Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Sep 19.
Published in final edited form as: Sleep Breath. 2008 Jun 13;13(1):35–41. doi: 10.1007/s11325-008-0198-2

Relationship between inflammation and cognitive function in obstructive sleep apnea

Alexander Haensel 1,2, Wayne A Bardwell 3,4, Paul J Mills 5, Jose S Loredo 6, Sonia Ancoli-Israel 7,8, Erin E Morgan 9, Robert K Heaton 10, Joel E Dimsdale 11,12
PMCID: PMC3175694  NIHMSID: NIHMS296983  PMID: 18551328

Abstract

Objectives

Obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) can have adverse effects on cognitive functioning, mood, and cardiovascular functioning. OSA brings with it disturbances in sleep architecture, oxygenation, sympathetic nervous system function, and inflammatory processes. It is not clear which of these mechanisms is linked to the decrease in cognitive functioning. This study examined the effect of inflammatory parameters on cognitive dysfunction.

Materials and methods

Thirty-nine patients with untreated sleep apnea were evaluated by polysomnography and completed a battery of neuropsychological tests. After the first night of evaluation in the sleep laboratory, blood samples were taken for analysis of interleukin 6, tumor necrosis factor-α (TNF-α), and soluble TNF receptor 1 (sTNF-R1).

Results

sTNF-R1 significantly correlated with cognitive dysfunction. In hierarchical linear regression analysis, measures of obstructive sleep apnea severity explained 5.5% of the variance in cognitive dysfunction (n.s.). After including sTNF-R1, percentage of variance explained by the full model increased more than threefold to 19.6% (F= 2.84, df=3, 36, p=0.05). Only sTNF-R1 had a significant individual relationship with cognitive dysfunction (β=0.376 t=2.48, p=0.02).

Conclusions

sTNF-R1 as a marker of chronic inflammation may be associated with diminished neuropsychological functioning in patients with OSA.

Keywords: Obstructive sleep apnea, Cognitive dysfunction, Cytokine, IL-6, TNF-α, sTNF-R1

Introduction

Obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) is characterized by chronically fragmented sleep and recurrent transient hypoxemia. Common sequelae of OSA include cardiovascular risk, excessive daytime sleepiness, mood disorder, and neuropsychological dysfunction [1, 2].

Obstructive sleep apnea can have a negative impact on cognitive functioning but the mechanisms responsible for this dysfunction have not been elucidated. A recent review analyzed patterns of neuropsychological deficits in OSA patients [3]. Attention–vigilance, executive functioning, and memory were impaired in at least 60% of the studies reviewed, and visuoconstruction and psychomotor functioning impairments were noted in 80% of the studies. A substantial impairment of vigilance and executive functioning in OSA patients was also found in a second meta-analysis [4]. In the latter review, general intelligence and verbal ability were unaffected by OSA and apnea's effects on visual and motor skills and memory were inconsistent.

It is unclear whether the disturbances in sleep architecture and/or the drops in oxygenation are the primary causes of cognitive problems in OSA. In a review of nine placebo-controlled studies that evaluated cognitive functioning pretreatment and posttreatment, continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP) treatment was only rarely superior to placebo [5]. These studies were also inconclusive regarding the effects of treatment on specific cognitive domains. CPAP's beneficial effects were inconsistent for sustained attention, memory, executive functioning, and psychomotor function in the above-mentioned meta-analysis [3].

Similarly, associations between apnea severity and cognitive dysfunction appear inconsistent. Although most studies have found an association between apnea severity variables and at least one domain of cognitive performance, only global cognitive functioning and attention–vigilance have been consistently related to apnea severity. In our placebo-controlled studies of CPAP treatment of OSA patients, we observed global cognitive improvement in one study [6] but not in another [7]. However, in both studies, vigilance was the only specific domain observed to improve in association with CPAP treatment [6, 7].

A possible mechanism responsible for cognitive dys-function in the OSA patient involves inflammatory cytokines. There is a significant association between peripheral inflammatory markers and cognitive performance. Peripheral proinflammatory cytokines such as interleukin-1 (IL-1) and IL-6 can affect central nervous system functioning by penetrating the blood-brain barrier directly through active transport mechanisms or indirectly through activation of the afferent vagus nerve [810]. Also, chronic elevation of proinflammatory cytokine levels can promote neurodegeneration and such chronic elevation is related to impairment of cognitive functioning [11]. Patients with diseases of cognitive function such as Alzheimer's disease or multi-infarct dementia show altered peripheral production of tumor necrosis factor-α (TNF-α;[12]). Poorer cognitive function in older, reasonably healthy African–Americans and Caucasian–Americans has also been associated with higher peripheral levels of IL-6, and higher IL-6 levels may be associated with cognitive decline in the general older population [11, 13, 14]. Thus, cytokine-mediated central inflammatory processes may be very relevant to cognition [15].

These observations on inflammation and cognition are germane to OSA, as numerous studies have reported increased levels of inflammation and inflammatory markers (IL-6, TNF-α, and C-reactive protein) in patients with OSA [16, 17]. The significant elevations of IL-6 and TNF-α are independent of body mass index (BMI) but relate to OSA severity, measured by the apnea–hypopnea index (AHI; [18]). Elevated cytokines, as part of a chronic inflammatory process, have been suggested as a possible mechanism for the increased cardiovascular risk associated with OSA [19].

In an attempt to answer whether cytokines play an important role in cognitive functioning of OSA patients, we compared the relative importance of markers of OSA severity and inflammation vis-à-vis neuropsychological functioning in OSA patients.

Materials and methods

Participants

People with a history suggestive of OSA were recruited by advertisement and word of mouth referral or were referred by local sleep centers, medical practitioners in the San Diego area, and by previous participants in our laboratory's studies. We limited enrollment to subjects in the age range of 30 to 65 years and with no more than 200% of ideal body weight as determined by Metropolitan Life Foundation height and weight tables [20].

Because we are trying to investigate specific OSA-related mechanisms, we used stringent exclusion criteria to minimize other sources of cognitive disturbance. Patients were excluded if they had a history of heart, liver, or renal disease, diabetes, psychosis, narcolepsy, current alcohol or drug use disorders, severe asthma, or cerebrovascular disease, or prior diagnosis or treatment of OSA. Pregnancy as well as current prescription medications except antihypertensive medication were additional criteria for exclusion. Patients receiving antihypertensive medication had their treatment tapered slowly in two to three steps depending on the patient's regular dosage. A 3-week drug washout period was observed before studying the patients. Individuals whose blood pressure exceeded 180/110 mmHg were returned to active treatment and were not studied in this protocol.

The project was approved by the University of California, San Diego, Human Subjects Committee and written informed consent was obtained from the subjects prior to participation in the study.

All subjects were screened for OSA using an unattended overnight home sleep recording system study (Stardust; Respironics Inc., Marietta, GA, USA). Subjects with an AHI (number of apneas plus hypopneas per hour of sleep) of at least 15 were admitted to the General Clinical Research Center (GCRC) Gillin Laboratory of Sleep and Chronobiology for three nights.

If the polysomnography recording on the first night confirmed an AHI≥15, subjects were considered to have presumptive OSA and were invited to continue in the study. None of the subjects had predominant central sleep apnea (defined as≥50% central respiratory events).

Measures

Sleep recordings

Sleep was monitored with the Grass Heritage digital polysomnograph (Model PSG36-2, Astro-Med, Inc., West Warwick, RI, USA). Central and occipital electroencephalogram, bilateral electrooculogram, submental and tibialis anterior electromyogram, electrocardiogram, body position, nasal airflow using a nasal cannula–pressure transducer, and naso-oral airflow using a thermistor were assessed. Respiratory effort was measured using chest and abdominal piezoelectric belts. Oxyhemoglobin saturation (SaO2) was monitored using a pulse oximeter (Biox 3740; Ohmeda: Louisville, CO, USA) and scored with Profox Software (Profox; Escondido, CA, USA). Sleep records were manually scored according to the criteria of Rechtshaffen and Kales [21]. AHI was calculated as the average of the total number of apneas (decrements in airflow of ≥90% from baseline for ≥10 s) and hypopneas (decrements in airflow of ≥50% but <90% from baseline for ≥10 s) experienced per hour of sleep [22].

Neuropsychological tests

Subjects were given the following battery at baseline: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III Digit Symbol, Symbol Search, Digit Span, and Letter–Number Sequencing; Brief Visuospatial Memory Test Revised; Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised; Trail Making A/B; Digit Vigilance Test; Stroop Color–Word Test; and Controlled Oral Word Association Test [2331]. These tests produced 15 raw scores per subject and assessed the following cognitive domains: speed of information processing; attention and working memory; executive functions; verbal learning and memory; visual learning and memory; and verbal fluency. The tests were administered at 1:00 pm by the same research personnel and required approximately 60 min to complete.

Psychological assessment

The Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CESD), a 20-item self-report scale, was used to assess depressive symptoms [32]. Scores ≥16 on the CESD suggest a diagnosis of dysthymia or major depression [33]. The CESD primarily taps cognitive–affective aspects of depression and has been shown to be useful in chronically ill groups experiencing fatigue (e.g., HIV, cancer), including OSA patients [3438].

Inflammatory markers

Blood was drawn upon awakening around 6:30 AM the morning after the first night of sleep in the GCRC. Blood was collected in ethylene diamine tetraacetic acid for IL-6, TNF-α, and soluble TNF receptor 1 (sTNF-R1). Early morning blood draw was accomplished before the subjects got out of bed in order to avoid possible effects of major movements on such levels. Plasma was stored at −80°C until further assessment was performed. IL-6, TNF-α, and sTNF-R1 levels were assessed by using a high-sensitivity enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay from R&D Systems (Minneapolis, MN, USA). The precision of the assays was as follows: intra-assay variations (CV) for IL-6, TNF-α, and sTNF-R1 were 2.0%, 8.0%, and 3.9%; interassay variations (CV) were 3.5%, 16.3%, and 4.8%, respectively. Sensitivity values for IL-6, TNF-α, and sTNF-R1 were <0.70 pg/ml, <0.18 pg/ml, and <0.52 pg/ml.

Data analyses

TNF-α data were log-transformed to approximate a normal distribution. Raw scores on the neuropsychological tests were converted to demographically converted T scores (controlling for age, education, gender, and ethnicity, as appropriate). Higher T scores indicate better performance. A deficit score was computed for each of the 15 individual test scores, according to the convention below; T scores were collapsed into groups from 0 to 5. The average of those scores is the global deficit score (GDS; [39]). A T score ≥40 (normal) yields a deficit score of 0; ≥35 and <40 (mildly impaired) yields a deficit score of 1; ≥30 and <35 (mild to moderate impairment) yields a deficit score of 2; ≥25 and <30 (moderate impairment) is scored as a 3; ≥20 and <25 (moderate to severe impairment) is scored as a 4; and <20 (severe impairment) is scored as a 5.

Pearson correlations were used to determine bivariate associations among the baseline GDS, BMI, CESD, OSA severity variables (AHI, %O2<90), and markers of inflammation (IL-6, TNF- α, sTNF-R1). Of the inflammatory markers, only sTNF-R1 showed a significant bivariate relationship with GDS; therefore, it was the only inflammatory marker included in the subsequent hierarchical analysis. Hierarchical linear regression analysis was used to model associations for GDS vs. OSA severity variables and sTNF-R1. While the OSA severity variables did not show significant bivariate relationships with GDS, we decided to take a conservative approach and include them in the hierarchical model to ensure that variance in cognitive functioning due to OSA severity was controlled.

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS statistical software (SPSS for Windows 12.0: SPSS Inc.; Chicago, IL, USA). Observations were considered significant at p<0.05.

Results

Demographics

Characteristics of patients are presented in Table 1. The majority of participants were male (85%) and Caucasian (72%). Subjects were middle aged (mean age 47.6±1.6 years) and mildly obese (BMI 31.1±0.9 kg/m2). On average, participants had severe OSA (mean AHI 63.9±4.9). Oxyhemoglobin desaturation during the night was mild with the mean of time spent at an oxygen saturation below 90% (%O2<90) at 7±2.1% of total time in bed.

Table 1.

Patient characteristics prior to treatment (±standard error of the mean; n=39)

Patients characteristics prior to treatment
Age (years) 47.6±1.6
Gender (%)
 Male 85
 Female 15
Ethnicity (%)
 Caucasian 72
 African-American 10
 Hispanic 5
 Asian 8
 Other 5
Education (%)
 Graduate school 10
 College graduate 39
 Partial college 28
 High school graduate 20
 Partial high school 3
BMI (kg/m2) 31.1±0.9
AHI 63.9±4.9
%O2<90 7±2.1
IL-6 (pg/ml) 3.6±0.4
TNF-α (pg/ml) 1.7 ±0.2
sTNF-R1 (pg/ml) 908.3 ±34.2
GDS 0.3 ±0.06

BMI Body mass index, AHI apnea-hypopnea index, %O2<90 oxygen saturation below 90%

Pearson's correlations

We first examined correlations between OSA severity variables (AHI and %O2<90) and the inflammatory markers. We observed a trend toward a positive association between AHI and sTNF-R1 (r=0.31, p=0.071). Correlations for GDS vs. patient characteristics, CESD, OSA severity variables, and inflammatory markers are shown in Table 2. sTNF-R1 was the only variable significantly correlated with GDS (r=0.387, p=0.015).

Table 2.

Pearson's correlations: patient characteristics vs. Global Deficit Score (n=39)

Patient Characteristics Pearson's r P
Age 0.144 0.382
BMI 0.081 0.625
AHI 0.024 0.885
%O290 0.0207. 0.0206.
Mean SaO2 −0.169 0.303
CESD −0.106 0.520
IL-6 0.234 0.170
TNF-α 0.009 0.958
sTNF-R1 0.387 0.015

Hierarchical linear regression analysis

Table 3 shows the results of the hierarchical linear regression analysis using GDS as the dependent variable and OSA severity variables and sTNF-R1 as the independent variables. In step 1, AHI and %O2<90 together explained 5.5% of the variance in GDS. Even though the OSA severity variables were nonsignificant in the bivariate correlation, we took a conservative approach by forcing AHI and %O2<90 together into the first step of a hierarchical regression analysis, to see if sTNF-R1 would subsequently contribute significantly to the prediction of neuropsychological functioning. In step 2, after including sTNF-R1 using forced entry, the percentage of variance in GDS explained by the full model increased more than threefold to 19.6% (F=2.84, df=3, 36, p=0.05). Only sTNF-R1 was a significant, individual predictor in the model (β=0.376, t=2.48, p=0.02).

Table 3.

Hierarchical linear regression analysis: global deficit score vs. markers of obstructive sleep apnea severity (AHI, %O2<90) and sTNF-Rl (n=39)

β SE B t score p value
Step 1: forced entry of OSA severity variablesa,R2=0.055
 %O2<90 0.279 0.001 1.43 0.16
 AHI −0.130 0.002 −0.67 0.51
Step 2: forced entry of OSA severity variables plus sTNF-R1b,R2=0.196
 %O2<90 0.255 0.001 1.40 0.17
 AHI −0.120 0.002 −0.66 0.52
 sTNF-Rl 0.376 <0.001 2.48 0.02
a

Model accounted for 5.5% of variance in global deficit score (F=1.04, df=2,37,p=0.36).

b

Model accounted for 19.6% of variance in global deficit score (F=2.84, df=3,36,p=0.05)

Discussion

This study examined the relationship between cognitive functioning, measures of sleep apnea severity, mood, and inflammatory markers in a sample of patients with severe untreated OSA. We observed a trend toward a positive association between OSA severity and inflammation. While the lack of a stronger association is likely due to limited statistical power, this trend nonetheless is in agreement with OSA inflammation links reported in the literature. [16, 17] The results of multivariate analyses suggest that sTNF-R1 was the most important predictor of cognitive status, accounting for almost 20% of variance in GDS. In other words, inflammation (as measured by sTNF-R1) explained three times as much variance of the GDS as did OSA severity (AHI and %O2 < 90).

Receptors for TNF-α occur physiologically as an inhibitor and modulator of TNF-α [40]. TNF receptor levels occur in much higher concentrations in the circulation than TNF-α, making assessment more reliable [41]. The difference in findings regarding TNF-R1 and TNF-α may also represent assay sensitivity, which is quite limited for TNF-α. There are different forms, sTNF-R1 and sTNF-R2 (also called sTNF-R p55 and sTNF-R p75), and both are shed into the circulation by various cell types such as activated lymphocytes, neutrophils, epithelial cells, and tumor cells. The receptors compete with cellular TNF receptors for TNF-α, thus changing its bioavailability. After a challenge to the immune system, TNF-R increases rapidly; increases are also found in clinical conditions such as rheumatoid arthritis and psoriasis [40, 42, 43]. sTNF-R1 shows a fairly consistent 24-h rhythm with peak values in the early morning hours, and sleep deprivation increases their values in healthy subjects [44, 45]. Less is known of the role of sTNF-R1 in OSA patients, but its substrate, TNF-α, shows an altered circadian variability in OSA [46]: OSA patients exhibit increased TNF-α concentrations in the afternoon instead of a normal peak during the night. It might therefore be interesting to study circadian variability of sTNF-R1 in future studies.

The impact of cytokines on cognitive disturbance has been studied during mild stimulation of primary host defense in healthy subjects [47]. In a double-blind crossover study, healthy volunteers were given intravenous injection of endotoxin or saline and neuropsychological tests were performed. Participants showed increased levels of TNF-α, sTNF-R, IL-6, IL-1 receptor antagonist, and cortisol as well as decreased verbal and nonverbal memory functions after injection. Thus, these authors found a significant positive correlation between cytokine secretion and decreased memory performance in healthy adults.

Our study has several limitations. We did not study the circadian variability of cytokines and different findings might have emerged had we examined such variability in relation to neuropsychological performance.

While OSA severity variables were not significantly associated with cognitive functioning, this lack of association could possibly be due to limited statistical power. Nonetheless, our results suggest that inflammatory changes in OSA may have a more important impact on cognitive functioning than the breathing disorder per se.

We excluded patients with major medical illnesses other than OSA and hypertension. These criteria resulted in a population that may not be representative of OSA patients normally seen in a sleep clinic. While our population might thus not be considered as “typical” OSA patients, our exclusion criteria served to limit the impact of several potential confounders (e.g., diabetes) that could affect cognitive dysfunction.

Another limitation to our findings relates to the impact of obesity on inflammatory markers. It is possible that body fat may be an undetected confounder of the reported relationship between inflammation and cognitive function. While BMI was unrelated to cognitive functions (see Table 2), it must be acknowledged that we have a limited range of BMI in the sample (from 23.1 to 50.2 kg/m2, mean BMI 31.1±0.9). It would actually be somewhat difficult to address this BMI–cognition–cytokine question in an apnea population for the following reasons: Apnea is strongly associated with increased BMI and thus nonobese apneics are relatively rare. On the other hand, if we enrolled apneics with severe obesity, we would be more likely to run into other confounding illnesses like diabetes, necessitating more complicated inferences regarding cognition. We have thus taken the middle course of studying moderate to severe apneics with moderate amounts of obesity. In an exploratory analysis, we included BMI as an independent variable in a hierarchical regression. The amount of explained variance in cognitive functioning did not increase beyond that observed in the previous model, and BMI was not a significant individual predictor. Therefore, to maximize power, we report the model that did not include BMI.

In conclusion, the findings of this study might help to disentangle the meaning of different types of factors that affect cognition in OSA. While these findings must be considered preliminary, they nonetheless suggest that inflammation in OSA patients may be an important factor to consider in understanding cognitive functioning in these patients. However, because we had no prior data to suggest that sTNF-R1 would be the strongest predictor, replication of these findings is essential. Future studies might explore the effects of CPAP on cognition and inflammatory factors.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by NIH grants HL044915, RR00827, and AG08415 and by the grant PBBSB115117 from the Swiss National Foundation.

References

  • 1.Cheshire K, Engleman H, Deary I, Shapiro C, Douglas NJ. Factors impairing daytime performance in patients with sleep apnea/hypopnea syndrome. Arch Intern Med. 1992;152:538–541. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Newman AB, Nieto FJ, Guidry U, Lind BK, Redline S, Pickering TG, Quan SF, Sleep Heart Health Study Research Group Relation of sleep-disordered breathing to cardiovascular disease risk factors: the Sleep Heart Health Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2001;154:50–59. doi: 10.1093/aje/154.1.50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Aloia MS, Arnedt JT, Davis JD, Riggs RL, Byrd D. Neuropsychological sequelae of obstructive sleep apnea–hypopnea syndrome: a critical review. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10:772–785. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704105134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Beebe DW, Groesz L, Wells C, Nichols A, McGee K. The neuropsychological effects of obstructive sleep apnea: a meta-analysis of norm-referenced and case-controlled data. Sleep. 2003;26:298–307. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.3.298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gay P, Weaver R, Loube D, Iber C. Evaluation of positive airway pressure treatment for sleep related breathing disorders in adults. Sleep. 2006;29:381–401. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.3.381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bardwell WA, Ancoli-Israel S, Berry CC, Dimsdale JE. Neuropsychological effects of one-week continuous positive airway pressure treatment in patients with obstructive sleep apnea: a placebo-controlled study. Psychosom Med. 2001 Jul;63:579–584. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200107000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lim W, Bardwell WA, Loredo JS, Kim E, Ancoli-Israel S, Morgan EE, Heaton R, Dimsdale JE. Effects of 2-week continuous positive airway pressure treatment vs. oxygen supplementation on neuropsychological functioning in patients with obstructive sleep apnea. J Clin Sleep Med. 2007;3:380–386. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Banks WA, Kastin AJ. Blood to brain transport of interleukin links the immune and central nervous systems. Life Sci. 1991;48:117–121. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(91)90385-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Maier S, Watkins LR. Cytokines for psychologists: implications for bidirectional immune-to-brain communication for understanding behavior, mood, and cognition. Psychol Rev. 1998;105:83–107. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.105.1.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Tracey KJ. The inflammatory reflex. Nature. 2002;420:853–859. doi: 10.1038/nature01321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Yaffe K, Lindquist K, Penninx BW, Simonsick EM, Pahor M, Kritchevsky S, Launer L, Kuller L, Rubin S, Harris T. Inflammatory markers and cognition in well-functioning African–American and white elders. Neurology. 2003;61:76–80. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000073620.42047.d7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.De Luigi A, Fragiacomo C, Lucca U, Quadri P, Tettamanti M, Grazia De Simoni M. Inflammatory markers in Alzheimer's disease and multi-infarct dementia. Mech Ageing Dev. 2001;122:1985–1995. doi: 10.1016/s0047-6374(01)00313-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Marsland AL, Petersen KL, Sathanoori R, Muldoon MF, Neumann SA, Ryan C, Flory JD, Manuck SB. Interleukin-6 covaries inversely with cognitive performance among middle-aged community volunteers. Psychosom Med. 2006;68:895–903. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000238451.22174.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Weaver JD, Huang MH, Albert M, Harris T, Rowe JW, Seeman TE. Interleukin-6 and risk of cognitive decline. Neurology. 2002;59:371–378. doi: 10.1212/wnl.59.3.371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wilson CJ, Finch CE, Cohen JH. Cytokines and cognition— the case for a head-to-toe inflammatory paradigm. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50:2041–2056. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50619.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Shamsuzzaman AS, Winnicki M, Lanfranchi P, Wolk R, Kara T, Accurso V, Somers VK. Elevated C-reactive protein in patients with obstructive sleep apnea. Circulation. 2002;105:2462–2464. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.0000018948.95175.03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Vgontzas AN, Papanicolaou DA, Bixler EO, Kales A, Tyson K, Chrousos GP. Elevation of plasma cytokines in disorders of excessive daytime sleepiness: role of sleep disturbance and obesity. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1997;82:1313–1316. doi: 10.1210/jcem.82.5.3950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Ciftci TU, Kokturk O, Bukan N, Bilgihan A. The relationship between serum cytokine levels with obesity and obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Cytokine. 2004;17:87–91. doi: 10.1016/j.cyto.2004.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Lavie P. Pro: sleep apnea causes cardiovascular disease. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2004;169:147–148. doi: 10.1164/rccm.2310010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Metropolitan Life Insurance Company Metropolitan height and weight tables. Stat Bull Metrop Life Found. 1983;64:3–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rechtshaffen A, Kales A. A manual of standard terminology: techniques and scoring system for sleep stages of human subjects. UCLA Brain Information Service/Brain Research Institute; Los Angeles: 1968. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Loredo JS, Ancoli-Israel S, Dimsdale JE. Effect of continuous positive airway pressure vs. placebo continuous positive airway pressure on sleep quality in obstructive sleep apnea. Chest. 1999;116:1545–1549. doi: 10.1378/chest.116.6.1545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Wechsler D. Wechsler adult intelligence scale—III. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Benedict RH. Brief visuospatial memory test-revised. Psychological Assessment Resources; Odessa: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Benedict RH, Schretlen D, Groninger L, Brandt J. Hopkins verbal learning test-revised: normative data and analysis of interform and test–retest reliability. Clin Neuropsychol. 1998;12:43–55. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Heaton RK, Miller SW, Taylor MJ, Grant I. Revised comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead-Reitan battery: demographic adjusted norms for African American and Caucasian adults. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; Lutz: 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lewis RF. Digit vigilance test. Psychological Assessment Resources; Odessa: 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Golden CJ. Stroop color and word test. Stoelting Co.; Chicago: 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assessment. Oxford University Press; New York: 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Heaton RK, Taylor MJ, Manly J. Demographic effects and use of demographically corrected norms with the WAIS-III and WMS-III. In: Tulsky DS, Saklaeske DH, Heaton EK, Bornstein R, Ledbetter ME, editors. Clinical interpretation of the WAIS-III and WMS-III. Academic; San Diego: 2003. pp. 181–210. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Benton AL. Differential behavioral effects of frontal lobe disease. Neuropsychologia. 1968;6:53–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Schulberg HC, Saul M, McClelland M, Ganguli M, Christy W, Frank R. Assessing depression in primary medical and psychiatric practices. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1985;42:1164–1170. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1985.01790350038008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Cockram A, Judd FK, Mijch A, Norman T. The evaluation of depression in inpatients with HIV disease. Aust NZ J Psychiatry. 1999;33:344–352. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1614.1999.00579.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hann D, Winter K, Jacobsen P. Measurement of depressive symptoms in cancer patients: evaluation of the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) J Psychosom Res. 1999;46:437–443. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(99)00004-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Bardwell WA, Berry CC, Ancoli-Israel S, Dimsdale JE. Psychological correlates of sleep apnea. J Psychosom Res. 1999;47:583–596. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(99)00062-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bardwell WA, Moore P, Ancoli-Israel S, Dimsdale JE. Does obstructive sleep apnea confound sleep architecture findings in subjects with depressive symptoms? Biol Psychiatry. 2000;48:1001–1009. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00887-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bardwell WA, Moore P, Ancoli-Israel S, Dimsdale JE. Fatigue in obstructive sleep apnea: driven by depressive symptoms instead of apnea severity? Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:350–355. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.2.350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Carey CL, Woods SP, Gonzalez R, Conover E, Marcotte TD, Grant I, Heaton RK, the HNRC Group Predictive validity of Global Deficit Scores in detecting neuropsychological impairment in HIV infection. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2004;26:307–319. doi: 10.1080/13803390490510031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Aderka D. The potential biological and clinical significance of the soluble tumor necrosis factor receptors. Cytokine Growth Factor Rev. 1996;7:231–240. doi: 10.1016/s1359-6101(96)00026-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Diez-Ruiz A, Tilz GP, Zangerle R, Baier-Bitterlich G, Wachter H, Fuchs D. Soluble receptors for tumour necrosis factor in clinical laboratory diagnosis. Eur J Haematol. 1995;54:1–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0609.1995.tb01618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Haack M, Schuld A, Kraus T, Pollmächer T. Effects of sleep on endotoxin-induced host responses in healthy men. Psychosom Med. 2001;63:568–578. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200107000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Mullington J, Korth C, Hermann DM, Orth A, Galanos C, Holsboer F, Pollmächer T. Dose-dependent effects of endotoxin on human sleep. Am J Physiol. 2000;278:R947–R955. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.2000.278.4.R947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Haack M, Pollmächer T, Mullington JM. Diurnal and sleep–wake dependent variations of soluble TNF- and IL-2 receptors in healthy volunteers. Brain Behavior Immunity. 2004;18:361–367. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2003.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Shearer WT, Reuben JM, Mullington JM, Price NJ, Lee BN, Smith EO, Szuba MP, Van Dongen HP, Dinges DF. Soluble TNF-α receptor 1 and IL-6 plasma levels in humans subjected to the sleep deprivation model of spaceflight. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2001;107:165–170. doi: 10.1067/mai.2001.112270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Entzian P, Linnemann K, Schlaak M, Zabel P. Obstructive sleep apnea syndrome and circadian rhythms of hormones and cytokines. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 1996;153:1080–1086. doi: 10.1164/ajrccm.153.3.8630548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Reichenberg A, Yirmiya R, Schuld A, Kraus T, Haack M, Morag A, Pollmächer T. Cytokine-associated emotional and cognitive disturbances in human. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:445–452. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.5.445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES