INTRODUCTION
Turner syndrome (TS) is a common genetic disorder characterized by the complete or partial absence of an X chromosome. The disorder affects approximately 1/2000 females with typical physical features including short stature, webbed neck, cardiovascular abnormalities and endocrine problems related to ovarian dysgenesis, such as amenorrhea and infertility.1 Females with Turner syndrome often demonstrate a distinctive neurocognitive profile characterized by full-scale IQs in the average range, with relative strengths in verbal abilities and weaknesses in visual-spatial skills and executive function.2, 3 There is also increasing evidence for deficits in the domain of social function in TS. While studies largely suggest that adult women with TS do not have increased incidence of psychiatric illness,4 there is some indication that global psychosocial functioning may be impaired.5, 6 Though outcome studies of adult women with TS are not always consistent,7-9 the extant literature indicates that relative to age-matched peers, these individuals experience higher incidence of anxiety, depression, poor body image, low self-esteem and self-perception of impaired social competence.7, 9 Investigation of other metrics of social function, such as achievement of independent living and successful romantic relationships, also indicate that a subset of adult women with TS are more socially impaired relative to their peers.10, 11
Studies of girls and adolescents with TS using parental ratings and self-report suggest similar difficulties in the form of immaturity, impaired social relationships and poor self-concept compared to peers.6, 12-15 There are also data suggesting that rates of ADHD and social anxiety may be higher in girls with TS relative to age-matched controls.16, 17 Though these data are compelling and of potential clinical importance, detecting the underlying cause of impaired social competence in TS has proven challenging. Studies to date indicate that impaired social function in TS is not related to IQ alone, nor to overall stigmata of disease, such as short-stature.5, 18 Data from studies of sex hormone replacement are similarly inconclusive, but also suggest that while induction of puberty improves self-concept and other aspects of cognition such as working memory,13, 19 difficulties in social competence continue to persist after sex hormone replacement.6, 9, 10 More recently, evidence of cognitive and neural correlates of dysfunctional social behavior in TS have been reported, including findings of aberrant face recognition abilities 20, 21 and affect recognition, particularly for fearful or angry faces.18, 20, 22
As yet, an overarching framework linking these findings to X-monosomy has not emerged, however such a framework is likely to involve complex interactions between genetic, hormonal and environmental influences. Animal studies have implicated estrogen as an integral component of social behavior,23, 24 making sex hormone deficiency in TS a compelling model for elucidating the effects of estrogen on human social cognitive processing. However, previous studies investigating social cognition in TS have focused primarily on adolescents or adult women with TS, presumably after exposure to exogenous estrogen, which is typically initiated by the age of 12-14 years in girls with TS who do not undergo spontaneous puberty.25 As such, previous research on social cognition in TS is potentially confounded by pubertal changes and hormone replacement therapy. The study presented here is one of the first of its kind to specifically include a sample of girls with TS in the prepubertal/pre-estrogen age range, between 3 and 12 years of age. By examining social competence in girls with TS prior to administration of estrogen, potential state-dependent influences of sex hormones on social cognition are minimized.
The purpose of this exploratory study was to examine elements of social competence and cognition in a pre-estrogen population of girls with TS (3-12 years), and to investigate if explanatory links exist between the domains of social competence and cognition. Studies of very young girls with TS can also provide critical knowledge about early development, and thus begin to fill existing gaps in our understanding of cognitive and behavioral trajectories across the lifespan of females with TS. Our a priori hypothesis was that girls with TS would perform more poorly on measures of social behavior and social cognition compared to peers. Furthermore we predicted that severity of deficits in social cognition would correlate with outcomes of social behavior. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study of its kind to investigate these measures in a pre-estrogen cohort of young girls with TS.
METHOD
Subjects
Forty-two subjects with Turner syndrome and 32 typically developing control girls were recruited through referrals from pediatric endocrinologists, advertisements in local parent organizations and the national Turner Syndrome Society network, and the Center for Interdisciplinary Brain Sciences Research (CIBSR) website (http://cibsr.stanford.edu) as part of a larger study examining genetic and neurobiological correlates of the cognitive-behavioral phenotype in TS. Participants ranged in age from 3 to 12 years. All participants were in good overall medical health. None of the study participants had evidence of current or past major neurological and psychiatric problems (TS participants with diagnoses of ADHD or anxiety were not excluded from the sample, however none of the girls with TS fulfilled full DSM-IV-TR criteria for these diagnoses). Only subjects with a monosomic 45X karyotype were included, confirmed by standard karyotype assessment. TS participants on growth hormone were included (26 of 39 participants), however none of the participants had started estrogen replacement. Parents of the TS group provided information about pubertal development based on Tanner staging; none of the participants scored >3 on breast or pubic hair scales. Socioeconomic status (SES) was derived from reported household income divided by median household income for the family’s area based on U.S. Census data by zip code (http://factfinder.census.gov/home/saff/main.html). All research was conducted at Stanford University. Informed consent and assent were obtained from all participants and study parameters were approved by the institutional IRB.
Intelligence testing measures
Participants between the ages of 3 to 5 years were administered the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence Third Edition (WPPSI)26 while the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children 4th Edition (WISC)27 was used for subjects aged 6-12 years. In addition to Full-scale IQ (FSIQ) scores, Verbal IQ (VIQ) scores were generated as defined by the Verbal Comprehension Index from the WISC-IV or the Verbal IQ score from the WPPSI. Performance IQ (PIQ) scores were defined by the Perceptual Reasoning Index from the WISC-IV or the Performance IQ score from the WPPSI. Participants were included if their full-scale IQ scores fell in the non-intellectually disabled range, IQ≥70. To preserve relative matching for cognitive level with girls in the TS group, healthy control girls with FSIQ>130 were excluded.
Neuropsychological Functioning
Neuropsychological functioning was assessed using the NEPSY28, a comprehensive cognitive/neuropsychological assessment tool for individuals age 3 to 12 years. The NEPSY generates scores for five domains: Attention/Executive Functions, Language, Sensorimotor Functions, Visuospatial Processing, and Memory and Learning. During the course of study recruitment, the NEPSY-II29 was released, which introduced a new Social Perception Domain score, including Affect Recognition and Theory of Mind subscales. Thus, while most subjects were assessed with the NEPSY, only a subset of later participants received the NEPSY-II Social Perception tests. Subscales contributing to the assessment of social cognition include the Memory for Faces task from the original version of the NEPSY, and the Affect Recognition and Theory of Mind subscales from the NEPSY-II. For the Theory of Mind subscale, conversion of raw scores to scaled values resulted in a significant decrease in reported values due to a low ceiling threshold, reducing total responses from 24 to 12 in TS participants and 22 to 11 in controls.
Psychosocial Assessments
Thirty-six TS and 21 control participants’ primary caregivers completed the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS).30 The SRS is a 65-item questionnaire that assesses specific and observable elements of social behavior based on frequency of occurrence. Domains include Social Awareness, Social Cognition, Social Communication, Social Motivation and Autistic Mannerisms. Participants were also assessed with the Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale: Second Edition (RCMAS-2)31 a 49-item self-report inventory used to measure symptoms of anxiety. RCMAS-2 subscales of interest were Physiological Anxiety, Worry, Social Anxiety, Defensiveness and Inconsistent Responding index. The scale is normed for ages 6-19 years, so younger participants did not receive a scaled score. Finally, the Behavioral Assessment System for Children (BASC-2)32 was used. The BASC is a parent report questionnaire that assesses emotional and behavioral functioning in individuals ages 2 to 18 years. For RCMAS and BASC assessments, T-scores are reported with a mean of 50 and standard deviation of 10.
Statistical Analyses
All data were first inspected for parametric properties. Independent two-tailed Student’s t-tests were initially used to analyze between-group differences in overall intelligence scores. As social competency and social cognition variables were correlated (see Table 1), we utilized univariate and multivariate analyses of covariance (M/ANCOVA) to assess predicted between-group differences for selected behavioral and cognitive variables related to social cognition and social function. Similar to studies of other genetic groups with visual-spatial dysfunction33, Verbal IQ was used as the primary covariate for intelligence in these ANCOVA models as PIQ scores are disproportionally biased by visual perceptual and visual spatial deficits present in girls with TS. Separate models assessing interaction effects of Group × Age and Group × VIQ were also conducted when appropriate. As the SRS is not specifically age-normed, age (centered around the mean age for the entire sample) was included as a covariate for MANCOVA analyses using SRS subscale scores as dependent variables. Statistical significance was defined as p<.05 and all analyses were performed using SPSS version 18 software.
Table 1.
NEPSY Memory Faces |
NEPSY Affect Recognition |
NEPSY Theory of Mind |
SRS Social Awareness |
SRS Social Cognition |
SRS Social Communication |
SRS Social Motivation |
SRS Autistic Mannerisms |
||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
NEPSY Memory for Faces |
Correlation | .327* | .694** | −.222 | −.369** | −.358* | −.182 | −.440** | |
Significance | -- | .042 | .004 | .122 | .008 | .011 | .205 | .001 | |
NEPSY Affect | Correlation | .373 | .154 | −.073 | −.036 | −.015 | −.172 | ||
Recognition | Significance | -- | .087 | .342 | .657 | .825 | .925 | .288 | |
NEPSY Theory of Mind |
Correlation | −.395 | −.319 | −.281 | −.092 | −.548* | |||
Significance | -- | .077 | .159 | .218 | .692 | .010 | |||
SRS Social | Correlation | .675** | .759** | .399** | .696** | ||||
Awareness | Significance | -- | .000 | .000 | .002 | .000 | |||
SRS Social | Correlation | .838** | .559** | .824** | |||||
Cognition | Significance | -- | .000 | .000 | .000 | ||||
SRS Social | Correlation | .626** | .829** | ||||||
Communication | Significance | -- | .000 | .000 | |||||
SRS Social | Correlation | .570** | |||||||
Motivation | Significance | -- | .000 | ||||||
SRS Autistic | Correlation | ||||||||
Mannerisms | Significance | -- |
Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed)
Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed)
RESULTS
Descriptive Statistics
After excluding individuals with IQs in the intellectually disabled and gifted ranges, participants included in the final analysis consisted of 39 girls with TS and 29 female controls. Groups were similar in demographic characteristics, including socioeconomic status, as measured by adjusted household income. Ethnicity of participants in both groups was primarily Caucasian (TS: 76%; Controls: 85%), Pearson’s chi-square test demonstrated that proportions of White and non-White participants were not significantly different between groups (χ2=.824, df=2, p=.364). Mean age in the two groups was similar as shown in Table 2. Age distributions in girls with TS (3-6 years: 43.6%, 7-9 years: 33.3%, 10-12 years: 23.1%) were similar to those of the control group (3-6 years: 44.8%, 7-9 years: 27.6%, 10-12 years: 27.6%). Pearson’s chi-square indicated that these frequencies were not significantly different between groups (χ2=.319, df=2, p=.853).
Table 2. Characteristics of Age, SES, and Cognitive Scores, by Group.
TS | Controls | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | P | |
Age | 7.89 (n=39) |
2.46 | 7.56 (n=29) |
2.81 | 0.604 |
SESa | 2.02 (n=39) |
1.11 | 2.06 (n=29) |
0.72 | 0.888 |
FSIQb | 92.18 (n=39) |
13.33 | 116.45 (n=29) |
8.30 | <0.001 |
WPPSI VIQc | 99.36 (n=11) |
12.66 | 114.10 (n=10) |
12.91 | 0.016 |
WPPSI PIQc | 90.45 (n=11) |
15.87 | 116.30 (n=10) |
12.50 | 0.001 |
WPPSI PSc | 91.17 (n=6) |
13.60 | 112.20 (n=5) |
11.95 | 0.025 |
WISC-IV VCId | 104.21 (n=28) |
15.84 | 116.00 (n=19) |
14.24 | 0.012 |
WISC-IV PRId | 88.46 (n=28) |
13.91 | 114.63 (n=19) |
8.12 | <0.001 |
WISC-IV WMd | 87.14 (n=28) |
13.11 | 106.21 (n=19) |
7.33 | <0.001 |
WISC-IV PSd | 88.15 (n=28) |
12.28 | 108.68 (n=19) |
11.16 | <0.001 |
Socioeconomic status (reported household income divided by median household income for geographical area)
Full Scale IQ
WPPSI 3rd Edition Verbal IQ, Performance IQ, Processing Speed
WISV 4th Edition Verbal Comprehension Index, Perceptual Reasoning Index, Working Memory, Processing Speed
Independent t-tests showed significant between-group differences in all IQ score measures, where girls with TS had lower scores compared to control peers. As expected, between-group differences were less apparent for WPPSI VIQ and WISC-IV VCI scores relative to WPPSI PIQ and WISC-IV PRI scores for girls with TS relative to controls. The means and standard deviations for demographic and cognitive data are listed in Table 2.
Behavioral Measures
SRS
Using univariate analysis and age and VIQ as covariates, the TS group had significantly higher ratings on the SRS total score relative to healthy controls (F1,54=6.724, p=.012), indicating greater overall impairments in social function. All SRS subscale scores were subsequently entered as dependent variables into a multivariate model with age and VIQ as covariates. The overall model was significant (Wilks Lambda F5,50=3.396, p=.010). Between-group effects were significant for all subscales (see Table 2) with the notable exception of the Social Motivation subscale at a threshold of p<.05. Using a conservative Bonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons at a threshold of p<.01 (.05/5 subtests), differences for Social Cognition (p=.004) and Autistic Mannerisms (p=.001) remained significant, and approached significance for Social Awareness (p=.018) and Social Communication (p=.014). Effects of age (p=.826) and VIQ (p=.488) were not significant, nor was the interaction term of Age × Group (p=.274).
Univariate analysis of RCMAS-2 Total Anxiety score using VIQ as a covariate did not show significant between-group differences (F1,40=0.825, p=.369). The multivariate model using VIQ as a covariate and all RCMAS subscales as dependent variables was also not significant (Wilks Lambda F5,36=1.135, p=.360). In contrast, the multivariate model for the BASC-2 subscale scores was significant (Wilks Lambda F12,51=3.433, p=.001). Follow-up univariate analyses demonstrated significant differences for the Hyperactivity, Atypicality, Attention, Social Skills, Activities of Daily Living and Functional Communication subscales, even when accounting for VIQ. There were no significant interaction effects of Group × Age or Group × VIQ for the RCMAS and BASC assessments.
Social Cognitive Measures
NEPSY Memory for Faces
36 girls with TS and 23 controls completed the Memory for Faces subtest. Univariate analysis indicated a significant difference between groups with TS performing at a lower level than controls after controlling for VIQ (F1,56=22.16, p<.001). There were no significant interaction effects.
NEPSY-II Affect Recognition
25 girls with TS and 22 controls completed the Affect Recognition subtest. Girls with TS exhibited lower performance compared to peers. However, between-group differences were not statistically significant after controlling for VIQ (F1,44=1.235, p=.272). No significant interaction effects were noted.
NEPSY-II Theory of Mind
As with the Affect Recognition task, this scale was only administered to participants after the release of the NEPSY-II, and has a low ceiling score. Twelve girls with TS and 11 controls were given scaled scores on the Theory of Mind subtest. Results showed that girls with TS had lower scores compared to controls when using VIQ as a covariate (F1,20=7.69, p=.012).
Growth Hormone Effects
Exploratory analyses were conducted within the overall TS group to determine if behavioral effects were associated with growth hormone treatment ongoing at the time of the evaluation (26 out of 39 participants). There were no significant differences on any behavioral or cognitive measure between these TS subgroups.
DISCUSSION
As predicted, we found that girls with TS performed more poorly than their peers on several measures of social competence and social cognition. In particular, girls with TS were found to have overall difficulty in social competence, with mean Total SRS scores in the clinically significant range (i.e., T score > 60).30 Moreover, effect sizes for these analyses were moderately large with the exception of the Social Motivation subscale. Girls with TS similarly placed in the mild-to-moderate range of clinical significance for the Social Cognition and Autistic Mannerisms SRS domains, which are designed to assess an individual’s ability to interpret social cues and the presence of restricted interests or stereotypic behaviors, respectively. Performance on SRS areas of Social Awareness and Social Communication also approached the clinical range, and represent an individual’s ability to sense reciprocal social behaviors and utilize expressive social communication. These ratings by parents of girls with TS indicate a perception of significant difficulty with social competence encompassing a broad range of social function.
Importantly, we also found dissociation in ratings for TS subjects on the Social Motivation subscale of the SRS versus other SRS subscale scores. Specifically, the Social Motivation subscale was the only domain in which the performance of girls with TS did not differ from their peers. This subscale is designed to measure motivation to engage in social behavior, as well as elements of social anxiety and inhibition. This SRS subscale profile suggests comparable levels of socially motivated behavior in TS relative to typically developing peers; however, this appetitive drive for social interaction appears to occur in the context of an impaired ability to implement effective social functioning. These findings provide a potential link between impaired social performance and observed decreases in self-esteem and self-concept in young girls with TS as reported in previous literature.17, 34 The extent to which this dissociation in social cognition/behavior contributes to subjective distress and impaired social function merits further investigation, with an emphasis on appetitive social behavior as a discriminating construct in the social cognitive profile of TS as compared to other conditions with social impairment.
Our findings contrast with earlier literature that have interpreted impairments of social functioning in TS to be analogous to those observed in individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD).35, 36 Our data instead suggest an alternative framework in which the social competency profile observed in TS might be discriminated from a more ‘typical’ profile associated with ASD based on differences in appetitive social behavior. Our findings also raise the larger question of understanding to what extent shared social behavioral symptomatology in TS and ASD reflects a common pathophysiological process. Certainly, the dissociation of scores on the SRS demonstrates the potential utility of this assessment instrument in girls with TS. One of the advantages of utilizing the SRS is that it is specifically designed to evaluate critical dimensions of social function in contrast to more generalized measures of adaptive function and behavior, such as the Child Behavior Checklist, which have been used in previous studies of TS.34, 37 Though originally designed and validated for screening and diagnosing children with ASD, the SRS has also been used for assessing social impairments in other conditions, such as mood and anxiety disorders.38 The unique social cognitive profile described in this study suggests its potential utility for identifying social impairments in TS.
The absence of significant anxiety as measured by the RCMAS-2 provides further support for a characteristic behavioral profile in TS that does not include wide-ranging elevated levels of psychopathology. BASC-2 scores similarly reflected the absence of group differences on scales of anxiety or depression. This instrument did demonstrate significant between-group differences on a cluster of measures related to ADHD and impaired executive function (Hyperactivity, Atypicality and Inattention), an area known to be a vulnerability for girls with TS. However, unlike the SRS, the average BASC subscale scores in the TS group did not reach a clinical threshold. Interestingly, girls with TS performed significantly more poorly on BASC-2 subscales of Activities of Daily Living and Functional Communication subscales, which generally measure overall adaptive functioning. The mean TS scores for these domains were also in the sub-clinical range.
Significant between-group differences in performance on the Social Perception domain of the NEPSY also provide evidence for social impairments in TS. Our data demonstrated significant deficits in this dimension, including differences on the Theory of Mind and Memory for Faces subscales. Furthermore, these differences survived covarying by VIQ suggesting that deficits are not due solely to problems with general cognitive level, but instead represent a more specific neurocognitive characteristic of TS. Our findings are also consistent with earlier evidence suggesting a pattern of social deficits in individuals with TS21, 39 and convergent data from neuroimaging studies that identify putative neurobiological correlates for these deficits. In particular, individuals with TS have enlarged amygdalae40, 41 and possible aberrant connectivity between the amygdala and fusiform face area42, regions which are integral to encoding the social salience of emotional stimuli.43
We were surprised to find that Affect Recognition was the only subscale that did not show a significant between-group difference (after controlling for VIQ). Deficits in affect recognition have been previously reported for both adolescents and adult women with TS20, 44 particularly for fearful or angry faces.18, 20, 22 Similarly, problems with other components of face processing such as abnormal eye-tracking scanpaths when viewing faces21 and gaze fixation and cueing difficulties45, 46 have been reported in TS. The fact that we did not detect affect recognition abnormalities in our sample may be attributed to several factors, the foremost being that the relatively small sample sizes of cohorts may not have provided sufficient power to observe a clinically significant difference on this measure. Another potential interpretation may arise from the younger age range of participants in our study and the estrogen-naïve status of the present cohort. As hormone replacement has become the standard course of clinical treatment for girls with TS who do not achieve spontaneous puberty,47 assessment of younger girls with TS represents a unique paradigm for furthering our understanding of the effects of sex hormones on the development of social cognition. Interestingly, studies of affect recognition in typically developing females over the course of the menstrual cycle indicate that phases with lower levels of estrogen correlate to higher accuracy in recognition of angry faces.48 While these findings contrast with the results in our prepubertal sample, they emphasize the need for further investigation into dosage effects of sex hormones on social behavior and cognition, particularly in regard to their influence on pubertal development and how this may impact a population of girls with typically absent or low levels of circulating sex hormones. We were not able to analyze this hypothesis more closely as Tanner staging data were only collected from the TS cohort and not from control participants, making it difficult to fully control for pubertal status. It is possible that some control participants included in our study may have experienced early pubertal changes. However, the presence of social competency deficits throughout the age range of our TS sample makes it unlikely that differences in social cognition can be attributed to potential pubertal changes alone, although this area certainly warrants closer examination in future studies.
Another limitation of our study includes the relatively broad range of ages of the participants. While analyses were appropriately controlled for age, social development represents a dynamic process, particularly when considering physiological and psychological changes occurring during puberty. Our results emphasize the need for longitudinal studies to further elucidate age-related influences on social cognitive processes in larger samples of girls with TS undergoing estrogen treatment. Investigation of the potential effects of parent-of-origin (i.e., X chromosome imprinting) on social cognition in larger TS cohorts would also be of interest.49 Lastly, our data relied largely on parent observation of social behaviors in children, which is a common method for assessment of social competency in both the clinical and research settings. However, these findings would be further enhanced by conducting direct observation of child social competency in future studies.
Overall, our findings build on previous literature by demonstrating significant differences in social competence and cognition in TS in a cohort of young girls with TS who have not yet received estrogen replacement therapy. Evidence of social cognitive differences in this population, even after controlling for differences in IQ, suggest increased risk for a characteristic profile of social behavioral problems that is emerging even in the earliest phases of development. These findings suggest that social deficits in TS are present early in development, and are independent from physiological and psychological changes that occur during induction of puberty with estrogen replacement. However, the actual trajectory of social cognitive development associated with induced puberty in TS is currently unknown, as this is a period of dramatic behavioral and neurobiological changes that is only just beginning to be understood in typically developing females as well.50 As such, future research is critical for determining whether behavioral differences observed in prepubertal girls with TS are later mitigated or influenced by administration of hormone replacement. Another strength of this study is the use of a specific instrument for assessing social behavior. Rather than relying on general behavioral assessments, the SRS allowed us to define more specific dimensions of social function and establishes a profile of social cognition that may increasingly help discriminate social difficulties in TS from other disorders with social dysfunction. Although previous studies of psychosocial function in TS have demonstrated global psychosocial problems, our data pinpoints specific components of social cognition impairments in a cohort of young girls prior to estrogen treatment. The observed differences provide a novel perspective into social cognition and functioning and provides a framework for better understanding the complex interaction between sex hormones and behavioral differences in TS. This comprehensive approach may allow for the future development of more clinically relevant assessments and treatment planning for young girls with TS.
Conclusions
Findings from our study underscore important differences in social cognition and performance in young girls with TS with estrogen deficiency. These data indicate the potential use of standardized assessments for social competence and social cognition in clinical treatment planning for girls with TS. A better understanding of social development is critical for school-age children to successfully deal with complex social demands throughout this age range, and even more so for girls with TS who may have significant impairments in this domain. The results of our study should encourage clinicians to pay greater attention to areas of social functioning in girls with TS as potential targets in an overall treatment plan. Furthermore, the implication of a distinct social cognitive profile in TS argues for a comprehensive, developmental approach to future research in this area.
Table 3. Performance on Social Responsiveness Scale Scores, by Group.
TS (n=37) |
Controls (n=21) |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | p | Cohen’s d | |
SRS Total Scoreb | 61.78 | 13.96 | 49.81 | 11.55 | 0.012 | 0.91 |
Social Awareness | 57.74 | 12.92 | 47.71 | 8.29 | 0.018 | 0.59 |
Social Cognition | 61.32 | 13.00 | 47.81 | 12.19 | 0.004 | 1.06 |
Social Communication | 59.77 | 12.80 | 49.81 | 10.31 | 0.014 | 0.83 |
Social Motivation | 54.49 | 10.34 | 52.19 | 11.41 | 0.753 | 0.21 |
Autistic Mannerisms | 69.10 | 15.60 | 52.76 | 13.30 | 0.001 | 1.10 |
Age and Verbal IQ score were used as covariates. Age × Group interaction term was tested in a separate model
SRS – Social Responsiveness Scale
Table 4. Performance on BASC-2 Scores, by Group.
TS (n=37) |
Controls (n=28) |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | p | Cohen’s d | |
BASC External Problems Composite | 52.23 | 9.2 | 50.46 | 9.52 | 0.723 | -- |
BASC Internal Problems Composite | 54.43 | 9.39 | 50.36 | 11.11 | 0.141 | -- |
BASC Behavioral Symptom Index | 55.49 | 10.05 | 49.57 | 9.98 | 0.066 | -- |
BASC Adaptive Skills Composite | 49.01 | 7.99 | 52.68 | 8.27 | 0.143 | -- |
BASC Hyperactivity | 58 | 11 | 50.48 | 9.61 | 0.012 | 0.72 |
BASC Atypicality | 57.14 | 12.48 | 47.38 | 10.16 | 0.006 | 0.85 |
BASC Attention | 56.28 | 10.98 | 48.46 | 8.13 | 0.028 | 0.79 |
BASC Social Skills | 55.39 | 7.79 | 50.36 | 7.97 | 0.042 | 0.64 |
BASC Activities of Daily Living | 46.22 | 8.81 | 52.29 | 8.82 | 0.016 | 0.69 |
BASC Functional Communication | 46.62 | 8.03 | 52.7 | 6.6 | 0.017 | 0.82 |
Verbal IQ score was used as a covariate
Table 5. Performance on NEPSY Social Domain Scores, by Group.
TS | Controls | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | p | Cohen’s d | |
NEPSY Memory for Faces | 8.96 (n=36) |
3.14 | 13.10 (n=23) |
3.19 | <0.001 | 1.31 |
NEPSY Affect Recognition | 9.49 (n=25) |
2.89 | 10.49 (n=22) |
2.92 | 0.272 | 0.34 |
NEPSY Theory of Mind | 8.69 (n=12) |
2.08 | 11.34 (n=11) |
2.10 | 0.012 | 1.27 |
Verbal IQ score was used as a covariate
Acknowledgements
This study was supported by a grant from the National Institutes of Health, R01-HD049653 (Dr. Allan Reiss, principal investigator). Dr. David Hong is supported by the T32-MH19908 training grant and the APIRE/Lilly Psychiatric Research Fellowship. The authors thank Dr. Booil Jo, PhD for her statistical input. Further support was provided from the Turner Syndrome Society US, Chain of Love foundation and the Turner Syndrome Foundation. We would also like to thank all participants and their families for their involvement in this research.
Footnotes
There are no conflicts of interest or industry support to disclose for this research.
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Morgan T. Turner syndrome: diagnosis and management. Am Fam Physician. 2007 Aug 1;76(3):405–410. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.LaHood BJ, Bacon GE. Cognitive abilities of adolescent Turner’s syndrome patients. J Adolesc Health Care. 1985 Sep;6(5):358–364. doi: 10.1016/s0197-0070(85)80003-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Rovet JF. The psychoeducational characteristics of children with Turner syndrome. J Learn Disabil. 1993 May;26(5):333–341. doi: 10.1177/002221949302600506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Delooz J, Van den Berghe H, Swillen A, Kleczkowska A, Fryns JP. Turner syndrome patients as adults: a study of their cognitive profile, psychosocial functioning and psychopathological findings. Genet Couns. 1993;4(3):169–179. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Downey J, Ehrhardt AA, Gruen R, Bell JJ, Morishima A. Psychopathology and social functioning in women with Turner syndrome. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1989 Apr;177(4):191–201. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198904000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.van Pareren YK, Duivenvoorden HJ, Slijper FM, Koot HM, Drop SL, de Muinck Keizer-Schrama SM. Psychosocial functioning after discontinuation of long-term growth hormone treatment in girls with Turner syndrome. Horm Res. 2005;63(5):238–244. doi: 10.1159/000085841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Boman UW, Bryman I, Halling K, Moller A. Women with Turner syndrome: psychological well-being, self-rated health and social life. J Psychosom Obstet Gynaecol. 2001 Jun;22(2):113–122. doi: 10.3109/01674820109049961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Verlinde F, Massa G, Lagrou K, et al. Health and psychosocial status of patients with turner syndrome after transition to adulthood: the Belgian experience. Horm Res. 2004;62(4):161–167. doi: 10.1159/000080099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lagrou K, Froidecoeur C, Verlinde F, et al. Psychosocial functioning, self-perception and body image and their auxologic correlates in growth hormone and oestrogen-treated young adult women with Turner syndrome. Horm Res. 2006;66(6):277–284. doi: 10.1159/000095547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Carel JC, Elie C, Ecosse E, et al. Self-esteem and social adjustment in young women with Turner syndrome--influence of pubertal management and sexuality: population-based cohort study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2006 Aug;91(8):2972–2979. doi: 10.1210/jc.2005-2652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Naess EE, Bahr D, Gravholt CH. Health status in women with Turner syndrome: a questionnaire study on health status, education, work participation and aspects of sexual functioning. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) 2009 May;72(5):678–684. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2265.2009.03715.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.McCauley E, Ito J, Kay T. Psychosocial functioning in girls with Turner’s syndrome and short stature: social skills, behavior problems, and self-concept. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1986 Jan;25(1):105–112. doi: 10.1016/s0002-7138(09)60606-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.McCauley E, Ross JL, Kushner H, Cutler G., Jr Self-esteem and behavior in girls with Turner syndrome. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1995 Apr;16(2):82–88. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.McCauley E, Feuillan P, Kushner H, Ross JL. Psychosocial development in adolescents with Turner syndrome. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2001 Dec;22(6):360–365. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200112000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Siegel PT, Clopper R, Stabler B. The psychological consequences of Turner syndrome and review of the National Cooperative Growth Study psychological substudy. Pediatrics. 1998 Aug;102(2 Pt 3):488–491. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Russell HF, Wallis D, Mazzocco MM, et al. Increased prevalence of ADHD in Turner syndrome with no evidence of imprinting effects. J Pediatr Psychol. 2006 Oct;31(9):945–955. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kilic BG, Ergur AT, Ocal G. Depression, levels of anxiety and self-concept in girls with Turner’s syndrome. J Pediatr Endocrinol Metab. 2005 Nov;18(11):1111–1117. doi: 10.1515/jpem.2005.18.11.1111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.McCauley E, Kay T, Ito J, Treder R. The Turner syndrome: cognitive deficits, affective discrimination, and behavior problems. Child Dev. 1987 Apr;58(2):464–473. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ross JL, Roeltgen D, Feuillan P, Kushner H, Cutler GB., Jr Use of estrogen in young girls with Turner syndrome: effects on memory. Neurology. 2000 Jan 11;54(1):164–170. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.1.164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Lawrence K, Kuntsi J, Coleman M, Campbell R, Skuse D. Face and emotion recognition deficits in Turner syndrome: a possible role for X-linked genes in amygdala development. Neuropsychology. 2003 Jan;17(1):39–49. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Mazzola F, Seigal A, MacAskill A, Corden B, Lawrence K, Skuse DH. Eye tracking and fear recognition deficits in Turner syndrome. Soc Neurosci. 2006;1(3-4):259–269. doi: 10.1080/17470910600989912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Romans SM, Stefanatos G, Roeltgen DP, Kushner H, Ross JL. Transition to young adulthood in Ullrich-Turner syndrome: neurodevelopmental changes. Am J Med Genet. 1998 Sep 1;79(2):140–147. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Choleris E, Ogawa S, Kavaliers M, et al. Involvement of estrogen receptor alpha, beta and oxytocin in social discrimination: A detailed behavioral analysis with knockout female mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2006 Oct;5(7):528–539. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00203.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Phan A, Lancaster KE, Armstrong JN, MacLusky NJ, Choleris E. Rapid effects of estrogen receptor alpha and beta selective agonists on learning and dendritic spines in female mice. Endocrinology. Apr;152(4):1492–1502. doi: 10.1210/en.2010-1273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Davenport ML. Approach to the patient with Turner syndrome. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2010 Apr;95(4):1487–1495. doi: 10.1210/jc.2009-0926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wechsler D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence - Third Edition. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio: 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Third Edition. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio: 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp SL. NEPSY: A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp SL. NEPSY Second Edition. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio: 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Constantino JN, Gruber CP. Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) Manual. Western Psychological Services; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Reynolds CR, Richmond BO. What I Think and Feel: a revised measure of Children’s Manifest Anxiety. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1997 Feb;25(1):15–20. doi: 10.1023/a:1025751206600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Doyle A, Ostrander R, Skare S, Crosby RD, August GJ. Convergent and criterion-related validity of the Behavior Assessment System for Children-Parent Rating Scale. J Clin Child Psychol. 1997 Sep;26(3):276–284. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2603_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Trauner DA, Spilkin AM, Williams J, Babchuck L. Specific cognitive deficits in young children with cystinosis: evidence for an early effect of the cystinosin gene on neural function. J Pediatr. 2007 Aug;151(2):192–196. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.02.062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lagrou K, Xhrouet-Heinrichs D, Heinrichs C, et al. Age-related perception of stature, acceptance of therapy, and psychosocial functioning in human growth hormone-treated girls with Turner’s syndrome. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1998 May;83(5):1494–1501. doi: 10.1210/jcem.83.5.4807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Donnelly SL, Wolpert CM, Menold MM, et al. Female with autistic disorder and monosomy X (Turner syndrome): parent-of-origin effect of the X chromosome. Am J Med Genet. 2000 Jun 12;96(3):312–316. doi: 10.1002/1096-8628(20000612)96:3<312::aid-ajmg16>3.0.co;2-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Marco EJ, Skuse DH. Autism-lessons from the X chromosome. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2006 Dec;1(3):183–193. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Rovet J, Ireland L. Behavioral phenotype in children with Turner syndrome. J Pediatr Psychol. 1994 Dec;19(6):779–790. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/19.6.779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Pine DS, Guyer AE, Goldwin M, Towbin KA, Leibenluft E. Autism spectrum disorder scale scores in pediatric mood and anxiety disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008 Jun;47(6):652–661. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31816bffa5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Lawrence K, Jones A, Oreland L, et al. The development of mental state attributions in women with X-monosomy, and the role of monoamine oxidase B in the sociocognitive phenotype. Cognition. 2007 Jan;102(1):84–100. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Skuse D, Morris J, Lawrence K. The amygdala and development of the social brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003 Dec;1008:91–101. doi: 10.1196/annals.1301.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kesler SR, Garrett A, Bender B, Yankowitz J, Zeng SM, Reiss AL. Amygdala and hippocampal volumes in Turner syndrome: a high-resolution MRI study of X-monosomy. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42(14):1971–1978. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Skuse DH, Morris JS, Dolan RJ. Functional dissociation of amygdala-modulated arousal and cognitive appraisal, in Turner syndrome. Brain : a journal of neurology. 2005;128(Pt 9):2084–2096. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Paton JJ, Belova MA, Morrison SE, Salzman CD. The primate amygdala represents the positive and negative value of visual stimuli during learning. Nature. 2006 Feb 16;439(7078):865–870. doi: 10.1038/nature04490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Ross JL, Kushner H, Zinn AR. Discriminant analysis of the Ullrich-Turner syndrome neurocognitive profile. Am J Med Genet. 1997 Oct 31;72(3):275–280. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-8628(19971031)72:3<275::aid-ajmg4>3.0.co;2-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Elgar K, Campbell R, Skuse D. Are you looking at me? Accuracy in processing line-of-sight in Turner syndrome. Proc Biol Sci. 2002 Dec 7;269(1508):2415–2422. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lawrence K, Campbell R, Swettenham J, et al. Interpreting gaze in Turner syndrome: impaired sensitivity to intention and emotion, but preservation of social cueing. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41(8):894–905. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00002-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Davenport ML. Moving toward an understanding of hormone replacement therapy in adolescent girls: looking through the lens of Turner syndrome. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1135:126–137. doi: 10.1196/annals.1429.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Guapo VG, Graeff FG, Zani AC, Labate CM, dos Reis RM, Del-Ben CM. Effects of sex hormonal levels and phases of the menstrual cycle in the processing of emotional faces. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009 Aug;34(7):1087–1094. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Skuse DH, James RS, Bishop DV, et al. Evidence from Turner’s syndrome of an imprinted X-linked locus affecting cognitive function. Nature. 1997 Jun 12;387(6634):705–708. doi: 10.1038/42706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Blakemore SJ, Choudhury S. Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006 Mar-Apr;47(3-4):296–312. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01611.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]