Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Sep 16.
Published in final edited form as: Psychiatr Q. 2011 Sep;82(3):191–206. doi: 10.1007/s11126-010-9160-0

Psychiatric Correlates of Behavioral Indicators of School Disengagement in the United States

Michael G Vaughn 1,, Jade Wexler 2, Kevin M Beaver 3, Brian E Perron 4, Gregory Roberts 5, Qiang Fu 6
PMCID: PMC3174864  NIHMSID: NIHMS322902  PMID: 20957435

Abstract

The current study examined relations between behavioral indicators of school disengagement and psychiatric disorders. Data was derived from a nationally representative sample of U.S. adults (N = 43,093). Indicators of school disengagement and diagnoses of personality, substance use, mood, and anxiety disorders were assessed with the Alcohol Use Disorder and Associated Disabilities Interview Schedule-DSM-IV-version. Findings from multinomial logistic regression analyses revealed that cumulative school disengagement is associated with increased odds of reporting a lifetime psychiatric disorder and general antisociality. Behavioral indicators of school disengagement such as absenteeism and cutting class are potentially important signs of psychiatric distress and conduct problems. In addition to attending to academic achievement outcomes school disengagement prevention strategies should consider targeting these psychiatric conditions in order to reduce school dropout.

Keywords: School dropout, School disengagement, Mental health, Comorbidity, Antisocial behavior

Introduction

The process of school disengagement not only culminates in school dropout but is also an indicator of subsequent life problems [1, 2]. School disengagement and dropout is a national problem that impacts society in a host of ways. High rates of student dropout lead to an economic drain on society [3, 4]. Currently, many jobs depend on having highly educated employees who can deal with complex issues such as increasing communication and technological demands. Students who drop out of high school thus lack appropriate academic and social skills and may not be able to deal with these complex issues or participate successfully in society. Dropouts from American high schools class of 2006, for example, cost the nation more than $309 billion in lost wages, taxes, and productivity over their lifetimes [5]. High school drop outs are more likely to have extra needs such as relying on government health care and related services, thus contributing to a drain on the economy. Overall, dropping out of middle and high school has become a large national concern with broad economic consequences, enhancing the need for educators and policy-makers to effectively and efficiently intervene.

Prior research on school dropout shows that it is typically a gradual process of disengaging or disconnecting from school both physically and mentally [6]. The process of disengagement starts early in the kindergarten years through a phase of “withdrawal,” intensifies during fourth to seventh grade with a “disengagement” phase, and results in many students dropping out of school by grade ten [7]. School disengagement has been viewed as synonymous with delinquency and conduct problems since the two often co-occur [8-11]. Although a number of risk factors for school disengagement have been identified such as academic performance (i.e., poor grades stemming from low literacy or verbal ability) and risk factors related to family or social reasons (i.e., students become parents, have to get a job to support their families, or have criminal parents), and dispositional factors (e.g., level of self-directedness) [12-16] it seems likely that school disengagement would be highly comorbid with several behavioral and psychiatric disorders. The impact of several life stressors likely can exacerbate the behavioral and psychiatric disorders. For example, when students transition from the elementary grades to the middle and high school grades, they confront many new academic and social challenges [7]. Academically, they must comprehend and learn a variety of content with complex vocabulary and challenging expository text. To keep up with these demands, they must possess at least average level literacy, verbal, and problem-solving skills. Socially, they may have difficulty fitting in with peers and often face an array of challenges at home. All of these factors may exacerbate the conjoint and intertwined risk of school disengagement and psychiatric comorbidity.

Despite the significance of school disengagement there have been few studies of school disengagement in relation to psychopathology and behavioral functioning using large representative samples. In a study of substance involvement and school dropout using the National Longitudinal Study of Young Adults, researchers Mensch and Kandel found that cigarette and illicit drug use, even after controlling for other risk factors, increased the probability of disengagement from school [17]. A 25 year prospective study employing a sample of 953 adults tracked since first grade found that alcohol dependence was predicted by early school disengagement and dropout [18]. Clearly, there is a gap in the research literature on the relationship between school disengagement and psychiatric comorbidity and filling this gap is necessary to shed light on these relations in order to inform future prevention efforts and inform policy.

Study Purpose

Although we realize that many processes such as academic achievement and cognitive processes are involved in disengagement from school, the focus of the present study is on behavioral indicators (absenteeism, cutting class) of school disengagement. Our purpose was to examine the sociodemographic, psychiatric, and substance use correlates of behavioral indicators of school disengagement using a nationally representative sample of U.S. adults. The primary aims were to (1) compare adults with a lifetime history of school disengagement to individuals without such a history with respect to sociodemographic variables, childhood and adult antisocial behaviors, and lifetime mood, anxiety, substance use, and personality disorders, and (2) to estimate the strength of the associations between these variables and indicators of school disengagement in controlled multivariate analyses. Two hypotheses were advanced. First, we hypothesized that there would be a gradient-based effect whereby increases in school disengagement would be associated with increases in the prevalence of antisocial behaviors and comorbid psychiatric disorders. Second, we hypothesized that school disengagment would be associated with externalizing disorders (i.e., substance use disorders and antisocial personality disorder) even after controlling for sociodemographic characteristics and lifetime psychiatric disorders. Although we realize that many processes such as academic achievement and cognitive processes are involved in disengagement from school, the focus of the present study is on behavioral indicators (absenteeism, cutting class) of school disengagement.

Method

Participants

Study findings are based on data from the 2001–2002 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC). NESARC is a nationally representative sample of 43,093 non-institutionalized U.S. residents aged 18 years and older [19]. The survey gathered background data and extensive information about a wide range of behaviors including substance use and comorbid psychiatric disorders, including personality disorders, from individuals living in households and group settings such as shelters, college dormitories, and group homes in all 50 states and the District of Columbia. NESARC utilized a multistage cluster sampling design, oversampling young adults, Hispanics, and African-Americans in the interest of obtaining reliable statistical estimation in these subpopulations, and to ensure appropriate representation of racial/ethnic subgroups. The overall response rate was 81%. Data were weighted at the individual and household levels to adjust for oversampling and non-response on demographic variables (i.e., age, race/ethnicity, sex, region, and place of residence). Data were also adjusted to be representative (based on region, age, race, and ethnicity) of the U.S. adult population as assessed during the 2000 Census. Study participants provided fully informed consent and The U.S. Census Bureau and the U.S. Office of Management and Budget approved the research protocol and informed consent procedures.

Diagnostic Assessment and Sociodemographic Measures

Data were collected through face-to-face structured psychiatric interviews conducted by U.S. Census workers trained by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism and U.S. Census Bureau. Interviewers administered the Alcohol Use Disorder and Associated Disabilities Interview Schedule—DSM-IV version (AUDADIS-IV), which in addition to extensive background and sociodemographic data provides diagnoses for mood, anxiety, personality, and substance use disorders. The AUDADIS IV has shown to have good-to-excellent reliability in assessing alcohol and drug use in the general population [20, 21].

The lifetime prevalence of school disengagement was assessed with three items embedded in the conduct disorder and part of the antisocial behavior interview module. All NESARC participants were asked the following questions: “In your entire life, did you ever often cut class, not go to class or go to school and leave without permission?”, “In your entire life, did you ever have a time when you were often absent from school, other than when caring for someone who was sick?” and “In your entire life, did you ever more than once quit a school program without knowing what you would do next?” NESARC respondents who did not answer yes to any of these three items were defined as engaged. Respondents who answered yes to one of these items were defined as being moderately disengaged, and those answering yes to two or three items were considered severely disengaged. The test–retest reliability for the antisocial personality disorder diagnosis was adequate (r = 0.69) [19]. The internal consistency reliability for the entire antisocial personality disorder criterion set was also good (α = 0.86) [22].

Consistent with current conceptualizations of personality disorders, including DSM-IV [23-25], personality disorder diagnoses reflected long-standing impairments, characteristic patterns of behavior, and exclusion of cases where substance use intoxication or withdrawal, other medication use, or physical illnesses could have contributed to reported Axis II personality disorder signs and symptoms. In addition to antisocial personality disorder, other personality disorders assessed included avoidant, dependent, obsessive–compulsive, paranoid, schizoid, and histrionic disorders. Family history of antisocial behavior based on any parental or sibling history of antisocial behavior was also assessed. Response categories for region of residence in U.S., urbanicity, race/ethnicity, sex, age, marital status, educational background, unemployment status, and individual and family income are listed in Table 1.

Table 1.

Sociodemographic characteristics of individuals with severe, moderate, and no history of school disengagement

Characteristic Engaged Moderate disengagement Severe disengagement Moderate disengagement Odds ratiob 95% CI Severe disengagement Odds ratiob 95% CI
N = 31755 % (CI)a N = 8546 % (CI) N = 1018 % (CI)
Sex
 Men 44.88 (44.18–45.59) 56.65 (55.30–57.99) 57.18 (52.88–61.37) 1.35 (1.26–1.44) 1.35 (1.11–1.65)
 Women 55.12 (54.41–55.82) 43.35 (42.01–44.70) 31.22 (22.44–41.59) 1.00 1.00
Race
 White 71.43 (68.03–74.61) 70.28 (67.18–73.20) 68.44 (63.46–73.04) 0.81 (0.70–0.93) 0.90 (0.47–1.73)
 African American 10.69 (9.48–12.02) 11.91 (10.51–13.46) 12.38 (9.66–15.73) 0.99 (0.85–1.15) 0.58 (0.25–1.36)
 Native American 1.80 (1.53–2.11) 2.93 (2.39–3.57) 3.82 (2.50–5.79) 0.95 (0.74–1.22) 1.03 (0.30–3.51)
 Indian/Alaska/Asian/Hawaiian Pacific 4.78 (3.73–6.12) 2.84 (2.21–3.64) 3.11 (1.80–5.33) 0.85 (0.67–1.08) 1.24 (0.37–4.15)
 Hispanic 11.30 (9.03–14.05) 12.05 (9.63–14.97) 12.26 (9.31–15.98) 1.00 1.00
Nativity
 Born in the U.S. 83.94 (80.34–87.00) 90.11 (88.05–91.85) 92.54 (89.10–94.95) 1.79 (1.52–1.98) 1.03 (0.41–2.57)
 Born outside the U.S. 16.06 (13.00–19.66) 9.89 (8.15–11.95) 7.46 (5.05–10.90) 1.00 1.00
Age (years)
 65+ 18.99 (18.19–19.82) 8.14 (7.44–8.89) 2.77 (1.80–4.25) 0.38 (0.34–0.44) 4.03 (1.71–9.52)
 50–64 22.77 (22.15–23.39) 16.21 (15.24–17.23) 10.26 (8.22–12.74) 0.51 (0.46–0.56) 3.02 (1.49–6.14)
 35–49 30.33 (29.57–31.09) 32.89 (31.57–34.240 36.57 (32.64–40.69) 0.71 (0.65–0.77) 1.42 (0.81–2.49)
 18–34 27.92 (27.01–28.84) 42.76 (41.27–44.26) 50.40 (46.34–54.45) 1.00 1.00
Education
 Less than high school 14.21 (13.17–15.31) 17.40 (16.12–18.76) 31.34 (27.43–35.54) 1.80 (1.60–2.01) 2.88 (1.53–5.43)
 High school graduate 28.95 (27.77–30.16) 31.30 (29.75–32.89) 26.55 (23.14–30.27) 1.31 (1.21–1.42) 2.72 (1.51–4.88)
 Some college 56.84 (55.56–58.11) 51.30 (49.46–53.14) 42.11 (37.70–46.65) 1.00 1.00
Income
 <19,999 22.98 (22.01–23.98) 23.76 (22.41–25.16) 29.52 (26.02–33.21) 1.00 (0.89–1.12) 1.62 (0.71–3.68)
 20,000–34,999 19.64 (18.95–21.34) 21.34 (20.16–22.56) 25.06 (21.46–29.03) 1.06 (0.95–1.18) 0.98 (0.39–2.46)
 35,000–69,999 32.33 (31.57–33.10) 32.26 (31.11–33.44) 30.88 (26.83–35.24) 0.99 (0.91–1.09) 0.76 (0.31–1.90)
 70,000+ 25.05 (23.63–26.52) 22.64 (20.97–24.40) 14.55 (11.46–18.30) 1.00 1.00
Marital status
 Never married 18.55 (17.63–19.50) 26.80 (25.28–28.38) 34.09 (30.20–38.21) 1.07 (0.98–1.16) 0.76 (0.44–1.33)
 Widowed/separated/divorced 17.92 (17.42–18.42) 15.17 (14.32–16.07) 29.16 (21.42–38.34) 0.97 (0.89–1.05) 1.79 (0.99–3.22)
 Married/cohabitating 63.54 (62.59–64.48) 58.03 (56.51–59.53) 50.76 (46.38–55.14) 1.00 1.00
Urbanicity
 Urban 28.71 (24.50–33.32) 31.30 (27.38–35.50) 30.76 (25.07–37.10) 0.08 (1.00–1.17) 1.49 (0.95–2.35)
 Rural 71.29 (66.68–75.50) 68.70 (64.50–72.62) 69.24 (62.90–74.93) 1.00 1.00
Region
 Northeast 19.99 (13.95–27.78) 18.77 (13.11–26.14) 19.28 (12.73–28.11) 0.91 (0.81–1.03) 0.84 (0.46–1.52)
 Midwest 23.17 (17.32–30.26) 23.38 (17.91–29.91) 25.65 (18.30–34.70) 0.80 (0.71–0.89) 0.43 (0.22–0.85)
 South 35.74 (29.41–42.61) 33.43 (27.53–39.91) 30.23 (23.67–37.71) 0.83 (0.74–0.92) 1.14 (0.64–2.04)
 West 21.10 (14.92–28.97) 24.42 (18.15–32.02) 24.84 (17.32–34.26) 1.00 1.00
a

CI confidence interval

b

OR odds ratio. OR values in bold are statistically significant based on a 95% CI that does not bound 1.0

c

Engaged individuals is the reference group for regression analysis

Statistical Analyses

Weighted prevalence estimates and standard errors were computed using SUDAAN Version 9.0 [26]. This software implements a Taylor series linearization to adjust standard errors of estimates for complex survey sampling design effects including clustered data. Cross tabulations were conducted with moderate and severe disengagement categories and sociodemographic variables and violent and non-violent antisocial behaviors. Multivariate multinomial logistic regression analyses were executed to assess the relationship of indicators of school disengagement to each psychiatric disorder while controlling for sociodemographic covariates and lifetime psychiatric diagnoses. Specifically, control variables used to reduce confounding included lifetime alcohol (alcohol abuse/dependence) and drug (abuse/dependence on heroin, hallucinogens, cocaine/crack, marijuana, stimulants, painkillers, tranquilizers, and sedatives) use disorders, nicotine dependence, pathological gambling, and lifetime DSM-IV mood (major depression, dysthymia, and bipolar disorder) and anxiety (social phobia, generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder, and specific phobia) disorders. Adjusted odds ratios (AORs) and 95% confidence intervals are presented to reflect association strength. Adjusted odds ratios were considered statistically significant only if associated confidence intervals did not include the value 1.0.

Results

Sociodemographic Characteristics across Categories of School Disengagement Severity

Table 1 displays sociodemographic characteristics of adults with a lifetime history of school engagement compared to persons who reported a lifetime history of moderate disengagement, and severe disengagement. Compared to engaged persons, those reporting a lifetime history of moderate and severe disengagement were more likely to be men (moderate OR = 1.35, 95% CI = 1.26–1.44, severe OR = 1.35, 95% CI = 1.11–1.65), born in the U.S. (moderate OR = 1.79, 95% CI = 1.52–1.98), and were uniformly more likely to be younger in age. There were little racial and ethnic differences, however, White individuals (moderate OR = 0.81, 95% CI = 0.70–0.93) were less likely to report moderate disengagement compared to other racial and ethnic groups. With respect to educational attainment, persons with a high school education (moderate OR = 1.31, 95% CI = 1.21–1.42, severe OR = 2.72, 95% CI = 1.51–4.88) or less than a high school education (moderate OR = 1.80, 95% CI = 1.60–2.01, severe OR = 2.88, 95% CI = 1.53–5.43) were more likely to report moderate and severe school disengagement compared to persons with some college education. Compared to persons from the western region of the U.S., individuals from the midwest (moderate OR = 0.80, 95% CI = 0.71–0.89, severe OR = 0.43, 95% CI = 0.22–0.85) and south (moderate OR = 0.83, 95% CI = 0.74–0.92) were significantly less likely to report a lifetime history of forms of school disengagement. There were no significant differences with respect to income and marital status.

School Disengagement and Associated Antisocial Behaviors

A consistent graded relationship was observed across the levels of engagement (See Table 2). That is, engaged respondents exhibited the lowest rates of antisocial behaviors, followed by those with a lifetime history of moderate disengagement. Respondents with severe disengagement exhibited substantially higher rates than the other two groups. Specifically, the prevalence of antisocial behaviors was typically five to ten times greater for respondents reporting a lifetime history of severe disengagement from school compared to respondents with no such history. Most prevalent behaviors were staying out late (70.67%, 95% CI = 66.40–74.60%), quitting a job without knowing where to find another (54.41%, 95% CI = 50.24–58.52%), and doing something that you could be arrested for (51.29%, 95% CI = 46.44–56.12%). Aggression and violent behaviors were also relatively high among the severely disengaged. For example, bullying or pushing others around (27.49%, 95% CI = 23.67–31.68%), getting into fight that came to swapping blows with husband/wife or boyfriend/girlfriend (25.56%, 95% CI = 22.22–29.21%), and hitting someone so hard that you injured them (21.14%, 95% CI = 17.62–25.15%). The least prevalent behavior was forcing someone to have sex (0.76%, 95% CI = 0.34–1.70%).

Table 2.

Associated antisocial behaviors of individuals with moderate, severe, and no history of disengagement

Behavior Engaged Moderate disengagement Severe disengagement χ2 P-value
N = 31,755 % (CI) N = 8546 % (CI) N = 1018 % (CI)
Violent
 Force someone to have sex 0.08 (0.05–0.14) 0.26 (0.16–0.41) 0.76 (0.34–1.70) 6.49 0.003
 Get into lots of fights that you started 1.15 (1.00–1.31) 6.69 (6.03–7.41) 16.69 (13.23–20.84) 58.48 <0.001
 Swapping blows with Husband/Wife or Boyfriend/Girlfriend 4.21 (3.85–4.60) 12.83 (11.95–13.77) 25.56 (22.22–29.21) 70.36 <0.001
 Use a weapon in a fight 1.30 (1.14–1.48) 6.24 (5.63–6.91) 13.20 (10.77–16.08) 53.60 <0.001
 Hit someone so hard that you injure them 3.32 (3.00–3.66) 13.55 (12.66–14.50) 21.14 (17.62–25.15) 73.45 <0.001
 Harass/threaten/blackmail someone 0.65 (0.54–0.80) 4.40 (3.83–5.04) 11.04 (8.43–14.33) 50.40 <0.001
 Bully/push people 3.12 (2.81–3.46) 14.65 (13.67–15.68) 27.49 (23.67–31.68) 72.55 <0.001
 Hurt an animal on purpose 1.22 (1.06–1.41) 3.34 (2.88–3.87) 6.40 (4.73–8.62) 30.88 <0.001
 Rob/mug someone or snatch a purse 0.08 (0.05–0.13) 0.82 (0.61–1.10) 1.71 (1.02–2.88) 19.62 <0.001
 Do things that could have easily hurt you/others 9.65 (8.83–10.53) 26.40 (24.82–28.04) 42.26 (37.71–46.96) 87.44 <0.001
 Physically hurt others on purpose 2.82 (2.54–3.13) 11.09 (10.18–12.07) 19.07 (16.25–22.24) 58.80 <0.001
Non-Violent
 Stay out late at night 16.64 (15.77–17.54) 52.85 (51.57–54.12) 70.67 (66.40–74.60) 81.84 <0.001
 Run away from home overnight 2.16 (1.95–2.39) 12.66 (11.73–13.65) 26.69 (23.64–29.98) 73.37 <0.001
 Quit a job without knowing where to find another 6.45 (6.00–6.93) 23.54 (22.22–24.91) 54.41 (50.24–58.52) 82.15 <0.001
 Travel around more than 1 month without plan 1.70 (1.52–1.90) 6.88 (6.22–7.59) 20.47 (17.32–24.02) 62.34 <0.001
 Have no regular place to live at least 1 month 1.24 (1.07–1.44) 5.99 (5.41–6.61) 18.37 (14.82–22.55) 61.38 <0.001
 Live with others at least 1 month 7.35 (6.73–8.02) 20.47 (19.39–21.58) 42.69 (38.66–46.80) 80.82 <0.001
 Lie a lot 2.13 (1.91–2.37) 13.15 (12.23–14.12) 31.60 (27.75–35.73) 67.32 <0.001
 Use a false or made up name/alias 0.92 (0.80–1.07) 5.23 (4.64–5.90) 12.96 (10.51–15.89) 46.50 <0.001
 Scam/con someone for money 0.41 (0.33–0.52) 4.05 (3.51–4.66) 12.88 (9.90–16.60) 41.72 <0.001
 Get three or more traffic tickets for reckless driving/causing accidents 6.26 (5.70–6.89) 15.67 (14.49–16.92) 21.58 (18.47–25.04) 58.11 <0.001
 Have a driver’s license suspended/revoked 5.15 (4.72–5.61) 15.42 (14.34–16.56) 21.47 (18.43–24.85) 74.35 <0.001
 Destroy others’ property 1.72 (1.52–1.95) 8.83 (8.00–9.74) 20.04 (17.06–23.39) 61.75 <0.001
 Fail to pay off your debts 2.29 (2.04–2.56) 8.71 (7.93–9.55) 19.99 (16.83–23.57) 57.88 <0.001
 Steal anything from others 5.41 (4.95–5.91) 18.55 (17.46–19.69) 34.85 (30.83–39.10) 72.23 <0.001
 Forge someone’s signature 1.10 (0.94–1.28) 4.93 (4.36–5.57) 10.30 (8.13–12.98) 42.42 <0.001
 Shoplift 6.70 (6.13–7.32) 24.03 (22.74–25.36) 39.97 (35.28–44.85) 73.17 <0.001
 Make money illegally 0.94 (0.80–1.10) 7.49 (6.72–8.33) 16.86 (14.06–20.07) 60.91 <0.001
 Do something you could have been arrested for 9.81 (9.06–10.61) 31.49 (29.95–33.06) 51.29 (46.44–56.12) 81.82 <0.001
 Set a fire on purpose 0.50 (0.40–0.62) 2.64 (2.22–3.13) 6.19 (4.58–8.32) 36.12 <0.001

Multivariate Multinomial Logistic Regression Analysis Examining Associations between School Disengagement Severity and Lifetime Psychiatric Comorbidity

Table 3 summarizes results from twenty-two multinomial logistic regression models that compare prevalence rates of lifetime psychiatric comorbidity for persons reporting moderate and severe levels of school disengagement. Recall that odds ratios are adjusted for sociodemographic factors (i.e., race, sex, education, marital status, age, income, region, and urbanicity), previously described lifetime DSM-IV psychiatric disorders, and family history of antisocial behavior. Across comorbid psychiatric disorders, there was a gradient-based response in that severe disengagement was associated with increased probability over and above moderate disengagement. The most prevalent psychiatric disorders among persons with a history of severe school disengagement was any lifetime alcohol use disorder (60.63%, CI = 56.34–64.75%), nicotine dependence (45.40%, CI = 40.97–49.90%), and major depressive disorder (40.52%, CI = 36.54–44.62%). A family history of antisocial behavior was also highly prevalent of the severely disengaged (52.46%, CI = 47.81–57.08%). Following adjustments, significant associations were found for several substance use disorders including nicotine dependence (moderate and severe), marijuana use disorder (moderate and severe), any alcohol use disorder (moderate and severe), and any drug use disorder (moderate and severe). Notably, severely disengaged persons were nearly three times (OR = 2.71, 95%CI = 2.11–3.49) more likely than engaged to possess an alcohol use disorder. Both moderate and severely disengaged were more likely to be diagnosed with major depression, bipolar disorder, and specific phobia. With respect to behavior and personality, a family history of antisocial behavior was more likely among moderate and severely disengaged along with antisocial personality disorder, which was large in effect for both moderate (OR = 3.90, 95% CI = 3.13–4.84) and severe (OR = 6.32, 95% CI = 4.70–8.49) groups. Finally, histrionic personality disorder was significantly more likely among the severely disengaged but not the moderately disengaged.

Table 3.

Associations of lifetime psychiatric comorbidity of individuals with severe, moderate, and no history of school disengagement

Comorbid psychiatric disorder Engaged Moderate disengagement Severe disengagement Multinomial regressionc
N = 31,755
% (CI)a
N = 8546
% (CI)
N = 1018
% (CI)
Moderate disengagement Odds ratiob 95% (CI) Severe disengagement Odds ratiob 95% (CI)
Nicotine dependence 13.15 (12.30–14.06) 31.40 (29.76–33.08) 45.40 (40.97–49.90) 1.62 (1.48–1.77) 1.68 (1.31–2.16)
Marijuana use disorder 4.68 (4.26–5.13) 18.76 (17.63–19.95) 37.76 (33.33–42.39) 1.62 (1.43–1.85) 2.71 (2.11–3.49)
Any alcohol use disorder 24.01 (22.60–25.48) 49.17 (47.27–51.08) 60.63 (56.34–64.75) 1.77 (1.64–1.91) 1.81 (1.46–2.25)
Any drug use disorder 2.68 (2.41–2.99) 12.16 (11.12–13.30) 25.54 (22.29–29.08) 1.34 (1.14–1.57) 1.54 (1.22–1.94)
Mood disorder
 Major depressive disorder 13.74 (13.06–14.44) 24.64 (23.46–25.85) 40.52 (36.54–44.62) 1.26 (1.14–1.38) 1.65 (1.31–2.07)
 Bipolar disorder 3.46 (3.17–3.78) 11.69 (10.83–12.60) 23.39 (19.95–27.22) 1.55 (1.33–1.81) 1.66 (1.26–2.18)
 Dysthymia 3.25 (3.00–3.53) 6.96 (6.34–7.64) 13.16 (10.67–16.13) 1.08 (0.93–1.27) 1.11 (0.79–1.57)
Anxiety disorder
 Panic disorder 3.35 (3.07–3.65) 5.87 (5.32–6.47) 9.03 (6.82–11.86) 1.05 (0.90–1.23) 0.98 (0.69–1.40)
 Social phobia 4.07 (3.72–4.46) 7.35 (6.61–8.15) 16.89 (13.86–20.42) 0.91 (0.78–1.07) 1.23 (0.91–1.67)
 Specific phobia 8.14 (7.59–8.73) 13.13 (12.13–14.19) 22.85 (19.50–26.57) 1.12 (1.01–1.24) 1.30 (1.02–1.65)
 Generalized anxiety disorder 3.32 (3.03–3.64) 6.30 (5.61–7.07) 12.90 (9.79–16.81) 1.02 (0.85–1.23) 1.10 (0.72–1.68)
Conduct disorder 0.58 (0.47–0.72) 2.73 (2.26–3.31) 1.77 (1.06–2.97) 3.54 (2.53–4.97) 2.19 (1.21–3.93)
Family history of antisocial behaviors 18.59 (17.62–19.61) 34.65 (33.00–36.34) 52.46 (47.81–57.08) 1.44 (1.33–1.57) 1.98 (1.59–2.47)
Psychotic disorder 0.53 (0.44–0.62) 1.24 (0.97–1.58) 3.92 (2.36–6.43) 0.89 (0.60–1.31) 1.01 (0.42–2.42)
Personality disorder
 Avoidant 1.49 (1.33–1.67) 4.32 (3.76–4.95) 11.60 (8.78–15.17) 1.25 (0.99–1.56) 1.40 (0.92–2.12)
 Dependent 0.26 (0.20–0.34) 0.83 (0.62–1.10) 4.34 (2.90–6.44) 1.06 (0.68–1.63) 1.65 (0.85–3.20)
 Obsessive–compulsive 6.20 (5.77–6.67) 12.63 (18.15–26.54) 22.06 (18.15–26.54) 1.23 (1.07–1.41) 1.33 (0.97–1.83)
 Paranoid 2.87 (2.61–3.14) 8.15 (7.42–8.96) 19.43 (16.09–23.28) 1.11 (0.94–1.31) 1.15 (0.81–1.63)
 Schizoid 2.13 (1.93–2.36) 5.67 (5.09–6.32) 11.96 (9.21–15.38) 1.19 (0.98–1.43) 1.19 (0.84–1.67)
 Antisocial 0.98 (0.84–1.15) 10.11 (9.21–11.09) 25.98 (22.36–29.97) 3.90 (3.13–4.84) 6.32 (4.70–8.49)
 Histrionic 1.03 (0.90–1.17) 3.61 (3.13–4.15) 11.80 (9.25–14.94) 1.15 (0.91–1.47) 1.83 (1.24–2.69)
a

CI confidence interval

b

OR odds ratio adjusted for sociodemographic variables, lifetime psychiatric disorders, and a family history of antisocial behavior. OR values in bold are statistically significant based on a 95% confidence interval that does not bound 1.0

c

Engaged individuals is the reference group for regression analysis

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the largest national epidemiological study examining the association between behavioral indicators of school disengagement and psychiatric diagnoses. Findings supported the two hypotheses tested—that there would be a gradient-based effect whereby increases in school disengagement would be associated with increases in the prevalence of antisocial behaviors and comorbid psychiatric disorders and that school disengagement would be associated with externalizing disorders (i.e., substance use disorders and antisocial personality disorder) even after controlling for sociodemographic characteristics and lifetime psychiatric disorders. With respect to sociodemographic patterns, the current investigation found that young males living in the Western region of the U.S. were more likely to be disengaged from school. There persons were also more likely to not have finished high school or go to college and were slightly more likely to be from urban areas. Notably, there were little differences with respect to race/ethnicity and income. One additional empirical trend was that persons born in the U.S. were nearly twice as likely as persons born outside of the U.S. to report school disengagement. This finding suggests that there may be something about American culture that may promote disengagement from school.

An additional finding is that both moderate and severe school disengagement is associated with numerous antisocial behaviors such as getting into numerous physical altercations, property destruction, lying, cruelty to animals, stealing, and harassment. As such, school disengagement can be seen if not as a marker for potential antisocial behavior phenotypes including conduct disorder and antisocial personality disorder, but part and parcel of these syndromes. Multivariate analyses, controlling for sociodemographic, lifetime psychiatric disorders, and family history of antisocial behavior, demonstrated that individuals with a lifetime history of severe school disengagement were approximately six times more likely to possess a antisocial personality disorder diagnosis than their school engaged peers. These findings support general theoretical viewpoints which suggest that school disengagement is part of a cumulative process of conduct problems which often result in dropout [11].

There was significant comorbidity between school disengagement and alcohol use disorder, cannabis use disorder, nicotine dependence, and any drug use disorder. These effects were relatively modest (odds ratios ranging from 1.34 to 1.81) except for alcohol use disorder with severely disengaged individuals being nearly three times more likely than engaged individuals to possess this disorder. This finding is noteworthy since it converges with prior research that showed a strong association with alcohol dependence over long periods of time [18].

There was also significant comorbidity between school disengagement and two mood disorders, major depression and bipolar disorder, and one anxiety disorder, specific phobia. To our knowledge, no other studies have documented these internalizing disorders in relation to school disengagement in a nationally representative sample. Bipolar disorder can be associated with significant affective lability including rage responses, difficulty getting along socially with others, and problems with task completion. The association with major depression, although a consequence rather than a precursor to school disengagement, impacts the motivational requirements needed to finish school and we speculate that aspect increments the probability of disengaging from school. Following adjustments, a significant association was also found for histrionic personality disorder. Histrionic personality disorder, often characterized by flagrantly provocative behavior, could serve to place individuals at higher risk for school disengagement perhaps via the stimulation of harsh counterresponses from school administrations.

Despite the serious consequences of school disengagement, there has been relatively few rigorous dropout prevention intervention studies conducted to confirm practices associated with effective dropout prevention. For example, in 2002 the Institute of Education Sciences created the What Works Clearinghouse (http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc) as a central source of scientific evidence of what works in education. Of the 11 dropout prevention programs cited on the What Works Clearinghouse, Check & Connect is the intervention cited as having the highest impact for helping students stay in school and is one of the most widely used dropout prevention/school engagement programs [27, 28]. Although Check & Connect is widely used and cited as a dropout prevention program with strong potential for decreasing dropout rates and increasing engagement in school, this intervention program has undergone only minimal rigorous experimental evaluation. Check & Connect has only two experimental studies associated with positive outcomes, and both of these studies were conducted in one particular area in the north west, targeting one specific population (primarily African American males receiving special education services), indicating a clear need for replication with other populations, as well as a need to identify other potentially effective intervention practices.

Because dropping out of school typically results from a gradual process of disengagement and based on our knowledge of likely correlates of indicators of school disengagement, it may be possible to not only establish risk profiles to better identify students most at risk for dropout [14], but it may also be possible to design dropout prevention interventions to target these areas of need. Additional protective factors for minority youth such as religiosity should also be evaluated [29-31]. Conducting rigorous experimental studies of dropout prevention to identify practices most associated with effective dropout prevention is essential to confirm practices that will keep more students engaged and in school.

Limitations

Study results require interpretation within the context of several limitations. First, considering the study data are cross-sectional, temporal ordering or variables does not permit firm conclusions regarding causal determinants. As such, reported findings cannot clarify the etiologic relationship between school engagement indicators and its correlates. For example, the use and abuse of drugs and alcohol may be associated with school disengagment due to its disinhibitng and harmful effects on neuroregulatory processes, thus facilitating an increase in academic problems. Conversely, the propensity to disengage from school, authority figures, and other institutions may also involve particular phenotypic characteristics that also include the propensity toward antisocial behavior in general, including alcohol and drug abuse. However, findings do suggest that school disengagement and psychopathology are intertwined. Longitudinal study designs beginning earlier in the life course that examine environmental stress in conjunction with genotypic information dynamically over time provide one way to elucidate the causal structure of school disengagement. An additional limitation is that the NESARC excludes persons under age 18 and therefore relies on retrospective respondent recall of school disengagement over potentially long periods of time. This could lead to underreporting or to biased reporting with younger respondents recalling better than older respondents. However, affirmative responses to school disengagement were also associated with lower levels of educational attainment thus demonstrating a degree of concurrent validity for these items. Although the NESARC is a nationally representative sample, it is uncertain how the association between school disengagement and psychiatric and substance use disorders would be similar or different if enriched correctional or clinical samples were used. In addition, the data on school disengagement did not include important academic and situational, data which is important to understanding the nature of school disengagement behaviors. School disengagement and dropout, although not without stigma, may be a more accepted behavior in certain contexts, particularly in areas where dropout rates are high [32]. Despite these limitations, study findings offer new and important epidemiologic insights into the problem of school disengagement in the U.S.

Acknowledgments

NESARC was funded by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism with additional support provided by the National Institute on Drug Abuse. The authors are grateful for support from NIH grants: DA021405 and K07CA104119 and partial support from the Greater Texas Foundation and the Meadows Center for Preventing Educational Risk at the University of Texas at Austin. The contents of the article are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official view of the National Institutes of Health.

Biographies

Michael G. Vaughn, PhD is currently Assistant Professor in the School of Social Work and Holds appointments in Public Policy and the Department of Community Health, Division of Epidemiology, Saint Louis University School of Public Health. His research interests include juvenile psychopathy, school dropout, adolescent substance abuse, self-regulation, and violence.

Jade Wexler, PhD is a senior research associate at the Meadows Center for Preventing Educational Risk. Her current research includes investigating effective response to intervention practices for older students with reading disabilities and effective methods to decrease dropout rates and increase school engagement for students at risk for dropping out of school.

Kevin M. Beaver, PhD is an assistant professor in the College of Criminology and Criminal Justice at Florida State University. His research focuses on the biosocial underpinnings to antisocial behaviors.

Brian E. Perron, PhD is an Assistant Professor at the University of Michigan, School of Social Work. His research focuses on issues related to the serious mental illnesses and substance use disorders. He is involved in a variety of research activities, including analysis of nationally representative data and clinic-based surveys, and collaborating on field-based interventions.

Greg Roberts, PhD is Director of the Vaughn Gross Center for Reading and Language Arts, Associate Director of the Meadows Center for Preventing Educational Risk, and Center Director for Evaluation and Analysis. His research interests include statistical modeling of the effects of reading interventions.

Qiang Fu, PhD is an Associate Professor in Community Health and the Director of Biostatistics Division at Saint Louis University School of Public Health. His research interests include latent variable analysis, longitudinal data analysis, behavior genetics, and psychiatric epidemiology of externalizing behaviors and substance use disorders.

Contributor Information

Michael G. Vaughn, Saint Louis University, 3550 Lindell Boulevard, St. Louis, MO 63103, USA mvaughn9@slu.edu

Jade Wexler, Meadows Center for Preventing Educational Risk, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA.

Kevin M. Beaver, College of Criminology and Criminal Justice, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA

Brian E. Perron, School of Social Work, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA

Gregory Roberts, Meadows Center for Preventing Educational Risk, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA.

Qiang Fu, Division of Biostatistics, School of Public Health, Saint Louis University, St. Louis, MO, USA.

References

  • 1.Davidoff A, Kenney G. Uninsured Americans with Chronic Health Conditions: Key Findings from the National Health Interview Survey. Washington, DC: Urban Institute; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Fagan J, Piper ES, Moore M. Contributions of delinquency and substance abuse to school dropout. Youth and Society. 1990;21:306–354. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.August D, Shanahan T, editors. Developing Literacy in Second-Language Learners. Philadelphia, PA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Dynarski M, Gleason P, Rangarajan A, Wood R. Impacts of Dropout Prevention Programs. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Alliance for Excellent Education: The High Cost of High School Dropouts: What the Nation Pays for Inadequate High Schools. Washington, DC: 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Finn JD. Withdrawing from school. Review of Educational Research. 1989;59:117–142. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Balfanz R, Herzog L, Mac Iver DJ. Preventing student disengagement and keeping students on the graduation path in urban middle-grades schools: Early identification and effective interventions. Educational Psychologist. 2007;42:223–235. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Farrington DP. Early predictors of adolescent aggression and adult violence. Violence and Victims 4I. 1989:79–100. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Harlow C. Bureau of Justice Statistics Special Report. Washington DC: U.S. Department of Justice; 2003. Education and Correctional Populations. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Phillips JC, Kelly DH. School failure and delinquency: Which causes which? Criminology. 1979;17:194–207. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Sweeten G, Bushway SD, Paternoster R. Does dropping out of school mean dropping into delinquency? Criminology. 2009;47:47–88. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Alexander KL, Entwisle DR, Kabbani NS. The dropout process in life course perspective: early risk factors at home and school. Teachers College Record. 2001;103:760–822. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Cairns RB, Cairns BD, Neckerman HJ. Early school dropout: Configurations and determinants. Child Developmen. 1989;60:1437–1452. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1989.tb04015.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Gleason P, Dynarski M. Do we know whom to serve? Issues in using risk factors to identify dropouts. Journal of Education for Students Placed at Risk. 2002;7:25–41. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Rosenthal BS. Nonschool correlates of dropout: An integrative review of the Literature. Children and Youth Services Review. 1998;20:413–433. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Rumberger RW. Why Students Drop Out of School? In: Orfield G, editor. Dropouts in America: Confronting the Graduation Rate Crises. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Education Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Mensch BS, Kandel DB. Dropping out of high school and drug involvement. Sociology of Education. 1988;61:95–113. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Crum RM, Ensminger ME, Ro MJ, McCord J. The association of educational achievement and school dropout with risk of alcoholism: a twenty-five-year prospective study of inner-city children. Journal of Students on Alcohol 59I. 1998:318–326. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1998.59.318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Grant BF, Dawson DA, Stinson FS, Chou PS, Kay W, Pickering R. The alcohol use disorder and associated disabilities interview schedule-IV (AUDADIS-IV): Reliability of alcohol consumption, tobacco use, family history of depression and psychiatric diagnostic modules in a general population sample. Drug Alcohol Dependency. 2003;71:7–16. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(03)00070-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Grant BF, Harford T, Dawson DA, Chou PS, Pickering RP. The alcohol use disorder and associated disabilities interview schedule (AUDADIS): Reliability of alcohol and drug modules in a general population sample. Drug and Alcohol Dependency. 1995;39:37–44. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(95)01134-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Hasin D, Carpenter KM, McCloud S, Smith M, Grant BF. The alcohol use disorders and associated disabilities interview schedule (AUDADIS): Reliability of alcohol and drug modules in a clinical sample. Drug and Alcohol Dependency. 1997;44:133–141. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(97)01332-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Blanco C, Grant J, Petry NM, Simpson HB, Alegria A, Liu S, et al. Prevalence and correlates of shoplifting in the United States: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions (NESARC) American Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;165:905–913. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07101660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Grant BF, Hasin D, Stinson FS, Dawson DA, Chou PS, Ruan WJ, et al. Prevalence, correlates, and disability of personality disorders in the United States: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2004a;65:948–958. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v65n0711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Grant BF, Stinson FS, Hasin DS, Dawson DA, Chou SP, Ruan WJ, et al. Co-occurrence of 12-month alcohol and drug use disorders and personality disorders in the United States: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004b;61:361–368. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.4.361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Goldstein RB, Grant BF, Juan WJ, Smith SM, Saha TD. Antisocial personality disorder with childhood- vs. adolescence-onset conduct disorder: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2006;194:667–675. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000235762.82264.a1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Research Triangle Institute. Software for Survey Data Analysis, SUDAAN. Version 9.0. Research Triangle Park, NC: Research Triangle Institute; 2004. Computer software. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Sinclair MF, Christenson SL, Thurlow ML. Promoting school completion of urban secondary youth with emotional or behavioral disabilities. Exceptional Children. 2005;71:465–482. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Sinclair MF, Christenson SL, Evelo DL, Hurley CM. Dropout prevention for youth with disabilities: Efficacy of a sustained school engagement procedure. Exceptional Children. 1998;65:7–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Hodge DR. Working with Hindu clients in a spiritually sensitive manner. Social Work. 2004;49:27–38. doi: 10.1093/sw/49.1.27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hodge DR, Williams TR. Assessing African American spirituality with spiritual eco-maps. Families in Society. 2002;83:585–595. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Koenig HG, McCullough ME, Larson DB. Handbook of religion and health. New York: Oxford University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Balfanz R, Legters N. Locating the Dropout Crises. Which High Schools Produce the Nation’s Dropouts? Where are They Located? Who Attends Them? Baltimore, MD: Center for Social Organization of Schools, John Hopkins University; 2004. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES