Abstract
Purpose
To examine how hours of sleep and wake times relate to between-person differences and day-to-day changes in diurnal cortisol rhythms in late adolescents
Methods
Older adolescents (N = 119) provided six cortisol samples (wakeup, +30min, + 2 hours, +8 hours, + 12 hours, and bedtime) on each of three consecutive days while wearing an actigraph. We examined how average (across 3 days) and day-to-day changes in hours of sleep and wake times related to diurnal cortisol patterns.
Results
On average, greater hours of sleep related steeper decline in cortisol across the days. Day-to-day analyses revealed that prior night’s hours of sleep predicted steeper diurnal slopes the next day, while greater waking cortisol levels and steeper slopes predicted greater hours of sleep and a later wake time the next day.
Conclusions
Our results suggest a bidirectional relationship between sleep and HPA axis activity.
Keywords: Actigraphy, Sleep, HPA axis, Cortisol, Diurnal Rhythms, Adolescence, Naturalistic, Diary Studies
Adolescence is marked by a biological shift in sleep patterns often resulting in more “owl-like” patterns of sleep (e.g., later bed times, later wake times) [1–3]. Such changes often conflict, particularly in later adolescence, with the growing social demands many individuals face [2, 4]. Findings indicate that late adolescents report more erratic sleep schedules, later waking times, and less sleep than early adolescents [4]. Less than 30% of late adolescents get the recommended 8 or more hours of sleep per night and nearly 40% of individuals report consistent poor sleep quality [5,6]. Such sleep patterns have negative implications for individuals’ outcomes including mood disorders [7], difficulties in school [8,9], and physical health (e.g., obesity[10]).
Sleep patterns have also been known to play an important role in regulating other aspects of physiology, including the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis. The HPA axis’ main hormone, cortisol, exhibits a strong diurnal rhythm with levels high at waking, increasing further to a peak 30 minutes after waking (called the cortisol awakening response [CAR]) and then decreasing rapidly across the day, reaching nadir around midnight [11]. These changes in cortisol coincide with individuals’ sleep cycles; low levels of cortisol are present during the first half of the night which is dominated by deep sleep [or slow wave sleep (SWS)], while increases in cortisol accompany the second half of the night which is dominated by rapid eye movement (REM) sleep (more wakeful period) [12,13].
This correspondence has been the topic of research, primarily among adults, revealing a bidirectional and complex relation between sleep and HPA axis functioning. That is, HPA axis functioning has been shown to influence sleep patterns [14,15] and changes in sleep patterns, specifically waketimes and hours of sleep, have been found to affect daytime cortisol rhythms [16, 17]. Surprisingly, however, the relation between sleep and HPA axis functioning has yet to be examined among adolescents. This is an important gap, given that adolescence is a critical developmental period in which physiological changes occur [18, 19] and sleep patterns are shifting (especially among late adolescents [20]), Further, recent research has linked diurnal cortisol functioning, specifically, flattened diurnal slopes across the waking day, to a variety of physical and mental health outcomes [21 – 24]. A better understanding of the relation of nighttime sleep and daytime cortisol rhythms may have important implications for our understanding of the development of a variety of disorders relating to both sleep and the HPA axis. In this study, we take an initial step in trying to understand associations between adolescent sleep and diurnal cortisol by examining how hours of sleep and wake times relate to late adolescents’ waking cortisol values, CAR, and diurnal decline across the waking day. Utilizing multiple salivary cortisol samples over consecutive days, we first examined between-person differences by investigating how individuals’ average (across days of sampling) hours of sleep and average wake times related to average cortisol rhythms. Then, to gain a better understanding of the reciprocal relation between sleep and HPA axis functioning, we investigated how within-person day-to-day variations in sleep patterns (i.e. wake time, hours of sleep) related to day-today variations in diurnal cortisol.
Method
Participants
Data for the current study come from a larger longitudinal study focused on adolescents from two diverse public high schools (one in the Midwest, one on the West Coast). For the larger study, participants high in neuroticism [25] were oversampled, resulting in 61% of the sample scoring in the top third of the neuroticism screener. Seventy-nine percent of the Midwest participants (n = 243) were randomly invited to participate in a longitudinal cortisol sampling protocol. Of those invited, 173 (71%) agreed to participate and completed Wave 1 cortisol sampling. The current study utilizes data from 152 adolescents who participated in Wave 2 of cortisol collection (approximately 1.5 years after Wave 1) at which point actigraph measures of sleep were added. Adolescents who were pregnant (n = 1), currently taking steroids (n = 7) or had missing data on study variables (n = 25) were excluded. This resulted in 119 adolescents (77% female). The greater proportion of females is accounted for by the fact that females on average report higher levels of neuroticism [26]. Of the current study’s sample, 64.7% scored in the top third of the neuroticism screener.1 Adolescents ranged in age from 17.93 to 20.10 years old (M = 19.01) and came from varying ethnic/racial backgrounds (see Table 1).
Table 1.
Variable | % | n | Mean | SD | Range |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age of adolescent | 19.01 | 0.42 | 17.93 – 20.10 | ||
ake time (self-report) hh:mm | 08:38 | 1:28 | 03:13 – 13:30 | ||
Wake time (Actigraph) hh:mm | 08:47 | 1:22 | 05:48 – 12:33 | ||
Hours of nighttime sleep (self-report) | 7.71 | 1.34 | 3.00 – 10.61 | ||
Hours of nighttime sleep (Actigraph) | 6.41 | 1.07 | 1.41 – 9.55 | ||
Sleep efficiency % (Actigraph) | 81.85 | 7.61 | 48.80 – 92.60 | ||
Hours of naptime (self-report) | 0.41 | 0.77 | 0.00 – 04.72 | ||
Average waketime cortisol (μg/dl) | 0.30 | 0.21 | 0.01 – 1.37 | ||
Average +30 min cortisol (μg/dl) | 0.50 | 0.25 | 0.01 – 1.78 | ||
Average + 3 hours cortisol (μg/dl) | 0.28 | 0.20 | 0.03 – 1.80 | ||
Average + 8 hours cortisol (μg/dl) | 0.16 | 0.11 | 0.03 – 0.82 | ||
Average + 12 hours cortisol (μg/dl) | 0.12 | 0.11 | 0.01 – 0.76 | ||
Average bedtime cortisol (μg/dl) | 0.10 | 0.11 | 0.01 – 0.71 | ||
Caffeine (drinks per day) | 0.91 | 1.40 | 0.00 – 8.00 | ||
Nicotine (cigarettes per day) | 0.28 | 0.82 | 0.00 – 5.00 | ||
Alcohol (drinks per week) | 4.38 | 7.45 | 0.00 – 50.00 | ||
Exercise (hours per week) | 2.77 | 4.07 | 0.00 – 30.00 | ||
Oral contraceptive use (females only) | 35.5 | 33 | |||
Asthma medication | 7.6 | 9 | |||
Depression medication | 4.2 | 5 | |||
Male | 21.8 | 26 | |||
European American | 61.4 | 73 | |||
African American | 17.6 | 21 | |||
Hispanic | 4.2 | 5 | |||
Multiple/other race | 16.8 | 20 |
Procedures
Participants were sent a study packet that contained an actigraph [27], dairy booklets, a mechanical kitchen timer (to assist with the timing of 2nd sample), straws, vials, labels and a health/medical questionnaire. Study personnel contacted participants to review the study protocol. Participants provided six salivary cortisol samples and completed six diary entries per day for three consecutive typical weekdays. Participants wore an actigraph the night before starting the cortisol sampling and left it on until the morning after the last day of the study. Participants were paid $30 for completion of the sampling protocol and given a summary of basic sleep statistics across the 3 days of sampling. As approved by our Institutional Review Board, individual sleep data were used only for descriptive and hypothesis testing purposes, not for assessment or diagnosis of sleep disorders.
Measures
Salivary cortisol
Saliva samples were gathered each day for three days, at wake-up, 30 minutes after waking, at bedtime and three semi-random times throughout the day signaled by the actigraph watch (approximately 2, 8, and 12 hours post-awakening)2. Participants expelled saliva through a small straw into a 2 mL polypropylene tube and labeled tubes with the time and date. Participants were instructed not to eat, drink, or brush their teeth 30 minutes before sampling. Samples were returned by mail, refrigerated at −20 degrees Celsius, and then sent on dry ice by courier to Biochemisches Labor, Trier, Germany to be assayed for cortisol. Cortisol levels are stable at room temperature for several weeks and are unaffected by shipping [28]. Assays were conducted using a time-resolved immunoassay with fluorometric detection (DELFIA; see reference 29 for greater assay description). Intra-assay coefficients of variation (CVs) were between 4.0% and 6.7%, and inter-assay CVs ranged from 7.1% to 9.0%.
Objective sleep
During the 3-day salivary cortisol data collection, individuals wore the Actiwatch Score (Phillips Respironics, Inc.), a wrist-based accelerometer placed on the non-dominant hand that quantifies movement across the waking day and during sleep. To score data, the Actiware-Sleep software (version 3.4) validated algorithm was used [30]3. Utilizing one minute epochs and based on significant movement after at least 10 minutes of inactivity, this algorithm calculates a variety of sleep parameters that include sleep end (wake time) and hours of sleep (sleep time excluding all periods of wakefulness during the total sleep period)[31]. Actigraph sleep estimates have been validated against concurrent polysomnography [32]. Aggregate sleep parameters were created by averaging individuals’ sleep parameters across the 3 sampling days; sleep parameters for each day were used in the day-to-day analyses.
Diary and health variables
Using paper and pencil dairies, adolescents reported on sleep and health behaviors for each day of cortisol sampling. Specifically, adolescents reported their waking time and previous night’s bedtime as well as any duration of time spent in a nap during the day. Adolescents also completed diary entries with each saliva sample, reporting whether they consumed caffeinated drinks, cigarettes, alcohol, medication or exercised within an hour of each sample. Youth also reported in a health questionnaire if they were taking birth control (females only) or anti-depressant medication.
Analytic Plan
A 3-level multilevel growth-curve analysis was utilized to account for the nested nature of our data [33,34]. This modeling controls for nonindependence associated with nesting and allows for levels of cortisol to be predicted by moment-level variables (Level 1), day-varying variables (Level 2), and individual-level variables (Level 3). In line with previous studies [34, 35], day level variables were added, both lagged (minus 1 day) and non-lagged (night after cortisol collection) at Level 2 to model, simultaneously, the effect of prior night sleep parameters on next-day cortisol, and the effect of cortisol on sleep later that evening. As recommended [36], Level 2 variables were centered within cluster (CWC) and Level 3 variables were centered at the grand mean (CGM). At Level 1, time was centered as hours since waking (e.g., waking = 0).
We first modeled the latent estimates of the parameters defining each individual’s diurnal cortisol rhythm. Next, we entered individuals’ 3-day average sleep parameters (i.e., wake time, hours of sleep) at Level 3 to examine their associations with average cortisol levels (See Equation 1, Table 2). Cortisol values are predicted by the time of each sample, scaled as hours since waking each day, such that the β000 (the intercept) reflects the average wakeup cortisol level across individuals, β100 reflects individuals’ average CAR4 and β200 reflects the average linear slope of participants’ diurnal cortisol rhythms5. Coefficients β001, β101, and β201 reflect the effect of average sleep parameters on average wakeup cortisol level, average CAR and average linear slope, respectively. Average wake time and sleep were entered in separate models, followed by an analysis that included both.
Table 2.
| ||
|
Finally, to understand the impact of changes in sleep parameters on day-to-day changes in cortisol rhythms within individuals, we examined the relation of sleep parameters the night before and after cortisol sampling to diurnal cortisol profiles each day by entering day level sleep parameters at Level 2 (See Equation 2, Table 2). For wake time, the sleep parameter before cortisol refers to the wake time in the morning before cortisol sampling that day, while sleep parameter after cortisol refers to wake time the morning after cortisol sampling. For hours of sleep, the sleep parameter before cortisol refers to the previous night’s hours of sleep before the start of the next day’s cortisol sampling, while the sleep parameter after cortisol refers to the hours of sleep after that day’s cortisol sampling. Coefficients γ01j, γ11j and γ21j reflect the relation of the sleep parameter the night before cortisol collection to subsequent day’s wakeup cortisol level, CAR, and linear slope, respectively, while coefficients γ02j, γ12j and γ22j reflect the relation of wakeup cortisol, CAR and linear slope, respectively to the sleep parameters taken that night, after cortisol sampling each day. Wake time and average hours of sleep were entered first separately and then simultaneously in the final model. For all analyses, adolescents’ diary reports of their caffeine, nicotine, and alcohol consumption, exercise, and medication use within the hour prior to each sample were entered as covariates at Level 1 while birth control, asthma, depression medication, and daytime sleep (naps) were entered as covariates at Level 3.
Results
See Table 1 for descriptive information. Cortisol values are presented in original μg/dL units in Table 1; because they were skewed (2.48) and kurtotic (9.57), natural log transformed values were used in all analyses. In line with recommendations [37], cortisol values were top coded at 1.80 μg/dl (equivalent to 50 nmol/L). The base model revealed the expected diurnal patterns; high values upon awakening (β000 = −1.40, SE = .06, p < .0001; equivant to .25 μg/dl.), a strong increase (68%) in levels in the first 30 minutes (CAR; β100 = .52, SE = .04, p < .0001), and approximately an 8% per hour decline in cortisol levels from wake up to bedtime6 (β200 = −.09, SE = .01, p < .0001).
Between-Individual Analysis
As seen in Table 3, average wake time did not relate to average diurnal cortisol profiles (Model 1). Average hours of sleep (Model 2), however, related to rate of decline in cortisol; greater hours of sleep related a steeper decline in cortisol across the day (1.1% steeper slope for every additional hour of sleep). These effects were maintained even after controlling for wake time (Model 3).
Table 3.
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | |
---|---|---|---|
Fixed effects | Coefficient (SE) | Coefficient (SE) | Coefficient (SE) |
Wake up cortisol level | |||
Intercept | −1.423 (.059)*** | −1.423 (.059)*** | −1.424 (.060)*** |
Average wake time | −0.005 (.036) | −0.009 (.036) | |
Average hours of sleep | 0.032 (.051) | 0.036 (.052) | |
CAR | |||
Intercept | 0.544 (.041)*** | 0.543 (.042)*** | 0.544 (.041)*** |
Average wake time | 0.017 (.032) | 0.020 (.032) | |
Average hours of sleep | −0.013 (.043) | −0.019 (.042) | |
Time since waking | |||
Intercept | −0.091 (.005)*** | −0.092 (.005)*** | −0.091 (.005)*** |
Average wake time | 0.008 (.004) | 0.010 (.004)* | |
Average hours of sleep | −0.009 (.005)* | −0.012 (.005)* |
Note.
p ≤ .05,
p< .01,
p< .001.
Race/ethnicity, gender, birth control, asthma medication, depression medication, and daytime sleep (naps) were added as covariates at Level 3. Caffeine use, nicotine use, and alcohol use, any medication and hours of exercise were added as covariates at Level 1.
Within-Individual Analysis
As seen in Table 4 (Model 1), when relating changes in sleep to changes in cortisol rhythms within individuals across multiple days, same day wake time was a significant predictor of subsequent wakeup cortisol values, CAR, and decline in cortisol. That is, adolescents who woke up later that day had higher wakeup cortisol, compared to days they woke up earlier (9.5% increase in morning cortisol for every 1 hour later wake time), a less pronounced CAR (10.5 % decrease in CAR for every 1 hour increase in wake time) and steeper decline in cortisol (1.3% steeper slopes for every 1 hour increase in wake time) across the day. In Model 2, prior nights’ hours of sleep predicted wakeup cortisol, CAR, and decline in cortisol; greater hours of sleep the night before predicted higher wakeup cortisol (15.5% increase for every hour increase in sleep), a lower CAR (10.4% decrease for every hour increase in sleep), and steeper decline in cortisol (1.3% steeper slope for every hour increase in sleep) the next day. Further, greater levels of wakeup cortisol predicted greater hours of sleep the next day.
Table 4.
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 3 | |
---|---|---|---|
Fixed effects | Coefficient (SE) | Coefficient (SE) | Coefficient (SE) |
Wake up cortisol level | |||
Intercept | −1.421 (.058)*** | −1.422 (.060)*** | −1.418 (.058)*** |
Same day wake time | 0.091 (.036)* | −0.002 (.037) | |
Next day wake time | 0.007 (.031) | −0.055 (.031) | |
Prior day hours of sleep | 0.144 (.035)*** | 0.150 (.040)*** | |
Next day hours of sleep | 0.079 (.028)** | 0.110 (.028)*** | |
CAR | |||
Intercept | 0.540 (.041)*** | 0.544 (.041)*** | 0.540 (.041)*** |
Same day wake time | −0.114 (.054)* | −0.056 (.059) | |
Next day wake time | −0.055 (.042) | −0.058 (.047) | |
Prior day hours of sleep | −0.110 (.043)* | −0.075 (.045) | |
Next day hours of sleep | −0.039 (.044) | 0.001 (.048) | |
Time since waking | |||
Intercept | −0.092 (.005)*** | −0.092 (.005)*** | −0.092 (.005)*** |
Same day wake time | −0.013 (.004)** | − 0.009 (.005) | |
Next day wake time | 0.007 (.004) | 0.015 (.004)*** | |
Prior day hours of sleep | −0.013 (.004)** | −0.010 (.004)* | |
Next day hours of sleep | −0.005 (.004) | −0.015 (.004)** |
Note.
p< .05,
p< .01,
p< .001.
Race/ethnicity, gender, birth control, asthma medication, depression medication were added as covariates at Level 3; Caffeine use, nicotine use, and alcohol use, any medication and hours of exercise were added as covariates at Level 1.
Results examining wake time and hours of sleep together (Model 3) revealed that prior and next day hours of sleep both predicted wakeup cortisol levels; greater hours of sleep the night prior predicted greater wakeup cortisol the next day, whereas greater wakeup cortisol that day related to greater hours of sleep that night (controlling for prior night hours of sleep). For diurnal slopes, a similar bidirectional relation emerged. Specifically, prior and next day hours of sleep both predicted steeper decline in cortisol slopes; greater hours of sleep the night before predicted a steeper decline in cortisol the subsequent day, and steeper slopes that day related to greater hours of sleep that night. Finally, next day wake time related to diurnal slopes, suggesting that flatter diurnal slopes that day predicted later waking times the following day.
Discussion
The current study took an important first step in understanding how typical hours of sleep and wake times relate to cortisol diurnal rhythms and how late adolescents’ day-to-day variation in sleep relates to day-to-day variation in cortisol. Our findings suggest that across individuals, average hours of sleep are a steeper diurnal decline in cortisol across the day, even after accounting for adolescents’ average wake times. These results are consistent with findings among adults [16] suggesting that individuals who sleep more have diurnal cortisol rhythms characterized by a steeper decline across the day and align with health findings suggesting that such patterns are considered normative and healthy [38]. The potentially bidirectional interrelations between sleep and the HPA axis [12], however, make it difficult to know if hours of sleep are impacting changes in diurnal rhythms or if diurnal rhythms are impacting hours of sleep.
A better understanding of causal directionality is gained from our day-to-day within-person examination. Findings suggest that on nights adolescents slept more, they woke up the next morning with higher waking cortisol levels and exhibited a steeper decline in their cortisol across the day. Further, on days adolescents had higher wakeup cortisol values and a steeper diurnal decline in their cortisol, they experienced greater hours of sleep that night and tended to wake up earlier the following day. An alternate way of viewing these day-to-day changes is that on days adolescents’ slept less, they woke up the following morning with lower waking cortisol levels and had flatter cortisol slopes that day. On days that adolescents had lower wakeup values and flatter slopes, they subsequently experienced less hours of sleep that night and woke up later the next day. It is important to emphasize that these day-to-day analyses examine within-person changes in sleep patterns and diurnal cortisol from one day to the next, rather than trait or between-person differences. As such, these analyses are less subject to third variable explanations, such as the possibility that stable genetic or personality factors determine both sleep timing and cortisol patterns.
Together, our findings suggest a bidirectional relation between sleep and the HPA axis. While the exact mechanisms underlying the relations between cortisol and the HPA axis are not fully known, our findings could be linked with the correspondence of sleep cycles and cortisol secretion; deeper sleep accompanies the quiescent period of the HPA axis, while greater cortisol output has been found during the later stages of sleep (dominated by REM sleep) [12,13]. Individuals who sleep less and wake earlier might have less time to “ramp up” their cortisol levels (which happen during REM sleep) and in turn end up with lower awakening cortisol and flatter slopes than individuals who slept more. Further, guided by previous laboratory findings that suggest that higher levels of cortisol have the ability to suppress SWS leading to subsequent sleep deprivation [39,40], individuals with flatter slopes (partially due to higher evening levels7) might have a difficult time falling asleep or staying asleep leading to fewer total hours of sleep. In an attempt to recover from sleep loss during the night, individuals then sleep later the next morning. Given that the specific sleep stages cannot be examined using actigraphy and we do not have overnight cortisol samples, these explanations remain speculative.
In sum, our study is the first to examine these relations in adolescents using an objective sleep measure and examining within-individual covariation between sleep and cortisol in a naturalistic setting. Further, our study examines these relations during a sensitive and unique developmental period in which changes in physiology and sleep patterns are especially relevant. [19, 20]. Our findings suggest that the choices made about sleep during this time may have consequences for physiology, and in turn, physiology (the HPA axis) attempts to adapt to late adolescents’ changing daily schedules. Despite these contributions, however, it is important to note several limitations of our study. First, while our findings suggest that sleep relates to fluctuation in diurnal cortisol, the changes were relatively small (1–2% change for diurnal slopes and 9–16% changes in waking cortisol). Second, we examined a limited number of days in our examination of day-to-day changes and might not be fully capturing the complexities of sleep and its relations to HPA axis functioning over a longer period of time. Third, we did not use electronic monitoring devices to track the exact timing of cortisol sampling; we are reliant on participant self-report of sampling timings. Failure to time samples appropriately, however, would most likely lead to a reduction in the size of our effects, rather than systematic bias in our data. Finally, individuals with greater neuroticism and females were over represented in our sample. We found no differences in study variables by neuroticism risk; however, future research should examine whether associations between sleep and HPA axis functioning are similar across personality characteristics and gender. Despite these limitations, our study provides an important first step in understanding the complex and bidirectional relations between adolescent sleep and HPA axis functioning.
Acknowledgments
Work on this paper was supported in part by a William T. Grant Scholars Award and an Institute for Policy Research Faculty Fellowship to Dr. Adam, and by NIMH R01 MH65652 (R. Zinbarg, P.I). Funding supporting the training of the first and third author was provided by the School of Social and Family Dynamics’ Intensive Summer Interdisciplinary Experience Graduate Fellowship.
Footnotes
Study variables were examined for differences by neuroticism risk score. Only differences in caffeine use and asthma medication emerged. High risk individuals reported greater caffeine use than low or medium risk individuals [F (2,116) = 3.04, p = .05], while low risk individuals were more likely to be taking asthma medication than the other two groups [χ2 (2) = 9.02, p < .05].
Participants were asked for their typical waketime for each day of the study and watches were programmed to signal 2, 8, and 12 hours from this time (day 2). Thirty minute variations were added for day 1 and day 3 to make signals less expected [i.e., day 1 participants took samples 30 min early (1.5, 7.5, and 11.5 hours from waking) and day 3 was 30 min later (2.5, 8.5, 12.5 hours from waking)].
Activity counts (A) within each epoch were calculated based on activity levels during the adjacent 2-min period using the following algorithm: A = E − 2(1/25) + E − 1(1/5) + E + E + 1(1/5) + E + 2(1/25). Threshold set to 40, with a range of 20 to 80.
Individuals’ 2nd sample (30 minutes after waking) was dummy coded where 1 = 2nd cortisol sample and 0 = all other samples.
The current analysis did not include a quadratic time variable because the inclusion of this term in the unconditional growth model did not improve our model fit ([χ2Δ (1) = .78, p = .37]). Given this, the slope can be interpreted as a linear decline across the day rather than the linear decline at waking.
Coefficients can be interpreted as the % change in outcome (cortisol) per unit change in the independent variable using the following formula β%change = [exp (βraw)] − 1 (Neter et al., 1990; Woolridge, 2000).
Time was centered at bedtime to examine if flatter slopes were partially due to higher evening levels in cortisol. Findings revealed that levels were trending [γ02 j = −.079, p = .18]; flatter lines were associated with less prior nights sleep partially due to higher evening cortisol levels.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Carskadon MA, Vieira C, Acebo C. Association between puberty and delayed phase preference. Sleep. 1993;16:258–262. doi: 10.1093/sleep/16.3.258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Dahl RE, Lewin DS. Pathways to adolescent health: Sleep regulation and behavior. J Adolesc Health. 2002;31(6S):175–184. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(02)00506-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA. Sleep schedules and daytime functioning in adolescents. Child Development. 1998;69:875–887. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ohayon MM, Roberts RE, Zulley J, et al. Prevalence and patterns of problematic sleep among older adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39:1549–1556. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200012000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lund HG, Reider BD, Whiting AB, et al. Sleep patterns and predictors of disturbed sleep in a large population of college students. J Adolesc Health. 2010;46:124–132. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Tsai L, Li S. Sleep patterns in college students: Gender differences and grade differences. J of Psychosom Research. 2004;56:231–237. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00507-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Peterson MJ, Benca RM. Sleep in mood disorders. Sleep Med Clin. 2008;3:231–249. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Fuligni AJ, Hardway C. Daily variations in adolescents’ sleep, activities, and psychological well being. J Res on Adolesc. 2006;16(3):353–378. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Dewald JF, Meijar AM, Oort FJ, et al. The influence of sleep quality, sleep duration and sleepiness on school performance in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis review. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14:179–189. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Agras WS, Hammer LD, McNicholas F, et al. Risk factors for childhood overweight: A prospective study from birth to 9. 5 years. J Pediatr. 2004;145:20–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2004.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Pruessner JC, Wolf OT, Hellhammer DH, et al. Free cortisol levels after awakening: A reliable biological marker for the assessment of adrenocortical activity. Life Sciences. 1997;61:2539–2549. doi: 10.1016/s0024-3205(97)01008-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Buckley TM, Schatzberg AF. Review: On the interactions of the hypothalamic–pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and sleep: Normal HPA axis activity and circadian rhythm, exemplary sleep disorders. J of Endocr & Metabolism. 2005;90:3106–3114. doi: 10.1210/jc.2004-1056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Born J, Muth S, Fehm HL. The significance of sleep onset and slow wave sleep for nocturnal release of growth hormone (gh) and cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1988;13:233–243. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(88)90021-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gillian JC, Jacobs LS, Fram DH, et al. Acute effect of glucocorticoid on normal human sleep. Nature. 1972;237:398–399. doi: 10.1038/237398a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bierwolf C, Struve K, Marshal L, et al. Slow wave sleep drives inhibition of pituitary-adrenal secretion in humans. J of Neuroendorcinology. 1997;9:479–484. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.1997.00605.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kumari M, Badrick E, Ferrie J, et al. Self reported sleep duration and sleep disturbance are independently associated with cortisol secretion in the Whitehall II study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2009;94:4801–4809. doi: 10.1210/jc.2009-0555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kudielka BM, Kirschbaum C. Awakening cortisol responses are influenced by health status and awakening time but not by menstrual cycle phase. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2003;28:35–47. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(02)00008-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Walker EF, Walder DJ, Reynolds F. Developmental changes in cortisol secretion in normal and at-risk youth. Dev and Psychopath. 2001;13:721–732. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401003169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Stroud LR, Foster E, Papandonatos GD, et al. Stress response and the adolescent transition: Performance versus peer rejection stressors. Dev and Pyschopathology. 2009;21:47–68. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Carskadon MA, Acebo C, Jenni OG. Regulations of adolescent sleep: Implications of behaviors. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2004;1021:276–291. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Adam EK, Doane LD, Zinbarg RE, et al. Prospective prediction of major depression disorder from cortisol awakening response in adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;36:921–931. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Boyle SH, Surwit RS, Georgiades A, et al. Depressive symptoms, race, and glucose concentrations. Diabetes Care. 2007;30:2484–8. doi: 10.2337/dc07-0258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Matthews K, Schwartz J, Cohen S, et al. Diurnal cortisol decline is related to coronary calcification: CARDIA study. Psychom Med. 2006;68:657–661. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000244071.42939.0e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Sephton SE, Sapolsky RM, Kraemer HC, et al. Diurnal cortisol rhythm as a predictor of breast cancer survival. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2000;92:994–1000. doi: 10.1093/jnci/92.12.994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Eysenck SB, Eysenck HJ, Barrett P. A revised version of the Psychoticism scale. Person Individ Diff. 1985;6(1):21–29. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Lynn R, Martin T. Gender differences in extraversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism in 37 Nations. J Soc Psych. 1997;137:369–373. doi: 10.1080/00224549709595447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Sadeh A, Acebo C. The role of actigraphy in sleep medicine. Sleep Med Rev. 2002;6:113–24. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2001.0182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Clements AD, Parker CR. The relationship between salivary cortisol concentrations in frozen versus mailed samples. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1998;23:613–616. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(98)00031-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Dressendorfer RA, Kirschbaum C, Rhode W, et al. Synthesis of a cortisol–biotin conjugate and evaluation as a tracer in an immunoassay for salivary cortisol measurement. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol. 1992;43:683–92. doi: 10.1016/0960-0760(92)90294-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Oakley NR. Validation with polysomnography of the sleepwatch sleep/wake scoring algorithm used by the Actiwatch Activity Monitoring System. Bend: MiniMitter, Cambridge Neurotechnology; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Littner M, Kushida CA, Anderson WM, et al. Practice parameters for the role of actigraphy in the study of sleep and circadian rhythms: an update for 2002. An American Academy of Sleep Medicine Practice Parameters. Standards of Practice Committee of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Sleep. 2003;26:337–41. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.3.337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Sadeh A, Hauri PJ, Kripke DF, Lavie P. The role of actigraphy in the evaluation of sleep disorders. Sleep. 1995;18:288–302. doi: 10.1093/sleep/18.4.288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Raudenbush SWB, Bryk AS, editors. Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods. Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Adam EK, Hawkley LC, Kudielka BM, et al. Day-to-day dynamics of experience-cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults. PNAS. 2006;103:17058–17063. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605053103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Doane LD, Adam EK. Loneliness and cortisol: Momentary, day-to-day, and trait associations. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35:430–441. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Enders CK, Tofighi D. Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: A new look at an old issue. Psychol Methods. 2007;12:121–138. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Nicolson NA. Measurement of cortisol. In: Luecken LJ, Gallo LC, editors. Handbook of Physiological Research Methods in Health Psychology. Sage Publications; 2008. pp. 37–74. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Adam EK, Kumari M. Assessing salivary cortisol in large-scale, epidemiological research. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(10):1423–1436. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Vazquez-Palacios G, Retana-Marquez S, Bonilla-Jaime H, et al. Further definition of the effect of corticosterone on the sleep-wake pattern in the male rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2001;70:305–310. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(01)00620-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Holsboer F, von Bardeleben U, Steiger A. Effects of intravenous corticotropin- releasing hormone upon sleep-related growth hormone surge and sleep EEG in man. Neuroendocrinology. 1998;48:32–38. doi: 10.1159/000124986. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]