Skip to main content
Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience logoLink to Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience
. 2010 Dec;12(4):503–515. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2010.12.4/imcgilchrist

Reciprocal organization of the cerebral hemispheres

Organización recíproca de los hemisferios cerebrales

Organisation réciproque des hémisphères cérébraux

Iain McGilchrist 1,*
PMCID: PMC3181995  PMID: 21319495

Abstract

The cerebral hemispheres are anatomically and neurophysiologically asymmetrical. The evolutionary basis for these differences remains uncertain. There are, however, highly consistent differences between the hemispheres, evident in reptiles, birds, and mammals, as well as in humans, in the nature of the attention each applies to the environment. This permits the simultaneous application of precisely focused, but narrow, attention, needed for grasping food or prey, with broad, open, and uncommitted attention, needed to watch out for predators and to interpret the intentions of conspecifics. These different modes of attention can account for a very wide range of repeated observations relating to hemisphere specialization, and suggest that hemisphere differences lie not in discrete functional domains as such, but distinct modes of functioning within any one domain. These modes of attention are mutually incompatible, and their application depends on inhibitory transmission in the corpus callosum. There is also an asymmetry of interaction between the hemispheres at the phenomenological level.

Keywords: brain, hemisphere, asymmetry, corpus callosum, attention, inhibition


The fact that the brain, an organ which exists precisely to make connections, has a deeply divided structure has remained largely unexplained and even unexamined. Nevertheless, speculation on the nature of the difference between the two cerebral hemispheres goes back more than two millennia: Greek physicians in the third century BC held that the right hemisphere was specialized for perception, and the left hemisphere for understanding.1 In more recent times, Wigan in 1844 deduced from a series of clinical cases that we 'must have two minds with two brains,' a redundancy which he thought protected against injury to one or other hemisphere, but with mental illness being the cost to the individual when they were in conflict.2 In the later 19th, and particularly in the 20th, century following the first callosotomy procedures of Sperry and Bogen, there arose a plethora of theories about the different functions the two hemispheres might perform, which broadly distinguished a verbal, rational, analytic left hemisphere from a visuospatially orientated, emotional, and holistic right hemisphere, though the evolutionary origin and basis of their anatomical and functional separation remained obscure.3 Subsequent research has in any case revealed that each hemisphere contributes to language, visuospatial skills, reason, and emotion, indeed to virtually every cerebral function, suggesting that the bihemispheric structure of the brain is an anomaly. At the same time, the persistence in popular culture of outdated characterizations of hemisphere difference has meant that the topic has somewhat fallen into disrepute.

Yet many important authors in the field (eg, Hellige,4 Ramachandran,5 Crow,6 Cutting7,8) accept that there is something manifestly important here that requires explanation. Hellige, while emphasizing that 'in the intact brain, it is rarely the case that one hemisphere can perform a task normally whereas the other hemisphere is completely unable to perform the task at all,' notes that 'the range of tasks showing hemispheric asymmetry is quite broad' and that 'thus far, it has not been possible to identify any single information-processing dichotomy that could account for anything close to this entire range of hemispheric asymmetries... Whatever links there might be between the various hemispheric asymmetries, they would seem to be determined in some other way or according to some other principle.'4 What might that principle be?

Brain asymmetries exist at many levels of description

The fact that hemispheric asymmetries exist at every level of description suggests that the interhemispheric distribution of neuropsychological functions is unlikely to be random. Such asymmetries exist at the gross anatomical level in the size, weight, and conformation of either hemisphere as a whole,9,10 but as well as differing in the size and shape of a number of defined brain areas,11 the hemispheres differ in the number of neurones,12 neuronal size,13 and the extent of dendritic branching within areas.14,15 The ratio of white to gray matter also differs, being higher in the right hemisphere.16,17 Neurochemically the hemispheres differ in their sensitivity to hormones18 and to pharmacological agents,19 and there are significant differences in the ratio of dopaminergic to noradrenergic neurotransmission.20,21

Functional independence of the hemispheres increases with evolution

Furthermore, the corpus callosum appears to be primarily involved in maintaining functional independence of the hemispheres. Though it contains an estimated 300 to 800 million fibers connecting topologically similar areas in either hemisphere, only 2% of cortical neurons are connected via the corpus callosum.22,23 What is more, a large number of these connections are functionally inhibitory24,25 Significant populations of cells projecting to the corpus callosum are GABA-ergic, and although the majority are glutamatergic, the excitatory fibers often terminate on interneurons whose function is inhibitory26,27 Stimulation of neurons in one hemisphere commonly results in an initial brief excitatory response, followed by a prolonged and often widespread inhibition in the contralateral hemisphere.28,29 Clearly the corpus callosum does also have excitatory functions, and both are necessary for normal human functioning,24,30 but the primary function of the corpus callosum may in fact be to allow reciprocal hemispheric inhibition.31-33 Separation of hemispheric function appears to accelerate with evolution, since interhemispheric connections decrease relative both to brain size,22 and to the degree of brain asymmetry34 In the ultimate case of H. sapiens, the twin hemispheres have been characterized as two autonomous systems.35

Attentional asymmetry in birds and animals

Functional brain asymmetries exist also in birds and animals. Lateralization of function is widespread in vertebrates,36 and appears to have evolutionary advantages. For example, Braun writes that ?the vast database of animal research [and] human neuropsychiatric research ... both clearly establish numerous important and spectacular specializations of the right hemisphere,' as well as of the left.37

It is argued here that these apparent specializations relate to differences in the mode of attention. Animals and birds experience competing needs. This can be seen at one level in terms of the types of attention they are required to bring to bear on the world. There is a need to focus attention narrowly and with precision, as a bird, for example, needs to focus on a grain of corn in order to distinguish it from the pieces of grit on which it lies. At the same time there is a need for open attention, as wide as possible, to guard against a possible predator. Chicks achieve this by prioritizing local information with the right eye (left hemisphere), and global information with the left eye (right hemisphere). Chicks that are properly lateralized are more able to use these two types of attention effectively than are those in which, experimentally, lateralization has not been permitted to develop (by depriving them of light exposure on day 19 of incubation).38 For many species of birds and animals there are biases at the population level towards watching out for predators with the left eye.38-46 Equally fixating an object, especially prey or food, is preferentially carried out using the right eye and foot.47

Individual animals with more strongly lateralized brains are better able, because of hemisphere specialization, to forage and remain aware of predators,48 and are more efficient,49 with shorter reaction times.50 But advantages accrue not only to the individual: being a more lateralized species at the population level carries advantages in social cohesion.51-53 The right hemisphere appears to be deeply involved in social functioning, not just in primates, where it is specialized in the expression of social feelings, but in lower animals and birds as well.38,39,54-56

Toads, for example, attend to their prey with the left hemisphere, but interact with their fellow toads using the right hemisphere,57 and while black-winged stilts peck more, and more successfully, at prey using the right eye, males are more likely to direct courtship displays to females that are seen with their left eye.55 In most animal species, intense emotional responses are related to the right hemisphere and inhibited by the left.61 Some of the same neuroendocrine hemisphere differences that characterize the human brain are already present in the brains of rats58,59: this may be related to the fact that in rats, as in humans, the right hemisphere is the main locus of early social experience.60

Lateralization brings evolutionary advantages, particularly in carrying out dual-attention tasks.41 In general terms, the left hemisphere yields narrow, focused attention, mainly for the purpose of getting and feeding. The right hemisphere yields a broad, vigilant attention, the purpose of which appears to be awareness of signals from the surroundings, especially of other creatures, who are potential predators or potential mates, foes, or friends; and it is involved in bonding in social animals. Individual human brains, like animal brains, that are less lateralized (as defined by handedness) than the norm appear to show global deficits. In humans this applies across all forms of reasoning, verbal and nonverbal.62 In a word, asymmetry pays.63

Attentional asymmetry in humans

This relationship between mode of attention and lateralization in animals also pertains in humans. It is conventional to distinguish five types of attention: vigilance, sustained attention, and alertness, forming the intensity axis, and focused attention and divided attention, forming the selectivity axis of attention.64 The different types of attention can be demonstrated to be distinct and independent of one another, and subserved by a number of different brain structures distributed extensively over the prefrontal, anterior cingulate, and posterior parietal areas of both hemispheric cortices. Within either hemisphere, and between hemispheres, the system of control processes is complex. However, some broad consistent differences in hemisphere specialization are striking.

Vigilance and sustained attention are grossly impaired in subjects with right-hemisphere lesions,65-67 especially right frontal lesions,68 and by contrast, are preserved in left-hemisphere lesions.69 Right-hemisphere lesions also lead to perceptuomotor slowing, a sign of diminished alertness, associated with lapses of attention, 70-76 though in one study slowing was associated with lesions in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.77 Studies in both normal subjects78,79 and split-brain subjects80 corroborate the role of the right hemisphere in the “intensity” aspects of attention, confirmed by neuroimaging.81,82

The other main axis of attention is selectivity (focused and divided attention). Although selective attention may be bilateral,83 deficits in focused attention are more severe with left-hemisphere injury,76,85 typically in the left caudate,77 or left anterior cingulate.84 Normal subjects show a left-hemisphere preference for choice reactions,86,87 and imaging suggests focused attention is associated with activity in the left orbitofrontal cortex and basal ganglia.88 By contrast, lesions in the right inferior parietal lobule cause the most serious impairment of global attention.89

As regards divided attention, the evidence is less conclusive. While some studies suggest that both left and right hemispheres are involved,77 there appears to be a clear primary role for the right hemisphere, especially the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.88,90

In summary, there is evidence of left-hemisphere dominance for local, narrowly focussed attention and righthemisphere dominance for broad, sustained, global, and flexible attention. 65,66,91-95 The scope of the right hemisphere's world is broad.96-97 Subjects with a right-hemisphere lesion start with the pieces and put them together to get the overall picture, whereas those with a left-hemisphere lesion prefer a global approach.98-102 Right-hemisphere damaged subjects seem unable to adjust the breadth of the “spotlight” of their attention and suffer 'an excessive and more or less permanent narrowing of their attentional window.'103

The attentional difference between the hemispheres has many consequences for the phenomenological world which each construes, potentially accounting for a range of apparent functional specializations of either hemisphere. These specializations, which the author has described in detail and discussed at length elsewhere,104 offer perceptual advantages that are reciprocally related, and are summarized in the following section.

Some consequences for hemispheric specialization

The above distinction in attention could be seen as offering the reciprocal possibilities of breadth and flexibility in apprehending the unpredictable and (as yet) unknown, versus the focus and precision required to grasp and use what is familiar and has already been prioritized as of interest.

The new versus the known

The right hemisphere alone attends to the peripheral field of vision from which new experience tends to come; only the right hemisphere can direct attention to what comes to us from the edges of our awareness, regardless of side.105,106 Anything newly entering our experiential world instantly triggers release of noradrenaline, mainly in the right hemisphere.96,107 Novel experience induces changes in the right hippocampus, but not the left.108 Phenomenologically it is the right hemisphere that is attuned to the apprehension of anything new.38,107,109-118

This difference is pervasive across domains. Not just new experience, but the learning of new information or new skills also engages right-hemisphere attention more than left,119,120 even if the information is verbal in nature.121,122 However, once the skills have become familiar through practice, they shift to being the concern of the left hemisphere,107 even for skills such as playing a musical instrument.123

The left hemisphere prioritizes the expected, and its process is predictive.124,125 This makes it more efficient in routine situations, but less efficient wherever the initial assumptions have to be revised,126,127 or when there is a need to distinguish old information from new material that may be consistent with it.128 Because the left hemisphere is drawn by its expectations, the right hemisphere outperforms the left whenever prediction is difficult because the situation is new to the subject.129 The link between the right hemisphere and what is new or emotionally engaging exists not just in humans, but already in higher mammals: for example, horses perceive new and possibly emotionally arousing stimuli with the left eye.130

Possibility versus predictability

The right hemisphere is more capable of a frame shift;131-133 the right frontal lobe is especially important for flexibility of thought, with damage in that area leading to perseveration.134-136 In problem solving, the right hemisphere presents an array of possible solutions, which remain “live” while alternatives are explored;137,138 the left hemisphere takes the single solution that seems best to fit what it already knows and latches onto it.21,139 Ramachandran's studies of anosognosia reveal a tendency for the left hemisphere to deny discrepancies that do not fit its already generated schema of things, a strategy that works well in familiar situations in which there are time-costs to exploring unnecessary possibilities. The right hemisphere, by contrast, is actively watching for discrepancies, more like a devil's advocate.140 Both approaches are needed, but are mutually contradictory.

These differences operate across all realms and apply equally to the verbal as to the visuospatial. For example, the left hemisphere operates focally, suppressing verbal meanings that are not currently relevant, whereas the right hemisphere recruits wider semantic associations,141-145 and the right posterior superior temporal sulcus may be selectively involved in verbal creativity146 In the “close” situation, by contrast, the left hemisphere actively suppresses the right, to exclude associations which are semantically only distantly related.147,148

Integration versus division

In general the left hemisphere is more closely interconnected within itself, and within regions of itself, than the right hemisphere.14,17 By contrast, the right hemisphere has a greater degree of myelination, facilitating swift transfer of information between the cortex and centers below the cortex,17 and greater connectivity in general.149 Functionally, its superior integration is evidenced by EEG measures150 and by the more diffuse but overlapping somatosensory projections and auditory inputs in the right hemisphere.151

At the experiential level it is also better able to integrate perceptual processes, particularly bringing together different kinds of information from different senses.109,154,155 By contrast, the left hemisphere may be 'inadequate for the more rapid complex syntheses achieved by the [right] hemisphere.'156

As mentioned, new stimuli lead to release of noradrenalin in the right hemisphere. Its neurons are relatively resistant to fatigue, so that exploratory attention is held open across a greater expanse of both space and time.21 The range of the right hemisphere is further increased by the fact that it has a longer working memory, and so is able both to access more information and hold it together at any one time for longer.157 It is capable of bearing more information in mind and doing so over longer periods, with greater specificity (which also means less susceptibility to degradation over time by memory).128,157-159 This broader field of attention, open to whatever may be, and coupled with greater integration over time and space, is what makes possible the recognition of broad or complex patterns, the perception of the “thing as a whole,” seeing the wood for the trees.160-163 In short, the left hemisphere takes a local short-term view, where the right hemisphere sees the bigger picture.

The whole versus the part

The link between the right hemisphere and holistic or Gestalt perception is one of the most reliable and durable of the generalizations about hemisphere differences, which follows from the differences in the nature of attention.95,164-168 The right hemisphere sees the whole, before whatever it is gets broken up into parts in our attempt to know it, and its holistic processing is not based on summation of parts. The right hemisphere, with its greater integrative power, is constantly searching for patterns in things, and its understanding is based on complex pattern recognition.164-169 On the other hand, the left hemisphere sees part-objects.118,170-172

Subjects with unilateral brain damage show complementary deficits in drawing skills, depending on whether it is right or left hemisphere function that is compromised. The productions of those with right-hemisphere damage, relying on their left hemisphere, lose overall coherence and integrity, and become so distorted they are barely recognizable: there is no grasp of the Gestalt. The drawings of those with left-hemisphere damage, by contrast, relying on their right hemisphere, exhibit relative poverty of detail, because the accent is on the shape of the whole.173,174

Context versus abstraction

For the same reason that the right hemisphere sees things as a whole, it also sees each thing in its context, as standing in a qualifying relationship with all that surrounds it, rather than taking it as a single isolated entity.129,175-176 Whatever is not explicit or literal, that requires contextual understanding, depends on the right frontal lobe for its meaning to be conveyed or received.176 The right hemisphere understands from indirect contextual clues, not only from explicit statement, whereas the left hemisphere will identify by concepts rather than from the experiential context (eg, identifies that it must be winter because it is “January,” not by looking at the trees).177,178

This difference is particularly important when it comes to language. Whereas the left hemisphere has more sophisticated syntax and a greater semantic range, the right hemisphere takes whatever is said within its entire context.179 It is specialized in pragmatics, the art of contextual understanding of meaning, and in using metaphor.180,181 The right temporal region appears to be essential for the integration of two seemingly unrelated concepts into a meaningful metaphoric expression.182 All conceptual thought is ultimately metaphorical in nature.183 The left hemisphere, because its thinking is decontextualized, tends towards a relatively inflexible following of the internal logic of the situation, even if this is in contravention of everything experience tells us.184

Individuals versus categories

At the same time it is the right hemisphere that stores details to distinguish specific instances.185 The right hemisphere presents individual, unique instances of things and individual, familiar, objects, where the left hemisphere represents categories of things, and generic, nonspecific objects.118,186-187 In keeping with this, the right hemisphere uses unique referents, where the left hemisphere uses non-unique referents.118,119,188-190 It is with the right hemisphere that we distinguish individuals of all kinds, places as well as faces.191 In fact it is precisely its capacity for holistic processing that enables the right hemisphere to recognize individuals, since individuals are Gestalt, indivisible, wholes.159,186,192 In keeping with the principle that it is not what is done, but how it is done, that distinguishes the two hemispheres, one cannot say that one hemisphere deals with single items (“units'”), and the other with “aggregates.” Both deal with “units” and both deal with “aggregates.” Thus, the right sees individual entities (its kind of units) in all their uniqueness, and it sees them as belonging in a contextual whole (its kind of aggregate), from which they are not divided. By contrast the left sees parts (its kind of units), which go to make up a something which it recognizes by the category to which it belongs (its kind of aggregate). However, the relationship between the smaller unit and the broader aggregate in either case is profoundly different: as is the mode of attention to the world with which it is associated.

Where the left hemisphere is more concerned with abstract categories and types, the right hemisphere is more concerned with the uniqueness and individuality of each entity189,193,194 The right hemisphere's role as what Ramachandran has described as the“anomaly detector” might in fact be seen rather as an aspect of its preference for things as they actually exist (which are never entirely static or congruent - always changing, never the same) over abstract representation, in which things become fixed and equivalent, types rather than individuals.

Where the left hemisphere utilizes abstract categories, the right hemisphere organizes experience according to specific exemplars.195-198 The left hemisphere takes an invariant or abstracted view in its representation of objects, where the right hemisphere uses stored “real world” views in order to group experience.195,199-200

The living versus the nonliving

The left hemisphere has more capacity for the abstract or impersonal, whereas, in keeping with its more contextualized view, the right hemisphere is less prone to abstraction, and prioritizes the personal.201,202

The left hemisphere is better at appreciating analytic or mechanical structures, whereas the right hemisphere is better adapted to an appreciation of wholes, such as living entities, that have not been put together from parts. The right hemisphere is more concerned with living individuals than manmade objects.203 The left hemisphere alone codes for nonliving things,204-207 while both hemispheres code for living things.206-208 However, at least one study has found a clean divide between the hemispheres, the left coding for the nonliving, and the right for the living, regardless of the task.209 The body image as a whole is a right-hemisphere entity, whereas body parts are the province of the left hemisphere.210

The left hemisphere codes for tools and machines. 118,204-206,208,209 Right-hemisphere damage leaves the ability to use simple tools unaltered, whereas left-hemisphere damage renders the sufferer incapable of using a hammer and nail, or a key and a padlock. However, right-hemisphere damage particularly impairs naturalistic actions involving a sequence of steps, for example making a cup of coffee or wrapping a present.211-212

Corballis writes that 'there is a case for supposing that the left side represents the fruits of human invention, including language, manufacture, and a partwise way of representing objects.'213 He draws attention to the affinity of the left hemisphere for everything it has itself made. The right temporal region, by contrast, appears to have areas not only specific for living things, but additionally for all that is specifically human.214-216 Such judgments of “humanness” are separate from the right hemisphere's superior ability to recognize faces.217

The need to reconcile irreconcilables

The narrow focus of attention of the left hemisphere predisposes it to adopt a part-wise representation of reality, which promotes the transformation of a continuous process into a series of static points. This may underlie the different contributions made by each hemisphere to the appreciation of music and the sense of time, as well as to spatial depth.104

The broader scope of attention of the right hemisphere, which sees the individual in relation to others - in animals and birds, to predator or conspecific - leads to its prominent role in social understanding in humans, including in “theory of mind,” and the expression of, and receptivity towards, social emotions. Decety and Chaminade note that 'self-awareness, empathy, identification with others, and more generally inter-subjective processes, are largely dependent upon ... right hemisphere resources.'218 These issues, which also have implications for the moral sense and the sense of the self, are complex and are explored at length elsewhere.104

Overall, there is the requirement, on the one hand, for a highly focused form of attention, directed towards an object that is already known, and which must be resolved precisely if it is to be grasped or used. This includes food or prey, but also anything that is to be used or manipulated. This is mediated by the left hemisphere, and crows already exhibit a strong right eye bias for tool manufacture, even where using the right eye makes the task more difficult.219,220 In man the left hemisphere is also the controller of the grasping right hand, and of the denotative and explicit (rather than connotative or implicit) aspects of language whereby we make meaning precise, or as we say “grasp” it (cf French com-prendre, German be-greifen). On the other hand, this narrowly focused kind of attention is disruptive of continuity and context, and there is a need for an attention of broader scope, mediated by the right hemisphere, that is not distinct, precise, and manipulative, but reciprocal, more tolerant of uncertainty and ambiguity, and intersubjective in nature.

The difference can be seen at its simplest in processing visual imagery. Blurred or indistinct images are easily processed by the right hemisphere, but not by the left, even where the nature of the task would suggest that it should be more problematic for the right hemisphere.221 One of the most consistent early findings in hemisphere specialization was that whenever an image is either only fleetingly presented, or presented in a degraded form, so that only partial information is available, a right-hemisphere superiority emerges.221-223 Sergent was able to demonstrate that this is the case even when the material is verbal, and its converse, namely that when images are presented for longer than usual, thus increasing their certainty and familiarity, a left-hemisphere superiority emerges, even in face recognition.224 According to Sergent, letters of the alphabet 'represent a finite set of stimuli that are sharply focused, familiar and overlearned,' whereas visual images 'represent a potentially infinite set of shapes of large visual angle size, with different levels of structure of unequal importance and salience that are most often unfamiliar to subjects.' Here a common thread unites, on the one hand, the left hemisphere's affinity for what it itself has made (here language), familiarity, certainty and finitude, and, on the other, the right hemisphere's affinity for all that is new, unknown, uncertain, and unbounded.93

The nature of interhemispheric relations

Given the difference in concerns, how are the relationships between the two hemispheres managed in practice?

The experience of callosotomy is instructive. Behavioral disturbances following callosotomy take the form not, as might have been expected, of loss of function, but, on the contrary, of failures of functional inhibition. In this respect, split-brain subjects are like patients who have suffered a stroke or other neurological injury affecting the corpus callosum: there is a problem of compromised interhemispheric inhibition.225 -226

The inhibitory nature of the corpus callosum is adaptive and creative, rather than restrictive, and the ability to maintain separation while communicating information is essential. Banich notes that:

“the major finding to come out of our laboratory since the mid-1980s is that interhemispheric interaction is much more than just a mechanism by which one hemisphere “photocopies” experiences and feelings for its partner. Interhemispheric interaction has important emergent functions - functions that cannot be derived from the simple sum of its parts ... the nature of processing when both hemispheres are involved cannot be predicted from the parts.”23

One of the many misconceptions of the popular culture surrounding hemisphere difference is that creativity is a function of the right hemisphere alone. Certainly there is plenty of evidence that the right hemisphere is important for creativity,227 which, given its ability to make more and wider-ranging connections, and to think more flexibly, is hardly surprising.228 But this is only part of the story. Both hemispheres are importantly involved. Creativity depends on the union of things that are also maintained separately - the precise function of the corpus callosum, both to separate and connect. Division of the corpus callosum does in fact impair creativity in individuals.229

Asymmetry of interhemispheric inhibition

Interhemispheric competition is differently regulated in different individuals. For certain tasks, one or other hemisphere generally tends to predominate, its particular cognitive and perceptual style as a whole being more suited to the task in question, but interindividual differences exist, with characteristic and consistent biases associated with differing degrees of arousal and activation in either hemisphere. This phenomenon is known as “hemispheric utilization bias” or “characteristic perceptual asymmetry.” 230-233

Through examining these perceptual asymmetries, the relationship between the hemispheres can be seen to be itself asymmetrical. For example, in experiments where a task is carried out requiring attention to the nonfavored visual field (the field contralateral to the nonfavored hemisphere), while irrelevant, distracting information is presented to the favored visual field, those subjects with a characteristic left-hemisphere bias found that the already strong tendency for the left hemisphere to prioritize the right visual field, and downplay the left visual field, was enhanced. This meant that the irrelevant information in the right field interfered with the task going on in the left field (controlled by the right hemisphere). But for those with a characteristic right-hemisphere bias, when conditions were reversed, no such competitive effects were seen: irrelevant information in the right hemisphere's favored left field did not interfere with the subject's ability to attend to the matter in hand going on, now, in the right field (the field favored by the left hemisphere).234 This confirms a more even distribution of “concern” in the right hemisphere than in the left, consonant with the well-known phenomenon of hemineglect following right-hemisphere insult, but going further. Having a “utilization bias” in favor of the left hemisphere intensifies this effect, whereas having a similar bias in favor of the right hemisphere does not.

Furthermore, in the majority of normal subjects transfer of information from left hemisphere to right hemisphere takes place more slowly than transfer from right to left,235-237 even where the task is by nature better suited to the right hemisphere.238 Interhemispheric competition is also revealed by response to injury. In 1890, BrownSéquard found he was able to reverse a paralysis caused by a lesion in one hemisphere of a frog by inflicting a similar lesion at the same point in the contralateral hemisphere.239 In accordance with this, if, following a brain injury, the contralateral hemisphere is disabled temporarily by transcranial magnetic stimulation, an improvement in function in the damaged hemisphere results.240,241 Similarly, a subsequent insult to the contralateral hemisphere yields an improvement in function in the originally damaged hemisphere.242 But such interhemispheric competition appears again to be asymmetrical, with the suppressive effect of the left hemisphere on the right being greater than that of the right on the left.240,243 Moreover, in chicks there is an asymmetry in favor of the right hemisphere, but when the commissures develop in adult birds, this permits the left hemisphere to have an inhibitory effect on the right hemisphere to a greater extent than the right hemisphere has on the left; severing of the commissures re-establishes the primary asymmetry4,244

Finally there may be costs as well as benefits to mutual inhibition, as callosotomy subjects reveal. Though they have handicaps, they can carry out some tasks more swiftly than normal subjects.245 For example, tasks involving focused attention usually engage primarily the left hemisphere. But in split-brain patients, the left hemisphere cannot so effectively inhibit the right, so that both are able to bring focused attention to bear (the right hemisphere can also yield focused attention), and both contribute, with the result that the task is carried out in half the time.

Conclusion

One possible explanation of the asymmetry and differential development of the cerebral hemispheres lies in the need to apply simultaneously mutually incompatible modes of attention to the world. This difference in attention can account for a wide range of so-called hemisphere specializations, and is facilitated by a corpus callosum which is able to balance facilitatory against inhibitory transmission. There is also an asymmetry in the way in which the two hemispheres mutually inhibit one another's contribution to the phenomenological world which favors the left hemisphere.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Lokhorst G-JC. The first theory about hemispheric specialization: fresh light on an old codex. J Hist Med Allied Sci. 1996;51:293–312. doi: 10.1093/jhmas/51.3.293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Wigan AL. A New View of Insanity: the Duality of the Mind Proved by the Structure, Functions and Diseases of the Brain, and by the Phenomena of Mental Derangement, and Shown to be Essential to Moral Responsibility. London, UK: Longman. 1844 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Harrington A. Medicine, Mind, and the Double Brain. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1987 [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hellige JB. Hemispheric Asymmetry: What's Right and What's Left. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1993 [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Ramachandran VS. Phantoms in the Brain: Human Nature and the Architecture of the Mind. London, UK: HarperCollins. 2005 [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Crow TJ. March 27, 1827 and what happened later - the impact of psychiatry on evolutionary theory. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2006;30:785–796. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Cutting J. The Right Cerebral Hemisphere and Psychiatric Disorders. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 1990 [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Cutting J. Scheler, phenomenology and psychopathology. Philos Psychiatr Psychol. 2009;16:143–159. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kertesz A., Polk M., Black SE., et al. Anatomical asymmetries and functional laterality. Brain. 1992;115:589–605. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.2.589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Damasio H. Human Brain Anatomy in Computerized Images. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2005 [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Galaburda AM. Anatomic basis of cerebral dominance. In: Davidson RJ, Hugdahl K, eds. Brain Asymmetry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1995:51–73. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Galaburda AM., Aboitiz F., Rosen GD., et al. Histological asymmetry in the primary visual cortex of the rat: implications for mechanisms of cerebral asymmetry. Cortex. 1986;22:151–160. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(86)80039-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Hayes TL., Lewis DA. Hemispheric differences in layer III pyramidal neurons of the anterior language area. Arch Neurol. 1993;50:501–505. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1993.00540050053015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Scheibel AB., Paul LA., Fried I., et al. Dendritic organization of the anterior speech area. Exp Neurol. 1985;87:109–117. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(85)90137-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Seldon HL. Structure of human auditory cortex: III. Statistical analysis of dendritic trees. Brain Res. 1982;249:211–221. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90055-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Allen JS., Damasio H., Grabowski TJ., et al. Sexual dimorphism and asymmetries in the gray-white composition of the human cerebrum. NeuroImage. 2003;18:880–894. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00034-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Gur RC., Packer IK., Hungerbuhler JP., et al. Differences in the distribution of gray and white matter in human cerebral hemispheres. Science. 1980;207:1226–1228. doi: 10.1126/science.7355287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lewis D., Diamond MC. The influence of gonadal steroids on the asymmetry of the cerebral cortex. In: Davidson RJ, Hugdahl K, eds. Brain Asymmetry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1995:31–50. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Glick SD., Carlson KL., Drew KL., et al. Functional and neurochemical asymmetry in the corpus striatum. In: Ottoson D, ed. Duality and Unity of the Brain. London: Macmillan. 1987:3–16. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Glick SD., Ross DA., Hough LB. Lateral asymmetry of neurotransmitters in human brain. Brain Res. 1982;234:53–63. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90472-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Tucker DM., Williamson PA. Asymmetric neural control systems in human self-regulation [review]. Psychol Rev. 1984;91:185–215. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Jäncke L., Steinmetz H. Anatomical brain asymmetries and their relevance for functional asymmetries. In: Hugdahl K, Davidson RJ, eds. The Asymmetrical Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2003:187–230. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Banich MT. Interaction between the hemispheres and its implications for the processing capacity of the brain. In: Hugdahl K, Davidson RJ, eds. The Asymmetrical Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2003:261–302. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Meyer B-U., Röricht S., Gräfin von Einsiedel H., et al. Inhibitory and excitatory interhemispheric transfers between motor cortical areas in normal subjects and patients with abnormalities of the corpus callosum. Brain. 1995;118:429–440. doi: 10.1093/brain/118.2.429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Röricht S., Irlbacher K., Petrow E., et al. Normwerte transkallosal und kortikospinal vermittelter Effekte einer hemisphärenselektiven elektromyographischer magnetischen Kortexreizung beim Menschen. Z Elektroenzephalogr Elektromyogr Verwandte Geb. 1997;28:34–38. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Conti F., Manzoni T. The neurotransmitters and postsynaptic actions of callosally projecting neurons. Behav Brain Res. 1994;64:37–53. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(94)90117-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Saron CD., Foxe JJ., Simpson GV., et al. Interhemispheric visuomotor activation: spatiotemporal electrophysiology related to reaction time. In: Zaidel E, Iacoboni M, eds. The Parallel Brain: the Cognitive Neuroscience of the Corpus Callosum. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2002:171–219. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Saron CD., Foxe JJ., Schroeder CE., et al. Complexities of interhemispheric communication in sensorimotor tasks revealed by high-density event-related potential mapping. In: Hugdahl K, Davidson RJ, eds. The Asymmetrical Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2003:341–408. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Allison JD., Meador KJ., Loring DW., et al. Functional MRI cerebral activation and deactivation during finger movement. Neurology. 2000;54:135–142. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.1.135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bloom JS., Hynd GW. The role of the corpus callosum in interhemispheric transfer of information: excitation or inhibition? Neuropsychol Rev. 2005;15:59–71. doi: 10.1007/s11065-005-6252-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Cook ND. Homotopic callosal inhibition. Brain Lang. 1984;23:116–125. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(84)90010-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hoptman MJ., Davidson RJ. How and why do the two cerebral hemispheres interact? Psychol Bull. 1994;116:195–219. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Chiarello C., Maxfield L. Varieties of interhemispheric inhibition, or how to keep a good hemisphere down. Brain Cogn. 1996;30:81–108. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1996.0006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hopkins WD., Marino L. Asymmetries in cerebral width in nonhuman primate brains as revealed by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Neuropsychologia. 2000;38:493–499. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00090-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Friedman A., Polson MC. The hemispheres as independent resource systems: limited-capacity processing and cerebral specialization. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1981;7:1031–1058. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.7.5.1031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rogers LJ., Andrew RJ. Comparative Vertebrate Lateralization. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2002 [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Braun CMJ. The evolution of hemispheric specialisation of antagonistic systems of management of the body's energy sources. Laterality. 2007;12:397–427. doi: 10.1080/13576500701458875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Rogers LJ. Evolution of hemispheric specialization: advantages and disadvantages. Brain Lang. 2000;73:236–253. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hoffman AM., Robakiewicz PE., Tuttle EM., et al. Behavioural lateralisation in the Australian magpie (Gymnorhina tibicen). Laterality. 2006;11:110–121. doi: 10.1080/13576500500376674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Rogers LJ., Kaplan G. An eye for a predator: lateralization in birds, with particular reference to the Australian magpie. In: Malashichev YB, Deckel AW, eds. Behavioral and Morphological Asymmetries in Vertebrates. Austin, TX: Landes Bioscience; 2006:47–57. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Rogers LJ., Zucca P., Vallortigara G. Advantages of having a lateralized brain. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2004;271(suppl 6):S420–S422. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2004.0200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Dharmaretnam M., Rogers LJ. Hemispheric specialization and dual processing in strongly versus weakly lateralized chicks. Behav Brain Res. 2005;162:62–70. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bugnyar T., Stöwe M., Heinrich B. Ravens, Corvus corax, follow gaze direction of humans around obstacles. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2004;271:1331–1336. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Evans CS., Evans L., Marler P. On the meaning of alarm calls - functional reference in an avian vocal system. Anim Behav. 1993;46:23–38. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Lippolis G., Bisazza A., Rogers LJ., et al. Lateralisation of predator avoidance responses in three species of toads. Laterality. 2002;7:163–183. doi: 10.1080/13576500143000221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Lippolis G., Westman W., McAllan BM., et al. Lateralisation of escape responses in the striped-faced dunnart, Sminthopsis macroura (Dasyuridae: Marsupialia). Laterality. 2005;10:457–470. doi: 10.1080/13576500442000210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Csermely D. Lateralisation in birds of prey: adaptive and phylogenetic considerations. Behav Processes. 2004;67:511–520. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2004.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Rogers LJ. Cognitive and social advantages of having a lateralized brain. In: Malashichev YB, Deckel AW, eds. Behavioral and Morphological Asymmetries in Vertebrates, Austin, TX: Landes Bioscience. 2006:129–139. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.McGrew WC., Marchant LF. Laterality of hand use pays off in foraging success for wild chimpanzees. Primates. 1999;40:509–513. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Fabre-Thorpe M., Fagot J., Lorincz E., et al. Laterality in cats: paw preference and performance in a visuomotor activity. Cortex. 1993;29:15–24. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(13)80208-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Bisazza A., Cantalupo C., Capocchiano M., et al. Population lateralisation and social behaviour: a study with sixteen species of fish. Laterality. 2000;5:269–284. doi: 10.1080/713754381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Rogers LJ., Workman L. Light exposure during incubation affects competitive behaviour in domestic chicks. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1989;23:187–198. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Halpern ME., Güntürkün O., Hopkins WD., et al. Lateralization of the vertebrate brain: taking the side of model systems. J Neurosci. 2005;25:10351–10357. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3439-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Fernández-Carriba S., Loeches A., Morcillo A., et al. Asymmetry in facial expression of emotions by chimpanzees. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:1523–1533. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00028-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Ventolini N., Ferrero EA., Sponza S., et al. Laterality in the wild: preferential hemifield use during predatory and sexual behaviour in the blackwinged stilt (Himantopus himantopus). Anim Behav. 2005;69:1077–1084. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Vallortigara G. Right hemisphere advantage for social recognition in the chick. Neuropsychologia. 1992;30:761–768. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(92)90080-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Vallortigara G., Rogers LJ., Bisazza A., et al. Complementary right and left hemifield use for predatory and agonistic behaviour in toads. NeuroReport. 1998;9:3341–3344. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199810050-00035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Glick SD., Meibach RC., Cox RD., et al. Multiple and interrelated asymmetries in rat brain. Life Sci. 1979;25:395–400. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(79)90272-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Sandhu S., Cook P., Diamond MC. Rat cerebral cortical estrogen receptors: male-female, right-left. Exp Neurol. 1986;92:186–196. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(86)90133-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Denenberg VH., Garbanati J., Sherman DA., et al. Infantile stimulation induces brain lateralization in rats. Science. 1978;201:1150–1152. doi: 10.1126/science.684436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Andrew RJ., Rogers LJ. The nature of lateralisation in tetrapods. In: Andrew RJ, Rogers LJ, eds. Comparative Vertebrate Lateralisation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2002:94–125. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Crow TJ., Crow LR., Done DJ., et al. Relative hand skill predicts academic ability: global deficits at the point of hemispheric indecision. Neuropsychologia. 1998;36:1275–1282. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00039-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Güntürkün O., Diekamp B., Manns M., et al. Asymmetry pays: visual lateralization improves discrimination success in pigeons. Curr Biol. 2000;10:1079–1081. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(00)00671-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.van Zomeren AH., Brouwer WH. Clinical Neuropsychology of Attention. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 1994 [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Jerison HJ. Vigilance: biology, psychology, theory and practice. In: Mackie RR, ed. Vigilance (NATO conference series). New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1977:27–40. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Dimond SJ. Disconnection and psychopathology. In: Gruzelier JH, FlorHenry P, eds. Hemisphere Asymmetries of Function in Psychopathology. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1979:35–47. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Rueckert L., Grafman J. Sustained attention deficits in patients with right frontal lesions. Neuropsychologia. 1996;34:953–963. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(96)00016-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Wilkins AJ., Shallice T., McCarthy R. Frontal lesions and sustained attention. Neuropsychologia. 1987;25:359–365. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(87)90024-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Korda RJ., Douglas JM. Attention deficits in stroke patients with aphasia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1997;19:525–542. doi: 10.1080/01688639708403742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Rousseaux M., Fimm B., Cantagallo A. Attention disorders in cerebrovascular diseases. In: Leclercq M, Zimmerman P, eds. Applied Neuropsychology of Attention. London: Psychology Press; 2002:280–304. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.de Renzi E., Faglioni P. The comparative efficiency of intelligence and vigilance tests in detecting hemispheric cerebral damage. Cortex. 1965;1:410–433. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Benson DF., Barton MI. Disturbances in constructional ability. Cortex. 1970;6:19–46. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(70)80034-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Howes D., Boller F. Simple reaction time: evidence for focal impairment from lesions of the right hemisphere. Brain. 1975;98:317–332. doi: 10.1093/brain/98.2.317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Nakamura R., Taniguchi R. Reaction time in patients with cerebral hemiparesis. Neuropsychologia. 1977;15:845–848. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(77)90020-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Tartaglione A., Bino G., Manzino M., et al. Simple reaction-time changes in patients with unilateral brain damage. Neuropsychologia. 1986;24:649–658. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(86)90004-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Sturm W., Büssing A. Einfluss der Aufgabenkomplexität auf hirnorganische Reaktionsbeeinträchtigungen - Hirnschädigungs - oder Patienteneffekt? Eur Arch Psychiatry Neurol Sci. 1986;235:214–220. doi: 10.1007/BF00379977. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Godefroy O., Lhullier C., Rousseaux M. Non-spatial attention disorders in patients with frontal or posterior brain damage. Brain. 1996;119:191–202. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.1.191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Sturm W., Reul J., Willmes K. Is there a generalised right hemisphere dominance for mediating cerebral activation? Evidence from a choice reaction experiment with lateralised simple warning stimuli. Neuropsychologia. 1989;27:747–751. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(89)90121-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Whitehead R. Right hemisphere processing superiority during sustained visual attention. J Cogn Neurosci. 1991;3:329–334. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1991.3.4.329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Dimond SJ. Performance by split-brain humans on lateralised vigilance tasks. Cortex. 1979;15:43–50. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(79)80005-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Lewin JS., Friedman L., Wu D., et al. Cortical localisation of human sustained attention: detection with functional MR using a visual vigilance paradigm. J Comput Assist Tomogr. 1996;20:695–701. doi: 10.1097/00004728-199609000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Pardo JV., Fox PT., Raichle ME. Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography. Nature. 1991;349:61–64. doi: 10.1038/349061a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Salmaso D., Denes G. Role of the frontal lobes on an attention task: a signal detection analysis. Percept Mot Skills. 1982;54:1147–1150. doi: 10.2466/pms.1982.54.3c.1147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Rousseaux M., Godefroy O., Cabaret M., et al. Analyse et évolution des déficits cognitifs après rupture des anévrysmes de l'artère communicante antérieure. Rev Neurol (Paris). 1996;152:678–687. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Dee HL., van Allen MW. Speed of decision-making processes in patients with unilateral cerebral disease. Arch Neurol. 1973;28:163–166. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1973.00490210043004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Bisiach E., Mini M., Sterzi R., et al. Hemispheric lateralisation of the decisional stage in choice reaction times to visual unstructured stimuli. Cortex. 1982;18:191–197. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(82)80002-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Jansen C., Sturm W., Willmes K. Sex-specific 'activation'-dominance of the left hemisphere for choice reactions: an experimental study regarding lateralisation of attention functions. Z Neuropsychol. 1992;3:44–51. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Corbetta M., Miezin FM., Dobmeyer S., et al. Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography. J Neurosci. 1991;11:2383–2402. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-08-02383.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Çiçek M., Gitelman D., Hurley RS., et al. Anatomical physiology of spatial extinction. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:2892–2898. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Vohn R., Fimm B., Weber J., et al. Management of attentional resources in within-modal and cross-modal divided attention tasks: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28:1267–1275. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Posner MI. Attention in cognitive neuroscience: an overview. In: Gazzaniga MS, ed. The Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1995:615–624. [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Ivry RB., Robertson LC. The Two Sides of Perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1998 [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Sergent J. The cerebral balance of power: confrontation or cooperation? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1982;8:253–272. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.8.2.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Robertson LC., Lamb MR. Neuropsychological contributions to theories of part/whole organisation. Cogn Psychol. 1991;23:299–330. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(91)90012-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.van Kleeck MH. Hemispheric differences in global versus local processing of hierarchical visual stimuli by normal subjects: new data and a metaanalysis of previous studies. Neuropsychologia. 1989;27:1165–1178. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(89)90099-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Mesulam M-M. Behavioral neuroanatomy: large-scale networks, association cortex, frontal syndromes, the limbic system and hemispheric specialization. In: Mesulam M-M, ed. Principles of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2000 [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Deouell LY., Ivry RB., Knight RT. Electrophysiologic methods and transcranial magnetic stimulation in behavioral neurology and neuropsychology. In: Feinberg TE, Farah MJ, eds. Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; 2003:105–134. [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Delis DC., Robertson LC., Efron R. Hemispheric specialisation of memory for visual hierarchical stimuli. Neuropsychologia. 1986;24:205–214. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(86)90053-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Delis DC., Kiefner MG., Fridlund AJ. Visuospatial dysfunction following unilateral brain damage: dissociations in hierarchical and hemispatial analysis. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1988;10:421–431. doi: 10.1080/01688638808408250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Siéroff E. Focussing on/in visual-verbal stimuli in patients with parietal lesions. Cogn Neuropsychol. 1990;7:519–594. [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Siéroff E. Les mécanismes attentionnels. In: Seron X, Jeannerod M, eds. Neuropsychologie humaine. Liège, France: Mardaga; 1994:127–151. [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Halligan PW., Marshall JC. Toward a principled explanation of unilateral neglect. Cogn Neuropsychol. 1994;11:167–206. [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Leclercq M. Theoretical aspects of the main components and functions of attention. In: Leclercq M, Zimmerman P, eds. Applied Neuropsychology of Attention. London, UK: Psychology Press; 2002:3–55. [Google Scholar]
  • 104.McGilchrist I. The Master and his Emissary: The Divided Brain and the Making of the Western World. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 2009 [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Schutz LE. Broad-perspective perceptual disorder of the right hemisphere. Neuropsychol Rev. 2005;15:11–27. doi: 10.1007/s11065-005-3585-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Posner M., Raichle M. Images of Mind. New York, NY: Scientific American Library; 1994 [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Goldberg E. The Executive Brain: Frontal Lobes and the Civilized Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2001 [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Tang AC. A hippocampal theory of cerebral lateralization. In: Hugdahl K, Davidson RJ, eds. The Asymmetrical Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2003:37–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Goldberg E., Costa LD. Hemispheric differences in the acquisition and use of descriptive systems. Brain Lang. 1981;14:144–173. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(81)90072-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Goldberg E., Podell K., Lovell M. Lateralization of frontal lobe functions and cognitive novelty. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994;6:371–378. doi: 10.1176/jnp.6.4.371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Treyer V., Buck A., Schnider A. Subcortical loop activation during selection of currently relevant memories. J Cogn Neurosci. 2003;15:610–618. doi: 10.1162/089892903321662985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Cotton B., Tzeng OJ., Hardyck C. Role of cerebral hemispheric processing in the visual half-field stimulus-response compatibility effect. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1980;6:13–23. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.6.1.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Persinger MA., Lalonde CA. Right to left hemispheric shift in occipital electroencephalographic responses to repeated Kimura figures. Percept Mot Skills. 2000;91:273–278. doi: 10.2466/pms.2000.91.1.273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Martin A., Wiggs CL., Weisberg J. Modulation of human medial temporal lobe activity by form, meaning and experience. Hippocampus. 1997;7:587–593. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1997)7:6<587::AID-HIPO1>3.0.CO;2-C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Gold JM., Berman KF., Randolph C., et al. PET validation of a novel prefrontal task: delayed response alternation (DRA). Neuropsychology. 1996;10:3–10. [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Berns GS., Cohen JD., Mintun MA. Brain regions responsive to novelty in the absence of awareness. Science. 1997;276:1272–1275. doi: 10.1126/science.276.5316.1272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Tulving E., Markowitsch HJ., Craik FE., et al. Novelty and familiarity activations in PET studies of memory encoding and retrieval. Cereb Cortex. 1996;6:71–79. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.1.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Cutting J. Principles of Psychopathology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 1997 [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Gardner H. The Shattered Mind. New York, NY: Vintage Books; 1974 [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Sperry RW. Consciousness, personal identity and the divided brain. In: Benson DF, Zaidel E, eds. The Dual Brain: Hemispheric Specialization in Humans. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1985:11–26. [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Mills DL., Coffey-Corina SA., Neville HJ. Language acquisition and cerebral specialization in 20-month-old infants. J Cogn Neurosci. 1993;5:317–334. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.3.317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Thal DJ., Marchman V., Stiles J., et al. Early lexical development in children with focal brain injury. Brain Lang. 1991;40:491–527. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(91)90145-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Bever TG., Chiarello RJ. Cerebral dominance in musicians and nonmusicians. Science. 1974;185:537–539. doi: 10.1126/science.185.4150.537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Podell K., Lovell M., Zimmerman M., et al. The Cognitive Bias Task and lateralised frontal lobe functions in males. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1995;7:491–501. doi: 10.1176/jnp.7.4.491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Phelps EA., Gazzaniga MS. Hemispheric differences in mnemonic processing: the effects of left hemisphere interpretation. Neuropsychologia. 1992;30:293–297. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(92)90006-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Brownell HH., Potter HH., Bihrle AM., et al. Inference deficits in right brain-damaged patients. Brain Lang. 1986;27:310–321. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(86)90022-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Molloy R., Brownell HH., Gardner H. Discourse comprehension by righthemisphere stroke patients: deficits of prediction and revision. In: Joanette Y Brownell HH, eds. Discourse Ability and Brain Damage: Theoretical and Empirical Perspectives. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag; 1990:113–130. [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Metcalfe J., Funnell M., Gazzaniga MS. Right hemisphere memory superiority: studies of a split-brain patient. Psychol Sci. 1995;6:157–164. [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Federmeier KD., Kutas M. Right words and left words: electrophysiological evidence for hemispheric differences in meaning processing. Cogn Brain Res. 1999;8:373–392. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(99)00036-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Larose C., Richard-Yris M-A., Hausberger M., et al. Laterality of horses associated with emotionality in novel situations. Laterality. 2006;11:355–367. doi: 10.1080/13576500600624221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Coulson S. Semantic Leaps: Frame-Shifting and Conceptual Blending in Meaning Construction. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2001 [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Aron AR., Fletcher PC., Bullmore ET., et al. Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:115–116. doi: 10.1038/nn1003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Nagahama Y., Okada T., Katsumi Y., et al. Dissociable mechanisms of attentional control within the human prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2001;11:85–92. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.1.85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Razani J., Boone KB., Miller BL., et al. Neuropsychological performance of right- and left-frontotemporal dementia compared to Alzheimer's disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2001;7:468–480. doi: 10.1017/s1355617701744037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Ruff RM., Allen CC., Farrow CE., et al. Figural fluency: differential impairment in patients with left versus right frontal lobe lesions. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1994;9:41–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Vanderhasselt MA., De Raedt R., Baeken C., et al. The influence of rTMS over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on intentional set switching. Exp Brain Res. 2006;172:561–565. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0540-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Richards L., Chiarello C. Activation without selection: parallel right-hemisphere roles in language and intentional movement? Brain Lang. 1997;57:151–178. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Posner MI. Time course of activating brain areas in generating verbal associations. Psychol Sci. 1997;8:56–59. [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Brownell HH., Simpson TL., Bihrle AM., et al. Appreciation of metaphoric alternative word meanings by left and right brain-damaged patients. Neuropsychologia. 1990;28:375–383. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(90)90063-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Ramachandran VS. Phantom limbs, neglect syndromes, repressed memories, and Freudian psychology. Int Rev Neurobiol. 1994;37:291–333. doi: 10.1016/s0074-7742(08)60254-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Yochim BP., Kender R., Abeare C., et al. Semantic activation within and across the cerebral hemispheres: what's left isn't right. Laterality. 2005;10:131–148. doi: 10.1080/13576500342000356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Coney J., Evans KD. Hemispheric asymmetries in the resolution of lexical ambiguity. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38:272–282. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00076-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Chiarello C. On codes of meaning and the meaning of codes: semantic access and retrieval within and between hemispheres. In: Beeman M, Chiarello C, eds. Right Hemisphere Language Comprehension: Perspectives from Cognitive Neuroscience. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1998:141–160. [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Seger CA., Desmond JE., Glover GH., et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for right-hemisphere involvement in processing unusual semantic relationships. Neuropsychology. 2000;14:361–369. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.3.361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Jung-Beeman M., Bowden EM., Haberman J., et al. Neural activity when people solve verbal problems with insight. PLoS Biol. 2004;2:E97. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Mashal N., Faust M., Hendler T., et al. An fMRI investigation of the neural correlates underlying the processing of novel metaphoric expressions. Brain Lang. 2007;100:115–126. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Chiarello C., Senehi J., Nuding S. Semantic priming with abstract and concrete words: differential asymmetry may be post-lexical. Brain Lang. 1987;31:43–60. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(87)90060-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Nakagawa A. Role of anterior and posterior attention networks in hemispheric asymmetries during lexical decisions. J Cogn Neurosci. 1991;3:313–321. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1991.3.4.313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Tucker DM., Roth DL., Bair TB. Functional connections among cortical regions: topography of EEG coherence. Electroencephalogr Clin. 1986;63:242–250. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(86)90092-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Liotti M., Tucker DM. Emotion in asymmetric corticolimbic network. In: Davidson RJ, Hugdahl K, eds. Human Brain Laterality. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 1994:389–424. [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Semmes J., Weinstein S., Ghent L., et al. Somatosensory Changes After Penetrating Brain Wounds in Man. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1960 [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Teuber HL., Battersby WS., Bender MB. Visual Field Defects after Penetrating Missile Wounds of the Brain. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1960 [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Milner B. Hemispheric. Specialization and Interactions. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1975 [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Chapanis L. Language deficits and cross-modal sensory perception. In: Segalowitz SJ, Gruber FA, eds. Language Development and Neurological Theory. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1977:107–120. [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Tucker DM. Developing emotions and cortical networks. In: Gunnar MR, Nelson CA, eds. Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology. Vol 24: Developmental Behavioral Neuroscience. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1992:75–128. [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Levy-Agresti J., Sperry RW. Differential perceptual capacities in major and minor hemispheres. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1968;61:1151. [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Kirsner K. Hemisphere-specific processes in letter-matching. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1980;6:167–179. doi: 10.1037/h0078140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Kirsner K., Brown H. Laterality and recency effects in working memory. Neuropsychologia. 1981;19:249–261. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(81)90109-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Marsolek CJ., Schacter DL., Nicholas CD. Form-specific visual priming for new associations in the right cerebral hemisphere. Mem Cognit. 1996;24:539–556. doi: 10.3758/bf03201082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Navon D. Forest before trees: the precedence of global features in visual perception. Cogn Psychol. 1977;9:353–383. [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Christman S. Cerebral Asymmetries in Sensory and Perceptual Processing. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1997 [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Beeman MJ., Bowden EM., Gernsbacher MA. Right and left hemisphere cooperation for drawing predictive and coherence inferences during normal story comprehension. Brain Lang. 2000;71:310–336. doi: 10.1006/brln.1999.2268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Young AH., Ratliff G. Visuospatial abilities of the right hemisphere. In: Young AH, ed. Functions of the Right Cerebral Hemisphere. London: Academic Press; 1983 [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Yoshida T., Yoshino A., Takahashi Y., et al. Comparison of hemispheric asymmetry in global and local information processing and interference in divided and selective attention using spatial frequency filters. Exp Brain Res. 2007;181:519–529. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-0948-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Evert DL., Kmen M. Hemispheric asymmetries for global and local processing as a function of stimulus exposure duration. Brain Cogn. 2003;51:115–142. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(02)00528-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.Fink GR., Marshall JC., Halligan PW., et al. Hemispheric asymmetries in global/local processing are modulated by perceptual salience. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37:31–40. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00047-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Heinze HJ., Hinrichs H., Scholz M., et al. Neural mechanisms of global and local processing: a combined PET and ERP study. J Cogn Neurosci. 1998;10:485–498. doi: 10.1162/089892998562898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 168.Lux S., Marshall JC., Ritzl A., et al. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of local/global processing with stimulus presentation in the peripheral visual hemifields. Neuroscience. 2004;124:113–120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.10.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Walsh K., Darby D. Neuropsychology, a Clinical Approach. 4th ed. New York, NY: Churchill, Livingstone; 1999 [Google Scholar]
  • 170.Bradshaw JL., Nettleton NC. Human Cerebral Asymmetry. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1983 [Google Scholar]
  • 171.Anderson JR. Cognitive Psychology and its Implications. New York, NY: Freeman; 1990 [Google Scholar]
  • 172.Stillings NA., Feinstein MH., Garfield J., et al. Cognitive Science: An Introduction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1987 [Google Scholar]
  • 173.McFie J., Zangwill O. Visual constructive disabilities associated with lesions of the left cerebral hemisphere. Brain. 1960;83:243–260. [Google Scholar]
  • 174.Warrington EK., James M., Kinsbourne M. Drawing disability in relation to laterality of cerebral lesion. Brain. 1966;89:53–82. doi: 10.1093/brain/89.1.53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 175.Kinsbourne M. Hemisphere interactions in depression. In: Kinsbourne M, ed. Cerebral Hemisphere Function in Depression. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1988:133–162. [Google Scholar]
  • 176.Alexander MP., Benson DF., Stuss DT. Frontal lobes and language. Brain Lang. 1989;37:656–91. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(89)90118-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 177.Blakeslee TR. The Right Brain. London: Macmillan, 1980 [Google Scholar]
  • 178.Deglin V. Split brain. Unesco Cour. 1976;29:4–31. [Google Scholar]
  • 179.Heilman KM., Scholes R., Watson RT. Auditory affective agnosia: disturbed comprehension of affective speech. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1975;38:69–72. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.38.1.69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 180.Bottini G., Corcoran R., Sterzi R., et al. The role of the right hemisphere in the interpretation of figurative aspects of language: a positron emission tomography activation study. Brain. 1994;117:1241–1253. doi: 10.1093/brain/117.6.1241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 181.Foldi NS., Cicone M., Gardner H. Pragmatic aspects of communication in brain-damaged patients. In: Segalowitz SJ, ed. Language Functions and Brain Organisation. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1983 [Google Scholar]
  • 182.Kaplan JA., Brownell HH., Jacobs JR., et al. The effects of right hemisphere damage on the pragmatic interpretation of conversational remarks. Brain Lang. 1990;38:315–333. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(90)90117-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 183.Pobric G., Mashal N., Faust M., et al. The role of the right cerebral hemisphere in processing novel metaphoric expressions: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2008;20:170–181. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 184.Lakoff G., Johnson M. Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and its Challenge to Western Thought. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1999 [Google Scholar]
  • 185.Deglin VL., Kinsbourne M. Divergent thinking styles of the hemispheres: how syllogisms are solved during transitory hemisphere suppression. Brain Cogn. 1996;31:285–307. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1996.0048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 186.Marsolek CJ. Abstract visual-form representations in the left cerebral hemisphere. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1995;21:375–386. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.21.2.375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 187.Brown HD., Kosslyn SM. Cerebral lateralization. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1993;3:183–186. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(93)90208-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 188.Kosslyn SM. Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres: a computational approach. Psychol Rev. 1987;94:148–175. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 189.Warrington EK., McCarthy RA. Categories of knowledge: further fractionations and an attempted integration. Brain. 1987;110:1273–1296. doi: 10.1093/brain/110.5.1273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 190.Bornstein B., Sroka H., Munitz H. Prosopagnosia with animal face agnosia. Cortex. 1969;5:164–169. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(69)80027-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 191.Landis T., Cummings JL., Benson DF., et al. Loss of topographic familiarity: an environmental agnosia. Arch Neurol. 1986;43:132–136. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1986.00520020026011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 192.Bourgeois MJ., Christman S., Horowitz IA. The role of hemispheric activation in person perception: evidence for an attentional focus model. Brain Cogn. 1998;38:202–219. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1998.1030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 193.Farah MJ. Disorders of visual-spatial perception and cognition. In: Heilman KM, Valenstein E, eds. Clinical Neuropsychology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2003:146–160. [Google Scholar]
  • 194.McCarthy RA., Warrington EK. Cogn Neuropsychol. London: Academic Press; 1990 [Google Scholar]
  • 195.Sergent J., Ohta S., MacDonald B. Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing: a positron emission tomography study. Brain. 1992;115:15–36. doi: 10.1093/brain/115.1.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 196.Laeng B., Chabris CF., Kosslyn SM. Asymmetries in encoding spatial relations. In: Hugdahl K, Davidson RJ, eds. The Asymmetrical Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2003:303–339. [Google Scholar]
  • 197.Laeng B., Zarrinpar A., Kosslyn SM. Do separate processes identify objects as exemplars versus members of basic-level categories? Evidence from hemispheric specialization. Brain Cogn. 2003;53:15–27. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00184-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 198.Koivisto M., Laine M. Strategies of semantic categorization in the cerebral hemispheres. Brain Lang. 1999;66:341–357. doi: 10.1006/brln.1998.2033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 199.Marsolek CJ. Dissociable neural subsystems underlie abstract and specific object recognition. Psychol Sci. 1999;10:111–118. [Google Scholar]
  • 200.Zaidel DW., Kosta A. Hemispheric effects of canonical views of category members with known typicality levels. Brain Cogn. 2001;46:311–316. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(01)80091-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 201.Kosslyn SM., Koenig O., Barrett A., et al. Evidence for two types of spatial representations: hemispheric specialization for categorical and coordinate relations. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1989;15:723–735. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.15.4.723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 202.van Lancker D. Personal relevance and the human right hemisphere. Brain Cogn. 1991;17:64–92. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(91)90067-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 203.Wallace GL., Canter GJ. Effects of personally relevant language materials on the performance of severely aphasic individuals. J Speech Hear Disord. 1985;50:385–390. doi: 10.1044/jshd.5004.385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 204.Lhermitte F., Chedru F., Chain F. À propos d'un cas d'agnosie visuelle. Rev Neurol (Paris). 1973;128:301–322. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 205.Gainotti G. The relationships between anatomical and cognitive locus of lesion in category-specific disorders. In: Forde EME, Humphreys GW, eds. Category Specificity in Brain and Mind. Hove, UK: Psychology Press; 2002:403–426. [Google Scholar]
  • 206.Martin A., Wiggs CL., Ungerleider LG., et al. Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature. 1996;379:649–652. doi: 10.1038/379649a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 207.Perani D., Cappa SF., Bettinardi V. Different neural systems for the recognition of animals and man-made tools. NeuroReport. 1995;6:1637–1641. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199508000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 208.Mummery CJ., Patterson K., Hodges JR., et al. Functional neuroanatomy of the semantic system: divisible by what? J Cogn Neurosci. 1998;10:766–777. doi: 10.1162/089892998563059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 209.Mummery CJ., Patterson K., Hodges JR., et al. Generating 'tiger' as an animal name or a word beginning with T: differences in brain activation. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996;263:989–995. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1996.0146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 210.Price CJ., Friston KJ. Functional imaging studies of category specificity. In: Forde EME, Humphreys GW, eds. Category Specificity in Brain and Mind. Hove, UK: Psychology Press; 2002:427–447. [Google Scholar]
  • 211.Gainotti G. What the locus of brain lesion tells us about the nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders: a review. Cortex. 2000;36:539–559. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70537-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 212.Hartmann K., Goldenberg G., Daumüller M., et al. It takes the whole brain to make a cup of coffee: the neuropsychology of naturalistic actions involving technical devices. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:625–637. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 213.Corballis MC. Sperry and the age of Aquarius: science, values and the split brain. Neuropsychologia. 1998;36:1083–1087. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00062-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 214.Schwartz MF., Buxbaum LJ., Montgomery MW., et al. Naturalistic action production following right hemisphere stroke. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37:51–66. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00066-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 215.Gainotti G., Barbier A., Marra C. Slowly progressive defect in recognition of familiar people in a patient with right anterior temporal atrophy. Brain. 2003;126:792–803. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 216.Giovanello KS., Alexander M., Verfaellie M. Differential impairment of person-specific knowledge in a patient with semantic dementia. Neurocase. 2003;9:15–26. doi: 10.1076/neur.9.1.15.14369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 217.Mendez MF., Lim GT. Alterations of the sense of 'humanness' in right hemisphere predominant frontotemporal dementia patients. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2004;17:133–138. doi: 10.1097/01.wnn.0000136593.21532.16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 218.Decety J., Chaminade T. When the self represents the other: a new cognitive neuroscience view on psychological identification. Conscious Cogn. 2003;12:577–596. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8100(03)00076-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 219.Hunt GR., Corballis MC., Gray RD. Laterality in tool manufacture by crows - neural processing and not ecological factors may influence “handedness” in these birds. Nature. 2001;414:707. doi: 10.1038/414707a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 220.Rutledge R., Hunt GR. Lateralised tool use in New Caledonian crows. Anim Behav. 2004;67:327–332. [Google Scholar]
  • 221.Cowin EL., Hellige JB. Categorical versus coordinate spatial processing: effects of blurring and hemispheric asymmetry. J Cogn Neurosci. 1994;6:156–164. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1994.6.2.156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 222.Hellige JB. Effects of perceptual quality and visual field of probe stimulus presentation on memory search for letters. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1980;6:639–651. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.6.4.639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 223.Bradshaw GJ., Hicks RE., Rose B. Lexical discrimination and letter-string identification in the two visual fields. Brain Lang. 1979;8:10–18. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(79)90035-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 224.Sergent J., Bindra D. Differential hemispheric processing of faces: methodological considerations and reinterpretation. Psychol Bull. 1981;89:541–554. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 225.Boroojerdi B., Diefenbach K., Ferbert A. Transcallosal inhibition in cortical and subcortical cerebral vascular lesions. J Neurol Sci. 1996;144:160–170. doi: 10.1016/s0022-510x(96)00222-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 226.Schnider A., Benson F., Rosner LJ. Callosal disconnection in multiple sclerosis. Neurology. 1993;43:1243–1245. doi: 10.1212/wnl.43.6.1243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 227.Harnad SR. Creativity, lateral saccades and the nondominant hemisphere. Percept Mot Skills. 1972;34:653–654. doi: 10.2466/pms.1972.34.2.653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 228.Bowden EM., Beeman MJ. Getting the right idea: semantic activation in the right hemisphere may help solve insight problems. Psychol Sci. 1998;9:435–440. [Google Scholar]
  • 229.Bogen JE., Bogen GM. Split-brains: interhemispheric exchange in creativity. In: Runco MA, Pritzker R, eds. Encyclopedia of Creativity. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1999 [Google Scholar]
  • 230.Levy J., Heller W., Banich MT., et al. Are variations among right-handed individuals in perceptual asymmetries caused by characteristic arousal differences between hemispheres? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1983;9:329–359. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.9.3.329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 231.Kim H., Levine SC. Variations in characteristic perceptual asymmetry: modality specific and modality general components. Brain Cogn. 1992;19:21–47. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(92)90036-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 232.Levine SC., Banich MT., Koch-Weser M. Variations in patterns of lateral asymmetry among dextrals. Brain Cogn. 1984;3:317–334. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(84)90024-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 233.Boles DB. Relationships among multiple task asymmetries. II. A largesample factor analysis. Brain Cogn. 1998;36:268–289. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1998.0995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 234.Spencer KM., Banich MT. Hemispheric biases and the control of visuospatial attention: an ERP study. BMC Neurosci. 2005;6:51. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-6-51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 235.Bisiacchi P., Marzi CA., Nicoletti R., et al. Left-right asymmetry of callosal transfer in normal human subjects. Behav Brain Res. 1994;64:173–178. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(94)90129-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 236.Brown WS., Larson EB., Jeeves M. Directional asymmetries in interhemispheric transmission time: evidence from visual evoked potentials. Neuropsychologia. 1994;32:439–448. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90089-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 237.Saron CD., Davidson RJ. Visual evoked potential measures of interhemispheric transfer time in humans. Behav Neurosci. 1989;103:1115–1138. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.103.5.1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 238.Larson EB., Brown WS. Bilateral field interactions, hemispheric specialization and evoked potential interhemispheric transmission time. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35:573–581. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(96)00099-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 239.Brown-Séquard C-E. Have we two brains or one? The Forum. 1890;9:627–643. [Google Scholar]
  • 240.Oliveri M., Rossini PM., Traversa R., et al. Left frontal transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces contralesional extinction in patients with unilateral right brain damage. Brain. 1999;122:1731–1739. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.9.1731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 241.Hilgetag CC., Théoret H., Pascual-Leone A. Enhanced visual spatial attention ipsilateral to rTMS-induced 'virtual lesions' of human parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:953–957. doi: 10.1038/nn0901-953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 242.Vuilleumier P., Hester D., Assal G., et al. Unilateral spatial neglect recovery after sequential strokes. Neurology. 1996;46:184–189. doi: 10.1212/wnl.46.1.184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 243.Kinsbourne M. Orientational bias model of unilateral neglect: evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. In: Robertson IH, Marshall JC, eds. Unilateral Neglect: Clinical and Experimental Studies. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1993:63–86. [Google Scholar]
  • 244.Güntürkün O., Böhringer PG. Lateralization reversal after intertectal commissurotomy in the pigeon. Brain Res. 1987;408:1–5. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)90351-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 245.Luck SJ., Hillyard SA., Mangun GR., et al. Independent attentional scanning in the separated hemispheres of split-brain patients. J Cogn Neurosci. 1994;6:84–91. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1994.6.1.84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Taylor & Francis

RESOURCES