Abstract
Grid cells in entorhinal cortex of freely-moving rats provide a strikingly periodic representation of self-location1 which is suggestive of very specific computational mechanisms2-4. However, their existence in humans and distribution throughout the brain are unknown. Here we show that the preferred firing directions of directionally-modulated grid cells in rat entorhinal cortex are aligned with the grids, and that the spatial organization of grid cell firing is more strongly apparent at faster than slower running speeds. Since the grids are also aligned with each other1,5, we predicted a macroscopic signal visible to functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in humans. We then looked for this signal as participants explored a virtual reality environment, mimicking the rats’ foraging task: fMRI activation and adaptation showing a speed-modulated 6-fold rotational symmetry in running direction. The signal was found in a network of entorhinal/subicular, posterior and medial parietal, lateral temporal and medial prefrontal areas. The effect was strongest in right entorhinal cortex, and the coherence of the directional signal across entorhinal cortex correlated with spatial memory performance. Our study illustrates the potential power of combining single unit electrophysiology with fMRI in systems neuroscience. Our results provide the first evidence for grid-cell-like representations in humans, and implicate a specific type of neural representation in a network of regions which support spatial cognition and also, intriguingly, autobiographical memory.
Grid cells recorded in medial entorhinal cortex of freely moving rodents fire whenever the animal traverses the vertices of an equilateral triangular grid covering the environment (see Fig. 1a), and may provide a neural substrate for path integration1-3,5,6. However, it is not known whether or not grid cells exist in humans, or how widespread the network of neurons with grid-like firing is, although the pre- and para-subiculum7 and posterior parietal cortex8 have been implicated.
Could grid cell firing be detected in the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) signal, which reflects changes in metabolic activity across thousands of individual neurons9? The grid patterns of neighbouring cells are offset so as to ‘tile’ the environment1, making systematic variation in population activity with position unlikely. However, three factors may allow detection using fMRI. (i) The angular orientations of the grids relative to the environment appears to be constant across cells, whether they are neighbouring1 or farther apart5 (Supplementary Fig. 1), and rotate coherently when salient distal cues are rotated1,6. The other two factors result from new analyses presented here. (ii) The firing of ‘conjunctive’ grid cells, found in the deeper layers of entorhinal cortex6 and in pre- and parasubiculum7, is modulated by running direction6. Here we show that the directions of modulation of conjunctive grid cells are aligned with the main axes of the grids (Fig. 1a-c). These first two factors will create systematic differences in neural population dynamics for runs aligned or misaligned with the main axes of the grids (Fig. 2). (iii) A third factor is running speed: the rate6 and inter-burst frequency10 of grid cell firing and the frequency with which individual firing fields in a grid are sampled increase with running speed. In addition, we show that the spatial organisation of grid cell firing is more strongly apparent during fast running than during slow running and immobility (Fig. 1d; see Methods for details and Supplementary Figs. 2-6 for further analyses and recording locations). Thus entorhinal grid cells form a coherent population in which the common effects of orientation and speed of movement could produce a macroscopic signal visible with fMRI.
We recorded whole brain fMRI data while human participants navigated within a virtual reality (VR) arena, mimicking the foraging task in rodents, see Methods for details. The arena comprised a grassy plain bounded by a cylindrical cliff, containing a single intra-maze landmark and surrounded by a background scene providing orientation cues rendered at infinity (Fig. 2). Participants collected and replaced objects found within the arena. Memory was measured as the proximity of the replacement location to the correct location (Supplementary Fig. 7 and ref.11).
If orientationally-aligned grid cell firing is present, there should be an effect of running direction with 6-fold rotational symmetry, reflecting the differences between running aligned or misaligned to the grid axes, and this effect should be modulated by running speed (Fig. 2). We defined an anatomical region of interest (ROI) on the entorhinal cortices, and examined one half of the fMRI data for modulation by running direction and speed to find the orientation of potential grids within each participant’s entorhinal cortex. Briefly, we used a quadrature filter to identify the orientation of any 6-peaked sinusoidal modulation of signal as a function of running direction: providing the potential grid orientation in each voxel (Supplementary Fig. 8 and Methods). The potential grid orientations were significantly clustered across voxels in the ROI in 34/42 participants (Rayleigh test, accounting for spatial smoothing, on each participant; P<<0.00001 for the population, Monte Carlo simulation; Supplementary Fig. 9). The mean grid orientation for the entorhinal ROI (φ) was defined as the population vector (vector sum of orientations weighted by amplitude), and calculated for data corresponding to fast, medium and slow runs.
We then examined the second half of the data for differential activation for fast, medium and slow runs aligned versus misaligned to each participant’s mean grid orientation. We looked for sinusoidal modulation of activation with 6-fold rotational symmetry aligned with each participant’s mean grid orientation across the whole brain (using the regressor cos(6[θ(t)–φ]) where θ(t) is running direction; Supplementary Fig. 8). We found a significant activation in the right entorhinal cortex for fast runs (Fig. 3a-b). Direct comparison of activation for fast runs aligned versus misaligned to each participant’s mean grid orientation showed a similar effect (Fig. 3d). Similar sinusoidal modulations were present in left entorhinal cortex, although slightly less reliably than on the right, and in the mean response of the entorhinal ROI (Supplementary Fig. 10). No activation elsewhere in the brain reached the significance level for this whole-brain analysis. Equivalent analyses for medium or slow runs, or looking for 4- or 8-fold (Fig. 3b-c) or even 5- or 7-fold rotational symmetry (Supplementary Fig. 11) failed to show significant activation. No significant activation was found when the analyses applied to the entorhinal ROI were performed to control ROIs in the posterior right hippocampal location associated with memory performance in this task11 or the visual cortical location showing adaptation to running direction (below), see Supplementary Fig. 12. Supplementary figures 13-15 show control analyses of behaviour in rats and humans.
Consistent with grid cells, the mean grid orientations in the 6-fold symmetry model varied randomly between participants, ruling out a role for specific visual features of the environment (Supplementary Fig. 16). Interestingly, the coherence of the potential grid orientations in each participant’s right entorhinal cortex was significantly correlated with that participant’s spatial memory performance (Fig. 3f). This parallels findings that entorhinal cortex activation can predict memory performance12,13, and provides a first indication that grid-like representations might usefully guide behaviour in mammals.
An independent indicator of variation in the dynamics of neuronal activity is provided by fMRI adaptation14-17, i.e., reduced signal from activation of a representation that was recently active compared to a previously inactive one. During runs aligned with the grid, a smaller proportion of grid cells will fire but will do so at a higher rate, while more grid cells will be active during misaligned runs but at a lower rate (Supplementary Fig. 17). Given the complex non-linear dynamical relationship between neuronal activation and fMRI adaptation9 we predicted that differences in the dynamics of neuronal activity when running aligned to the grids compared to misaligned would cause differences in fMRI adaptation, and that these differences would also vary with running speed.
We first checked whether the more basic effect of fMRI adaptation to running direction occurred within our virtual navigation paradigm. We made separate adaptation regressors (the logarithm of the time since last running in the current direction) for fast, medium and slow runs. We found significant adaptation to running direction in visual cortex extending into posterior parahippocampal/lingual and retrosplenial cortices, with larger effects for fast than for slow runs, see Fig. 4a-b. The adaptation effect is independent of variations in angular velocity (see Methods) and is consistent with adaptation to the background visual scene (which is rendered at infinity; viewing direction is the same as running direction), as previously found with static scenes in similar areas14,16. It is also consistent with view-specific single unit responses in humans18 or with the presence of head-direction cells19 which have been reported in retrosplenial cortex20.
Given the robust fMRI adaptation to virtual running direction, we tested our specific prediction that adaptation in entorhinal cortex is modulated by running direction with 6-fold rotational symmetry and by running speed. Accordingly we looked for regions showing a reduction in fMRI signal according to how recently participants were running at 60° to the current direction which was greater for fast than for slow running. Unlike our analysis of whole brain activation as a function of mean grid orientation in the entorhinal ROI (Fig. 3), this analysis can be directly applied to the whole brain without reference to specific ROIs. As predicted, right entorhinal cortex (extending into subiculum) showed 60° shifted directional adaptation, independent of any effect of basic directional adaptation, as did regions in posterior parietal, lateral temporal and medial prefrontal cortices, see Fig. 4c. Medial parietal cortex also showed 60°shifted directional adaptation, in addition to the basic directional adaptation described above, consistent with the presence of both visual/head-direction and grid-like representations, see Supplementary Fig 18. The adaptation effect in all of these regions was strongly modulated by the participants’ virtual running speed, being higher for fast compared to slow runs (Fig. 4d) and the effect was specific to 60° rather than 45° or 90° shifted directional adaptation (Fig. 4e).
Do these remote regions showing 60° shifted directional adaptation also show the sinusoidal modulation of activation seen in entorhinal cortex during fast running, consistent with the presence of grid-like representations in these regions too? We examined the mean response to the sinusoidal regressor defined in entorhinal cortex in 8mm-radius ROIs centred on the locations of peak adaptation. The responses in medial prefrontal (one-tailed t-test; P=0.008), medial parietal (P=0.002) and lateral temporal (left, P=0.019; right, P=0.0005) ROIs, but not the posterior parietal ROI (P=0.058), reached significance for this specific prediction. Overall, these results suggest a network of regions containing coherently aligned neural representations with 6-fold rotational symmetry, although the responses in remote regions were weaker than in right entorhinal cortex (e.g. not reaching significance in the initial whole-brain split-half analysis), possibly reflecting a lower concentration of grid-like cells.
Our results provide the first evidence that human entorhinal cortex encodes virtual movement direction with 6-fold symmetry, consistent with a coherently-oriented population of grid cells similar to those found in rat entorhinal cortex1,5,6 and pre- and parasubiculum7. The dependence of directional modulation on running speed is consistent with the effects of speed on the firing rate6,10 and apparent spatial organisation (Fig. 1d) of grid cells, while effects of speed on other aspects of behaviour may also contribute (see e.g., Supplementary Fig. 14). The relationship to spatial memory of the directional coherence of potential grids across entorhinal cortex provides a first indication that grid-like representations might usefully guide behaviour.
Because we can only measure effects of direction and speed (not location) in the fMRI signal, our findings could reflect the presence of grid cells, or movement-related responses from head-direction19, or ‘conjunctive’ directional grid6,7 cells, if they form coherent populations whose firing has 6-fold rotational symmetry. We showed that conjunctive grid cells from rat entorhinal cortex have such an organisation (Fig. 1). Our finding of similar and aligned fMRI responses from subicular, posterior/medial parietal, lateral temporal, and medial prefrontal cortices suggest that populations with similar proporties also exist elsewhere, a prediction directly testable in rodents (see also refs.7,8). These results outline a circuit for navigation, consistent with suggestions that medial and lateral temporal, posterior and medial parietal and medial prefrontal areas cooperate to support spatial cognition2,8,11,21,22, and implicate a specific type of underlying neural representation.
Our study illustrates the ability to infer neural representations in humans by using fMRI in conjunction with single unit recording in behaving animals, promising a coherent understanding of behaviour at the neural and systems levels. The observed grid-like representations support spatial memory (Fig. 3f) and are found in a circuit of regions which strikingly overlaps the network for autobiographical memory and imagery, e.g. refs.21,23,24. These types of regularly repeating representation may provide a clue to the neural basis of autobiographical memory, perhaps encoding temporal as well as spatial context3,4,25-27 for combination with parallel networks representing non-spatial information25,28-30.
Methods Summary
Single unit recording experiment in rats
Single unit recordings were made from the medial entorhinal cortex of 17 male, Lister hooded rats, in accordance with appropriate Home Office Project and Personal licences. Each animal received a single microdrive carrying four tetrodes. While animals foraged for sweetened rice in a familiar environment, their positions and concomitant action potentials were captured using a standard procedure5. Grid cells were identified on the basis of their hexagonal spatial firing pattern, the regularity of which was assessed using a gridness measure1,5. Post-recording animals were sacrificed, perfused with phosphate buffered saline and then with 4% paraformaldehyde. Histology was conducted to confirm recording locations. See methods and ref.5 for details.
Functional neuroimaging experiment in humans
Forty-two male participants took part in this experiment (aged 18-31, mean age 23.1 years). The study was approved by the local Research Ethics Committee. Functional images were acquired on a 3T Siemens Allegra scanner using a gradient-echo EPI pulse sequence. Imaging data were analyzed with SPM5 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/), including standard preprocessing procedures (spatial realignment, correction for differences in slice acquisition time, spatial normalization and smoothing) and modelling the data by a set of general linear models. During the virtual reality task, participants were presented with a first-person perspective of a grassy plane surrounded by a circular cliff. Participants moved the viewpoint by using their right hand to operate keys to move forward and turn left or right. Throughout the experiment, participants collected and replaced everyday objects within the arena. See methods and ref.11 for details.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We gratefully acknowledge K. Jeffery and J. O’Keefe for providing help and facilities for single unit recording, the Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging for providing help and scanning facilities, J. Krupic and R. Hayman for help with single unit data collection, A. Jeewajee for help with analyses, J. King for help with VR programming, and useful discussions with P. Dayan, K. Friston, U. Frith, C. Hall, A. Jeewajee, J. O’Keefe and M. Witter. This work was funded by the UK Medical Research Council and the European Union (SpaceBrain grant).
Footnotes
Supplementary Information is linked to the online version of the paper at www.nature.com/nature.
Author Contributions C.F.D., C.B. and N.B. jointly conceived and designed the experiments. C.F.D. performed the fMRI experiment and data analyses; C.B. performed the single-unit experiment and data analyses; N.B. gave direction on analyses; all authors discussed the analyses and results and contributed to the paper.
Author Information Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints.
References
- 1.Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, Moser MB, Moser EI. Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature. 2005;436:801–806. doi: 10.1038/nature03721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.McNaughton BL, Battaglia FP, Jensen O, Moser EI, Moser MB. Path integration and the neural basis of the ‘cognitive map’. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2006;7:663–678. doi: 10.1038/nrn1932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Burgess N, Barry C, O’Keefe J. An oscillatory interference model of grid cell firing. Hippocampus. 2007;17:801–812. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hasselmo ME. A model of episodic memory: mental time travel along encoded trajectories using grid cells. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2009;92:559–573. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.07.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Barry C, Hayman R, Burgess N, Jeffery KJ. Experience-dependent rescaling of entorhinal grids. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:682–684. doi: 10.1038/nn1905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Sargolini F, et al. Conjunctive representation of position, direction, and velocity in entorhinal cortex. Science. 2006;312:758–762. doi: 10.1126/science.1125572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Boccara CN, et al. Grid cells in presubiculum and parasubiculum. FENS Abstr. 2008;vol.4 128.21. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Whitlock JR, Sutherland RJ, Witter MP, Moser MB, Moser EI. Navigating from hippocampus to parietal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2008;105:14755–14762. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0804216105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Logothetis NK. What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature. 2008;453:869–878. doi: 10.1038/nature06976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Jeewajee A, Barry C, O’Keefe J, Burgess N. Grid cells and theta as oscillatory interference: electrophysiological data from freely moving rats. Hippocampus. 2008;18:1175–1185. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Doeller CF, King JA, Burgess N. Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2008;105:5915–5920. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801489105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Fernandez G, Brewer JB, Zhao Z, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD. Level of sustained entorhinal activity at study correlates with subsequent cued-recall performance: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study with high acquisition rate. Hippocampus. 1999;9:35–44. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1999)9:1<35::AID-HIPO4>3.0.CO;2-Z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Olsen RK, et al. Performance-related sustained and anticipatory activity in human medial temporal lobe during delayed match-to-sample. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:11880–11890. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2245-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Epstein R, Graham KS, Downing PE. Viewpoint-specific scene representations in human parahippocampal cortex. Neuron. 2003;37:865–876. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00117-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Grill-Spector K, Henson R, Martin A. Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10:14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Park S, Intraub H, Yi DJ, Widders D, Chun MM. Beyond the edges of a view: boundary extension in human scene-selective visual cortex. Neuron. 2007;54:335–342. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Bakker A, Kirwan CB, Miller M, Stark CE. Pattern separation in the human hippocampal CA3 and dentate gyrus. Science. 2008;319:1640–1642. doi: 10.1126/science.1152882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ekstrom AD, et al. Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation. Nature. 2003;425:184–188. doi: 10.1038/nature01964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Taube JS. Head direction cells and the neuropsychological basis for a sense of direction. Prog. Neurobiol. 1998;55:225–256. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(98)00004-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Chen LL, Lin LH, Green EJ, Barnes CA, McNaughton BL. Head-direction cells in the rat posterior cortex. I. Anatomical distribution and behavioral modulation. Exp. Brain Res. 1994;101:8–23. doi: 10.1007/BF00243212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Byrne P, Becker S, Burgess N. Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery. Psychol Rev. 2007;114:340–375. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Wolbers T, Wiener JM, Mallot HA, Buchel C. Differential recruitment of the hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex, and the human motion complex during path integration in humans. J Neurosci. 2007;27:9408–9416. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2146-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Maguire EA. Neuroimaging studies of autobiographical event memory. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond B Biol. Sci. 2001;356:1441–1451. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2001.0944. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL. Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;8:657–661. doi: 10.1038/nrn2213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.O’Keefe J, Nadel L. The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford University Press; 1978. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Howard MW, Fotedar MS, Datey AV, Hasselmo ME. The Temporal Context Model in spatial navigation and relational learning: Toward a common explanation of medial temporal lobe function across domains. Psychol. Rev. 2005;112:75–116. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Pastalkova E, Itskov V, Amarasingham A, Buzsaki G. Internally generated cell assembly sequences in the rat hippocampus. Science. 2008;321:1322–1327. doi: 10.1126/science.1159775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Gaffan D. Scene-specific memory for objects: a model of episodic memory impairment in monkeys with fornix transection. J. Cogn Neurosci. 1994;6:305–320. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1994.6.4.305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Knierim JJ. Neural representations of location outside the hippocampus. Learn. Mem. 2006;13:405–415. doi: 10.1101/lm.224606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Eichenbaum H, Yonelinas AP, Ranganath C. The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;30:123–152. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.