Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Sep 15.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2007 Spring;19(2):497–521. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070241

Childhood Adversity and Youth Depression: Influence of Gender and Pubertal Status

Karen D Rudolph 1, Megan Flynn 1
PMCID: PMC3174013  NIHMSID: NIHMS314228  PMID: 17459181

Abstract

This research examined three possible models to explain how childhood social adversity and recent stress interact to predict depression in youth: stress-sensitization, stress-amplification, and stress-inoculation. Drawing from a stress-sensitization theory of depression, we hypothesized that exposure to childhood adversity, in the form of disruptions in critical interpersonal relationships, would lower youths’ threshold for depressive reactions to recent interpersonal stress. We expected that this pattern of stress sensitization would be most salient for girls negotiating the pubertal transition. These hypotheses were examined in two studies: a longitudinal, questionnaire-based investigation of 399 youth (M age = 11.66 years) and a concurrent, interview-based investigation of 147 youth (M age = 12.39 years). Findings supported the role of stress-sensitization processes in pubertal girls and prepubertal boys, and stress-amplification processes in prepubertal girls. Childhood social adversity specifically predicted sensitization to recent interpersonal, but not noninterpersonal, stress. These findings build on prior theory and research by suggesting that early adversity exerts context-specific effects that vary across gender and development. Future research will need to identify the specific mechanisms underlying this stress-sensitization process.

Keywords: childhood adversity, depression, gender, puberty

Childhood Adversity and Youth Depression: Influence of Gender and Pubertal Status

In light of abundant research attesting to the pivotal role of stress in the development of psychopathology (for a review, see Grant et al., 2003), developmental scientists have focused growing attention on the psychological and biological consequences of stress (Boyce et al., 1998; Cicchetti & Walker, 2001; Curtis & Cicchetti, 2003). Of particular concern is the potential long-term impact of stress experienced early in life on subsequent vulnerability to psychopathology (Boyce & Ellis, 2005; O’Connor, 2003). Notably, theory and research consistently implicate early adversity, particularly in the form of family disruption and parental loss, as a significant risk factor for depression (e.g., Brown & Harris, 1978; Daley, Hammen, & Rao, 2000; Goodyer & Altham, 1991; Kessler & Magee, 1993; Lizardi & Klein, 2000; for a review, see Goodman, 2002). However, minimal research explores how early adversity and recent stress jointly contribute to the onset and course of depression. Consistent with emerging models of early experience (Boyce & Ellis, 2005; O’Connor, 2003), the present research examined the hypothesis that exposure to childhood social adversity heightens youths’ reactivity to subsequent stress, resulting in depression.

Theoretical Models of the Consequences of Childhood Adversity

Three theoretical models might explain how childhood social adversity influences subsequent stress reactivity: stress-amplification, stress-sensitization, and stress-inoculation (see Figure 1). These models all take a diathesis-stress form, wherein the interaction between individual susceptibility (in this case, exposure to childhood adversity) and recent stressful experiences determines risk for psychopathology (Monroe & Simons, 1991; Sameroff, 1987).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Figure 1

Hypothetical diathesis-stress interactions depicting (a) stress amplification (an interaction in which childhood adversity creates a stronger risk for depression in individuals exposed to high levels of stress than those exposed to mild levels of stress, (b) stress sensitization (an interaction in which lower levels of stress are necessary to precipitate depression in individuals exposed to high than low levels of childhood adversity), and (c) stress inoculation (an interaction in which childhood adversity buffers youth from depression in the face of stress).

Stress-amplification model

According to a stress-amplification model, childhood adversity amplifies depressive reactions to recent stress – that is, youth with a history of adversity would demonstrate higher levels of depression than those without a history of adversity when exposed to severe but not mild recent stress (see Figure 1a). Youth without a history of adversity would be expected to demonstrate low levels of depression regardless of their exposure to recent stress. Considerable theory and research implicate the role of stress-amplification processes in the onset and course of depression in youth. For example, cognitive vulnerability-stress theories of depression propose that youth with maladaptive cognitions (e.g., negative inferential styles, dysfunctional cognitions) experience depression when exposed to high levels of stress (Abramson, Metalsky, & Alloy, 1989; Alloy, Kelly, Mineka, & Clements, 1990; Hankin & Abramson, 2001). Similarly, genetic vulnerability-stress theories propose that genetic liability to depression is expressed only in presence of heightened stress (Caspi et al., 2003; Eley et al., 2004). Consistent with these theories, a growing body of research supports the idea that youth with pre-existing vulnerabilities experience depression when exposed to high levels of recent stress (Abela, 2001; Eley et al., 2004; for a review, see Hankin & Abramson, 2001).

Stress-sensitization model

According to a stress-sensitization model (see Figure 1b), childhood adversity reduces an individual’s threshold for depressive reactions to recent stress – that is, youth with a history of adversity would require only mild stress to trigger depression, whereas youth without a history of adversity would require more severe stress to trigger depression. This stress-sensitization model is consistent with one of the pioneering diathesis-stress perspectives, which suggested that individuals with a pre-existing vulnerability to psychopathology might require lower levels of stress (e.g., challenges encountered in daily living) to trigger the onset of symptoms than those without a pre-existing vulnerability (Zubin & Spring, 1977).

Whereas a stress-amplification model has predominated in theory and research aimed at explaining the role of cognitive and genetic vulnerability to depression (Abela, 2001; Caspi et al., 2003; Eley et al., 2004; Hankin & Abramson, 2001; for exceptions, see Segal, Williams, Teasdale, & Gemar, 1996 regarding cognitive sensitization; Kendler, Thornton, & Gardner, 2001 regarding genetic sensitization), a stress-sensitization model has predominated in theory and research aimed at explaining the role of early life experience in depression vulnerability (Gold, Goodwin, & Chrousos, 1988; Hammen, Henry, & Daley, 2000; Harkness, Bruce, & Lumley, in press; Monroe & Harkness, 2005; Pine & Charney, 2002; Post, 1992; Post, Rubinow, & Ballenger, 1984). Specifically, stress-sensitization theory posits that stressful physiological and social experiences – either internal (e.g., prior symptoms of depression) or external (e.g., early life adversity) are encoded in the brain in the form of “memory traces” (Post, 1994, p. 31) that heighten reactivity of the stress-response system, such that lower levels of stress are necessary to trigger depression (Gold et al., 1988; Post, 1992).

Although few investigators have studied the interaction between early adversity and recent stress in the prediction of depression, two studies provide preliminary support for a stress-sensitization model. In one study, depressed adolescents who were exposed to parental abuse and neglect reported lower levels of stressful life events prior to the onset of their depression than depressed adolescents without a history of abuse (Harkness et al., in press). In a second study, young women who were exposed to childhood adversity (e.g., parental divorce, death of a parent, parent psychopathology) were more likely to become depressed following exposure to mild stress than women without a history of adversity (Hammen et al., 2000). These findings are consistent with the idea that youth who are exposed to prior adversity are reactive to lower levels of stress than those who are not exposed to adversity.

Stress-inoculation model

According to a stress-inoculation model (see Figure 1c), childhood adversity actually buffers youth from the adverse effects of recent stress – that is, youth with a history of adversity would demonstrate reduced stress reactivity. Specifically, this model suggests that prior stress exposure exerts a “steeling” effect (Chorpita & Barlow, 1998; Garmezy, 1986), such that youth are less reactive to subsequent stress. For example, exposure to adversity might provide youth with an opportunity to develop adaptive coping skills to deal with later stress. In this case, one would expect that youth who were not exposed to adversity would demonstrate higher levels of depression in the face of stress than those who were exposed to adversity. Some research supports the occurrence of stress-inoculation in the form of diminished biological stress reactivity in humans and animals exposed to adversity (for a review, see Boyce & Ellis, 2005).

Integration of the three models

Stress-amplification and stress-sensitization models are similar in their predictions that a history of adversity enhances stress reactivity; however, they differ in their predictions concerning the level of stress at which this stress reactivity is most salient. Specifically, a stress-amplification model suggest that heightened stress reactivity occurs primarily at severe levels of stress, thereby resulting in a bigger difference in depression between those with and without a history of adversity at severe than mild levels of stress. In contrast, a stress-sensitization model suggests that heightened stress reactivity occurs primarily at mild levels of stress, thereby resulting in a bigger difference in depression between those with and without a history of adversity at mild than severe levels of stress. A stress-sensitization model allows for the possibility that youth with a history of adversity also show somewhat greater reactivity to severe stress than those without a history of adversity (i.e., severe stress might be sufficient but not necessary to trigger depression in youth with a history of adversity), but it is also possible according to this model that severe stress is sufficient to precipitate equally high levels of depression even in youth with no pre-existing vulnerability. A stress-inoculation model, in contrast, suggests that youth with a history of adversity actually show diminished stress reactivity, resulting in lower levels of depression in the face of stress.

All three of these models are consistent with developmental perspectives on psychopathology that capture the dynamic transactions occurring between youth and their environments over the course of development (Boyce et al., 1998; Curtis & Cicchetti, 2003; Lerner, 1978; O’Connor, 2003; Sameroff, 1975). Consistent with a developmental psychopathology perspective, earlier experiences contribute to an unfolding cycle of person-by-environment transactions and interactions across development. That is, stress creates enduring changes in maturing biological and psychological systems; these changes, in turn, heighten or reduce vulnerability to future adverse environmental forces. According to these theories, therefore, experience not only serves as a social context for development, but actually might influence the biological and psychological make-up of the developing individual, thereby influencing future vulnerability or resilience.

Although each of these models could explain the long-term effects of childhood adversity, prior theory (Gold et al., 1988; Pine & Charney, 2002; Post, 1992; Post et al., 1984) and research (Hammen et al., 2000; Harkness et al., in press) most strongly implicate stress-sensitization processes when understanding the influence of adversity on depressive reactions to subsequent stress. In the present research, therefore, we anticipated that youth with a history of adversity, reflected in exposure to disruption in close interpersonal relationships (e.g., interparental conflict, parental divorce, death of a family member or close friend, chaotic family circumstances), would demonstrate heightened stress reactivity in the form of a lower threshold for depressive reactions to recent stress than youth without a history of adversity.

Domain Specificity of Stress Reactivity

One limitation of prior theory and research on the interactive influence of childhood adversity and recent stress is a failure to distinguish among different domains of stress. However, diathesis-stress “match” models (e.g., Hammen & Goodman-Brown, 1990; Turner & Cole, 1994) suggest that risk for depression might depend on a match between the domain of vulnerability and the domain of stress. Because the present research focused on adversity in the form of disruption in interpersonal relationships, we anticipated that exposure to adversity would be particularly likely to influence future reactivity to interpersonal rather than noninterpersonal stressors.

Developmental and Sex Differences in Stress Sensitization

Although stress-sensitization theory focuses on the role of early adversity in future stress reactivity, it expands little on the time frame across which these processes unfold. Moreover, direct supportive evidence comes exclusively from studies of adolescents (Hammen et al., 2000; Harkness et al., in press). However, it is possible that there are particular developmental stages during which youth with a history of adversity are especially likely to demonstrate a lower threshold of stress reactivity. For instance, vulnerable youth might be particularly sensitive to stress-sensitization effects during developmental periods that involve novelty and disruption, such as the transition through puberty. The pubertal transition is associated with a confluence of biological, psychological, and social developments and challenges (e.g., Ge, Lorenz, Conger, Elder, & Simons, 1994; Petersen & Hamburg, 1986; Susman & Rogol, 2004). This backdrop of change in adolescents’ lives might heighten their reactivity to even mild stressors, thereby activating stress-sensitization processes in youth exposed to prior adversity. In fact, it is possible that prior to the pubertal transition, a stress-amplification or stress-inoculation model of adversity holds, such that exposure to adversity either amplifies depressive reactions only when youth are exposed to more severe stress, or even buffers youth against depressive reactions to stress. Alternatively, exposure to adversity might have little impact on depression under any circumstances prior to puberty.

The transition through puberty might be especially likely to activate stress-sensitization processes linked to depression in girls. Research reveals that adolescent girls are more sensitive than boys to the adverse effects of stress, particularly stressors that involve the disruption of interpersonal relationships (e.g., Ge et al., 1994; Rudolph, 2002). This heightened stress sensitivity and associated risk for depression might be due to a variety of female-linked characteristics that emerge or intensify during adolescence, including hormonal changes, personality attributes that involve a focus on relationships, and maladaptive coping styles (for reviews, see Hankin & Abramson, 2001; Nolen-Hoeksema & Girgus, 1994; Rose & Rudolph, 2006; Rudolph, Hammen, & Daley, 2005). In the present study, therefore, we predicted that sensitization to interpersonal stress, but not noninterpersonal stress, would be most salient in girls undergoing the transition to puberty. That is, we anticipated that pubertal girls who were exposed to social adversity would demonstrate higher levels of depression than those who were not exposed to adversity at mild levels of interpersonal stress (i.e., a lower threshold for stress reactivity). At severe levels of interpersonal stress, we expected that even the pubertal girls who were not exposed to adversity would experience heightened depression.

Overview of the Present Research

Two studies were conducted to examine the joint contribution of childhood social adversity and recent stress to depression. The first study was a short-term (6-month) prospective investigation in a sample of 399 early adolescents. Adolescents completed a series of questionnaires that assessed exposure to a significant family disruption, experience of stressful life events, and anhedonia. We focused on anhedonia in this study because contemporary models of emotional distress (Watson et al., 1995) suggest that depression is uniquely reflected in symptoms of anhedonia (e.g., inability to experience pleasure, feelings of boredom, social withdrawal). The second study was a concurrent investigation in a sample of 147 early to mid adolescents. Adolescents and their primary female caregiver completed a series of semi-structured interviews that assessed exposure to childhood adversity (specifically, interpersonal disruptions), experience of stressful life events, and clinical depression. These two studies had complementary strengths—the first allowed for an examination of the direction of effect between stress and subsequent depression, adjusting for prior levels of depression, whereas the second provided in-depth multi-informant assessments of the key study constructs. In both studies, adolescents provided reports of their pubertal status, allowing us to examine whether depressive reactions to adversity differed across maturational stages.

Study 1

Method

Participants

Participants were 399 5th and 6th graders (185 girls, 214 boys; M age = 11.66 years, SD = .68) from several schools in a Midwestern school district. These youth were drawn from a sample of 537 youth who participated in two waves of a longitudinal study examining the transition to adolescence. Of the eligible students in the targeted grades, 97% participated in the initial wave of the larger study, resulting in a highly representative sample. The 399 youth included in the present research were selected based on their completion of the Pubertal Development Scale (PDS; Petersen, Crockett, Richards, & Boxer, 1988) at the second wave of the study. Non-completion of the measure was due to time constraints that prevented youth from completing the measure or to youth declining the measure. The 399 youth were diverse in ethnicity (66.2% White, 28.1% African American, 4.0% Asian American, and 1.9% other or unknown) and socioeconomic status. Based on available information for 91% of the youth, 31% received a federally subsidized school lunch.

Youth who did and did not complete the PDS did not significantly differ in their level of Wave 1 anhedonia, t(532) = 1.82, ns, Wave 1 or Wave 2 interpersonal stress, t(523) = 1.82, ns, and t(515) = 1.12, ns, Wave 1 or Wave 2 noninterpersonal stress, t(523) = 1.02, ns, and t(513) = -.35, ns, exposure to a family disruption, χ2(1) = 1.02, ns, or receipt of a federally subsidized school lunch, χ2(2) = 4.16, ns. The two groups did differ in sex, χ2(1) = 12.42, p < .001, ethnicity (white versus ethnic minority), χ2(1) = 4.31, p < .05, age, t(535) = 4.35, p < .001, and Wave 2 anhedonia, t(528) = 3.33, p < .001. Specifically, youth who did not complete the measure were more likely to be girls, members of ethnic minority groups, older (11.94 vs. 11.66 years), and more anhedonic at Wave 2. Of the 399 youth, 94% had complete data for both waves of the study and were included in the present analyses. Youth with and without complete data did not significantly differ on the key study variables.

Procedures

Participants completed the measures at two waves, separated by approximately six months, during a classroom administration. Researchers read each item and response option aloud as participants followed along and recorded their responses. Table 1 presents intercorrelations among anhedonia, interpersonal and noninterpersonal stress, and family disruption (0 = absent; 1 = present).

Table 1.

Intercorrelations Among the Variables - Study 1

1 2 3 4
1. Anhedonia .38*** .34*** .15**
2. Interpersonal Life Stress .68*** .37***
3. Noninterpersonal Life Stress .30***
4. Family Disruption
**

p < .01

***

p < .001.

Measures

Anhedonia

Adolescents completed the anhedonia subscale from the Youth Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (Y-MASQ; Rudolph et al., 2006) to assess experiences of anhedonia during the previous two weeks. This subscale includes eight items (e.g., “I felt depressed.” “I did not enjoy anything.” “I felt like nothing really mattered to me.” “I did not feel like doing things that I usually like to do.”), rated on a five-point scale (1 = Not at all to 5 = Very Much). This subscale was derived from factor analysis of the Y-MASQ (Rudolph et al., 2006) and allowed us to distinguish depression from generalized emotional distress or negative affect. The subscale had high internal consistency in the present sample (αs = .80 and .83 at Waves 1 and 2, respectively) and was relatively stable over time (average r = .45, p < .001). The anhedonia subscale correlated with scores on the Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI; Kovacs, 1980/81; average within-wave r = .63, p < .001). Scores were computed as the average of the eight items, with higher scores reflecting more anhedonia (M = 1.65; SD = .66; range = 1 to 4.75). Although the study involved a representative community sample, a considerable number of youth experienced significant depressive symptoms. Specifically, 15% of youth had CDI scores in the moderate to severe range (i.e., 13 or above).

Life stress

Adolescents completed a modified version of the Life Events Questionnaire (Robinson, Garber, & Hilsman, 1995). Several events from the original 35-item version were omitted because they were less relevant to this age group (e.g., pregnancy, beginning or ending of jobs). A few events were added that were not widely represented in the original version (e.g., peer events and academic failure events). The final revised version included 37 stressful events across different life domains (e.g., family, peer, school, health, finances).

Adolescents first indicated whether they had experienced each of the events in the last six months. Then they rated the stressfulness of each experienced event on a 5-point scale (1 = Not Bad at All to 5 = Horrible). Because ratings of stressfulness might be confounded with exposure to prior stress or symptoms of depression, a nomothetic method was used to determine the event weightings (see Caldwell, Rudolph, Troop-Gordon, & Kim, 2004; Turner & Cole, 1994). Specifically, the average impact rating across all adolescents who had experienced each event was calculated. This average rating was then used to weight the event for each adolescent. This procedure yielded information about the objective impact of the stressors. Each event was categorized as interpersonal (events that involve a significant interaction between the youth and another person or events that directly affect the relationship between the youth and another person; e.g., a close friend moved away) or noninterpersonal (e.g., failure of a class). High reliability (95% agreement) was found for event categorization. Separate interpersonal stress scores (M = 8.38; SD = 7.48, range = 0 – 45.68) and noninterpersonal stress scores (M = 7.93; SD = 6.61, range = 0 – 37.71) were calculated by summing the event weightings across relevant events experienced by each youth.

Family disruption

Adolescents completed a questionnaire assessing their exposure to family disruption involving a significant parent-child separation or loss (Rudolph, Kurlakowsky, & Conley, 2001). First, adolescents responded to two general probes regarding whether they had experienced a significant separation from their mother or father. If either of these items was positively endorsed, adolescents indicated the cause of the separation or loss based on a checklist of eight possible reasons: parental divorce or separation, parental death, placement in foster care, placement with another relative, parental abandonment (i.e., never lived with parent), parental hospitalization, parent taking care of sick relative, and parent away for job. These reasons were generated based on a review of relevant research (e.g., Kessler, Davis, & Kendler, 1997; Kessler & Magee, 1993) and on prior data from an open-ended questionnaire that elicited information from youth and parents about causes for parent-child separation or loss. Adolescents also indicated the duration of the separation or loss (less than one month, more than one month, more than one year, all their life). Because the focus in the present study was on major interpersonal disruptions, the disruption group included adolescents who experienced separations of greater than one year and the no disruption group included adolescents who experienced no separations or separations of less than one year.

Based on these guidelines, 251 adolescents experienced no significant disruption and 130 adolescents experienced a significant disruption involving one (82.3%) or both (17.7%) parents (data were missing from 18 adolescents). Of those adolescents who experienced a significant separation from their mother, almost 90% endorsed placement in foster care (27.6%), placement with another relative (27.6%), parental divorce or separation (20.7%), or maternal abandonment (i.e., never lived with mother; 10.3%) as the reason for the separation. A few adolescents (n = 4) endorsed maternal hospitalization (6.9%), maternal death (3.4%), and mother was away for job (3.4%).

Of those adolescents who experienced a significant separation from their father, about 90% endorsed parental divorce or separation (58.5%), paternal abandonment (17.9%), placement in foster care (6.5%), or placement with another relative (6.5%) as the reason. A few adolescents (n = 13) endorsed father was away for job (4.9%), paternal death (3.3%), father taking care of sick relative (.8%), and other (e.g., paternal imprisonment; 1.6%) as the reason. In cases where adolescents experienced more than one significant separation from their mother or father, the separation of the longest duration was used to categorize them.

Pubertal development

Youth completed the Pubertal Development Scale (PDS; Petersen et al., 1988) to assess their level of physical maturation. The PDS is composed of five questions that assess stage of growth spurt, body hair, skin changes, voice changes and facial hair (boys), and breast development and menarcheal status (girls). Each item is rated on a 4-point scale (1 = No development, 2 = Development has just begun, 3 = Development is definitely underway, 4 = Development is complete). Onset of menarche is rated using a dichotomous response (1 = No, 4 = Yes). The PDS has been well-validated across several studies (Brooks-Gunn, Warren, Rosso, & Gargiulo, 1987; Petersen et al., 1988). Scores on the PDS are moderately correlated with physician ratings of the Tanner stages (Brooks-Gunn et al., 1987). This measure was added to the study at Wave 2, and thus scores are only available at this time (α = .70 for girls and α =.56 for boys).

Youth were divided into two groups based on their scores on this measure: youth scoring at or below a 2 (prepubertal) and youth scoring above a 2 (pubertal). This cut-off reflects a conceptually meaningful categorization of youth whose development had not yet begun or was barely underway versus those whose development was underway.

Results

Overview of Analytic Approach

To examine whether the effect of childhood adversity varied at different developmental stages, separate analyses were conducted in pubertal and prepubertal youth. Specifically, a series of hierarchical multiple regression analyses was conducted to examine whether a history of family disruption moderated the effect of recent stress (interpersonal and noninterpersonal) on subsequent anhedonia. For each analysis, Wave 1 anhedonia was adjusted for in the first step, the main effects of recent stress, family disruption, and sex were entered in the second step, all two-way interactions were entered in the third step, and the three-way interactions were entered in the fourth step. The continuous predictors were mean-centered. Effects for interpersonal and noninterpersonal stress were entered in the same equations to examine their independent contribution. When the effects for noninterpersonal stress were nonsignificant, follow-up regressions were conducted excluding these effects. Significant interactions were interpreted by solving the unstandardized regression equation to predict anhedonia from stress for the disruption and no disruption groups. Findings for interpersonal stress are summarized in the text, Table 2, and Figure 2. Because the findings for noninterpersonal stress were nonsignificant, these are summarized briefly in the text. The table and figure display results of the analyses for interpersonal stress that exclude the nonsignificant effects of noninterpersonal stress.

Table 2.

Predicting Anhedonia Over Time From Recent Interpersonal Stress, Family Disruption, and Interpersonal Stress × Family Disruption Interaction

Predictors β t ΔR2
Pubertal Girls
(n = 111)
Step 1 Anhedonia .34   3.43*** .21
Step 2 Stress .58   3.60***
Family Disruption −.12 −1.31 .05
Step 3 Stress ×
Family Disruption −.36 −2.38* .04
Prepubertal Girls
(n = 70)
Step 1 Anhedonia .53   4.91*** .41
Step 2 Stress .25   1.56 .03
Family Disruption .00     .04 .03
Step 3 Stress ×
Family Disruption −.07   −.48 .00
Pubertal Boys
(n = 93)
Step 1 Anhedonia .36   3.59** .13
Step 2 Stress −.10   −.80 .01
Family Disruption .11   1.04 .01
Step 3 Stress ×
Family Disruption .06     .49 .00
Prepubertal Boys
(n = 102)
Step 1 Anhedonia .37   3.77*** .18
Step 2 Stress .32   2.07* .01
Family Disruption −.01   −.14 .01
Step 3 Stress ×
Family Disruption −.29 −1.99* .03
*

p < .05

**

p < .01

***

p < .001.

Note. βs and ts represent standardized coefficients and t statistics at the final step. ΔR2 represents percent of variance accounted for at each step.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Interpersonal Stress × Family Disruption interactions in the prediction of subsequent anhedonia for (a) pubertal girls and (b) prepubertal boys. The analyses adjust for prior anhedonia.

Pubertal Youth

In pubertal youth, a significant Interpersonal Stress × Family Disruption × Sex interaction was found, β = -.40, t = −1.99, p < .05. The Noninterpersonal Stress × Family Disruption × Sex interaction, β = .01, t = .03, ns, and the Noninterpersonal Stress × Family Disruption interaction, β = -.15, t = -.83, ns, were nonsignificant. Thus, effects for noninterpersonal stress were excluded from follow-up analyses. To clarify the nature of the Interpersonal Stress × Family Disruption × Sex interaction, separate regressions were conducted in pubertal girls and boys. As displayed in Table 2, the two-way interaction for interpersonal stress was significant in girls but not boys. Decomposition of the interaction in girls revealed that interpersonal stress was significantly associated with subsequent anhedonia in youth who had not experienced a significant family disruption, β = .35, t = 3.39, p < .001, but not in those who had, β = .24, t = 1.24, ns. As reflected in Figure 2a, the nature of this interaction was consistent with a stress-sensitization effect. Specifically pubertal girls who were exposed to a family disruption were more anhedonic at Wave 2, after adjusting for Wave 1 anhedonia, even following the experience of mild to moderate interpersonal stress. Pubertal girls who were not exposed to a family disruption showed low levels of Wave 2 anhedonia when they experienced mild stress, but levels more similar to those of the disruption group when they experienced high levels of stress. For pubertal boys, interpersonal stress did not significantly predict Wave 2 anhedonia.

Prepubertal Youth

In prepubertal youth, the main effects, two-way interactions, and three-way interactions for both interpersonal and noninterpersonal stress were nonsignificant, |βs| ≤ .18, |ts| ≤ 1.21, ns.

Summary of Study 1 Results

Results from Study 1 indicated that a history of family disruption lowered pubertal girls’ threshold for reactivity to interpersonal stress, such that they displayed heightened anhedonia, adjusting for earlier anhedonia, following even mild levels of stress. In contrast, pubertal girls with no history of family disruption displayed heightened anhedonia only when they were exposed to high levels of interpersonal stress. Stress-sensitization effects did not emerge in the context of noninterpersonal stress.

Although no significant main effects or interaction effects were found in prepubertal youth in the preliminary analysis, the pattern of effects in Table 2 indicated a significant Interpersonal Stress × Family Disruption interaction in prepubertal boys. Thus, we further probed the nature of this interaction on an exploratory basis (see Figure 2b). Decomposition of the interaction revealed that interpersonal stress was associated with subsequent anhedonia in youth who had not experienced a significant family disruption, β = .22, t = 1.98, p = .05, but not in those who had, β = -.03, t = -.19, ns. As reflected in the figure, prepubertal boys who were exposed to a family disruption were more anhedonic at Wave 2, after adjusting for Wave 1 anhedonia, even following the experience of mild interpersonal stress. Prepubertal boys who were not exposed to a family disruption showed low levels of Wave 2 anhedonia when they experienced mild stress, but levels similar to those of the disruption group when they experienced high levels of stress. Although this pattern was similar to that observed in pubertal girls, it needs to be interpreted with caution due to the absence of a significant three-way interaction with sex.

Study 2

Method

Participants

Participants were 147 4th – 8th graders (85 girls, 62 boys; M age = 12.39 years, SD = 1.23; 78.9% White, 12.9% African American, and 8.2% other) and their female caregivers who participated in a study of youth depression. Families had a range of income levels [$0–14,999 (6.3%), $15–29,999 (9.2%), $30–44,999 (23.9%), $45–59,999 (25.4%), $60,000–74,999 (15.5%), $75,000–89,999 (6.3%), and over $90,000 (13.4%)]. Families resided in small urban and rural Midwestern towns. Of the 147 female caregivers, 89.1% were biological mothers, 3.4% adoptive mothers, 2.7% aunts, 2.0% grandmothers, 2.0% stepmothers, and .7% other. Fifteen of the youth did not have a male caregiver in the home. The remainder of the male caregivers included 68.7% biological fathers, 13.6% stepfathers, 2.7% adoptive fathers, 1.4% uncles, 1.4% grandfathers, and 2.0% other.

These participants were a subset of 167 youth who comprised the full sample for a larger study of youth depression. The 147 youth were selected based either on their completion of the Pubertal Development Scale (n = 132) or the availability of information regarding their menarcheal status (n = 15 girls). Because menarcheal status was strongly correlated with pubertal status as measured by the remainder of the PDS items (r = .69, p < .001), these girls were included in the analyses. Furthermore, menarcheal status has been used as a sole index of puberty in prior studies of depression (Hayward, Gotlib, Schraedley, & Litt, 1999). The subset of youth who were included and excluded from the present analyses did not differ in ethnicity (White versus minority), χ2(1) = .81, ns, age, t(165) = .57, ns, depression, t(165) = 1.15, ns, interpersonal or noninterpersonal stress, ts(165) = .29 and 1.36, ns, or adversity, t(165) = .32, ns. They did differ in sex, χ2(1) = 19.67, p < .001. However, the sex difference was not due to a differential completion rate of the PDS in girls versus boys, χ2(1) = .59, ns, but rather the fact that we were able to include a small subset of girls but not boys who had not completed the PDS. Analyses were conducted with and without the girls who were included based on menarcheal status, and the pattern of results was highly similar. We chose to report results from the analyses that included these girls to maximize our inclusion of youth from the original study.

Participants for the larger study were recruited based on school-wide screenings for depressive symptoms using the Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI; Kovacs, 1980/81). Youth with a range of CDI scores were recruited for the study, oversampling those with CDI scores above 18. Because some of the youth who participated in the school-wide screenings also participated in Study 1, there was some overlap in the samples for the two studies (13% of the youth in Study 1 also participated in Study 2).

Procedures

Primary female caregivers were contacted by telephone to determine their interest in participating in the study. Participating families met with two interviewers for a three- to four-hour assessment. One interviewer conducted diagnostic interviews and the other interviewer conducted life stress interviews. Caregivers and youth were interviewed separately. Families were given a financial reimbursement and youth were given a gift certificate for their participation.

Measures

Table 3 presents intercorrelations among the measures. It is notable that noninterpersonal stress was not significantly associated with depression, interpersonal stress, or childhood adversity in this study, whereas these correlations were significant and quite a bit larger in Study 1. This discrepancy might be due to a better differentiation between interpersonal and noninterpersonal stress using interview-based measures than questionnaire-based measures.

Table 3.

Intercorrelations Among the Variables - Study 2

1 2 3 4
1. Depression .37*** −.07 .26**
2. Interpersonal Life Stress .07 .57***
3. Noninterpersonal Life Stress .05
4. Childhood Adversity
**

p < .01

***

p < .001.

Depression

The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Epidemiologic Version-5 (K-SADS-E; Orvaschel, 1995) was administered to youth and their caregivers to assess youth depression. Interviews were conducted by a faculty member in clinical psychology, a post-doctoral student in clinical psychology, several psychology graduate students, or a post BA-level research assistant. Interviewers received intensive training in the assessment and diagnosis of psychopathology in youth, and all interviews were coded through consultation with a clinical psychology faculty member. A best-estimate approach (Klein, Ouimette, Kelly, Ferro, & Riso, 1994) was used to combine information from the caregiver and youth.

Based on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders criteria (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) regarding the number, severity, and frequency of symptoms and resulting impairment, ratings of depressive symptoms were assigned on a 5-point scale: 0 = No symptoms, 1 = Mild symptoms, 2 = Moderate symptoms (consistent with a minor depressive episode), 3 = Diagnosis with mild to moderate impairment, 4 = Diagnosis with severe impairment. Separate ratings were assigned for each category of depression (e.g., Major Depression, Dysthymia) and each episode of depression during the year preceding the interview, including the present. These ratings were then summed to create continuous depression scores (M = .86, SD = 1.58, range = 0 – 9). Higher ratings reflected more severe symptoms within a single diagnostic category, the presence of symptoms from multiple categories, and/or multiple episodes of depression (see Rudolph et al., 2000). Of the 147 youth, 12.9% met diagnostic criteria for major depression or dysthymia within the past year (i.e., a rating of 3 or 4 for at least one episode of major depression or dysthymia). An additional 10.2% had moderate symptoms of major depression, dysthymia, or other depressive disorders within the past year (i.e., a rating of 2 for at least one episode of major depression, dysthmia, or other depressive diagnosis).

Reliability of these ratings was evaluated based on independent coding of 25% of the audiotaped interviews. Strong reliability was found for both continuous (one-way random-effects intraclass correlation coefficient = .98) and categorical (Cohen’s weighted ĸ = .86) depression scores.

Life stress

Youth and their caregivers completed the Youth Life Stress Interview, an adaptation of the Child Episodic Life Stress Interview (Rudolph & Hammen, 1999; Rudolph et al., 2000). This semi-structured interview assesses the nature and intensity of life stress using the contextual threat method (Brown & Harris, 1978). Specific, detailed probes were used to elicit objective information separately from youth and caregivers about youths’ experience of episodic stressors across several life domains during the preceding year (e.g., school, same- and opposite-sex peer relationships, parent-child relationships, health). First, interviewers provided a general probe to elicit information about the occurrence of stressful events in the past year. Following the general probe, interviewers probed for specific stressful events within a variety of domains (e.g., an argument with a parent, failure of a test, a friend moving away, a serious injury). Based on detailed follow-up questions concerning the timing, duration, and context of the stressors, interviewers prepared a narrative summary of each event. This information was presented to a team of coders, blind to the youth’s diagnosis and subjective reaction to the events.

Consensual ratings were assigned by integrating information across youth and caregiver reports. For each life event, the team rated on a 5-point scale the objective stress or negative impact that would be associated with the event for a typical youth in the same circumstances (events that were rated “no negative stress/impact” were excluded from analyses). The team also categorized each event according to its content: interpersonal (events that involved a significant interaction between the youth and another person or events that directly affected the relationship between the youth and another person) and noninterpersonal. For example, an argument with a parent or a friend moving away would be coded as interpersonal stressors, whereas failing a test or being ill would be coded as noninterpersonal stressors. Composite scores of interpersonal stress (M = 9.05, SD = 7.06, range = 0 – 36) and noninterpersonal stress (M = 6.29, SD = 4.41, range = 0 – 21.50) were calculated by summing the impact ratings across relevant events.

For reliability purposes, information about 160 life events was presented to two independent coding teams. High reliability was found for ratings of episodic stress impact (ICC = .95) and event content (Cohen’s ĸ = .92).

Childhood adversity

The Lifetime Adversity section of the Youth Life Stress Interview, developed for the present study, was administered to youth and their caregivers. This interview assesses youths’ exposure to severe negative life events and circumstances across their lifetime (up until one year prior to the interview) using the same type of contextual threat method as the assessment of recent life events. This time frame was used to ensure that reports of childhood adversity did not overlap with reports of recent stress, which were assessed in the year prior to the interview. First, a general probe was used to assess exposure to particularly stressful events and circumstances, emphasizing that the focus was on very serious rather than everyday problems. Second, specific probes were used to assess the occurrence of the following types of adversity: death of a close family member or friend, extended separation from parents, parental separation or divorce, exposure to severe marital conflict, and severe chronic illness of a close family member or friend. Follow-up questions were asked as needed to assess the context of the event. An independent rating team provided a consensual rating on a 10-point scale that reflected the overall level of adversity experienced by youth, considering the likely impact of the events for a typical child in the same circumstances (M = 3.27, SD = 2.00, range = 1 – 10). For reliability purposes, two independent teams of raters coded a subset of 40 interviews. High reliability was found for ratings of adversity (ICC = .99).

Pubertal development

Youth completed the PDS (Petersen et al., 1988) to assess their level of pubertal development (see description in Study 1). The measure was available for 132 youth (α = .86 for girls and .67 for boys). Youth who were missing this measure either entered the study before we began to administer the scale (n = 24) or declined completing the scale (n = 11). As noted earlier, information was available about menarcheal status for an additional 15 girls.

Results

Overview of Analytic Approach – Continuous Scores

To examine whether the effect of childhood adversity varied at different developmental stages, separate analyses were once again conducted in pubertal and prepubertal youth. Specifically, a series of hierarchical multiple regression analyses was conducted to examine whether a history of adversity moderated the association between recent stress (interpersonal and noninterpersonal) and depression. For each analysis, the main effects of recent stress and adversity were entered in the first step and the two-way interactions were entered in the second step. The predictors were mean-centered. Effects for interpersonal and noninterpersonal stress were entered in the same equations to examine their independent contribution. Significant interactions were interpreted by solving the unstandardized regression equation to predict depression from stress at low (−1 SD), moderate (mean), and high (+1 SD) levels of adversity. Findings for interpersonal stress are summarized in the text, Table 4, and Figure 3. Because the findings for noninterpersonal stress were nonsignificant, these are summarized briefly in the text. The table and figure display results of the analyses for interpersonal stress that exclude the nonsignificant effects of noninterpersonal stress.

Table 4.

Predicting Depression From Recent Interpersonal Stress, Childhood Adversity, and Interpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity Interaction

Predictors β t ΔR2
Pubertal Girls
(n = 54)
Step 1 Stress .51   3.27**
Childhood Adversity .10     .62 .18
Step 2 Stress ×
Childhood Adversity −.29 −1.84+ .05
Prepubertal Girls
(n = 31)
Step 1 Stress .47   3.20**
Childhood Adversity .14     .88 .43
Step 2 Stress ×
Childhood Adversity .35    2.33* .10
Pubertal Boys
(n = 35)
Step 1 Stress −.06    −.31
Childhood Adversity .32    1.48 .04
Step 2 Stress ×
Childhood Adversity −.27  −1.25 .05
Prepubertal Boys
(n = 27)
Step 1 Stress .31    1.08
Childhood Adversity .34    1.20 .07
Step 2 Stress ×
Childhood Adversity −.54  −2.28* .17
+

p < .10

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

Note. βs and ts represent standardized coefficients and t statistics at the final step. ΔR2 represents percent of variance accounted for at each step.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Figure 3

Interpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interactions in the prediction of depression for (a) pubertal girls, (b) prepubertal girls, and (c) prepubertal boys.

Pubertal Youth

The regression analysis for pubertal girls revealed a marginally significant Interpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction, β = -.29, t = −1.76, p < .10, and a nonsignificant Noninterpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction, β = .07, t = .55, ns (see Table 4 for results of the analysis that excludes noninterpersonal stress). Decomposition of the Interpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction revealed that recent interpersonal stress was more strongly associated with depression in girls who were exposed to low levels of adversity, β = .67, t = 3.43, p < .001, than in those who were exposed to high levels of adversity, β = .34, t = 2.14, p < .05. Stress was moderately associated with depression in pubertal girls who were exposed to moderate levels of adversity, β = .51, t = 3.27, p < .01. As reflected in Figure 3a, the nature of this interaction was consistent with a stress-sensitization effect. Specifically, pubertal girls who were exposed to high levels of adversity showed some symptoms of depression even when they experienced mild to moderate recent interpersonal stress. Pubertal girls who were exposed to low levels of adversity showed very low levels of symptoms when they experienced mild interpersonal stress. At high levels of interpersonal stress, pubertal girls experienced symptoms of depression between those reflecting a minor and major depressive episode, regardless of their exposure to adversity.

The regression analysis for pubertal boys revealed a nonsignificant Interpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction, β = -.22, t = -.82, ns, and a nonsignificant Noninterpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction, β = -.07, t = -.29, ns.

Prepubertal Youth

The regression analysis for prepubertal girls revealed a significant Interpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction, β = .56, t = 3.17, p < .01, and a nonsignificant Noninterpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction, β = .26, t = 1.66, ns (see Table 4 for results of the analysis that excludes noninterpersonal stress). Decomposition of the Interpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction revealed that interpersonal stress was significantly associated with depression in girls exposed to high, β = .80, t = 3.96, p < .001, and moderate, β = .47, t = 3.20, p < .01, levels of adversity, but not those exposed to low levels of adversity, β = .14, t = .69, ns. As reflected in Figure 3b, the nature of this interaction was consistent with a stress-amplification effect. Specifically, prepubertal girls who were exposed to high levels adversity showed heightened depression when exposed to severe but not mild levels of recent interpersonal stress. Prepubertal girls without a history of adversity showed low levels of depression regardless of their exposure to recent stress.

The regression analysis for prepubertal boys revealed a significant Interpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction, β = -.56, t = −2.18, p < .05, and a nonsignificant Noninterpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction, β = -.02, t = -.09, ns (see Table 4 for results of the analysis that excludes noninterpersonal stress). Decomposition of the Interpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction revealed that recent interpersonal stress was associated with depression in boys who were exposed to low levels of adversity, β = .66, t = 1.82, p < .10, but not those exposed to moderate, β = .31, t = 1.08, ns, or high, β = -.05, t = -.16, ns, levels of adversity. As reflected in Figure 3c, the nature of this interaction was consistent with a stress-sensitization effect. Specifically, prepubertal boys who were exposed to high levels of adversity showed significant symptoms of depression even when they experienced mild to moderate recent interpersonal stress. Prepubertal boys who were exposed to low levels of adversity showed very low levels of symptoms when they experienced mild interpersonal stress. At high levels of interpersonal stress, prepubertal boys experienced symptoms of depression between those reflecting a minor and major depressive episode, regardless of their exposure to adversity.1

Overview of Analytic Approach – Categorical Scores

An additional set of logistic regression analyses was conducted to examine whether a similar pattern of effects arose for interpersonal stress when predicting categorical depression scores. Because of the limited sample size of youth who met diagnostic criteria for depression, particularly when the youth were further divided by sex and pubertal status, these analyses distinguished between youth who experienced no or mild symptoms of depression versus youth who met diagnostic criteria (i.e., ratings of 3 or 4 on the 0 – 4 scale) or who met criteria for moderate symptoms (i.e., ratings of 2 on the 0 – 4 scale; for example, youth who experienced minor depressive episodes or youth who experienced significant symptoms of dysthymia for less than one year). Parallel to the analyses for continuous scores, the main effects of recent stress and childhood adversity were entered in the first step and the two-way interaction was entered in the second step. The predictors were mean-centered. The dependent variable in each equation was the presence or absence of significant depression. Figure 4 presents graphic depictions of the interactions. These graphs show the percentage of youth who met criteria for significant depression at low (bottom third of sample), moderate (middle third of sample), and high (top third of sample) levels of interpersonal stress and childhood adversity.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Figure 4

Percent of youth with significant depression (moderate symptoms or diagnoses) in low, moderate, and high childhood adversity and interpersonal stress groups for (a) pubertal girls, (b) prepubertal girls, and (c) prepubertal boys.

Pubertal Youth

The logistic regression analysis for pubertal girls revealed a significant main effect of interpersonal stress, B = .18, SE = .07, Δχ2(1) = 8.57, p < .01, a nonsignificant main effect of childhood adversity, B = .51, SE = .28, Δχ2(1) = .38, ns, and a significant Interpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction, B = -.05, SE = .02, Δχ2(1) = 5.78, p < .05. As reflected in Figure 4a, the nature of this interaction was consistent with a stress-sensitization effect. Specifically, pubertal girls who were exposed to high levels of adversity were highly likely to experience significant depression when they experienced moderate (67%) or high (60%) stress. Even at mild stress levels, 25% of pubertal girls exposed to high adversity experienced significant depression, compared to none of the girls exposed to low adversity. Girls exposed to low and moderate levels of adversity appeared unaffected by moderate stress, but were reactive to high levels of stress, although not as reactive as girls exposed to high adversity.

The logistic regression analysis for pubertal boys revealed a nonsignificant main effect of interpersonal stress, B = .07, SE = .16, Δχ2(1) = .01, ns, a nonsignificant main effect of childhood adversity, B = .62, SE = .45, Δχ2(1) = 1.29, ns, and a nonsignificant Interpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction, B = -.10, SE = .10, Δχ2(1) = 2.27, ns.

Prepubertal Youth

The logistic regression analysis for prepubertal girls revealed a significant main effect of interpersonal stress, B = .46, SE = .28, Δχ2(1) = 9.95, p < .01, a significant main effect of childhood adversity, B = .81, SE = .58, Δχ2(1) = 4.17, p < .05, and a significant Interpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction, B = .34, SE = .19, Δχ2(1) = 4.58, p < .05. As reflected in Figure 4b, the nature of this interaction was consistent with a stress-amplification effect. Specifically, prepubertal girls exposed to high adversity were more likely to experience significant depression at high (83%) but not mild or moderate (0%) stress levels. Prepubertal girls exposed to low or moderate adversity rarely experienced significant depression regardless of their level of stress (0 – 17%).

The logistic regression analysis for prepubertal boys revealed a significant main effect of interpersonal stress, B = .16, SE = .09, Δχ2(1) = 4.49, p < .05, a nonsignificant main effect of childhood adversity, B = .43, SE = .37, Δχ2(1) = .12, ns, and a significant Interpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction, B = -.05, SE = .03, Δχ2(1) = 5.31, p < .05. As reflected in Figure 4c, the nature of this interaction generally was consistent with a stress-sensitization effect. Specifically, prepubertal boys who were exposed to high levels of adversity were highly likely to experience significant depression when they experienced moderate (100%) or high (60%) stress but not mild stress (0%). Prepubertal boys exposed to low levels of adversity experienced significant depression only when exposed to high levels of stress. For boys exposed to moderate levels of adversity, 50 −67% experienced significant depression across stress levels.

Summary of Study 2 Results

Results from Study 2 indicated that a history of social adversity lowered the threshold of stress reactivity in pubertal girls and prepubertal boys. That is, these two groups demonstrated symptoms of depression even in the presence of mild to moderate levels of recent interpersonal stress. At severe levels of recent interpersonal stress, pubertal girls and prepubertal boys demonstrated heightened depression regardless of their history of adversity. In contrast, a history of social adversity amplified the adverse effect of recent interpersonal stress in prepubertal girls, such that heightened depression occurred only in prepubertal girls exposed to high levels of adversity and severe recent stress. Results were quite consistent across continuous and categorical operationalizations of depression, although results based on the categorical approach should be interpreted with some caution given some of the small sample sizes within particular groups.

Discussion

The present research examined the joint contribution of childhood social adversity and recent stress to depression in youth. Three models of risk were considered to explain the influence of adversity: stress-amplification, stress-sensitization, and stress-inoculation. Findings yielded support for the first two models but not the third. Specifically, a history of adversity heightened reactivity to subsequent interpersonal stress. The pattern of effects differed across pubertal stage and sex.

Stress-Sensitization and Stress-Amplification Processes

Consistent with a stress-sensitization model of depression (Gold et al., 1988; Post, 1992; Post et al., 1984), pubertal girls and prepubertal boys with a history of adversity demonstrated a lower threshold of reactivity to recent interpersonal but not noninterpersonal stress than those without a history of adversity. Using a continuous approach to measuring depression, youth with a history of adversity demonstrated higher levels of depression than youth without a history of adversity when exposed to mild or moderate recent stress. At severe levels of stress, either a smaller difference emerged (in the case of pubertal girls) or youth with and without a history of adversity demonstrated similar levels of depression (in the case of prepubertal boys). This smaller gap in depression between the two groups was due to the fact that youth without a history of adversity showed significantly higher levels of depression when exposed to severe than to mild stress. Notably, this pattern was observed across two studies using different methodologies. Results were quite consistent when a categorical index of depression was used. Moreover, analyses using a categorical approach further refined the nature of the stress-sensitization effect by suggesting that the most dramatic effect (i.e., a particularly big difference between stress reactivity in youth who were and were not exposed to social adversity) occurs at moderate levels of stress, although for pubertal girls even mild stress was associated with a small heightened risk for depression.

These effects are consistent with the idea that exposure to adversity sensitizes the stress-response system, such that youth show depressive responses to even relatively mild stress. Because earlier levels of symptoms were adjusted in Study 1, these results are not due merely to the stability of depression in youth with a history of adversity. Although lower levels of stress were sufficient to produce depressive symptoms in youth with a history of adversity, severe stress was still capable of producing depression when it did occur (Monroe & Harkness, 2005), as reflected in the elevated levels of depression in vulnerable youth who experienced high levels of stress.

Evidence for a stress-sensitization effect in pubertal girls was consistent with our hypotheses concerning risk for depression in this group. Prior theory and research suggest that adolescent girls are more reactive to interpersonal stress than are adolescent boys (Ge et al., 1994; Rudolph, 2002), including certain forms of family adversity (Hetherington, 1989; Hetherington & Stanley-Hagan, 1999). Moreover, the transition through puberty has been implicated as a particularly high-risk period for depression onset in girls (Angold, Costello, & Worthman, 1998; Hayward et al., 1999; Wichstrom, 1999). Physical changes of puberty not only reflect underlying biological processes (i.e., hormonal changes) that might increase risk, but also are accompanied by changes in self-perception and social expectations that pose emotional challenges to maturing youth. Girls might face particular challenges due to their dissatisfaction with the physical changes (e.g., increase in body fat and weight; Petersen & Crockett, 1985) coupled with heightened disruption in female relationships during this period (Gore, Aseltine, & Colton, 1993; Rudolph & Hammen, 1999). This backdrop of normative changes and disruption during the pubertal transition might lead even mild interpersonal stressors to “tip the scale” toward depression in girls with a history of adversity.

Interestingly, a stress-amplification effect of adversity was observed in prepubertal girls in Study 2. This contrasting pattern was due to differences in stress reactivity between prepubertal and pubertal girls at both mild and severe stress levels. At mild levels of stress, prepubertal girls with a history of adversity showed low levels of depression, in contrast to the higher levels shown by pubertal girls. Perhaps girls with a history of adversity are less reactive to mild interpersonal stress prior to, than during, puberty because their coping resources are not strained by the developmental challenges of puberty. At severe levels of stress, prepubertal girls without a history of adversity did not show heightened depression, in contrast to pubertal girls without a history of adversity. Girls without a pre-existing vulnerability might only demonstrate adverse effects of interpersonal stress when they are negotiating the simultaneous challenges of puberty (Simmons & Blyth, 1987). Notably, the absolute level of depression (when measured on a continuum) observed in prepubertal girls exposed to both a history of adversity and severe stress was still lower than that of pubertal girls exposed to both risk factors, consistent with a heightened risk for depression in pubertal girls. It is noteworthy that a stress-amplification effect was not found for prepubertal girls in Study 1. This discrepancy could be due to a variety of differences in the measurement approaches across the two studies, including the use of a more comprehensive assessment of childhood adversity in Study 2, as well as the use of self-report questionnaires in Study 1 versus interview-based measures in Study 2. Additional research is needed to replicate this effect.

Evidence for a stress-sensitization effect in prepubertal boys was not consistent with our hypotheses concerning the heightened risk associated with puberty and with being a girl. Interestingly, however, this effect is consistent with research suggesting that prepubertal boys might be particularly sensitive to certain types of family adversity, such as divorce and its associated risks (Hetherington, 1989). Moreover, less mature pubertal status has been linked to greater risk for internalizing symptoms and depressive disorders in boys (Angold et al., 1998; Laitinen-Krispijn, van der Ende, & Verhulst, 1999). Because some of the physical changes of puberty (e.g., increased size and muscle mass) and the social value of these changes (e.g., athleticism, status in the peer group) are desirable in boys (Siegel, Yancey, Aneshensel, & Schuler, 1999), prepubertal boys actually might be more vulnerable to depressive responses to interpersonal stress than pubertal boys. This combined sensitivity to family adversity and to recent interpersonal stress might activate the stress-sensitization process in prepubertal boys.

In sum, this research supported both stress-sensitization and stress-amplification models of childhood adversity and depression, although more consistent support was found for stress sensitization. Whereas stress-sensitization processes are consistent with a seldom-mentioned form of diathesis-stress model, wherein a diathesis lowers the level of stress necessary to precipitate psychopathology (Zubin & Spring, 1977), stress-amplification processes are consistent with a more conventional form of diathesis-stress model, wherein a diathesis is not translated into psychopathology unless youth are exposed to high levels of stress (Monroe & Simons, 1991; Sameroff, 1987).

Mechanisms Underlying Stress Amplification and Stress Sensitization

Although the present research did not examine the specific mechanisms mediating stress-amplification and stress-sensitization processes, these effects are consistent with theory and research implicating early adversity as a risk factor for both biological and psychological vulnerability to stress. Research in both humans and animals suggests that early exposure to adverse environmental conditions creates changes in the biological systems underlying stress responses (for reviews, see Boyce & Ellis, 2005; Goodman, 2002; Heim & Nemeroff, 2001). For example, exposure to family adversity, such as separation from caregivers, parental loss, maltreatment, institutionalization, and maternal stress, is associated with dysregulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (e.g., Essex, Klein, Cho, & Kalin, 2002; Gunnar, Morison, Chisolm, & Schuder, 2001; Hart, Gunnar, & Cicchetti, 1995; Sanchez, Ladd, & Plotsky, 2001; Suomi, 1991). Early adversity also contributes to the emergence of dysfunctional social-cognitive processes (e.g., Alloy et al., 2001; Garber & Flynn, 2001; Gibb et al., 2001; Rudolph et al., 2001; Hankin & Abramson, 2001; Rose & Abramson, 1992), insecure attachment (Bowlby, 1969; O’Connor, 1993), and emotion regulation difficulties (Cicchetti & Aber, 1986; Cicchetti & Toth, 1998). Collectively, these studies suggest that early adversity fosters individual differences in the neurobiological and psychological underpinnings of stress reactivity and depression.

One question that arises, however, is when stress reactivity will be expressed in the form of stress amplification versus stress sensitization. Substantial research on vulnerability-stress models of depression yields evidence for stress amplification (e.g., Abela, 2001; Caspi et al., 2003; Hankin, Abramson, & Siler, 2001; Robinson et al., 1995). Why, then, did stress-sensitization effects predominate in the present research? Notably, this pattern of effects is consistent with other research on the interaction between lifetime adversity and recent stress in the prediction of depression (Hammen et al., 2000; Harkness et al., in press), as well as with research suggesting that severe stressful events more frequently precede episodes of depression in some individuals than in others (e.g., Frank, Anderson, Reynolds, Ritenour, & Kupfer, 1994). Perhaps a history of social adversity represents a more pervasive diathesis for depression than specific individual vulnerabilities (e.g., a negative cognitive style, poor coping skills). The development of multiple vulnerabilities resulting from a history of adversity not only might heighten youths’ reactivity to severe stressors, as typically proposed, but also might foster depressive responses to minor challenges, particularly during high-risk developmental periods (e.g., the pubertal transition in girls). Consistent with the idea that stress reactivity can be expressed in different forms, recent theory and research suggest complex and multi-determined interactions between early adversity and recent stressful environments in the prediction of health and psychopathology (Boyce & Ellis, 2005). Thus, additional research is needed to determine under which circumstances stress-sensitization versus stress-amplification models hold, as well as to determine whether unique circumstances might set the stage for stress-inoculation in the form of resilience to depression in the face of stress.

Implications for Developmental Models of Early Experience

The present research contributes to emerging models of early experience (e.g., O’Connor, 2003) by providing insight into one way in which adversity alters youths’ reactivity to later perturbations in their social contexts. Rather than acting in an additive way to determine risk, earlier experiences interact with recent interpersonal stress to determine risk for depression. However, this conceptualization is more dynamic than traditional diathesis-stress models in that the diathesis is presumed to be activated by early stress, rather than to reflect a stable, dispositional vulnerability.

This research also takes a first step toward understanding the developmental context of risk for depression resulting from childhood adversity. Specifically, findings for girls revealed that stress-amplification processes were activated during the prepubertal period and stress-sensitization processes were activated during the pubertal period. In contrast, findings for boys revealed that stress-sensitization processes were activated during the prepubertal period, whereas pubertal boys seemed unaffected by interpersonal stress regardless of their history of adversity. Other developmental questions await attention. For example, developmental theories suggest possible sensitive periods during which certain experiences might contribute to certain types of outcomes (Curtis & Cicchetti, 2003; O’Connor, 2003). Thus, it is important not only to identify when youth with a history of adversity are most sensitive to recent stress, but also to examine how adversity that occurs at different life stages influences subsequent stress reactivity. Relatedly, the mechanisms underlying stress-sensitization and stress-amplification processes might vary according to the stage of exposure to adversity. For example, exposure to adversity early in life might influence the attachment system and subsequent ability to engage in effective emotion-regulation efforts in the face of stress, whereas exposure during middle childhood might undermine the development of children’s sense of self and the emergence of adaptive cognitive systems. Finally, effects might vary across different stages in the course of depression (Harkness et al., in press). Overall, future research would benefit from a more explicit examination of the timing of adversity and the emergence of particular types of vulnerabilities that drive stress reactivity across development.

Limitations of the Present Research

The present research provides only a preliminary step toward understanding stress-sensitization and stress-amplification processes associated with social adversity. It is important to note that our assessments of adversity collapsed across varying experiences because an insufficient number of youth were exposed to each type of adversity to conduct separate analyses. In Study 1 we categorized youth according to whether or not they had experienced a family disruption. In Study 2 we created a composite index of social adversity. Composite indexes of risk can be useful because they capture naturally occurring “packages” of correlated stressors (Kazdin & Kagan, 1994, p. 39; Kessler et al., 1997). For this reason, researchers studying childhood adversity and contextual risk often create such composites (e.g., Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Toth, 1998; Dawson et al., 2003; Ellis, Essex, & Boyce, 2005; Hammen et al., 2000). However, collapsing across types of adversity did not allow us to examine whether specific disruptions were particularly potent in triggering stress-sensitization or stress-amplification processes. Because the consequences of adversity might differ according to its type and associated circumstances, additional research is necessary to unpack the various components of childhood adversity that might set into motion stress-sensitization or stress-amplification process.

Future research also needs to track the emergence of stress reactivity over an extended time period. Theories regarding the role of early experience in vulnerability to psychopathology (O’Connor, 1993; Post, 1992; Post et al., 1984) propose a process that unfolds over time and across developmental stages. In the present study, we examined stress reactivity in youth with and without a history of adversity who differed in their stage of pubertal development, but were not able to follow youth across developmental stages. The use of a prospective design in Study 1 did provide the opportunity to track changes in depression following recent stress, which ensured that our results were not due merely to the stability of depression in youth with a history of adversity. However, longer-term investigations are needed to follow ongoing interactions and transactions among youth and their environments across development. Such investigations would clarify whether a history of adversity promotes stress reactivity similarly at early and later stages of life, or whether reactivity to stress intensifies over the course of development.

Finally, it is important to note that both genetic liability and environmental experiences likely contribute to stress-reactivity profiles (Boyce & Ellis, 2005; Ellis et al., 2005). For example, Kendler and colleagues (2001) suggest that a genetic predisposition to depression might be expressed in the form of a “prekindling” effect, such that lower levels of stress are required to precipitate depression in individuals with than without a genetic liability. It is possible that the association between exposure to adversity and stress reactivity in the present study reflects a common genetic vulnerability. Future research therefore needs to investigate the intersection between genetically and environmentally mediated influences on stress reactivity. Relatedly, recent research has identified distinct components of depression that are reflected in traitlike versus statelike factors (Cole, Nolen-Hoeksema, Girgus, & Paul, 2006). It would be interesting to examine whether stress reactivity profiles linked to early adversity differ according to whether depression is operationalized as a traitlike or statelike construct.

Conclusions

The present research revealed that a history of adversity heightened youths’ stress reactivity either by lowering their threshold of responsiveness to stress, in the case of pubertal girls and prepubertal boys, or by amplifying the adverse effects of severe stress, in the case of prepubertal girls. Heightened stress reactivity occurred in the context of recent interpersonal but not noninterpersonal stress. This research suggests that stress-sensitization and stress-amplification theories provide useful broad developmental frameworks for conceptualizing and studying how early experience and recent stress intersect in their contribution to psychopathology. Moreover, these findings implicate the critical importance of early intervention programs that interrupt these processes before they set the stage for a self-perpetuating course of depression across development.

Acknowledgements

We express our appreciation to the students, teachers, and principals of the participating schools for their facilitation of this study. We also thank Constance Hammen, Kate Harkness, and Eva Pomerantz for their helpful comments; Melissa Caldwell, Alyssa Clark, Colleen Conley, Alison Dupre, Heidi Gazelle, and Kathryn Kurlakowsky for their assistance in data collection and management; and Shannon Daley for consultation on statistical analyses. This research was supported by a University of Illinois Research Board Beckman Award, a William T. Grant Foundation Faculty Scholars Award, and National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH59711 awarded to Karen D. Rudolph.

Footnotes

1

Because of the partial overlap in participants across Studies 1 and 2, we reanalyzed the data omitting youth who had participated in Study 1. The pattern of results was highly similar, with one exception: The Interpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction was only marginally significant in prepubertal boys due to the decrease in the sample size. Effect sizes were slightly larger in the reduced sample with the exception of a smaller effect size for the interaction in pubertal boys (which was consistent with the prediction that this effect would not be significant in pubertal boys). Specifically, the Interpersonal Stress × Childhood Adversity interaction accounted for an additional 9% of the variance in pubertal girls, 11% in prepubertal girls, 1% in pubertal boys, and 19% in prepubertal boys, beyond the main effects. Even though the effect sizes were slightly larger in these analyses, we chose to present the findings for the full sample given the relatively small ns in each group in the reduced sample, which would preclude our ability to conduct categorical analyses.

References

  1. Abela J. The hopelessness theory of depression: A test of the diathesis-stress and causal mediation components in third and seventh grade children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2001;29:241–254. doi: 10.1023/a:1010333815728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Abramson LY, Metalsky GI, Alloy LB. Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review. 1989;96:358–372. [Google Scholar]
  3. Alloy LB, Abramson LY, Tashman NA, Berrebbi DS, Hogan ME, Whitehouse WG, et al. Developmental origins of cognitive vulnerability to depression: Parenting, cognitive, and inferential feedback styles of the parents of individuals at high and low cognitive risk for depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2001;25:397–423. [Google Scholar]
  4. Alloy LB, Kelly KA, Mineka S, Clements CM. Comorbidity of anxiety and depressive disorders: A helplessness-hopelessness perspective. In: Maser JD, Cloninger CR, editors. Comorbidity of mood and anxiety disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1990. pp. 499–543. [Google Scholar]
  5. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: Author; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  6. Angold A, Costello EJ, Worthman CM. Puberty and depression: The roles of age, pubertal status and pubertal timing. Psychological Medicine. 1998;28:51–61. doi: 10.1017/s003329179700593x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: Vol. I. Attachment. New York: Basic Books; 1969. [Google Scholar]
  8. Boyce WT, Ellis BJ. Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17:271–301. doi: 10.1017/s0954579405050145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Boyce WT, Frank E, Jensen PS, Kessler RC, Nelson CA, Steinberg L the MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Psychopathology and Development. Social context in developmental psychopathology: Recommendations for future research from the MacArthur Network on Psychopathology and Development. Development and Psychopathology. 1998;10:143–164. doi: 10.1017/s0954579498001552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brooks-Gunn J, Warren MP, Rosso J, Gargiulo J. Validity of self-report measures of girls’ pubertal status. Child Development. 1987;58:829–841. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brown GW, Harris TO. Social origins of depression: A study of psychiatric disorder in women. New York: Free Press; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  12. Caldwell MS, Rudolph KD, Troop-Gordon W, Kim D. Reciprocal influences among relational self-views, social disengagement, and peer stress during early adolescence. Child Development. 2004;75:1140–1154. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00730.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, et al. Influence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science. 2003;301:386–389. doi: 10.1126/science.1083968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chorpita BF, Barlow DH. The development of anxiety: The role of control in the early environment. Psychological Bulletin. 1998;124:3–21. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cicchetti D, Aber JL. Early precursors of later depression: An organizational perspective. Advances in Infancy Research. 1986;4:87–137. [Google Scholar]
  16. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Toth SL. Maternal depressive disorder and contextual risk: Contributions to the development of attachment insecurity and behavior problems in toddlerhood. Development and Psychopathology. 1998;10:283–300. doi: 10.1017/s0954579498001618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cicchetti D, Toth SL. The development of depression in children and adolescents. American Psychologist. 1998;53:221–241. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.53.2.221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cicchetti D, Walker EF. Stress and development: Biological and psychological consequences. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:413–418. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cole DA, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Girgus J, Paul G. Stress exposure and stress generation in child and adolescent depression: A latent trait-state-error approach to longitudinal analyses. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2006;115:40–51. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.1.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Curtis WJ, Cicchetti D. Moving research on resilience into the 21st century: Theoretical and methodological considerations in examining the biological contributors to resilience. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:773–810. doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Daley SE, Hammen C, Rao U. Predictors of first onset and recurrence of major depression in young women during the five years following high school graduation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2000;109:525–533. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dawson G, Ashman SB, Panagiotides H, Hessl D, Self J, Yamada E, et al. Preschool outcomes of children of depressed mothers: Role of maternal behavior, contextual risk, and children’s brain activity. Child Development. 2003;74:1158–1175. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Eley TC, Sugden K, Corsico A, Gregory AM, Sham P, McGuffin P, et al. Gene-environment interaction analysis of serotonin system markers with adolescent depression. Molecular Psychiatry. 2004;9:908–915. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Ellis BJ, Essex MJ, Boyce WT. Biological sensitivity to context: II. Empirical explorations of an evolutionary-developmental theory. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17:303–328. doi: 10.1017/s0954579405050157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Essex MJ, Klein MH, Cho E, Kalin NH. Maternal stress beginning in infancy may sensitize children to later stress exposure: Effects on cortisol and behavior. Biological Psychiatry. 2002;52:776–784. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01553-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Frank E, Anderson B, Reynolds CF, Ritenour A, Kupfer DJ. Life events and the research diagnostic criteria endogenous subtype. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1994;51:519–524. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950070011005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Garber J, Flynn C. Predictors of depressive cognitions in young adolescents. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2001;25:353–376. [Google Scholar]
  28. Garmezy N. Developmental aspects of children’s responses to the stress of separation and loss. In: Rutter M, Izard CE, Reid PB, editors. Depression in young people: Developmental and clinical perspectives. New York: Guilford; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  29. Ge X, Lorenz FO, Conger RD, Elder GG, Simons RL. Trajectories of stressful life events and depressive symptoms during adolescence. Developmental Psychology. 1994;30:467–483. [Google Scholar]
  30. Gibb BE, Alloy LB, Abramson LY, Rose DT, Whitehouse PD, Hogan ME, et al. History of childhood maltreatment, negative cognitive styles, and episodes of depression in adulthood. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2001;25:425–446. [Google Scholar]
  31. Gold PW, Goodwin FK, Chrousos GP. Clinical and biochemical manifestations of depression: Relation to the neurobiology of stress. New England Journal of Medicine. 1988;319:348–353. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198808113190606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Goodman SH. Depression and early adverse experiences. In: Gotlib IH, Hammen C, editors. Handbook of depression. New York, NY: Guilford; 2002. pp. 245–267. [Google Scholar]
  33. Goodyer IM, Altham PME. Lifetime exit events and recent social and family adversities in anxious and depressed school-age children and adolescents: I. Journal of Affective Disorders. 1991;21:219–228. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(91)90001-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gore S, Aseltine RH, Colton ME. Gender, social-relational involvement, and depression. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 1993;3:101–125. [Google Scholar]
  35. Grant KE, Compas BE, Stuhlmacher AF, Thurm AE, McMahon SD, Halpert JA. Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology: Moving from markers to mechanisms of risk. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129:447–466. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Gunnar MR, Morison SJ, Chisolm K, Schuder M. Salivary cortisol levels in children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:611–628. doi: 10.1017/s095457940100311x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hammen C, Goodman-Brown T. Self-schemas and vulnerability to specific life stress in children at risk for depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1990;14:215–227. [Google Scholar]
  38. Hammen C, Henry R, Daley SE. Depression and sensitization to stressors among young women as a function of childhood adversity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68:782–787. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hankin BL, Abramson LY. Development of gender differences in depression: An elaborated cognitive vulnerability-transactional stress theory. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:773–796. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hankin BL, Abramson LY, Siler M. A prospective test of the hopelessness theory of depression in adolescence. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2001;25:607–632. [Google Scholar]
  41. Harkness KL, Bruce AE, Lumley MN. The role of childhood abuse and neglect in the sensitization to stressful life events in adolescent depression. In press, Journal of Abnormal Psychology. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.4.730. (in press). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hart J, Gunnar MR, Cicchetti D. Altered neuroendocrine activity in maltreated children related to depression. Development and Psychopathology. 1995;8:201–214. [Google Scholar]
  43. Hayward C, Gotlib IH, Schraedley PK, Litt IF. Ethnic differences in the association between pubertal status and symptoms of depression in adolescent girls. Journal of Adolescent Health. 1999;25:143–149. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(99)00048-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Heim C, Nemeroff CB. The role of childhood trauma in the neurobiology of mood and anxiety disorders: Preclinical and clinical studies. Biological Psychiatry. 2001;49:1023–1039. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01157-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hetherington EM. Coping with family transitions: Winners, losers, and survivors. Child Development. 1989;60:1–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1989.tb02690.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hetherington EM, Stanley-Hagan M. The adjustment of children with divorced parents: A risk and resiliency perspective. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1999;40:129–140. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kazdin AE, Kagan J. Models of dysfunction in developmental psychopathology. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 1994;1:35–52. [Google Scholar]
  48. Kendler KS, Thornton LM, Gardner CO. Genetic risk, number of previous depressive episodes, and stressful life events in predicting onset of major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;158:582–586. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.4.582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kessler RC, Davis CG, Kendler KS. Childhood adversity and adult psychiatric disorder in the U.S. National Comorbidity Survey. Psychological Medicine. 1997;27:1101–1119. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797005588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kessler RC, Magee W. Childhood adversities and adult depression: Basic patterns of association in a U.S. national survey. Psychological Medicine. 1993;23:679–690. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700025460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Klein DN, Ouimette PC, Kelly HF, Ferro T, Riso LP. Test-retest reliability of team consensus best-estimate diagnoses of Axis I and II disorders in a family study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1994;151:1043–1047. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.7.1043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kovacs M. Rating scales to assess depression in school-aged children. Acta Paedopsychiatry. 1980/81;46:305–315. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Laitinen-Krispijn S, vanderEnde J, Verhulst FC. The role of pubertal progress in the development of depression in early adolescence. Journal of Affective Disorders. 1999;54:211–215. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(98)00166-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Lerner RM. Nature, nurture, and dynamic interactionism. Human Development. 1978;21:1–20. [Google Scholar]
  55. Lizardi H, Klein DN. Parental psychopathology and reports of the childhood home environment in adults with early-onset dysthymic disorder. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2000;188:63–70. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200002000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Monroe SM, Harkness KL. Life stress, the “kindling” hypothesis, and the recurrence of depression: Considerations from a life stress perspective. Psychological Review. 2005;112:417–445. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Monroe SM, Simons AD. Diathesis-stress theories in the context of life stress research: Implications for the depressive disorders. Psychological Bulletin. 1991;110:406–425. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.3.406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Girgus JS. The emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;115:424–443. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. O’Connor TG. Early experiences and psychological development: Conceptual questions, empirical illustrations, and implications for intervention. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:671–690. doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Orvaschel H. Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Epidemiologic Version-5. Nova Southeastern University; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  61. Peterson AC, Crockett L. Pubertal timing and grade effects on adjustment. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1985;14:191–206. doi: 10.1007/BF02090318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Petersen AC, Crockett L, Richards M, Boxer A. A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1988;17:117–133. doi: 10.1007/BF01537962. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Petersen AC, Hamburg BA. Adolescence: A developmental approach to problems and psychopathology. Behavior Therapy. 1986;17:480–499. [Google Scholar]
  64. Pine DS, Charney DS. Children, stress, and sensitization: An integration of basic and clinical research on emotion? Biological Psychiatry. 2002;52:773–775. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01569-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Post RM. Transduction of psychosocial stress into the neurobiology of recurrent affective disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1992;149:999–1010. doi: 10.1176/ajp.149.8.999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Post RM. Mechanisms underlying the evolution of affective disorders: Implications for long-term treatment. In: Grunhaus L, Greden JF, editors. Severe depressive disorders. Progress in psychiatry. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. pp. 23–65. [Google Scholar]
  67. Post RM, Rubinow DR, Ballenger JC. Conditioning, sensitization, and kindling implications for the course of affective illness. In: Post RM, Ballenger JC, editors. Neurobiology of mood disorders. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins; 1984. pp. 432–466. [Google Scholar]
  68. Robinson NS, Garber J, Hilsman R. Cognitions and stress: Direct and moderating effects on depressive versus externalizing symptoms during the junior high school transition. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1995;104:453–463. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.104.3.453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Rose DT, Abramson LY. Developmental predictors of depressive cognitive style: Research and theory. In: Cicchetti D, Toth SL, editors. Developmental perspectives on depression. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press; 1992. pp. 323–349. [Google Scholar]
  70. Rose A, Rudolph KD. A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: Potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132:98–131. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.98. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Rudolph KD. Gender differences in emotional responses to interpersonal stress during adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2002;30:3–13. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(01)00383-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Rudolph KD, Hammen CL. Age and gender as determinants of stress exposure, generation, and reactions in youngsters: A transactional perspective. Child Development. 1999;70:660–677. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Rudolph KD, Hammen C, Burge D, Lindberg N, Herzberg D, Daley SE. Toward an interpersonal life-stress model of depression: The developmental context of stress generation. Development and Psychopathology. 2000;12:215–234. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400002066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Rudolph KD, Hammen C, Daley SE. Mood disorders. In: Mash EJ, Wolfe DA, editors. Behavioral and emotional disorders in adolescents. New York: Guilford; 2005. pp. 300–342. [Google Scholar]
  75. Rudolph KD, Kurlakowsky KD, Conley CS. Developmental and social contextual origins of depressive control-related beliefs and behavior. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2001;25:447–475. [Google Scholar]
  76. Rudolph KD, Laurent J, Joiner T, Catanzaro S, Lambert SM, Osborne L, et al. Development and validation of the Youth Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire (Y-MASQ) 2006 Manuscript in preparation. [Google Scholar]
  77. Sameroff A. Transactional models in early social relations. Human Development. 1975;18:65–79. [Google Scholar]
  78. Sameroff AJ. The social context of development. In: Eisenberg N, editor. Contemporary topics in developmental psychology. New York, NY: Wiley; 1987. pp. 273–291. [Google Scholar]
  79. Sanchez MM, Ladd CO, Plotsky PM. Early adverse experience as a developmental risk factor for later psychopathology: Evidence from rodent and primate models. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:419–450. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401003029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Segal ZV, Williams JM, Teasdale JD, Gemar M. A cognitive science perspective on kindling and episode sensitization in recurrent affective disorder. Psychological Medicine. 1996;26:371–380. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700034760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Siegel JM, Yancey AK, Aneshensel CS, Schuler R. Body image, perceived pubertal timing, and adolescent mental health. Journal of Adolescent Health. 1999;25:155–165. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(98)00160-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Simmons RG, Blyth DA. Moving into adolescence: The impact of pubertal change and school context. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  83. Suomi SJ. Adolescent depression and depressive symptoms: Insights from longitudinal studies with rhesus monkeys. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1991;20:273–287. doi: 10.1007/BF01537612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Susman EJ, Rogol A. Puberty and psychological development. In: Lerner R, Steinberg L, editors. Handbook of Adolescent Psychology. New York: Wiley; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  85. Turner JE, Cole DA. Development differences in cognitive diatheses for child depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1994;22:15–32. doi: 10.1007/BF02169254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Watson D, Weber K, Assenheimer JS, Clark LA, Strauss ME, McCormick RA. Testing a tripartite model: I. Evaluating the convergent and discriminant validity of anxiety and depression symptom scales. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1995;104:3–14. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.104.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Wichstrom L. The emergence of gender difference in depressed mood during adolescence: The role of intensified gender socialization. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35:232–245. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Zubin J, Spring B. Vulnerability: A new view of schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1977;86:103–126. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.86.2.103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES