Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Soc Sci Med. 2011 Jul 12;73(7):1088–1096. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.06.011

Childhood socio-economic status and the onset, persistence, and severity of DSM-IV mental disorders in a US national sample

Katie A McLaughlin a, Joshua Breslau b, Jennifer Greif Green c, Matthew D Lakoma d, Nancy A Sampson e, Alan M Zaslavsky e, Ronald C Kessler e,*
PMCID: PMC3191493  NIHMSID: NIHMS311298  PMID: 21820781

Abstract

Although significant associations between childhood socio-economic status (SES) and adult mental disorders have been widely documented, SES has been defined using several different indicators often considered alone. Little research has examined the relative importance of these different indicators in accounting for the overall associations of childhood SES with adult outcomes. Nor has previous research distinguished associations of childhood SES with first onsets of mental disorders in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood from those with persistence of these disorders into adulthood in accounting for the overall associations between childhood SES and adult mental disorders. Disaggregated data of this sort are presented here for the associations of childhood SES with a wide range of adult DSM-IV mental disorders in the US National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R), a nationally representative sample of 5,692 adults. Childhood SES was assessed retrospectively with information about parental education and occupation and childhood family financial adversity. Associations of these indicators with first onset of 20 DSM-IV disorders that included anxiety, mood, behavioral, and substance disorders at different life course stages (childhood, adolescence, early adulthood, and mid-later adulthood) and the persistence/severity of these disorders were examined using discrete-time survival analysis. Lifetime disorders and their ages-of-onset were assessed retrospectively with the WHO Composite International Diagnostic Interview. Different aspects of childhood SES predicted onset, persistence, and severity of mental disorders. Childhood financial hardship predicted onset of all classes of disorders at every life-course stage with odds-ratios (ORs) of 1.7–2.3. Childhood financial hardship was unrelated, in comparison, to disorder persistence or severity. Low parental education, although unrelated to disorder onset, significantly predicted disorder persistence and severity, whereas parental occupation was unrelated to onset, persistence, or severity. Some, but not all, of these associations were explained by other co-occurring childhood adversities. These specifications have important implications for mental health interventions targeting low-SES children.

Keywords: USA, Childhood, Socio-economic Status (SES), Poverty, Childhood Adversity, Mental Disorder, Anxiety Disorder, Mood Disorder

Accumulating evidence suggests that childhood socio-economic status (SES) is an important marker of early-life environmental conditions that has lasting effects on health across the life-course (Davey Smith, Blane, & Hawthorne, 1997; Marmot, Shipley, Brunner, & Hemingway, 2001; Power, Atherton, Strachan, Shepherd, Fuller, Davis et al., 2007). Although much of the research on this topic has focused on physical health and mortality (Cohen, Janicki-Deverts, Chen, & Matthews, 2010; Galobardes, Lynch, & Davey Smith, 2004), significant associations between childhood SES and adverse mental health outcomes in adulthood also have been reported in numerous studies (Fan & Eaton, 2001; Gilman, Kawachi, Fitzmaurice, & Buka, 2003; Gilman, Kawachi, Fitzmaurice, & Buka, 2002; Lundberg, 1993; Marmot et al., 2001; Power & Manor, 1992). However, knowledge of the relationship between childhood SES and adult mental health remains cursory due to several pervasive limitations in existing research. First, the specific dimensions of childhood SES that predict adult mental health remain to be identified due to a lack of studies that include a range of childhood SES indicators. Second, the extent to which childhood SES is associated with specific mental disorders or to other aspects of psychiatric morbidity such as disorder persistence and severity is unclear, as most studies have examined associations only with psychological distress. Third, because previous research has not had information on the age of onset of psychiatric outcomes, we know little about when in the life-course childhood SES first has effects on mental health. Finally, although the association between childhood SES and adult mental health may be accounted for by a wide range of other childhood adversities (CAs) that co-occur with low SES, existing research has largely failed to control for these important confounders. We address these gaps in the literature in the current report.

Dimensions of Socioeconomic Status

Childhood SES might contribute to the subsequent development of psychopathology through a number of pathways that are differentially related to aspects of childhood SES (Braveman, Cubbin, Egerter, Chideya, Marchi, Metzler et al., 2005; Galobardes, Shaw, Lawlor, Lynch, & Davey Smith, 2006; Krieger, Williams, & Moss, 1997; Lynch & Kaplan, 2000). The three primary indicators of childhood SES are parental educational attainment, parental occupational status, and financial adversity. Low educational attainment reflects a persistent disadvantage in relative social position that is a marker of one’s access to resources, knowledge, and social structures that promote health and well-being. Perceptions of low relative status may have pernicious psychological effects by engendering social comparisons that generate negative affect and feelings of low self-worth (Wilkinson, 1996; Wilkinson & Pickett, 2007). Parental occupation also is associated with prestige and with differential exposure to harsh work conditions that could have spillover effects into the levels of stress experienced by children at home. In contrast, earning income that is inadequate for subsistence is an indicator of deprivation in actual resources that are needed to sustain health, such as adequate food, shelter, and access to health care. Threatened or actual material deprivation resulting from financial hardship may generate anxiety and worry that increase risk for psychopathology.

Although it is likely that these indicators have differential associations with mental health outcomes, many studies have utilized a single indicator of SES or a single composite made up by combining information across indicators (Gilman et al., 2003; Marmot et al., 2001; Power, Hertzman, Matthews, & Manor, 1997; Power & Manor, 1992). Previous studies have therefore been unable to disentangle the effects of different aspects of childhood SES on adult psychopathology. Consideration of the effects of these indicators simultaneously may reveal differentiation that has theoretical and practical significance (Kessler, 1982). For example, one previous study found that economic hardship in childhood was associated with adult psychological distress whereas parental educational attainment was not (Lahelma et al., 2006), suggesting that the focus of preventive interventions should be on financial stress.

Dimensions of Psychiatric Morbidity

Most research on the long-term effects of childhood SES has examined its associations with psychological distress rather than with psychiatric disorders (Lahelma et al., 2006; Lundberg, 1993; Marmot et al., 2001; Power & Manor, 1992). Although some prior studies have examined mental disorders, many have focused exclusively on major depression (Fan & Eaton, 2001; Gilman et al., 2003; Ritsher, Warner, Johnson, & Dohrenwend, 2001; Stansfeld, Clark, Rodgers, Caldwell, & Power, 2008), precluding examination of differential effects of childhood SES across the full range of mental disorders. Although numerous psychiatric outcomes were examined in one prior study of adolescent mental disorders (Miech et al., 1999), we are unaware of similar studies examining adult psychopathology. Importantly, no attempt has been made in past studies to distinguish associations of childhood SES with first onsets of mental disorders at different life-course stages. This is an important omission, because a majority of adults with mental disorders met criteria for a disorder in childhood or adolescence (Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas, & Walters, 2005; Kim-Cohen, Caspi, Moffitt, Harrington, Milne, & Poulton, 2003) before their adult SES was established. Because early-onset disorders are associated with the development of comorbid disorders later in life, the associations of childhood SES with adult disorders might be explained completely by greater onset of childhood disorders among individuals with low SES. Alternatively, associations of childhood SES with adult mental health may simply reflect the persistence of childhood disorders into adulthood. Prior studies of childhood SES have, with rare exceptions (Gilman et al., 2003), failed to distinguish effects on disorder onset from disorder persistence. Distinguishing between these two possibilities is important, because the intervention implications would be quite different if childhood SES predicts new onsets of mental disorders in adulthood than if childhood SES were simply associated with a more chronic course of disorders beginning earlier in life.

Other Childhood Adversities

Low childhood SES is associated with increased exposure to a range of other CAs such as parental psychopathology, maltreatment, and family violence (Tracy, Zimmerman, Galea, McCauley, & Stoep, 2008; Turner, Finkelhor, & Ormrod, 2006) that are associated with psychiatric disorders in adulthood (Green, McLaughlin, Berglund, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky et al., 2010; Kessler, Davis, & Kendler, 1997; McLaughlin, Green, Gruber, Sampson, Zaslavsky, & Kessler, 2010a, b). These CAs are likely confounders of the childhood SES-adult psychopathology association. Although several prior studies have examined this possibility, they have been restricted in the types of CAs considered (Gilman et al., 2002; Lundberg, 1993; Mäkinen, Laaksonen, Lahelma, & Rahkonen, 2006; Sadowski, Ugarte, Kolvin, Kaplan, & Barnes, 1999; Veijola, Puukka, Lehtinen, Moring, Lindholm, & Vaisanen, 1998). Childhood SES remained significantly associated with adult mental health after controlling for CAs in several of these studies (Lundberg, 1993; Sadowski et al., 1999) but not in others (Mäkinen et al., 2006).

We address these limitations in past work using data from a national survey of the US population. We disaggregate the associations of lifetime DSM-IV disorders with three different childhood SES indicators (parental education, parental occupation, and financial hardship) as they separately predict the onset, persistence, and severity of mental disorders at various life-course stages. In addition, we examine these associations after controlling for a wide range of other CAs.

METHODS

Sample

The NCS-R was a face-to-face household survey of English-speaking respondents ages 18 and older carried out between February 2001 and April 2003 in a nationally-representative multi-stage clustered area probability sample of the US household population (Kessler & Merikangas, 2004). The response rate was 70.9%. The survey was administered in two parts. Part I included a core diagnostic assessment of mental disorders (n=9,282). Part II assessed risk factors, consequences, other correlates, and additional disorders that were administered to all Part I respondents who met lifetime criteria for a disorder plus a probability subsample of other respondents (n=5,692). The Part I sample was weighted to adjust for differential probabilities of selection within households. The Part II sample was weighted to adjust for selection probabilities. A final weight adjusted the Part II sample to match the 2000 census population on a cross-classification of a number of geographic and socio-demographic variables. All analyses are based on the weighted Part II sample. More information about the NCS-R sampling design and weighting is reported elsewhere (Kessler, Berglund, Chiu, Demler, Heeringa, Hiripi et al., 2004). Recruitment and consent procedures were approved by the human subjects committees of Harvard Medical School and the University of Michigan.

Measures

Diagnostic Assessment

NCS-R diagnoses are based on Version 3.0 of the World Health Organization Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) (Kessler & Üstun, 2004), a fully-structured lay-administered interview that generates diagnoses according to DSM-IV criteria. Diagnoses comprise four broad classes that include 20 specific disorders: Mood disorders [major depressive disorder, dysthymic disorder, bipolar I and II disorder, and sub-threshold BPD], anxiety disorders (panic disorder, agoraphobia without a history of panic disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, specific phobia, social phobia, post-traumatic stress disorder, separation anxiety disorder), behavior disorders (intermittent explosive disorder, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, oppositional-defiant disorder, conduct disorder), and substance disorders (alcohol abuse, alcohol dependence with abuse, drug abuse, drug dependence with abuse). The prevalence and distribution of these disorders in the NCS-R has been reported previously (Kessler et al., 2005). As detailed elsewhere (Kessler et al., 2004), blinded clinical reappraisal interviews with a probability sub-sample of NCS-R respondents found generally good concordance between DSM-IV diagnoses based on the CIDI and those based on clinical interviews.

The CIDI assessed age-of-onset (AOO) of disorders retrospectively. A special question sequence was used to improve accuracy of onset reporting: “Can you remember your exact age the very first time when you (had the symptom/the syndrome)?” Respondents who answered “no” were probed for a bound of uncertainty by moving up the age range incrementally (e.g. “Was it before you went to school?”; “Was it before you were a teen?”). Onset was set at the upper end of the bound of uncertainty (e.g., age 12 for respondents who reported that onset was before the beginning of their teens). Experimental research has shown that this approach yields more plausible responses than standard AOO questions (Knauper, Cannell, Schwarz, Bruce, & Kessler, 1999). Time-since-onset (TSO) was calculated by subtracting AOO from age at interview.

Disorder Persistence

Persistence of disorders is a function of both episode duration and recurrence risk. Longitudinal studies can calculate persistence directly from information about duration of incident episodes, time to recurrence after incident episode offset, duration of second episodes, and so on, although this is difficult logistically even in multi-wave prospective studies (Yonkers, Bruce, Dyck, & Keller, 2003). It is impossible to measure persistence directly using retrospective assessments, but persistence can be estimated indirectly from the ratio of current prevalence to lifetime prevalence. This approach uses only part of the information about recency of disorders, though, as data are also collected on age at offset of the most recent episode among lifetime cases in the CIDI. This information can be used to estimate disorder persistence more accurately using a special class of survival models, described in more detail below.

Disorder Severity

The Sheehan Disability Scales (SDS) (Leon, Olfson, Portera, Farber, & Sheehan, 1997) were used to assess disorder-specific impairment in role performance in work, household maintenance, social life, and intimate relationships for mood, anxiety, and behavior (but not substance) disorders. The SDS asked respondents to think of the month in the past year when the focal disorder was most severe and to rate on a 0–10 visual analog scale how much the disorder caused impairments in each domain (where 0=none, 1–3=mild, 4–6=moderate, 7–9=severe, and 10=very severe). Respondents who received a score of severe or very severe in any of the four domains were classified ‘severe’ for the current analyses.

Childhood SES

Parental education was classified into four categories: less than high school graduation (0–11 years), high school graduation (12 years), some post high school education (13–15 years), and college graduation (16+ years). Occupations were classified into 28 categories and 10 major groups defined by the International Standard Classification of Occupations of the International Labour Organization (ISCO-88) (Hussmanns, Mehran, & Verma, 1990). The 10 major groups were further classified into skill-level categories defined by ISCO-88 as elementary (unskilled and semi-skilled labor), low-average prestige (clerks, service and sales, skilled workers, craft and related, and plant/machine operators), high-average prestige (technicians), and high prestige (professionals). Although not assigned a skill level in the ISCO-88 classification, members of the armed forces were classified as low-average prestige and legislators were classified as high prestige. Respondents were classified in terms of the higher education and occupation level of either parent if scores were available for both and at elementary occupation if neither parent worked. Financial hardship was assessed in a question that asked respondents whether their family ever received government assistance in the form of food stamps, welfare checks, or other forms of financial aid before they were 18 years of age.

Childhood Adversities

Eleven CAs occurring before age 18 were assessed in the NCS-R. These include three types of interpersonal loss (parental death, parental divorce, and other loss of contact with parents), four types of parental maladjustment (mental illness, substance abuse, criminality, and inter-parental violence), three types of maltreatment (physical abuse, sexual abuse, neglect), and serious respondent physical illness. The interpersonal losses were assessed with measures of parental death, divorce, and other parental separations (e.g., adoption, foster placement, living with other relatives instead of parents). Parental mental illness (major depression, generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder, antisocial personality disorder) and substance abuse were assessed with the Family History Research Diagnostic Criteria Interview (Endicott, Andreasen, & Spitzer, 1978) and its extensions (Kendler, Silberg, Neale, Kessler, Heath, & Eaves, 1991). Family violence and physical abuse were assessed with a modified version of the Conflict Tactics Scale (Straus, 1979). Sexual abuse was assessed with questions about repeated fondling, attempted rape, and rape. Neglect was assessed using questions commonly used in studies of child welfare (Courtney, Piliavin, Grogan-Kaylor, & Nesmith, 1998).

Data Analysis

Associations between childhood SES and disorder onset were examined using discrete-time survival analysis with person-year the unit of analysis (Singer & Willett, 1993). Initial analyses were carried out by stacking the 20 separate disorder-specific person-year files and including 19 dummy predictor variables that distinguished among these files, thereby forcing the estimated slopes of the disorders on childhood SES to be the same across all disorders. Disorder-specific models were subsequently examined to investigate differences in slopes across disorders. The initial model examined the effects of all childhood SES variables simultaneously. Subsequent models added controls for number and type of CAs (Green et al., 2010). These models were examined in the total data array and in subsamples defined by life-course stage (childhood: ages 4–12; adolescence: ages 13–19; early adulthood: ages 20–29; and middle-later adulthood: ages 30+).

Associations between childhood SES and disorder persistence were examined using a special class of survival models known as backward recurrence models (Alison, 1984; Yamaguchi, 2003). These models use a person-year survival approach to predict current prevalence among lifetime cases and recency of the last episode of a focal disorder among respondents with a lifetime history of that disorder who are not in episode at the time of interview. The outcome is the number of years between the age at interview and the most recent episode. Longer survival times indicate longer periods of remission. Person-years prior to the most recent episode are censored. These models controlled for AOO and TSO.

The associations of childhood SES with disorder severity were investigated using logistic regression models that were estimated among respondents with an episode of each focal disorder in the 12 months prior to the survey to predict probability of being classified as ‘severe’ on any of the four subscales of the SDS. Logistic regression coefficients and their standard errors were exponentiated and are reported as odds-ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs). All models included controls for age at interview, gender, and race-ethnicity (Non-Hispanic White, Non-Hispanic Black, Hispanic, Other). Because the NCS-R data are both clustered and weighted, the design-based Taylor series method implemented in the SUDAAN software system (Research Triangle Institute, 2002) was used to estimate standard errors. Significance was evaluated using .05-level two-sided tests.

RESULTS

Associations of childhood adversities with childhood SES and DSM-IV/CIDI disorders

We first conducted a series of analyses to determine whether CAs were associated with childhood SES and mental disorders and thus should be controlled in subsequent analysis. The 11 CAs were related both to childhood SES and to psychiatric disorders. Childhood SES indicators were examined as predictors of each of the 11 CAs in a series of logistic regression models, with controls for age, gender, and race/ethnicity. Financial hardship was associated with 10 of the 11 CAs, whereas parental educational attainment and occupation were associated with 5 and 6 of the CAs. (Table 1)

Table 1.

Multivariate associations (odds-ratios) between childhood SES and childhood adversitiesa (n=4,780)

Childhood Adversities
Parent
Died
Parent
Divorce
Other
Parent Loss
Parent
Mental
Parent
Substance
Parent
Criminal
Family
Violence
Physical
Abuse
Sexual
Abuse
Neglect Physical
Illness
N % (95% CI) % (95% CI) % (95% CI) % (95% CI) % (95% CI) % (95% CI) % (95% CI) % (95% CI) % (95% CI) % (95% CI) % (95% CI)

Parent Education
 0–11 years 1073 0.8 0.6–1.3 1.0 0.7–1.4 2.4 1.3–4.2 0.8 0.5–1.1 1.2 0.8–1.9 2.4 1.4–4.2 2.0 1.4–2.7 1.7 1.1–2.5 0.7 0.5–1.0 2.4 1.5–3.9 1.2 0.8–1.8
 12 years 1856 1.2 0.7–1.8 1.3 1.0–1.6 2.2 1.3–4.0 0.9 0.6–1.1 1.4 0.9–2.1 2.0 1.2–3.2 1.6 1.1–2.2 1.3 1.0–1.8 1.1 0.8–1.6 2.1 1.3–3.4 0.8 0.6–1.2
 13–15 years 708 0.9 0.5–1.4 1.5 0.9–2.5 1.4 0.7–2.9 0.8 0.5–1.2 1.5 0.9–2.5 1.4 0.7–2.7 1.3 1.0–1.8 1.4 0.9–2.0 1.5 1.0–2.3 2.1 1.3–3.4 1.2 0.8–1.7
 16+ years 1143 - -- - -- - -- - -- - -- - -- - -- - -- - -- - -- - --
  χ 23 (p-value) 3.5 (.330) 6.0 (.110) 12.8 (.010)* 2.0 (.580) 3.7 (.300) 20.8 (<.001)* 21.2 (<.001)* 7.0 (.070) 10.2 (.020)* 19.0 (<.001)* 5.9 (.110)
Parent Occupation
 Never worked 18 1.3 0.2–7.6 0.4 0.1–1.7 28.7 10.5–78.7 2.0 0.4–9.2 3.5 1.0–12.0 7.2 1.1–48.6 1.0 0.3–4.1 2.4 0.7–8.1 2.0 0.4–9.4 3.9 0.9–16.2 1.2 0.1–10.0
 Elementary 459 1.2 0.7–2.0 0.8 0.5–1.4 2.2 1.2–3.7 0.8 0.4–1.4 1.3 0.9–2.0 2.1 1.2–3.5 1.1 0.7–1.9 1.1 0.8–1.5 1.0 0.6–1.6 1.2 0.5–2.7 1.2 0.8–2.0
 Low-average prestige 2362 0.9 0.6–1.3 1.2 1.0–1.5 1.9 1.3–2.9 1.2 1.0–1.5 2.1 1.6–2.6 2.0 1.3–3.0 1.8 1.4–2.2 1.6 1.2–2.0 1.1 0.8–1.5 1.7 1.1–2.8 1.3 1.0–1.8
 High-average prestige 481 1.1 0.7–1.8 1.3 0.8–2.1 1.5 0.9–2.6 1.2 0.9–1.7 1.6 1.0–2.6 1.4 0.8–2.4 1.8 1.2–2.7 1.0 0.7–1.6 1.2 0.7–1.8 1.6 0.8–3.0 1.0 0.6–1.8
 High prestige 1460 - -- - -- - -- - -- - -- - -- - -- - -- - -- - -- - --
  χ 24 (p-value) 1.9 (.750) 4.6 (.330) 51.7 (<.001)* 5.0 (.290) 31.4 (<.001)* 13.1 (.010)* 32.0 (<.001)* 14.1 (.010)* 1.9 (.760) 15.2 (<.010)* 3.6 (.470)
Financial Hardship
 Yes 323 2.4 1.5–3.9 3.2 2.0–5.2 3.5 2.4–5.3 3.2 2.3–4.5 4.7 3.3–6.8 3.8 2.7–5.3 4.8 3.2–7.0 3.4 2.3–4.9 2.5 1.6–4.0 5.0 3.5–7.2 1.2 0.8–1.8
 No 4457
  χ21 (p-value) 13.4 (<.001)* 24.3 (<.001)* 36.6 (<.001)* 47.8 (<.001)* 69.7 (<.001)* 58.2 (<.001)* 61.8 (<.001)* 38.1 (<.001)* 14.8 (<.001)* 76.7 (<.001)* 0.5 (.470)
a

Model controlled for age, gender, and race/ethnicity

Associations between CAs and psychiatric disorders were examined in a series of logistic regression models predicting the subsequent onset of each disorder, controlling for age, gender, and race/ethnicity. Of the 11 CAs, 10 were associated significantly with disorder onset (ORs=1.1–1.9). Detailed associations between CAs and disorders in the NCS-R have been reported previously (Green et al., 2010).

Associations between childhood SES and first onset of DSM-IV/CIDI disorders

In the initial model examining the associations of childhood SES with first onset of the 20 pooled DSM-IV/CIDI disorders, only financial hardship was a significant predictor of disorder onset (OR=1.9, χ21=42.4, p<.001). (Table 2) After controlling for CAs, the association between financial hardship and disorder onset was no longer significant.

Table 2.

Multivariate associations (odds ratios) between childhood SES and the onset of DSM-IV/CIDI disordersa,b

Any Disorder Mood Anxiety Substance Behavior
OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)

I. Without CA controlsb
Parent occupation
 Never worked/Elementary 0.9(0.8–1.2) 0.9 (0.7–1.2) 1.0 (0.8–1.4) 0.7 (0.5–1.1) 1.0 (0.7–1.6)
 Low-average prestige 1.1(0.9–1.2) 1.0 (0.9–1.2) 1.1 (1.0–1.3) 1.0 (0.8–1.3) 1.1 (0.8–1.5)
 High-average prestige 0.9(0.8–1.1) 0.8 (0.6–1.0) 0.9 (0.7–1.1) 1.0 (0.7–1.4) 1.0 (0.7–1.4)
 High prestige - - - - -
  χ 23 4.6 5.3 5.3 3.2 0.4
Parent Education
 0–11 years 1.0(0.8–1.2) 0.9 (0.7–1.1) 1.0 (0.8–1.2) 0.9 (0.7–1.3 1.2 (0.8–1.8)
 12 years 1.0(0.9–1.2) 0.9 (0.8–1.1) 1.0 (0.8–1.2) 1.1 (0.8–1.4) 1.1 (0.8–1.5)
 13–15 years 1.0(0.9–1.2) 0.9 (0.7–1.1) 1.1 (0.9–1.3) 1.0 (0.7–1.4) 1.2 (0.9–1.7)
 16+ years - - - - -
  χ23 0.4 1.4 0.9 1.4 2.2
Financial Hardship
 Yes 1.9*(1.6-2.3) 1.7* (1.4–2.1) 1.9* (1.5–2.3) 2.1* (1.6–2.6) 2.1* (1.6–2.8)
  χ21 42.4* 22.4* 35.1* 33.5* 23.3*
II. With CA controlsc
 Parent occupation
 Never worked/Elementary 0.9(0.7–1.1) 0.9 (0.7–1.2) 1.0 (0.8–1.3) 0.7 (0.5–1.0) 0.9 (0.6–1.3)
 Low-average prestige 1.0(0.8–1.1) 0.9 (0.8–1.1) 1.0 (0.9–1.2) 0.9 (0.7–1.1) 1.0 (0.7–1.4)
 High-average prestige 0.8(0.7–1.0) 0.8 (0.6–0.9) 0.8 (0.7–1.0) 1.0 (0.7–1.3) 1.0 (0.7–1.3)
 High prestige - - - - -
  χ23 4.2 7.4 5.3 3.3 0.3
Parent Education
 0–11 years 1.0(0.8–1.2) 0.9 (0.7–1.2) 1.0 (0.8–1.2) 1.0 (0.7–1.3) 1.1 (0.8–1.6)
 12 years 1.0(0.9–.2) 0.9 (0.8–1.1) 1.0 (0.9–1.2) 1.1 (0.8–1.4) 1.1 (0.8–1.4)
 13–15 years 1.0(0.9–1.2) 0.9 (0.7–1.1) 1.1 (0.9–1.2) 1.0 (0.8–1.3) 1.2 (0.9–1.7)
 16+ years - - - - -
  χ3 0.4 1.8 0.9 0.9 1.8
Financial Hardship
 Yes 1.2(1.0–1.4) 1.2 (0.9–1.4) 1.2 (1.0–1.6) 1.2 (0.9–1.5) 1.2 (0.9–1.6)
  χ21 2.5 1.8 3.3 0.9 1.0
   (n1/n2) d (4,780/344,299) (4,780/96,537) (4,780/130,781) (4,780/76,671) (4,780/28,646)
*

Significant at the 0.05 level, two-tailed

a

Models were estimated in a discrete-time survival framework with childhood SES variable and controls used to predict the first onset of each outcome disorder. There were a total of 4,780 respondents with information on childhood SES included in the analysis and we considered a total of 20 outcome disorders.

b

Model included all childhood SES variables and controlled for person-year, age, sex, race, and the diagnostic categories included in the pooled data file.

c

Model included covariates for the 11 CAs. See Methods Section for details.

d

n1=the number of respondents included in the analysis; n2=the number of person-years included in the analysis.

We next replicated the analysis to predict disorders separately within classes. Financial hardship predicted onset of each of the four classes of disorders (ORs=1.7–2.1, χ21=22.5–35.1, p<.001). None of these associations were sustained after adjustment for CAs.

Associations between childhood SES and disorder onset at different life-course stages

Replication in subsamples of person-years defined by life-course stage showed that financial hardship was associated with disorder onset at every life-course stage (ORs=1.4–2.3, χ21=6.2–43.1, p=.010-<.001). (Table 3) The magnitude of this association decreased significantly with age ( χ23=9.8, p<.05). In the model that includes CA controls, financial hardship remained significantly associated with disorder onset only in childhood (OR=1.3, χ21=4.7, p=.030). Parent occupation also predicted disorder onset in childhood (χ23=8.9, p=.030), with respondents whose parents had high-average prestige occupations at lower risk for disorder onset (OR=0.8) than respondents whose parents had high prestige occupations.

Table 3.

Multivariate associations (odds ratios) between childhood SES and the onset of DSM-IV/CIDI disorders at four life-course stagesa

Person-years
4–12 13–19 20–29 30+
OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)

I. Without CA controls
Parent Occupation
 Never worked/Elementary 1.0 (0.7–1.3) 0.9 (0.6–1.3) 1.0 (0.8–1.4) 0.9 (0.6–1.4)
 Low-average prestige 1.0 (0.9–1.3) 1.0 (0.8–1.3) 1.0 (0.8–1.2) 1.2*(1.0–1.6)
 High-average prestige 0.8 (0.7–1.0) 1.0 (0.8–1.3) 1.0 (0.7–1.2) 0.9 (0.6–1.2)
 High prestige - - - -
  χ 23 5.9 1.1 0.4 7.7*
Parent Education
 0–11 years 1.0 (0.8–1.3) 1.0 (0.7–1.3) 0.9 (0.6–1.2) 1.0 (0.7–1.4)
 12 years 1.0 (0.8–1.3) 1.0 (0.8– 1.2) 1.0 (0.8– 1.3) 1.0 (0.8– 1.2)
 13–15 years 1.2 (1.0– 1.5) 1.0 (0.8– 1.2) 1.0 (0.8– 1.3) 0.9 (0.6– 1.2)
 16+ years - - - -
   χ 23 3.9 0.2 2.3 0.6
Financial Hardship
 Yes 2.3* (1.8– 2.9) 1.9* (1.5– 2.4) 1.8* (1.4– 2.4) 1.4* (1.1– 1.9)
  χ 21 43.1* 24.1* 16.4* 6.2*
II. With CA controls
Parent Occupation
 Never worked/Elementary 0.9 (0.7– 1.1) 0.9 (0.6– 1.4) 1.0 (0.8– 1.3) 0.9 (0.6– 1.3)
 Low-average prestige 0.9 (0.8– 1.1) 0.9 (0.8– 1.1) 0.9 (0.8– 1.1) 1.1 (0.9– 1.4)
 High-average prestige 0.8 (0.6– 0.9) 0.9 (0.7– 1.2) 0.9 (0.7– 1.1) 0.8 (0.6– 1.2)
 High prestige - - - -
  χ 23 8.9* 0.8 1.9 4.6
Parent Education
 0–11 years 1.0 (0.8– 1.3) 1.0 (0.7– 1.3) 0.9 (0.7– 1.1) 1.0 (0.8– 1.4)
 12 years 1.0 (0.9– 1.3) 1.0 (0.8– 1.2) 1.0 (0.8– 1.3) 1.0 (0.8– 1.3)
 13–15 years 1.2 (1.0– 1.4) 1.0 (0.8– 1.2) 1.0 (0.8– 1.3) 0.9 (0.7– 1.2)
 16+ years - - - -
  χ 23 4.0 0.3 4.1 0.9
Financial Hardship
 Yes 1.3* (1.0– 1.6) 1.1 (0.9– 1.4) 1.2 (0.9– 1.6) 1.0(0.8– 1.4)
  χ 21 4.7* 0.7 2.0 0.1
    (n1/n2)b (4,780/81,668) (4,780/61,777) (4,615/74,026) 3,693/126,828)
*

Significant at the 0.05 level, two-tailed

a

See footnote 1 to Table 2 for a description of the dataset, overall modeling approach, and control variables.

b

n1=the number of respondents included in the analysis; n2=the number of person-years included in the analysis.

Associations between childhood SES and persistence of DSM-IV/CIDI disorders

Childhood SES was not associated with persistence of pooled DSM-IV/CIDI disorders in the backward recurrence model. (Appendix table available at: http://www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/ncs/publications.php. [INSERT LINK TO ONLINE FILES].) Disaggregation of this model, though, revealed differential associations across the four disorder classes. Parental education was associated with persistence of behavior disorders (χ23=9.6, p=.020), with persistence greatest among respondents whose parents had less than a high school education (OR=1.6) relative to those whose parents had a college education. This association persisted even after controlling for CAs (χ23=12.1, p=.010). None of the other indicators of childhood SES predicted disorder persistence.

Associations between childhood SES and severity of DSM-IV/CIDI disorders

Lower parental education (ORs=1.5–1.8, χ23=10.4, p=.020) and financial hardship (OR=1.7, χ21=4.9, p=.030) were associated with elevated odds of being classified as severe on the SDS. (Table 5) The disorders of respondents whose parents had a high school education or less (ORs=1.6–1.8) were more likely to be severe than those of respondents whose parents had a college education. When controls for CAs were added to the model, parental education continued to predict disorder severity for all disorder classes ( χ23=8.5–13.3, p=.040–.010), such that the disorders of respondents whose parents had less than a college education were most likely to be severe.

Sensitivity Analysis

We evaluated whether the associations between parental SES and disorder severity changed when we adjusted for the respondent’s educational attainment and past-year family income in the model that included controls for CAs. Childhood parental education, which was the only significant SES indicator in that model, remained a significant predictor of disorder severity for anxiety, mood, and behavior disorders when we controlled respondent education and past-year income (χ23=9.3–14.1, p=.003–.026).

DISCUSSION

This study provides useful new information about the associations of childhood SES with the onset, course, and severity of mental disorders. The reliance on retrospective reports is a potential limitation because of the possibility of recall bias (Simon & von Korff, 1995) which may persist despite the use of methods for prompting accurate recall of the onset and offset of disorders (Knauper et al., 1999). Although retrospective studies in representative samples such as this provide an opportunity to investigate broad population patterns, replication of our findings in prospectively followed cohorts, such as the Great Smoky Mountain and Dunedin studies, is an important goal for future research. An additional noteworthy limitation is that we did not control for adult SES in our analysis of disorder onset and persistence. Although some studies have found that the association between childhood SES and adult mental health disappears after accounting for adult SES (Marmot et al., 2001; Stansfeld et al., 2008), others have found that the association persists (Huurre, Aro, & Rahkonen, 2003; Mäkinen et al., 2006; Power et al., 2007) Importantly, the association between childhood SES and adult mental disorder cannot be explained by adult SES if disorder onset occurs prior to the establishment of adult SES, a critical limitation that has not been addressed in existing research. Indeed, we found that childhood SES was most strongly related to disorder onsets occurring in childhood and adolescence, before the establishment of adult SES. Moreover, our sensitivity analysis showed that the associations between parental education and severity of adult disorders remained significant after adjustment for the respondent’s SES in adulthood. Finally, this study relied on data from a U.S. sample. These findings therefore warrant replication in samples drawn from other settings and cultural contexts.

Within the context of these limitations the results of this study extend the literature on childhood SES and psychiatric disorders by showing that this relationship is more complex than has previously been shown to be the case, with different indicators of childhood SES predicting onset, persistence, and severity of a wide range of mental disorders. Four results are especially noteworthy. First, we found that the traditional indicators of childhood SES, parent education and occupation, did not predict disorder onset, but that financial hardship predicted disorder onset at every stage of the life-course. This finding is consistent with evidence that childhood financial hardship is more strongly associated with mental disorders than parental education and occupation (Lahelma et al., 2006), and suggests that material deprivation is more strongly associated with the onset of psychiatric disorders than indicators of relative status.

Second, financial hardship was associated with elevated risk of initial disorder onset across all four disorder classes considered here and in all four life-course stages, although the magnitude of this association decreased significantly with age. These findings are consistent with previous research showing a strong relationship between child poverty and child psychopathology (McLeod & Shanahan, 1996; Goodman et al., 2003), and extend this literature by documenting a breadth of the association between childhood financial hardship and mental disorders over the life-course that has, to our knowledge, never before been demonstrated. This pattern of findings adds to a growing evidence base supporting social causation rather than social selection theories of SES and psychopathology (Johnson et al., 1999; Ritsher et al., 2001).

Third, the association between financial hardship and subsequent onset of mental disorders remained significant only in childhood once we controlled for CAs, suggesting that exposure to CAs explains the association between financial hardship and onset of mental disorders in adolescence and adulthood. These findings provide indirect evidence for the role of early exposure to stress as a pathway linking childhood SES to adult mental health, in addition to the previously documented socio-economic pathway (Stansfeld et al., 2008). This finding has implications for interventions aimed at reducing the mental health consequences of childhood poverty and underscores the importance of ameliorating the adverse emotional effects of a wide range of CAs when targeting preventive interventions to children in low-SES families.

Fourth, we found associations between parental education and disorder persistence and severity, which were not explained by CAs. Low parental education was associated with persistence of behavior disorders, and with severity of mood, anxiety, and behavior disorders. The number of these associations is striking, especially because parental education was not a significant predictor of disorder onset. These findings suggest that, in contrast to our results for disorder onset, disadvantaged relative position in childhood predicts disorder persistence and severity in adulthood more strongly than material deprivation. Parental education may be a marker of access to resources that promote recovery from mental illness, such as mental health treatment, or may confer protection against the adverse social consequences of mental disorders. For example, greater educational attainment is associated with reduced stigma towards individuals with mental illness (Corrigan & Watson, 2007), which may improve parental support for individuals with psychiatric disorders. The fact that behavior disorders were found to be involved in many of these associations is noteworthy. It may be that the relatively early age-of-onset of behavior disorders (Kessler et al., 2005; Nock, Kazdin, Hiripi, & Kessler, 2007) accounts for the fact that childhood SES is more consistently associated with the persistence-severity of these disorders, a possibility that requires further investigation.

Importantly, we found that active associations of childhood SES with psychiatric disorders continue to exist in adulthood, particularly with disorder persistence and severity. We can conclude from the associations documented here that no single underlying causal pathway is likely to explain the numerous patterns that make up the overall relationship between childhood SES and psychiatric morbidity. Although we saw that a number of the component associations are due to a larger constellation of CAs associated with low childhood SES, others persist even after controlling for these CAs. A number of plausible biological, psychological, structural, and socio-economic mediators are likely involved in these remaining associations (Cohen et al., 2010). Investigation of the relative strength of these mediators requires more focused study in future investigations.

Table 4.

Multivariate associations (odds ratios) between childhood SES and severe scores on the Sheehan Disability scales among respondents with 12-month DSM-IV/CIDI disorders (n=3,448)a

Any Disorder Any Mood Any Anxiety Any Disruptive Behaviore
OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR(95% CI)

I. Without CA controlsb
Parent Occupation
 Never worked/Elementary 1.0 (0.6– 1.6) 1.2 (0.5– 2.7) 1.0 (0.6– 1.7) 1.8 (0.7– 5.1)
 Low-average prestige 1.0 (0.7– 1.4) 0.9 (0.5– 1.5) 1.1 (0.8– 1.6) 0.9 (0.4– 1.7)
 High-average prestige 1.0 (0.7– 1.6) 1.0 (0.4– 2.4) 1.2 (0.8– 1.8) 0.8 (0.2– 3.0)
 High prestige - - - -
  χ 23 0.1 0.7 1.4 2.3
Parent Education
 0–11 years 1.6* (1.0– 2.7) 2.6 (1.3– 5.4) 1.4 (0.8– 2.5) 1.5 (0.7– 3.2)
 12 years 1.8* (1.2– 2.5) 1.5 (0.8– 2.7) 1.6* (1.1– 2.4) 1.6 (0.8– 3.2)
 13–15 years 1.5* (1.0– 2.4) 2.2 (1.1– 4.4) 1.4 (0.8– 2.4) 0.5 (0.2– 1.2)
 16+ years - - - -
  χ23 10.4* 13.3* 10.6* 8.0*
Financial Hardship
 Yes 1.7* (1.1– 2.8) 1.5 (0.8– 2.9) 1.8* (1.0– 3.0) 1.3 (0.5– 3.1)
  χ 21 4.9* 1.6 4.3* 0.3
II. With CA controlsc
Parent Occupation
 Never worked/Elementary 1.0 (0.6– 1.7) 1.4 (0.6– 3.3) 1.0 (0.6– 1.7) 2.8(0.7– 10.2)
 Low-average prestige 0.9 (0.7– 1.2) 0.8 (0.5– 1.4) 1.0 (0.7– 1.4) 0.8 (0.3– 2.0)
 High-average prestige 1.0 (0.7– 1.6) 0.9 (0.4– 2.2) 1.1 (0.7– 1.7) 1.0 (0.3– 3.8)
 High prestige - - - -
  χ 23 0.8 1.9 0.3 6.7
Parent Education
 0–11 years 1.7* (1.1– 2.6) 2.6* (1.3– 5.3) 1.5 (0.9– 2.5) 1.2 (0.5– 3.1)
 12 years 1.8* (1.3– 2.4) 1.5 (0.8– 2.7) 1.7* (1.3– 2.3) 1.4 (0.6– 3.4)
 13–15 years 1.6* (1.0– 2.4) 2.4* (1.2– 4.8) 1.6* (1.0– 2.4) 0.3 (0.1– 1.0)
 16+ years - - - -
  χ23 13.3* 13.1* 14.5* 8.5*
Financial Hardship
 Yes 1.4 (0.9– 2.4) 1.4 (0.7– 2.8) 1.4 (0.8– 2.6) 1.2 (0.6– 2.6)
  χ 21 2.0 0.8 1.5 0.2
    (n1)d (9,432) (1,243) (4,128) (527)
*

Significant at the .05 level, two-sided test.

a

Models were estimated using logistic regression with severity defined as a score of 7–10 on any of the 4 subscales on the Sheehan Disability Scale among those with a 12-month diagnosis

b

Model included all childhood SES variables and controlled for age category, time since onset, sex, race, and diagnosis category.

c

Model included additional covariates for the 11 CAs.

d

n1=the number of disorders included in the analysis.

e

Conduct disorder is not included as the Sheehan scale was not administered for conduct disorder.

Research Highlights.

  • Childhood financial hardship predicts the first onset of DSM-IV mood, anxiety, behavior, and substance disorders, but did not predict either persistence or severity of these disorders.

  • Financial hardship predicts disorder onsets in childhood, adolescence and adulthood, but parent education/occupation do not

  • Childhood financial hardship predicts disorder onsets only in childhood after controlling for other childhood adversities

  • Although unrelated to disorder onsets, low parental education is associated with greater severity of adult mental disorders

  • Low parental education is associated with greater persistence of disruptive behavior disorders

Acknowledgments

This project was supported by a National Institute of Mental Health grants (K01MH092526) to K. McLaughlin and (K01MH085710) to J. Green. The National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) is supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH; U01-MH60220) with supplemental support from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA), the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA), the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation (RWJF; Grant 044780), and the John W. Alden Trust. Collaborating NCS-R investigators include Ronald C. Kessler (Principal Investigator, Harvard Medical School), Kathleen Merikangas (Co-Principal Investigator, NIMH), James Anthony (Michigan State University), William Eaton (The Johns Hopkins University), Meyer Glantz (NIDA), Doreen Koretz (Harvard University), Jane McLeod (Indiana University), Mark Olfson (New York State Psychiatric Institute, College of Physicians and Surgeons of Columbia University), Harold Pincus (University of Pittsburgh), Greg Simon (Group Health Cooperative), Michael Von Korff (Group Health Cooperative), Philip S. Wang (NIMH), Kenneth Wells (UCLA), Elaine Wethington (Cornell University), and Hans-Ulrich Wittchen (Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry; Technical University of Dresden). The views and opinions expressed in this report are those of the authors and should not be construed to represent the views of any of the sponsoring organizations, agencies, or U.S. Government. A complete list of NCS publications and the full text of all NCS-R instruments can be found at http://www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/ncs. Send correspondence to ncs@hcp.med.harvard.edu.

The NCS-R is carried out in conjunction with the World Health Organization World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative. We thank the staff of the WMH Data Collection and Data Analysis Coordination Centres for assistance with instrumentation, fieldwork, and consultation on data analysis. These activities were supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH070884), the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation, the Pfizer Foundation, the US Public Health Service (R13-MH066849, R01-MH069864, and R01 DA016558), the Fogarty International Center (FIRCA R03-TW006481), the Pan American Health Organization, Eli Lilly and Company, Ortho-McNeil Pharmaceutical, Inc., GlaxoSmithKline, and Bristol-Myers Squibb. A complete list of WMH publications can be found at http://www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/wmh/.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Alison PD. Survival analysis of backward recurrence times. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1984;80:315–322. [Google Scholar]
  2. Braveman PA, Cubbin C, Egerter S, Chideya S, Marchi KS, Metzler M, et al. Socioeconomic status in health research: one size does not fit all. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2005;294:2879–2888. doi: 10.1001/jama.294.22.2879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Chen E, Matthews K. Childhood socioeconomic status and adult health. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1186:37–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05334.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Corrigan PW, Watson AC. The stigma of psychiatric disorders and the gender, ethnicity, and education of the perceiver. Community Mental Health Journal. 2007;43:439–458. doi: 10.1007/s10597-007-9084-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Courtney ME, Piliavin I, Grogan-Kaylor A, Nesmith A. Foster youth transitions to adulthood: A longitudinal view of youth leaving care. Madison, WI: Institute for Research on Poverty; 1998. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Davey Smith G, Blane D, Hawthorne V. Lifetime socioeconomic position and mortality: Prospective observational study. British Medical Journal. 1997;314:547–552. doi: 10.1136/bmj.314.7080.547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Endicott J, Andreasen N, Spitzer RL. Family history research diagnostic criteria. New York, NY: Biometrics Research, NY State Psychiatric Institute; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  8. Fan AP, Eaton WW. Longitudinal study assessing the joint effects of socioeconomic status and birth risks on adult emotional and nervous conditions. British Journal of Psychiatry Supplement. 2001;40:s78–83. doi: 10.1192/bjp.178.40.s78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Galobardes B, Lynch JW, Davey Smith G. Childhood socioeconomic circumstances and cause-specific mortality in adulthood: Systematic review and interpretation. Epidemiologic Reviews. 2004;26:7–21. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxh008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Galobardes B, Shaw M, Lawlor DA, Lynch JW, Davey Smith G. Indicators of socioeconomic position (part 1) Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health. 2006;60:7–12. doi: 10.1136/jech.2004.023531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gilman SE, Kawachi I, Fitzmaurice GM, Buka L. Socio-economic status, family disruption and residential stability in childhood: Relation to onset, recurrence and remission of major depression. Psychological Medicine. 2003;33:1341–1355. doi: 10.1017/s0033291703008377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gilman SE, Kawachi I, Fitzmaurice GM, Buka SL. Socioeconomic status in childhood and the lifetime risk of major depression. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2002;31:359–367. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Goodman E, Slap GB, Huang B. The public health impact of socioeconomic status on adolescent depression and obesity. American Journal of Public Health. 2003;93:1844–1850. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.11.1844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Green JG, McLaughlin KA, Berglund P, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, et al. Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) I: Associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;62:113–123. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hussmanns R, Mehran F, Verma V. Surveys of economically active population, employment, unemployment and underemployment: An ILO manual on concepts and methods. Geneva: International Labour Office; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  16. Huurre T, Aro H, Rahkonen O. Well-being and health behaviour by parent socioeconomic status: A follow-up study of adolescents aged 16 until age 32 years. Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2003;38:249–255. doi: 10.1007/s00127-003-0630-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Johnson JG, Cohen P, Dohrenwend BP, Link BG, Brook JS. A longitudinal investigation of social causation and social selection processes involved in the association between socioeconomic status and psychiatric disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1999;108:490–499. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.108.3.490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kendler KS, Silberg JL, Neale MC, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ. The family history method: Whose psychiatric history is measured? American Journal of Psychiatry. 1991;148:1501–1504. doi: 10.1176/ajp.148.11.1501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kessler RC. A disaggregation of the relationship between socioeconomic status and psychiatric disorders. American Sociological Review. 1982;47:752–764. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Chiu WT, Demler O, Heeringa S, Hiripi E, et al. The US National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R): Design and field procedures. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2004;13:69–92. doi: 10.1002/mpr.167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62:593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kessler RC, Davis CG, Kendler KS. Childhood adversity and adult psychiatric disorder in the US National Comorbidity Survey. Psychological Medicine. 1997;27:1101–1119. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797005588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kessler RC, Merikangas KR. The National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R): Background and aims. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2004;13:60–68. doi: 10.1002/mpr.166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kessler RC, Üstun TB. The World Mental Health (WHM) Survey Initiative Version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2004;13:93–121. doi: 10.1002/mpr.168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kim-Cohen J, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Harrington H, Milne BJ, Poulton R. Prior juvenile diagnoses in adults with mental disorder: Developmental follow–back of a prospective-longitudinal cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:709–717. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.7.709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Knauper B, Cannell CF, Schwarz N, Bruce ML, Kessler RC. Improving the accuracy of major depression age of onset reports in the US National Comorbidity Survey. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 1999;8:39–48. [Google Scholar]
  27. Krieger N, Williams DR, Moss NE. Measuring social class in US public health research: concepts, methodologies, and guidelines. Annual Review of Public Health. 1997;18:341–378. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.18.1.341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lahelma E, Laaksonen M, Martikainen P, Rahkonen O, Sarlio-Lähteenkorva S. Multiple measures of socioeconomic circumstance and common mental disorders. Social Science & Medicine. 2006;63:1383–1399. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.03.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Leon AC, Olfson M, Portera L, Farber L, Sheehan DV. Assessment of psychiatric impairment in primary care with the Sheehan Disability Scale. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine. 1997;27:93–105. doi: 10.2190/T8EM-C8YH-373N-1UWD. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lundberg O. The impact of childhood living conditions on illness and mortality in adulthood. Social Science & Medicine. 1993;36:1047–1052. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(93)90122-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lynch J, Kaplan C. Socioeconomic position. In: Kawachi I, Berkman LF, editors. Social epidemiology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2000. pp. 13–35. [Google Scholar]
  32. Mäkinen T, Laaksonen M, Lahelma E, Rahkonen O. Associations of childhood circumstances with physical and mental functioning in adulthood. Social Science & Medicine. 2006;62:1831–1839. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.08.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Marmot M, Shipley M, Brunner E, Hemingway H. Relative contribution of early life and adult socioeconomic factors to adult morbidity in the Whitehall II study. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health. 2001;55:301–307. doi: 10.1136/jech.55.5.301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber M, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky A, Kessler RC. Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) III: Associations with functional impairment related to DSM-IV disorders. Psychological Medicine. 2010a;40:847–859. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709991115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber M, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky A, Kessler RC. Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R): II. Associations with persistence of DSM-IV disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010b;62:124–132. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. McLeod JD, Shanahan MJ. Trajectories of poverty and children’s mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1996;37:207–220. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Miech RA, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Wright BRE, Silva PA. Low socioeconomic status and mental disorders: A longitudinal study of selection and causation during young adulthood. American Journal of Sociology. 1999;104:1096–1131. [Google Scholar]
  38. Nock MK, Kazdin AE, Hiripi E, Kessler RC. Lifetime prevalence, correlates, and persistence of oppositional defiant disorder: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry. 2007;48:703–713. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01733.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Power C, Atherton K, Strachan DP, Shepherd P, Fuller E, Davis A, et al. Life-course influences on health in British adults: Effects of socio-economic position in childhood and adulthood. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2007;36:532–539. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyl310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Power C, Hertzman C, Matthews S, Manor O. Social differences in health: life-cycle effects between ages 23 and 33 in the 1959 British birth cohort. American Journal of Public Health. 1997;87:1499–1503. doi: 10.2105/ajph.87.9.1499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Power C, Manor O. Explaining social class differences in psychological health among young adults: a longitudinal perspective. Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology. 1992;27:284–291. doi: 10.1007/BF00788900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Research Triangle Institute. SUDAAN: Professional software for survey data analysis [computer program] Research Triangle Park, N.C: Research Triangle Institute; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  43. Ritsher JEB, Warner V, Johnson JG, Dohrenwend BP. Inter-generational longitudinal study of social class and depression: A test of social causation and social selection models. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;178(suppl 40):s84–s90. doi: 10.1192/bjp.178.40.s84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Sadowski H, Ugarte B, Kolvin I, Kaplan C, Barnes J. Early life family disadvantages and major depression in adulthood. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;174:112–120. doi: 10.1192/bjp.174.2.112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Simon GE, von Korff M. Recall of psychiatric history in cross-sectional surveys: Implications for epidemiologic research. Epidemiologic Reviews. 1995;17:221–227. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.epirev.a036180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Singer JD, Willett JB. It’s about time: using discrete-time survival analysis to study duration and timing of events. Journal of Educational Statistics. 1993;18:155–195. [Google Scholar]
  47. Stansfeld SA, Clark C, Rodgers B, Caldwell T, Power C. Childhood and adulthood socio-economic position and midlife depressive and anxiety disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;192:152–153. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.043208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Straus MA. Measuring intrafamily conflict and violence: The Conflict Tactics (CT) Scales. Journal of Marriage & the Family. 1979;41:75–88. [Google Scholar]
  49. Tracy M, Zimmerman FJ, Galea S, McCauley E, Stoep AV. What explains the relation between family poverty and childhood depressive symptoms? Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2008;42:1163–1175. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Turner HA, Finkelhor D, Ormrod R. The effect of lifetime victimization on the mental health of children and adolescents. Social Science & Medicine. 2006;62:13–27. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.05.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Veijola J, Puukka P, Lehtinen V, Moring J, Lindholm T, Vaisanen E. Sex differences in the association between childhood experiences and adult depression. Psychological Medicine. 1998;28:21–27. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797006089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Wilkinson RG. Unhealthy societies: The affliction of inequality. London: Routledge; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  53. Wilkinson RG, Pickett KE. The problems of relative deprivation: Why some societies do better than others. Social Science & Medicine. 2007;65:1965–1978. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.05.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Yamaguchi K. Accelerated failure-time mover-stayer regression models for the analysis of last episode data. Sociological Methodology. 2003;33:81–110. [Google Scholar]
  55. Yonkers KA, Bruce SE, Dyck IR, Keller MB. Chronicity, relapse, and illness-course of panic disorder, social phobia, and generalized anxiety disorder: Findings in men and women from 8 years of follow-up. Depression & Anxiety. 2003;17:173–179. doi: 10.1002/da.10106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES