.03; t(17) = 6.54, p < 0.001). This was not the case, however, when low-confidence judgments were given (mean = 0.52, SE = 0.02; t(17) < 1). Thus, because both recognition and source memory performance were poor for low-confidence responses, the fMRI subsequent memory analyses focused on comparison of HCHs versus Ms.

Valid versus invalid memory performance.

The need to reorient attention to invalidly cued objects at study negatively impacted subsequent memory for those objects (Table 2). Specifically, invalidly cued objects were later confidently recognized less often than were validly cued objects (t(17) = 5.18, p < 0.0005). However, when invalidly cued objects were subsequently recognized with high confidence, source memory performance was equivalent to that for validly cued objects (t(17) < 1). This finding suggests that the following neuroimaging comparisons of valid and invalid subsequent memory effects (HCH vs M) were not biased in favor of conditions associated with superior source memory. Such a bias would be introduced, however, if subsequent memory analyses were expanded to include all recognized items (rather than restricted to HCHs), as source memory was better for all hits in the valid versus invalid conditions (t(17) = 1.9, p < 0.04). This finding of biased source memory for all hits, but unbiased source memory for high-confidence hits, reinforces the restriction of subsequent memory analyses to high-confidence hits. Finally, study task RTs conditionalized as a function of later memory performance revealed that study RTs did not differ as a function of subsequent memory (Table 1) (valid HCH vs M: t(17) < 1; invalid HCH vs M: t(17) < 1). This finding rules out the possibility that the neural subsequent memory effects were simply a consequence of the amount of time spent initially processing study items (i.e., differential “duty cycles”).

Table 2.

Recognition and source memory performance with source memory conditionalized on recognition memory

Recognition memory (proportion of old or new items)
Source memory (proportion correct)
High confidence
Low confidence
Hits False alarms Hits False alarms High confidence Low confidence
Validly cued items 0.37 (0.04) 0.04 (0.01) 0.19 (0.02) 0.13 (0.02) 0.72 (0.04) 0.52 (0.03)
Invalidly cued items 0.26 (0.04) 0.21 (0.02) 0.69 (0.04) 0.50 (0.03)

Values are given as mean (SE).

Neuroimaging findings

The present experiment was designed to directly assess the degree to which top-down and bottom-up attention mechanisms contribute to the formation of event memories. Our analysis strategy first considered the factors of attention and encoding separately, and then examined the relationship between them by (1) investigating regional overlap between attention and encoding effects, and (2) investigating connectivity effects.

Neural correlates of top-down and bottom-up attention

We first examined whether our attention paradigm gave rise to patterns of activity similar to those observed in prior Posner cueing studies (which primarily used detection tasks with repeated simple shapes, whereas our paradigm included a discrimination task and trial-unique meaningful objects). Based on prior studies, top-down attention effects were predicted (1) to be elicited by the arrow cues to shift attention and (2) to vary in duration according to the interval over which attention was maintained (the variable CSI) (see Experimental procedures). Supporting these predictions, a parametric modulation analysis revealed that cue-related activity (cue > fixation) varied positively according to CSI in the frontal eye fields (FEFs), the left mIPS, extending into SPL (Fig. 2; Table 3). These findings are consistent with an extensive literature suggesting that these frontoparietal regions form a dorsal attention network (Corbetta et al., 1993; Nobre et al., 1997; Kastner et al., 1999; Hopfinger et al., 2000; Sylvester et al., 2007; for review, see Corbetta et al., 2008).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Top-down and bottom-up attention effects. Top, Regions that exhibit cue-related activity that was modulated according to the duration over which attention was to be maintained (green; top-down attention effects); regions that exhibit greater activity in response to objects that appeared in an unexpected versus expected location (red; bottom-up attention effects). Regional overlap of the two effects appears in yellow. Effects are thresholded at p < 0.001 on a standardized brain (PALS-B12 atlas using Caret5: http://brainvis.wustl.edu/wiki/index.php/Caret:About). Bottom, The observed hemodynamic responses are plotted for relevant clusters (data pooled across activated voxels falling within a 6-mm-radius sphere centered on the peak voxel in the cluster; note that top-down attention effects were elicited by cues, and bottom-up attention effects by objects). L IPS/SPL, Left IPS/SPL; L TPJ, left TPJ; R TPJ, right TPJ.

Table 3.

Parietal and frontal regions exhibiting top-down and bottom-up attention effects, subsets of which demonstrate overlap with subsequent memory effects

∼BA MNI coordinates
Peak Z (no. voxels) SMEs
x y z
Top-down attention Overlap with Positive SMEs
    R FEF 6/4 39 −3 63 4.04 (172) 39 −3 63
        R FEF 4/6 27 −9 57 3.35
        R FEF 4 30 −3 66 3.45 30 −3 66
        R precentral gyrus 4 39 −6 51 4.03 45 0 57
        R SEF/CEF 6 48 0 48 4.02 48 0 48
    L FEF 6/4 −33 −6 60 5.17 (480) −30 −3 45
        L FEF 4 −15 −9 63 3.37
        L precentral gyrus 4 −45 −6 45 4.87 −45 −6 45
        L precentral gyrus 4 −36 −18 51 3.71
        L SEF/CEF 6 −6 6 54 4.28 −3 3 57
    L mIPS/SPL 7 −30 −48 57 3.89 (125)
        L mIPS/SPL 7 −27 −60 57 3.75 −27 −57 51
        L mIPS 7 −30 −42 48 3.74
        L mIPS 7 −36 −30 39 3.20
Bottom-up attention Overlap with Negative SMEs
    L IFS 46 −30 30 24 3.57 (37)
        L IFS 46 −36 24 36 3.47
        L IFG 46 −42 30 30 3.48
    R IFG 44 54 21 27 3.28 (4)
    R anterior insula 16 48 21 −3 3.97 (82)
        R anterior insula 16 42 27 −6 3.92
    L anterior insula 16 −36 18 −3 3.57 (13)
    L SEF 6/8 −9 9 54 3.29 (4)
    R FEF 6 45 3 48 4.79 (306)
        R FEF 6 30 3 54 4.23
        R SEF 6 12 9 60 4.37
        R MFG 44/6 36 9 33 3.55
    L FEF 6 −24 0 54 4.70 (96)
    R TPJ 40 63 −36 36 4.93 (597) 57 −39 36
        R TPJ 40 54 −39 30 4.72 51 −39 33
        R TPJ 40 63 −42 24 4.72 60 −45 30
        R TPJ 40 60 −51 15 4.45 60 −54 24
        R lateral IPS 7 45 −45 51 3.68 48 −42 51
        R mIPS 7 36 −51 42 4.25
        R precuneus/posterior cingulate 7/31 18 −54 42 3.55
    L lateral IPS 7 −30 −45 39 4.98 (590)
        L lateral IPS 7 −27 −69 36 4.49
        L TPJ 40 −48 −51 39 4.23 −48 −51 39
        L TPJ 40 −57 −57 24 3.63 −57 −57 24
        L precuneus 7 −12 −57 51 4.36 −9 −60 45
    R precuneus/posterior cingulate 7/31 15 −66 54 3.84 (82)
        R precuneus/posterior cingulate 7/31 9 −63 42 3.62 9 −63 42
        R precuneus/posterior cingulate 7/31 12 −54 54 3.37

Given the large spatial extent of the clusters, peaks and subpeaks listed; coordinates with associated cluster size denote cluster peak, and subpeaks are contiguous local maxima >8 mm apart. Clusters that also exhibit subsequent memory effects (i.e., survived the conjunction analysis between attention and memory contrasts) are denoted in the last columns (peak coordinates identified from the conjunction analysis listed). Z values refer to the peak or subpeak of the listed cluster. L, Left; R, right; SEF, supplementary eye field; CEF, cingulate eye field; IFS, inferior frontal sulcus; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; MFG, medial frontal gyrus; SME, subsequent memory effect.

Confirming the outcome of the parametric modulation analysis, analysis of nonmodulated cue effects (i.e., the canonical cue-related HRF) revealed the same set of regions (bilateral FEF and left mIPS/SPL). Notably, this contrast also identified an additional set of regions in visual cortex (bilateral visual cortex, centered on [12, −99, 6] and [−18, −93, 6]; z = 4.17), consistent with the idea that the nonmodulated effects reflect a combination of low-level visual and high-level attention-related processes. Finally, to determine which nonmodulated effects exhibited a sustained response, we inclusively masked the nonmodulated effects (p < 0.001) with the dispersion derivative contrast (p < 0.05). The outcome of this procedure revealed 34 voxels in mIPS/SPL (centered on [−24, −57, 54]; z = 3.66) that showed a more sustained response than the canonical cue-related HRF, further confirming the findings from the parametric modulation analysis.

To identify neural correlates of bottom-up attention, objects appearing in unexpected locations were contrasted with those appearing in expected locations (i.e., invalidly > validly cued objects). Multiple regions were more active when the object was invalidly cued, including bilateral TPJ (Fig. 2; Table 3). This pattern is consistent with the proposal that TPJ is a key component of a “ventral attention network” that mediates stimulus-driven reorienting of attention (Corbetta et al., 2008). Additional regions revealed in this contrast included FEF and IPS (Fig. 2; Table 3), a finding consistent with the hypothesis that stimulus-driven reorienting of attention triggers recruitment of the dorsal attention network (Corbetta et al., 2002, 2008; Giessing et al., 2006; Shulman et al., 2009). That is, effects revealed by this contrast may reflect the consequences of a stimulus-driven salience calculation (mediated in part by TPJ), which in turn serves to drive shifts in the locus of visuospatial attention (partially mediated by an FEF-IPS/SPL network) (Burrows and Moore, 2009; Shulman et al., 2009).

It is notable that these “validity effects” appear to overlap with the CSI-varying cue-related effects in medial—but not lateral—IPS (Fig. 2, yellow). This apparent dissociation would extend recent evidence suggesting that lateral and medial IPS functionally differ (Nelson et al., 2010), with mIPS differentially tracking demands on top-down attention (Hutchinson et al., 2009; Sestieri et al., 2010; Uncapher et al., 2010). Here, mIPS was engaged during the components of the Posner task that are thought to recruit top-down attention (most directly evidenced by the parametrically modulated cue-related contrast, and indirectly evidenced by the validity contrast, for reasons described in the previous paragraph). Interestingly, this mIPS/SPL region appears to anatomically overlap with that revealed in a recent meta-analysis of the top-down attention literature (compare Fig. 2, present mIPS/SPL top-down effects, yellow + green, Uncapher et al., 2010, their Fig. 1, top-down attention Activation Likelihood Estimation map).

Importantly, the apparent selectivity of the present top-down effects to mIPS/SPL, not including lateral IPS, was confirmed by a disjunction analysis (i.e., identification of regions that exhibit significant effects in one contrast but not in the other). To identify regions that exhibited validity effects but not CSI-varying cue-related activity, we masked the validity contrast (p < 0.001) with the parametrically modulated cue-related contrast (thresholded at a lenient level, p < 0.10, to create a stringent mask). Importantly, left lateral IPS survived this disjunction analysis, indicating that it exhibited validity effects but no evidence of cue-related activity. We also performed the reverse disjunction analysis, masking the cue-related contrast (p < 0.001) with the validity contrast (thresholded at a lenient level, p < 0.10). This procedure did not reveal a disjunction in left mIPS/SPL (nor in FEF), suggesting that medial (and not lateral) parietal subregions are engaged during both the cue-related and validity contrasts (again, this finding is compatible with the view that top-down attention mechanisms can be triggered by stimulus-driven salience that drives the reorienting of attention). Together, these findings support the hypothesis that lateral and medial IPS functionally differ, with mIPS/SPL differentially tracking demands on top-down attention.

Neural correlates of episodic encoding

Prior studies identifying the neural correlates of encoding have contrasted study items that were subsequently remembered versus forgotten (Brewer et al., 1998; Wagner et al., 1998; Henson et al., 1999) (for review, see Wagner et al., 1999; Paller and Wagner, 2002; Spaniol et al., 2009). Accordingly, we first analyzed stimulus-related activity on trials most analogous to prior subsequent memory experiments, namely, trials on which the object appeared in an expected location (validly cued trials), and was later remembered with high confidence (HCH) versus later forgotten (M). Consistent with the literature on MTL- and PPC-encoding effects (for respective review, see Davachi, 2006; Uncapher and Wagner, 2009), this contrast revealed positive subsequent memory effects (HCH > M) in multiple regions, including bilateral hippocampus and right posterior IPS (Fig. 3; Table 4). Also evident were bilateral clusters that encompassed fusiform and lateral occipital (LO) cortex (Fig. 3); these ventral temporal-occipital foci appear to include the LO complex, which is implicated in visual object representation (Malach et al., 1995; Grill-Spector et al., 2001; Grill-Spector and Malach, 2004). In support of this interpretation, an analysis of common objects vs greebles identified a large swath of activity overlapping these fusiform and LO subsequent memory effects (available from the corresponding author upon request). Additionally, a positive subsequent memory effect was evident in left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), a region consistently associated with episodic encoding success for verbalizable and/or meaningful stimuli (Kirchhoff et al., 2000; Baker et al., 2001; Davachi et al., 2001; Otten and Rugg, 2001b; Clark and Wagner, 2003). [Note that subsequent memory effects were computed by comparing all HCHs (i.e., collapsed across source memory accuracy) to misses; to identify effects of source memory, we compared HCHs with and without source memory in the subset of participants (n = 15) with five or more trials in each condition. No region demonstrated greater encoding activity for HCHs with source memory at standard statistical thresholds (p < 0.001), whereas activation in right TPJ was greater during HCHs without source memory. Because these source memory analyses were underpowered, however, interpretative caution is warranted.]

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Subsequent memory effects. Left, Greater activity in response to validly cued objects that were later confidently remembered relative to forgotten (positive subsequent memory effects; warm colors) was observed in bilateral hippocampus, left VLPFC, right posterior IPS, and bilateral LO/fusiform. By contrast, right TPJ exhibited greater activity in response to validly cued objects that were later forgotten relative to confidently remembered (negative subsequent memory effects; cool colors). Hippocampal effects are displayed on mean across-subject anatomical images; all other effects are surface rendered on a standardized brain. Display thresholds are p < 0.001. Right, Observed hemodynamic responses to objects are plotted for relevant clusters (data pooled across all voxels in the cluster). L Hip, Left hippocampus; R Hip, right hippocampus; R pIPS, right posterior IPS; R TPJ, right TPJ; R LO/Fus, right lateral occipital and fusiform cortex; L VLPFC, left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; L LO/Fus, left lateral occipital and fusiform cortex.

Table 4.

Brain regions associated with encoding success (positive subsequent memory effects) or failure (negative subsequent memory effects) for items presented in expected locations (valid trials) or unexpected locations (invalid trials)

∼BA MNI coordinates
Peak Z (no. voxels)
x y z
Valid trials
    Positive subsequent memory effects
        L IFG 45 −39 24 15 3.29 (4)
        L IFG/inferior frontal junction 44/6 −42 0 27 4.14 (93)
        R posterior/medial SFG 6/4 6 −18 57 3.43 (4)
        L putamen −33 0 −9 3.69 (8)
        R putamen 27 −6 −12 3.57 (28)
        L posterior hippocampus −24 −33 −6 3.60 (19)
        R posterior hippocampus 33 −39 −6 3.71 (14)
        R fusiform 37 48 −54 −6 4.08 (55)
        L fusiform 37 −48 −57 −6 3.90 (97)
        R posterior IPS 7/19 33 −72 33 4.08 (18)
    Negative subsequent memory effects
        R medial superior rostral gyrus 10 15 60 15 4.23 (40)
        R anterior/medial SFG 9 15 42 42 3.38 (15)
        R TPJ 40/39 51 −48 39 3.99 (98)
        BL posterior cingulate 31 −6 −69 36 3.69 (36)
Invalid trials
    Positive subsequent memory effects
        R fusiform 37 39 −45 −15 3.40 (6)
    Negative subsequent memory effects
        R DLPFC 9 45 12 45 3.60 (6)
        R IPS/TPJ 7/40 36 −39 42 3.56 (52)
        R SPL 5 24 −48 63 3.31 (4)
        R precuneus 7 12 −63 51 4.01 (7)
        R parietal-occipital sulcus 18/17 6 −78 39 3.26 (6)

Z values refer to the peak of the cluster (subpeaks available from the corresponding author upon request). L, Left; R, right; BL, bilateral; SFG, superior frontal gyrus; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

Positive subsequent memory effects are often accompanied by negative subsequent memory effects, or regions that show an enhanced response to items later forgotten relative to items later remembered. Consistent with prior reports (Otten and Rugg, 2001a; Wagner and Davachi, 2001; Daselaar et al., 2004; Gonsalves et al., 2004; Reynolds et al., 2004; Turk-Browne et al., 2006; Chua et al., 2007; Otten, 2007; Park and Rugg, 2008), negative subsequent memory effects (M > HCH) were observed in right vPPC, including TPJ, and in medial aspects of parietal and prefrontal cortex (Fig. 3; Table 4). These negative subsequent memory effects, as well as the preceding positive subsequent memory effects, were associated with object-related activity (analogous analyses of cue-related activity did not reveal significant correlates of subsequent memory at standard statistical thresholds) (Otten et al., 2006).

Overlap of attention and encoding effects

Stimulus-related signals that vary with subsequent memory may reflect a variety of processes. We therefore adopted a functional localizer logic, using the top-down attention contrast to constrain the process space being investigated. To do so, we looked within the regions engaged during top-down attention (operationalized as parametrically modulated cue-related activity) to assess whether stimulus period activity in these regions tracked subsequent memory. In other words, to determine whether the neural correlates of top-down attention overlapped with those of successful episodic encoding, we used a conjunction analysis between the regions identified by the parametrically modulated cue-related contrast and those exhibiting positive object-related subsequent memory effects. Specifically, the cue-related contrast (thresholded at p < 0.001) was inclusively masked with the positive subsequent memory contrast for validly cued objects (thresholded at p < 0.05). This conjunction analysis (at a conjoint threshold of p < 0.0005) revealed that the bilateral FEF and left mIPS/SPL regions that exhibited a top-down cueing effect were again engaged when objects appeared (postcue), and this engagement was predictive of subsequent memory (Fig. 4, green; Table 3, denoted in final column). [Note that additional analyses comparing HCHs with and without source memory failed to reveal a difference in encoding activation in left mIPS/SPL and FEF. Again, due to concerns about power, future studies are needed to determine whether left mIPS/SPL activation during encoding is specifically predictive of memory for item–location associations and/or is related to item memory.]

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Overlap of attention and encoding effects. Top, Regions exhibiting both top-down attention and positive subsequent memory effects for validly cued objects are displayed in green, and those showing both bottom-up attention and negative subsequent memory effects are displayed in blue. Data displayed at conjoint thresholds of p < 0.0005. Bottom, Observed hemodynamic responses are plotted for PPC clusters, according to subsequent memory, and validity condition at study (data pooled across all voxels in each cluster). L mIPS/SPL, Left mIPS/SPL; L TPJ, left TPJ; R TPJ, right TPJ; SME, subsequent memory effect; HCH valid, validly cued HCHs; M valid, validly cued Ms; HCH invalid, invalidly cued HCHs; M invalid, invalidly cued Ms.

Negative subsequent memory effects in TPJ have been posited to reflect the disruptive consequences of the bottom-up capture of attention during encoding, perhaps marking the diversion of attention by irrelevant event information (Otten and Rugg, 2001a; Wagner and Davachi, 2001; Uncapher and Wagner, 2009). Here, we explicitly tested this account by examining the relationship between the parietal structures engaged during the bottom-up capture of attention (i.e., greater activation to invalidly cued relative to validly cued objects) and those demonstrating negative subsequent memory effects for validly cued objects. Importantly, inclusive masking of the contrast identifying bottom-up attention effects (thresholded at p < 0.001) with that revealing negative subsequent memory effects (thresholded at p < 0.05) revealed overlap in bilateral TPJ (Fig. 4, blue; Table 3, denoted in final column). This pattern suggests that, at least for validly cued objects, greater TPJ activation may reflect the capture of bottom-up attention by irrelevant event features. Qualitative inspection of the observed hemodynamic responses in bilateral TPJ (Fig. 4) further indicated that TPJ responded maximally to items that appeared outside the current focus of attention (invalidly cued objects) and were later forgotten, and the least to items that appeared where expected (validly cued objects) and were later remembered. This observation of a negative subsequent memory effect in TPJ on invalidly cued trials—wherein bottom-up attention was presumably captured by the to-be-encoded object—was unexpected, as the capture of bottom-up attention by such objects was predicted a priori to foster their encoding. Accordingly, we next explored the subsequent memory effects on invalid trials in greater detail.

Encoding of objects outside the focus of attention

Prior subsequent memory studies have investigated encoding correlates for stimuli appearing in expected locations. In most cases, the spatial contingency was limited to one location (central fixation); in others, this contingency extended to multiple locations (Cansino et al., 2002; Sommer et al., 2005a,b; Uncapher et al., 2006; Uncapher and Rugg, 2009). To our knowledge, no study has examined whether encoding mechanisms are similar or different for stimuli presented outside versus inside the current focus of top-down attention. The present design—in which spatial expectations were probabilistically confirmed (validly cued objects) or violated (invalidly cued objects)—provided leverage on this question, though we note that this analysis is inherently underpowered due to the relatively lower frequency of invalidly cued objects (with an average of 62 trials contributing to subsequent memory analyses of invalid objects vs 214 for valid objects). (Note that the limited number of invalid trials precluded subsequent source memory analyses from being conducted.)

To complement the preceding subsequent memory analysis for validly cued objects, we first identified positive and negative subsequent memory effects for invalidly cued objects [i.e., (HCH > M)invalid and (M > HCH)invalid]. A positive subsequent memory effect for invalidly cued objects was observed in right fusiform (Fig. 5A; Table 4); the absence of other positive correlates of encoding at the standard statistical threshold (p < 0.001) is likely due to low power. A follow-up statistically independent analysis of the percentage signal change for this cluster further revealed a modest, but significant, positive subsequent memory effect for validly cued objects (t(17) = 2.34; p < 0.02). Strikingly, the negative subsequent memory analysis revealed unexpected effects in dPPC, including in right IPS and SPL (Fig. 5B; Table 4) (similar effects were observed in left IPS/SPL when the statistical threshold was relaxed to p < 0.005). This was surprising given the finding from a recent meta-analysis that all prior negative subsequent memory effects in parietal cortex have been reported in vPPC, with none in dPPC (Uncapher and Wagner, 2009). Even in the few studies that have directly manipulated goal-directed attention in a subsequent memory paradigm (Kensinger et al., 2003; Uncapher and Rugg, 2005, 2008, 2009), there was no dPPC activity reported to be associated with subsequent forgetting.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Subsequent memory effects for items outside the current focus of attention (invalid trials). A, A positive subsequent memory effect for invalidly cued items was observed in right fusiform. B, Negative subsequent memory effects for invalidly cued items (blue) were observed in ventral parietal regions, but also—strikingly—in dorsal parietal regions (see text). Regions that demonstrated a greater subsequent memory effect for objects appearing inside versus outside the focus of attention (valid vs invalid) are displayed in red. A subset of regions demonstrating this interaction also showed a negative subsequent memory effect for invalid items: right IPS and SPL (purple). C, To illustrate the nature of the interaction in right IPS and SPL, subsequent memory effects for valid and invalid conditions are plotted for each region (clusterwise mean parameter estimates and SEs shown for clusters identified from the interaction contrast). All display thresholds are p < 0.001. R Fus, Right fusiform cortex; R IPS, right IPS; R SPL, right SPL; L SPL, left SPL; SME, subsequent memory effect.

We next sought to identify regions where subsequent memory effects differed according to whether objects appeared in expected, relative to unexpected, locations. Voxel-level interaction analyses [i.e., (HCH > M)invalid > (HCH > M)valid, and vice versa] revealed that, while no regions showed positive encoding-related activity that was greater for invalid than valid trials (even when the threshold of the interaction was dropped to p < 0.01), several regions—including the right IPS and SPL regions previously identified—exhibited the opposite pattern (valid > invalid subsequent memory effects) (Fig. 5B; Table 5), including the left LO/fusiform region that showed a positive subsequent memory effect when processing validly cued objects (Fig. 3). Thus, stronger preparatory coupling between mIPS/SPL regions that mediate top-down attention and LO/fusiform regions that represent visual object form appears to facilitate encoding of subsequently encountered objects. Consistent with this interpretation, an across-subject regression revealed that subjects who showed stronger subsequent memory connectivity between mIPS/SPL and LO/fusiform (during the cue period) tended to demonstrate superior recognition memory at test (i.e., exhibited higher Pr for high-confidence judgments) relative to subjects who showed weaker connectivity effects (r(15) = 0.414, p < 0.05) (Fig. 6A).

Table 5.

Brain regions where subsequent memory effects differ according to whether items appear in expected versus unexpected locations

∼BA MNI coordinates
Peak Z (no. voxels)
x y z
Valid > invalid subsequent memory effects
    L anterior SFG 9 −18 45 27 3.24 (5)
    R putamen 27 −6 −6 3.65 (17)
    R central sulcus 3/4 51 −12 39 3.40 (12)
    L central sulcus 3/4 −48 −12 42 3.46 (26)
    R SPL 7 30 −39 63 4.25 (26)
    R IPS 7 42 −45 45 3.41 (8)
    R precuneus 7 12 −60 51 4.03 (16)
    L precuneus 7 −12 −66 51 3.23 (4)
Invalid > valid subsequent memory effects
Figure 6.

Figure 6.

Parietal attention seeds show connectivity that differs according to encoding success or failure. A, Regions whose connectivity with left mIPS/SPL (green sphere, peak mIPS/SPL coordinates of the top-down attention contrast are illustrated in Fig. 2) was stronger during preparatory periods of trials resulting in objects being remembered versus forgotten (positive subsequent connectivity effects; warm colors); regions showing the opposite effect, or stronger connectivity with left mIPS/SPL during preparatory periods leading to forgotten versus remembered objects (negative subsequent connectivity effects) are displayed in cool colors [note that the small cluster in dorsal AnG (coordinates 39, − 57, 33) is in fact spatially contiguous with the large ventral AnG cluster that has been surface rendered dorsally (for details, see Table 6)]. Scatter plot illustrates that individual differences in the strength of the positive subsequent connectivity effect in LO/fusiform during encoding correlates with across-subject differences in later memory performance, such that the stronger the mIPS/SPL–LO/fusiform connectivity, the superior the later memory performance. B, Regions whose connectivity with left or right TPJ (Fig. 2, red spheres; peak TPJ coordinates from the bottom-up attention contrast illustrated) was stronger during the presentation of objects that were later remembered versus forgotten, and vice versa (positive and negative subsequent connectivity effects). The coloring scheme is the same as that used in A. L mIPS/SPL, Left mIPS/SPL; L TPJ, left TPJ; R TPJ, right TPJ; L LO/Fus; left LO/fusiform; R LO/Fus, right LO/fusiform; R AnG, right AnG.

In addition to connectivity increases during the preparatory period that tracked later memory outcome, mIPS/SPL also exhibited connectivity decreases with other regions (i.e., showed weaker connectivity during trials where the object was later remembered vs forgotten). These regions included a large cluster in right angular gyrus (AnG) (Fig. 6A; Table 6). AnG is regarded as a key parietal component in the default-mode network, which is thought to be involved in internally oriented or self-related cognition (for review, see Buckner et al., 2008, Bressler and Menon, 2010). Thus, one interpretation of this greater mIPS/SPL–AnG connectivity during trials where objects are later forgotten versus remembered is that directing attention internally before the external presentation of a stimulus may be detrimental to the encoding of that information.

Table 6.

Regions where connectivity with attention-related seed regions differs according to later memory success or failure

∼BA MNI coordinates
Peak Z (no. voxels)
x y z
Top-down attention seed
    Left mIPS/SPL
        Greater connectivity during encoding success
            R medial thalamus 6 0 −6 2.84 (7)
            R superior temporal sulcus 22 57 −39 6 3.57 (17)
            L fusiform 37 −36 −51 −9 2.89 (5)
            L LO cortex 37 −51 −63 −6 3.06 (6)
            L LO cortex 37 −45 −78 −3 3.20 (33)
            BL medial occipital cortex 17 0 −96 9 2.87 (4)
        Greater connectivity during encoding failure
            R midbrain 3 −36 −27 2.65 (4)
            R AnG 39 42 −57 27 3.51 (84)
Bottom-up attention seeds
    Right TPJ
        Greater connectivity during encoding success
        Greater connectivity during encoding failure
            BL medial SFG 9 0 51 39 3.12 (17)
            L IFG 45/44 −51 24 15 2.83 (5)
            BL anterior cingulate 24 6 9 27 2.98 (13)
            BL medial thalamus 0 3 −9 3.00 (7)
            R parahippocampal gyrus 36 24 −33 −21 2.99 (8)
            L parahippocampal gyrus/fusiform 36/37 −24 −36 −21 3.16 (21)
            R retrosplenial 29 21 −51 9 3.07 (26)
            L retrosplenial 29 −15 −51 12 3.33 (47)
            BL retrosplenial 29 0 −63 12 2.81 (6)
            R LO cortex 19 45 −63 −9 3.11 (15)
    Left TPJ
        Greater connectivity during encoding success
            L putamen −24 9 12 3.01 (4)
            R precentral gyrus 4 60 −3 42 2.94 (30)
            L retrosplenial 29 −9 −39 21 2.78 (4)
            R posterior cingulate 31 6 −63 36 2.90 (9)
        Greater connectivity during encoding failure
            L DLPFC 46 −39 33 9 2.84 (6)
            BL medial thalamus −3 −3 −9 2.79 (5)
            R fusiform 37 27 −39 −24 3.21 (22)
            L fusiform 37 −30 −42 −21 3.06 (19)
            L parahippocampal gyrus 36 −27 −48 −6 2.90 (8)
            R parahippocampal gyrus 36 36 −51 −9 3.41 (15)
            L LO cortex 19 −39 −60 −6 2.73 (5)
            L LO cortex 19 −39 −75 −3 2.92 (6)
            L parietal-occipital sulcus 18/17 −12 −81 24 3.06 (4)

DLPFC, Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

Bottom-up attention seeds.

We next investigated whether the bilateral TPJ regions exhibiting bottom-up attention effects (during the stimulus-processing period) showed connectivity that differed according to later memory. We therefore looked for subsequent memory connectivity effects using the right and left TPJ regions as seeds. We found no regions that showed positive subsequent memory connectivity effects with the right TPJ seed and only a few that did so with the left TPJ seed (Table 6). By contrast, the TPJ seeds showed robust negative subsequent memory connectivity effects with a broad set of regions (Table 6). Strikingly, the LO/fusiform region that showed positive connectivity effects with the mIPS/SPL seed showed the opposite pattern of connectivity with TPJ (Fig. 6B). The TPJ seeds also showed negative subsequent memory connectivity effects with bilateral regions encompassing parahippocampal and fusiform cortices (Fig. 6B); parahippocampal cortex has been associated with the spatial encoding of objects (for review, see Eichenbaum and Lipton, 2008). Thus, successful object encoding appears to be associated with reduced coupling between TPJ regions (implicated in reflexive orienting) and LO/fusiform regions (implicated in visual object representation), as well as with parahippocampal regions (implicated in the encoding of objects in space).

Eye movements

Eye movements were not monitored in the scanner, raising the possibility that the observed subsequent memory effects could, in theory, reflect differential BOLD responses associated with eye movement patterns (e.g., objects fixated might be associated with more effective encoding and thus a higher likelihood of being subsequently remembered). While the absence of eye-tracking data precludes a definitive assessment of this possibility, we believe it unlikely for a number of reasons. First, it is unlikely that extensive eye movements occurred during scanning, because participants were able to maintain accurate fixation during the behavioral training session, and an extensive literature suggests that participants tend not to saccade when trained to covertly orient (Posner and Cohen, 1980). Second, if eye movements did occur, to the extent that differences in fixating the to-be-remembered object account for differences in subsequent memory outcome, one might predict that RTs during the object discrimination task would be faster for objects in fixation relative to those peripherally viewed. However, RTs were comparable during the encoding of later remembered and later forgotten stimuli (see Behavioral performance) (Table 1). Third, subsequent memory effects have been consistently observed in IPS across studies in which all stimuli fall at the center of the visual field (for review, see Uncapher and Wagner, 2009). Under such conditions, subjects learn that the center of the visual field is task/goal-relevant, and it is to their advantage to fixate this region to effectively perceive and make decisions about presented stimuli. Fourth, even under conditions where stimuli are presented rapidly in the center of the visual field, making eye movements suboptimal and therefore unlikely, subsequent memory effects are observed in IPS. For instance, Otten and Rugg (2001b) identified subsequent memory effects in IPS/SPL for items presented for 300 ms, with no RT differences between items that were later remembered versus forgotten.

Finally, we note that studies that have systematically mapped the effects of eye movements or overt shifts of attention have consistently identified bilateral parietal activity, corresponding to saccades to contralateral visual hemifields (for review, see Silver and Kastner, 2009). Our critical analysis (overlap of top-down attention effects and subsequent memory effects) identified unilateral activity in left IPS (Fig. 4). This lateralization of effects was not due to thresholding, as the effects remained left lateralized when the threshold of the attention effects was reduced from p < 0.001 to a liberal value of 0.01. Given that the items were presented with equal probability to the two sides of the screen, it seems unlikely that fluctuations in the magnitude of the unilateral IPS effects reflect eye movements to both sides of the screen. We would note that this pattern of left-lateralized IPS top-down attention effects has also been found in prior Posner studies, where eye movements were monitored during scanning and found to be negligible (Doricchi et al., 2010).

Summary of fMRI findings

The univariate data demonstrated a relationship between neural correlates of attention and episodic encoding of objects: top-down attention effects and encoding success effects overlapped in dPPC (mIPS/SPL), and bottom-up attention effects and encoding failure effects overlapped in vPPC (TPJ). The deployment of top-down attention interacted with stimulus location to either promote memory (if the stimulus appeared in the cued location) or hinder memory (if it appeared in the noncued location). The multivariate connectivity findings suggest that episodic encoding of objects is influenced by parietal interactions with regions representing visual object information (LO/fusiform), with a positive influence from top-down attention-related regions (mIPS/SPL) and a negative influence from bottom-up attention-related regions (TPJ).

Discussion

Based on prior observations of encoding success effects localized predominantly to dPPC and encoding failure effects localized exclusively to vPPC, we previously speculated that top-down and bottom-up attention may have distinct influences on event encoding (Uncapher and Wagner, 2009). The present study directly tested this dual-attention encoding hypothesis by manipulating top-down and bottom-up attention during an incidental encoding paradigm. Three main findings advance understanding of how attention regulates memory formation. First, for objects appearing in expected locations, dPPC regions engaged during the controlled allocation of visuospatial attention were positively correlated with episodic memory formation, whereas vPPC regions engaged during stimulus-driven attentional capture were negatively correlated with memory formation. Second, we provide novel evidence that the deployment of top-down attention is not always beneficial for encoding, as the dorsal attention network was also associated with encoding failure when objects appeared outside the focus of attention. Finally, connectivity analyses revealed that, during the formation of memories for objects, top-down and bottom-up attention appear to have opposite influences on perceptual cortical areas that subserve visual object representation, suggesting that one manner in which attention modulates memory is by altering the perceptual processing of to-be-encoded stimuli.

Top-down attention and episodic encoding

We propose that the observed overlap between neural correlates of top-down attention and encoding success in dPPC is indicative of the extent to which the deployment of top-down attention supports memory encoding. In other words, the overlap of effects may reflect engagement of top-down attention mechanisms during the study task, which in turn increases the probability that attended information will progress through the cortical hierarchy to converge on MTL mechanisms for encoding into memory.

Top-down attention can enhance the firing rate of neurons representing goal-relevant elements of an experience (Desimone and Duncan, 1995; McAdams and Maunsell, 1999; Treue and Martinez-Trujillo, 1999; Boynton, 2005) (for review, see Treue, 2001), and attention is thought to influence between-region communication by altering oscillatory coupling (Saalmann et al., 2007; Gregoriou et al., 2009a), preparing regions to receive input at the most excitable phase of their oscillatory activity (Gregoriou et al., 2009b). The hippocampus, by virtue of its apical position in the neural processing hierarchy (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991), is proposed to be the recipient of cortically processed event features (for review, see Eichenbaum et al., 2007), and cortical–hippocampal coupling is associated with successful episodic encoding (Fell et al., 2001). By facilitating the cortical representations of goal-relevant stimuli, top-down attention—partially subserved by an FEF–mIPS/SPL network—could foster the propagation of higher-fidelity representations to the hippocampus, resulting in a higher probability of stimulus encoding into memory (“biased input hypothesis”) (Uncapher and Rugg, 2009).

The present data lend support for this perspective. First, activation in mIPS/SPL exhibited a top-down attention effect during the preparatory period and predicted later memory success during the object processing period. Second, activity in this mIPS/SPL region predicted response profiles in object-sensitive regions of ventral temporo-occipital cortex, namely LO and fusiform. Importantly, this functional coupling was stronger during preparatory periods of trials for which the subsequently presented object was later remembered versus later forgotten, and the strength of this mIPS/SPL–LO/fusiform coupling difference correlated with across-subject differences in later memory performance. Finally, activity in bilateral fusiform, LO, and hippocampus was greater during the viewing of objects that would be later remembered versus forgotten. Collectively, these findings are consistent with a role of top-down attention in promoting encoding by (1) preparing object representation processes in LO/fusiform for incoming information, with a stronger drive promoting better encoding, and (2) fostering effective propagation of these putatively higher-fidelity visual representations to the hippocampus. Future studies using intracranial electrocorticography (Jacobs and Kahana, 2010), with independent electrodes in LO/fusiform and in hippocampus, will provide a means to test the role of attention-mediated oscillatory coupling in visual object encoding into episodic memory.

Bottom-up attention and episodic encoding

The dual-attention encoding hypothesis posits that memory failure can result when attention is captured by event information that is not the target of a subsequent retrieval attempt. That is, if attention is shifted away from information that will be the target of subsequent retrieval, encoding will be hindered for this information. Here, the overlap between bottom-up attention and negative subsequent memory effects for validly cued objects in bilateral TPJ lends support for the proposal that attention capture can hinder event encoding. Factors that engage the ventral attention network are debated, but at least include an unexpected or salience dimension (Posner and Cohen, 1984; Jonides and Yantis, 1988). One possibility is that attention was captured by nonexperimental variables (e.g., unexpected change in scanner noise, urge to move, or internally oriented cognition) or stimulus-related variables (e.g., oddly shaped or shaded object), consistent with data indicating that TPJ is engaged by not only spatial but also nonspatial (feature-based) information (Linden et al., 1999; Marois et al., 2000; Braver et al., 2001; Kiehl et al., 2001; Serences et al., 2005). Thus, while future studies are needed to gain leverage on the nature of the attention-capturing information leading to subsequent forgetting of target objects, the present data provide evidence that a reorienting mechanism in TPJ is directly associated with later forgetting.

Intuitively, it is easy to understand that attentional capture of extrastimulus information would draw processing resources from the target stimulus, leading to poorer memory for the stimulus. What is less clear is what happens when the attention-capturing information is stimulus related. Recent studies reveal an equivocal pattern: while items that are distinctive in some way (and therefore attention capturing) often enjoy a mnemonic advantage (von Restorff, 1933), this is not always the case. Strange et al. (2000) reported an interaction of the “von Restorff effect” and depth of processing. Perceptually distinctive items were better remembered only if studied under “shallow” encoding conditions (where superficial aspects of the items were emphasized) (Fabiani et al., 1990), whereas emotionally distinctive items were always remembered better. Importantly, semantically distinctive items were remembered worse when attention was paid to that dimension during study. Summerfield and Mangels (2006) additionally showed that items appearing at unpredictable times (distinctive in the temporal dimension) were more poorly remembered than predictable items.

Here we examined the consequences on memory when items appeared in an unexpected location. Like the semantic and temporal dimensions, items that appeared in an unexpected location suffered a mnemonic disadvantage. As in previous studies, this may be due to an interaction between our encoding task and the attention-capturing dimension. If attention to the unexpected spatial location occurred at the expense of some other memory-promoting dimension, such as semantic elaboration, memory for the objects would suffer. Another explanatory factor may be the degree to which attention-capturing information is used as a retrieval cue. To the degree that spatial information is a poor retrieval cue, attention being captured by the spatial dimension at study may hinder later memory performance.

The present findings also advance understanding of how memories are formed for objects appearing outside the current focus of attention (i.e., invalidly cued objects). Our data revealed that, rather than recruiting a new set of mechanisms, the mechanisms associated with encoding objects outside attentional focus—in fusiform cortex—are a subset of those engaged when objects are within attentional focus. Strikingly, presenting objects outside attentional focus also revealed a novel negative subsequent memory effect in the dorsal attention network (SPL). To date, activation in this top-down network has been exclusively associated with subsequent memory success, rather than failure (Uncapher and Wagner, 2009). This negative effect reversed to a positive effect when objects appeared in the expected location, suggesting that top-down biasing of attention interacts with expectation to either promote or hinder memory formation. Thus, previous findings that the top-down attention network exclusively promotes memory encoding may be due to a lack of expectation violations in prior studies. Collectively, the present findings highlight the role of perceptual cortices in visual object encoding, and the modulatory influence that attention has on activity in these regions.

Summary

The present data provide strong evidence that top-down and bottom-up attention mechanisms in parietal cortex influence episodic encoding, with mIPS/SPL-mediated top-down attention generally serving to promote memory formation, and TPJ-mediated bottom-up attention serving to hinder memory formation (at least within the present experimental context). Here we offer a mechanistic explanation for how to reconcile effects of attention that may appear inconsistent across the literature (Uncapher and Wagner, 2009); namely, that the presence of positive or negative subsequent memory effects in dorsal or ventral PPC can be predicted based on whether one exerts experimental control over the focus of attention. These findings of direct overlap between attention and episodic encoding appear to stand in contrast to the pattern observed during episodic retrieval, where the overlap between parietal attention and memory effects is a topic of current debate (Cabeza et al., 2008; Ciaramelli et al., 2008; Hutchinson et al., 2009; Uncapher et al., 2010). As such, the present data highlight the importance of attention during event processing for later remembering (Craik et al., 1996).

Footnotes

This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants 5R01-MH080309 and F32-MH084475.

References

  1. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Nonlinear spatial normalization using basis functions. Hum Brain Mapp. 1999;7:254–266. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)7:4&#x0003c;254::AID-HBM4&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-G. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage. 2005;26:839–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baker JT, Sanders AL, Maccotta L, Buckner RL. Neural correlates of verbal memory encoding during semantic and structural processing tasks. Neuroreport. 2001;12:1251–1256. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200105080-00039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beck DM, Kastner S. Top-down and bottom-up mechanisms in biasing competition in the human brain. Vis Res. 2009;49:1154–1165. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.07.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blumenfeld RS, Ranganath C. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex promotes long-term memory formation through its role in working memory organization. J Neurosci. 2006;26:916–925. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2353-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Boynton GM. Attention and visual perception. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005;15:465–469. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Braver TS, Barch DM, Gray JR, Molfese DL, Snyder A. Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11:825–836. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.9.825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bressler SL, Menon V. Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14:277–290. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brewer JB, Zhao Z, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD. Making memories: brain activity that predicts how well visual experience will be remembered. Science. 1998;281:1185–1187. doi: 10.1126/science.281.5380.1185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Burrows BE, Moore T. Influence and limitations of popout in the selection of salient visual stimuli by area V4 neurons. J Neurosci. 2009;29:15169–15177. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3710-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cabeza R, Ciaramelli E, Olson IR, Moscovitch M. The parietal cortex and episodic memory: an attentional account. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:613–625. doi: 10.1038/nrn2459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cansino S, Maquet P, Dolan RJ, Rugg MD. Brain activity underlying encoding and retrieval of source memory. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12:1048–1056. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.10.1048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chua EF, Schacter DL, Rand-Giovannetti E, Sperling RA. Evidence for a specific role of the anterior hippocampal region in successful associative encoding. Hippocampus. 2007;17:1071–1080. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Ciaramelli E, Grady CL, Moscovitch M. Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: a hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:1828–1851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Clark D, Wagner AD. Assembling and encoding word representations: fMRI subsequent memory effects implicate a role for phonological control. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:304–317. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00163-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Dobmeyer S, Shulman GL, Petersen SE. Attentional modulation of neural processing of shape, color, and velocity in humans. Science. 1990;248:1556–1559. doi: 10.1126/science.2360050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Shulman GL, Petersen SE. A PET study of visuospatial attention. J Neurosci. 1993;13:1202–1226. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-03-01202.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Corbetta M, Kincade JM, Ollinger JM, McAvoy MP, Shulman GL. Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:292–297. doi: 10.1038/73009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Corbetta M, Kincade JM, Shulman GL. Neural systems for visual orienting and their relationships to spatial working memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14:508–523. doi: 10.1162/089892902317362029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL. The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron. 2008;58:306–324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Craik FIM, Lockhart RS. Levels of processing: a framework for memory research. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav. 1972;11:671–684. [Google Scholar]
  24. Craik FIM, Govoni R, Naveh-Benjamin M, Anderson ND. The effects of divided attention on encoding and retrieval processes in human memory. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1996;125:159–180. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.125.2.159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Daselaar SM, Prince SE, Cabeza R. When less means more: deactivations during encoding that predict subsequent memory. Neuroimage. 2004;23:921–927. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Davachi L. Item, context and relational episodic encoding in humans. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2006;16:693–700. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Davachi L, Maril A, Wagner AD. When keeping in mind supports later bringing to mind: neural markers of phonological rehearsal predict subsequent remembering. J Cogn Neurosci. 2001;13:1059–1070. doi: 10.1162/089892901753294356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. de Fockert J, Rees G, Frith C, Lavie N. Neural correlates of attentional capture in visual search. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16:751–759. doi: 10.1162/089892904970762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1995;18:193–222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Doricchi F, Macci E, Silvetti M, Macaluso E. Neural correlates of the spatial and expectancy components of endogenous and stimulus-driven orienting of attention in the Posner task. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20:1574–1585. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Downar J, Crawley AP, Mikulis DJ, Davis KD. The effect of task relevance on the cortical response to changes in visual and auditory stimuli: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2001;14:1256–1267. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Eichenbaum H, Lipton PA. Towards a functional organization of the medial temporal lobe memory system: role of the parahippocampal and medial entorhinal cortical areas. Hippocampus. 2008;18:1314–1324. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C. The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30:123–152. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Fabiani M, Karis D, Donchin E. Effects of mnemonic strategy manipulation in a Von Restorff paradigm. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1990;75:22–35. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(90)90149-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Fell J, Klaver P, Lehnertz K, Grunwald T, Schaller C, Elger CE, Fernández G. Human memory formation is accompanied by rhinal-hippocampal coupling and decoupling. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:1259–1264. doi: 10.1038/nn759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC. Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex. 1991;1:1–47. doi: 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fisher R. London: Oliver and Boyd; 1950. Statistical methods for research workers. [Google Scholar]
  38. Friston KJ, Buechel C, Fink GR, Morris J, Rolls E, Dolan RJ. Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage. 1997;6:218–229. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Friston KJ, Fletcher P, Josephs O, Holmes A, Rugg MD, Turner R. Event-related fMRI: characterizing differential responses. Neuroimage. 1998;7:30–40. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Friston KJ, Glaser DE, Henson RN, Kiebel S, Phillips C, Ashburner J. Classical and Bayesian inference in neuroimaging: applications. Neuroimage. 2002;16:484–512. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gauthier I, Tarr MJ. Becoming a “Greeble” expert: exploring mechanisms for face recognition. Vis Res. 1997;37:1673–1682. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(96)00286-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Giessing C, Thiel CM, Rösler F, Fink GR. The modulatory effects of nicotine on parietal cortex activity in a cued target detection task depend on cue reliability. Neuroscience. 2006;137:853–864. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Gonsalves B, Reber PJ, Gitelman DR, Parrish TB, Mesulam MM, Paller KA. Neural evidence that vivid imagining can lead to false remembering. Psychol Sci. 2004;15:655–660. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00736.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H, Desimone R. High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science. 2009a;324:1207–1210. doi: 10.1126/science.1171402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H, Desimone R. Long-range neural coupling through synchronization with attention. Prog Brain Res. 2009b;176:35–45. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17603-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Grill-Spector K, Malach R. The human visual cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004;27:649–677. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Grill-Spector K, Kourtzi Z, Kanwisher N. The lateral occipital complex and its role in object recognition. Vis Res. 2001;41:1409–1422. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(01)00073-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Henson R. Efficient experimental design for fMRI. In: Friston K, Ashburner J, Kiebel S, Nichols T, Penny W, editors. Statistical parametric mapping: the analysis of brain images. Elsevier: London; 2007. pp. 193–210. [Google Scholar]
  49. Henson RN, Rugg MD, Shallice T, Josephs O, Dolan RJ. Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 1999;19:3962–3972. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-03962.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hopfinger JB, Buonocore MH, Mangun GR. The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:284–291. doi: 10.1038/72999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hutchinson JB, Uncapher MR, Wagner AD. Posterior parietal cortex and episodic retrieval: convergent and divergent effects of attention and memory. Learn Mem. 2009;16:343–356. doi: 10.1101/lm.919109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Ikkai A, Curtis CE. Cortical activity time locked to the shift and maintenance of spatial attention. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:1384–1394. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Indovina I, Macaluso E. Dissociation of stimulus relevance and saliency factors during shifts of visuospatial attention. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:1701–1711. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Jacobs J, Kahana MJ. Direct brain recordings fuel advances in cognitive electrophysiology. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14:162–171. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Jonides J, Yantis S. Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention. Percept Psychophys. 1988;43:346–354. doi: 10.3758/bf03208805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kastner S, Ungerleider LG. Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:315–341. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kastner S, Pinsk MA, De Weerd P, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG. Increased activity in human visual cortex during directed attention in the absence of visual stimulation. Neuron. 1999;22:751–761. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80734-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kensinger EA, Clarke RJ, Corkin S. What neural correlates underlie successful encoding and retrieval? A functional magnetic resonance imaging study using a divided attention paradigm. J Neurosci. 2003;23:2407–2415. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-06-02407.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kiehl KA, Laurens KR, Duty TL, Forster BB, Liddle PF. Neural sources involved in auditory target detection and novelty processing: an event-related fMRI study. Psychophysiology. 2001;38:133–142. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Kincade JM, Abrams RA, Astafiev SV, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study of voluntary and stimulus-driven orienting of attention. J Neurosci. 2005;25:4593–4604. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0236-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kirchhoff BA, Wagner AD, Maril A, Stern CE. Prefrontal-temporal circuitry for episodic encoding and subsequent memory. J Neurosci. 2000;20:6173–6180. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-16-06173.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lazar NA, Luna B, Sweeney JA, Eddy WF. Combining brains: a survey of methods for statistical pooling of information. Neuroimage. 2002;16:538–550. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Linden DE, Prvulovic D, Formisano E, Völlinger M, Zanella FE, Goebel R, Dierks T. The functional neuroanatomy of target detection: an fMRI study of visual and auditory oddball tasks. Cereb Cortex. 1999;9:815–823. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.8.815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Malach R, Reppas JB, Benson RR, Kwong KK, Jiang H, Kennedy WA, Ledden PJ, Brady TJ, Rosen BR, Tootell RB. Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1995;92:8135–8139. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.18.8135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Marois R, Leung HC, Gore JC. A stimulus-driven approach to object identity and location processing in the human brain. Neuron. 2000;25:717–728. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81073-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. McAdams CJ, Maunsell JH. Effects of attention on orientation-tuning functions of single neurons in macaque cortical area V4. J Neurosci. 1999;19:431–441. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-01-00431.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Moscovitch M, Umilta C. Modularity and neuropsychology: modules and central processes in attention and memory. In: Schwartz MF, editor. Modular deficits in Alzheimer's disease. Cambridge, MA: MIT/Bradford; 1990. pp. 1–59. [Google Scholar]
  68. Nelson SM, Cohen AL, Power JD, Wig GS, Miezin FM, Wheeler ME, Velanova K, Donaldson DI, Phillips JS, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. A parcellation scheme for human left lateral parietal cortex. Neuron. 2010;67:156–170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Nobre AC, Sebestyen GN, Gitelman DR, Mesulam MM, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD. Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. Brain. 1997;120:515–533. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.3.515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Noudoost B, Chang MH, Steinmetz NA, Moore T. Top-down control of visual attention. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010;20:183–190. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Otten LJ. Fragments of a larger whole: retrieval cues constrain observed neural correlates of memory encoding. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:2030–2038. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Otten LJ, Rugg MD. When more means less: neural activity related to unsuccessful memory encoding. Curr Biol. 2001a;11:1528–1530. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(01)00454-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Otten LJ, Rugg MD. Task-dependency of the neural correlates of episodic encoding as measured by fMRI. Cereb Cortex. 2001b;11:1150–1160. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.12.1150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Otten LJ, Quayle AH, Akram S, Ditewig TA, Rugg MD. Brain activity before an event predicts later recollection. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:489–491. doi: 10.1038/nn1663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Paller KA, Wagner AD. Observing the transformation of experience into memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2002;6:93–102. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01845-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Park H, Rugg MD. The relationship between study processing and the effects of cue congruency at retrieval: fMRI support for transfer appropriate processing. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:868–875. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Posner MI, Cohen Y. Attention and the control of movements. In: Stelmach GE, Requin J, editors. Tutorials in motor behavior. New York: North-Holland; 1980. pp. 243–258. [Google Scholar]
  78. Posner MI, Cohen Y. Components of visual orienting. In: Bouma H, Bowhuis D, editors. Attention and performance X. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1984. pp. 531–556. [Google Scholar]
  79. Reynolds JR, Donaldson DI, Wagner AD, Braver TS. Item- and task-level processes in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: positive and negative correlates of encoding. Neuroimage. 2004;21:1472–1483. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.10.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Rugg MD, Otten LJ, Henson RN. The neural basis of episodic memory: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2002;357:1097–1110. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Saalmann YB, Pigarev IN, Vidyasagar TR. Neural mechanisms of visual attention: how top-down feedback highlights relevant locations. Science. 2007;316:1612–1615. doi: 10.1126/science.1139140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Serences JT, Shomstein S, Leber AB, Golay X, Egeth HE, Yantis S. Coordination of voluntary and stimulus-driven attentional control in human cortex. Psychol Sci. 2005;16:114–122. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00791.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Sestieri C, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Attention to memory and the environment: functional specialization and dynamic competition in human posterior parietal cortex. J Neurosci. 2010;30:8445–8456. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4719-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Shulman GL, Astafiev SV, Franke D, Pope DL, Snyder AZ, McAvoy MP, Corbetta M. Interaction of stimulus-driven reorienting and expectation in ventral and dorsal frontoparietal and basal ganglia-cortical networks. J Neurosci. 2009;29:4392–4407. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5609-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Silver MA, Kastner S. Topographic maps in human frontal and parietal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13:488–495. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Sommer T, Rose M, Gläscher J, Wolbers T, Büchel C. Dissociable contributions within the medial temporal lobe to encoding of object-location associations. Learn Mem. 2005a;12:343–351. doi: 10.1101/lm.90405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Sommer T, Rose M, Weiller C, Büchel C. Contributions of occipital parietal and parahippocampal cortex to encoding of object-location associations. Neuropsychologia. 2005b;43:732–743. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Spaniol J, Davidson PS, Kim AS, Han H, Moscovitch M, Grady CL. Event-related fMRI studies of episodic encoding and retrieval: meta-analyses using activation likelihood estimation. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:1765–1779. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Strange BA, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Dolan RJ. Brain mechanisms for detecting perceptual semantic and emotional deviance. Neuroimage. 2000;12:425–433. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Summerfield C, Mangels JA. Dissociable neural mechanisms for encoding predictable and unpredictable events. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:1120–1132. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Sylvester CM, Shulman GL, Jack AI, Corbetta M. Asymmetry of anticipatory activity in visual cortex predicts the locus of attention and perception. J Neurosci. 2007;27:14424–14433. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3759-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Szekely A, Jacobsen T, D'Amico S, Devescovi A, Andonova E, Herron D, Lu CC, Pechmann T, Pleh C, Wicha N, Federmeier K, Gerdjikova I, Gutierrez G, Hung D, Hsu J, Iyer G, Kohnert K, Mehotcheva T, Orozco-Figueroa A, Tzeng A, Tzeng O, Arevalo A, Vargha A, Butler AC, Buffington R, Bates E. A new on-line resource for psycholinguistic studies. J Mem Lang. 2004;51:247–250. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Treue S. Neural correlates of attention in primate visual cortex. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24:295–300. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01814-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Treue S, Martínez Trujillo JC. Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex. Nature. 1999;399:575–579. doi: 10.1038/21176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Turk-Browne NB, Yi DJ, Chun MM. Linking implicit and explicit memory: common encoding factors and shared representations. Neuron. 2006;49:917–927. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.01.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Uncapher MR, Rugg MD. Effects of divided attention on fMRI correlates of memory encoding. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005;17:1923–1935. doi: 10.1162/089892905775008616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Uncapher MR, Rugg MD. Fractionation of the component processes underlying successful episodic encoding: a combined fMRI and divided-attention study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2008;20:240–254. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Uncapher MR, Rugg MD. Selecting for memory? The influence of selective attention on the mnemonic binding of contextual information. J Neurosci. 2009;29:8270–8279. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1043-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Uncapher MR, Wagner AD. Posterior parietal cortex and episodic encoding: insights from fMRI subsequent memory effects and dual-attention theory. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2009;91:139–154. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Uncapher MR, Otten LJ, Rugg MD. Episodic encoding is more than the sum of its parts: an fMRI investigation of multifeatural contextual encoding. Neuron. 2006;52:547–556. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Uncapher MR, Hutchinson JB, Wagner AD. A roadmap to brain mapping: toward a functional map of human parietal cortex. Neuron. 2010;67:5–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.06.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. von Restorff H. Über die wirkung von bereichsbildungen im spurenfeld. Psychol Res. 1933;18:299–342. [Google Scholar]
  103. Vossel S, Thiel CM, Fink GR. Cue validity modulates the neural correlates of covert endogenous orienting of attention in parietal and frontal cortex. Neuroimage. 2006;32:1257–1264. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Wagner AD, Davachi L. Cognitive neuroscience: forgetting of things past. Curr Biol. 2001;11:R964–R967. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(01)00575-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Wagner AD, Schacter DL, Rotte M, Koutstaal W, Maril A, Dale AM, Rosen BR, Buckner RL. Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science. 1998;281:1188–1191. doi: 10.1126/science.281.5380.1188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Wagner AD, Koutstaal W, Schacter DL. When encoding yields remembering: insights from event-related neuroimaging. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1999;354:1307–1324. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1999.0481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Worsley KJ, Marrett S, Neelin P, Vandal AC, Friston KJ, Evans AC. A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation. Hum Brain Mapp. 1996;4:58–73. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1996)4:1<58::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-O. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES