Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Oct 13.
Published in final edited form as: Am J Community Psychol. 2008 Dec;42(3-4):266–282. doi: 10.1007/s10464-008-9207-7

Predicting Different Patterns of Sexual Identity Development Over Time Among Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Youths: A Cluster Analytic Approach

Margaret Rosario 1, Eric W Schrimshaw 2, Joyce Hunter 3
PMCID: PMC3192490  NIHMSID: NIHMS326598  PMID: 18972204

Abstract

Recent research has suggested that the sexual identity development of lesbian, gay, and bisexual (LGB) youths may not follow a single pattern, but may follow variety of pathways. Although some research documenting variability in identity development exists, unclear are the potential individual and social contexts that predict these different patterns, as well as the contexts that predict changes in identity integration over time. This report longitudinally examined these issues in an ethnically diverse sample of 156 LGB youths (ages 14–21) in New York City. Cluster analytic techniques identified two patterns of sexual identity formation (i.e., early and more recent), and three patterns of sexual identity integration (i.e., high, middling, and low). Gender, age, and sexual abuse were found to predict identity formation, and gay-related stress, social support, negative social relationships, sexual orientation, sexual identity, and gender were found to predict patterns of sexual identity integration and changes in integration over one year. These findings document different patterns of sexual identity development and identify potential contextual barriers and facilitating factors that may be used to develop interventions to promote healthy LGB identity development.

Keywords: coming-out process, psychosexual development, self-disclosure, internalized homophobia, gay-related stress, social support

The sexual identity development of lesbian, gay, and bisexual (LGB) individuals, often referred to as the “coming-out process,” has become the focus of a growing theoretical and empirical literature. Although early models of sexual identity development hypothesized a series of linear developmental stages (see Eliason, 1996, for review), more recent work has documented various departures that call into question the hierarchical nature of linear stage models. As such, some researchers have called for examination of individual variability in the sexual identity development of LGB youths (e.g., Savin-Williams, 2001; Schneider, 2001). The current report examines potential variability in the sexual identity development of LGB youths over the course of one year. Further, this report examines the potential individual and social contexts that may explain these different patterns of sexual identity development. Thus, the report has potential implications for advancing our theoretical understanding of the sexual identity development of LGB youths and it provides evidence on factors that may impede or promote healthy LGB identity development.

Models of Sexual Identity Development

Despite decades of theoretical work, the proliferation of models describing the sexual identity development of LGB individuals continues (e.g., Cass, 1979; Chapman & Brannock, 1987; Fassinger & Miller, 1996; Minton & McDonald, 1984; Meyer & Schwitzer, 1999; Troiden, 1989). These models propose that LGB individuals progress through a linear series of stages starting with first awareness of same-sex attractions through an eventual acceptance, disclosure, and integration of that identity. Nevertheless, such linear stage models have been critiqued for failing to recognize the diversity of the coming-out process (e.g., Horowitz & Newcomb, 2001; Kitzinger & Wilkinson, 1995; Sophie, 1985/86). Indeed, the proliferation of models attests to the diversity of experiences. For example, some models propose that sexual activity occurs prior to disclosure of sexual identity to others (e.g., Chapman & Brannock, 1987; Fassinger & Miller, 1996; Minton & McDonald, 1984; Rotheram-Borus & Fernandez, 1995) and other models stipulate that sexual activity follows disclosure (e.g., Meyer & Schwitzer, 1999; Morris, 1997). Such theoretical discrepancies suggest the likelihood of multiple pathways for the sexual identity development of LGB individuals.

In this report, we conceptualize sexual identity development as consisting of identity formation and identity integration (Morris, 1997; Rosario, Schrimshaw & Hunter, 2004; Rosario, Schrimshaw, Hunter, & Braun, 2006). The former involves the initiation of a process of self-discovery and exploration. It includes becoming aware of one’s emerging sexual orientation, questioning whether one may be LGB, and engaging in sexual relations with the same sex (Cass, 1979; Chapman & Brannock, 1987; Morris, 1997; Troiden, 1989). Identity integration entails an acceptance and commitment to one’s sexual identity. Specifically, identity integration involves engaging in LGB-related social activities, transforming negative attitudes toward homosexuality into positive attitudes, feeling comfortable with other individuals potentially knowing about one’s identity, and disclosing one’s LGB identity to others (Morris, 1997; Rosario, Hunter, Maguen, Gwadz, & Smith., 2001). Beyond defining identity formation and integration, we hypothesize that they are related, such that identity formation is associated with greater integration. However, we do not specify an order as to when development occurs within identity formation or integration because the patterns of sexual identity development are likely to be diverse.

Variability in Sexual Identity Development

Some researchers have called for a focus on the diversity in the coming-out process (e.g., Diamond, 2003; Schneider, 2001). Savin-Williams (2001) has argued for the examination of “differential developmental trajectories,” suggesting that the coming-out process is not an identical process for all, but rather that LGB individuals experience a diversity of developmental pathways. He has suggested the need to examine within-group differences in the coming-out process, particularly by gender, ethnicity, and age cohort. Indeed, a growing literature has examined differences in the ages at initiating various milestones of identity development, particularly by gender and, to a lesser extent, by ethnicity and age (D’Augelli & Hershberger, 1993; Dubé & Savin-Williams, 1999; Floyd & Bakeman, 2006; Grov, Bimbi, Nanín, & Parsons, 2006; Maguen, Floyd, Bakeman, & Armistead, 2002; Rosario et al., 1996; 2004). However, even these examinations of variability are limited to comparisons or differences between sociodemographic classifications.

More consistent with the notion of variability in development are studies that have examined different patterns or pathways through the coming-out process (e.g., Dubé, 2000; Floyd & Stein, 2002; Maguen et al., 2002; Kahn, 1991; Rust, 1993; Schindhelm & Hospers, 2004; Savin-Williams & Diamond, 2000). For example, studies have documented variability in the order of various psychosexual milestones, such that some LGB individuals report their first same-sex sexual experience prior to disclosing their sexual identity, but others report disclosing their identity prior to any sexual contact (Dubé, 2000; Maguen et al., 2002; Savin-Williams & Diamond, 2000; Schindhelm & Hospers, 2004). Even greater variability has been observed by Floyd and Stein (2002), who identified five empirically-derived patterns of development among LGB youths, including youths who progressed through the coming-out process at a younger age, others who progressed though the process at a later age, those who started at a later age but quickly progressed through developmental milestones, and those who started at an early age but who were then slow to progress to disclosure to others. Despite the findings, these studies of the coming-out process are limited by their use of retrospective data. Retrospective reports are prone to distortions based on the individual’s current condition (Henry, Moffitt, Caspi, Langley, & Silva, 1994; Ross, 1989). Researchers have argued that LGB sexual identity development must be studied prospectively (Boxer & Cohler, 1989; D’Augelli, 1994).

Few prospective longitudinal studies have examined the coming-out process. The available studies have documented considerable consistency, as well as change, over time in sexual identity, attractions, and behaviors (Diamond, 2000; 2005; Dickson, Paul, & Herbison, 2003; Rosario et al., 2006; Stokes, Damon, & McKirnan, 1997; Stokes, McKirnan, & Burzette, 1993). However, potential changes in aspects of identity integration over time and potential variability in these changes remain unstudied. One expects considerable variability in the patterns of identity integration, given variability in patterns of identity formation (e.g., Floyd & Stein, 2002). For example, some LGB youths might report high levels on all aspects of identity integration (e.g., positive attitudes, high comfort, high disclosure), others might be less self-accepting, and still others might be high on some aspects (e.g., positive attitudes toward their sexuality) but low on other aspects (e.g., reluctant to disclose their identity).

Explanation of Variability in Sexual Identity Development

As important as it is to document the various patterns of identity formation and integration, it is equally important to understand the potential reasons for this variability. In addition, potential predictors of changes in identity formation and integration may be critical for developing programs and services both to reduce barriers that impede healthy identity development and to promote conditions that facilitate it. Nevertheless, understanding the potential individual and social contexts that predict different patterns of identity formation and integration remains under-examined.

We hypothesize that different patterns of identity formation and integration may vary in logical ways. Specifically, we hypothesize that gender, age, and sexual abuse are related to identity formation. The limited empirical literature indicates that young men are more likely than young women to follow a trajectory were they engaged in same-sex behavior prior to identifying as gay (Savin-Williams & Diamond, 2000). Older LGB individuals have been found to follow an identity formation pattern that differs from those of younger ages, even among youths (Floyd & Stein, 2002). Further, it has been found that experiences of childhood sexual abuse may lead to earlier sexual identity formation among LGB individuals (Matthews, Hughes, Tartaro, 2006; Morris & Balsam, 2003; Robohm, Litzenberger, & Pearlman, 2003), and may alter the pattern of identity formation (Bartholow et al., 1994). Given that sexual abuse is 2 to 16 times more prevalent among LGB youths (see Saewyc, 2006, for a review), this may be a critical factor for many LGB youths.

We also hypothesize that a number of individual and social context factors are related to different patterns of identity integration, with some factors (i.e., negative social relationships) serving as barriers to identity integration and other factors (i.e., positive social relationships) as facilitators of identity integration. Although no studies were identified which examined predictors of change in identity integration, several cross-sectional studies support our hypotheses. Potential negative relationships (e.g., negative social relationships, gay-related stressors) were found to be associated with lower levels of identity integration (D’Augelli, Hershberger, & Pilkington, 1998; Pilkington & D’Augelli, 1995), whereas positive relationships (e.g., supportive friends and family) were associated with greater integration (Elizur & Mintzer, 2001; Hershberger & D’Augelli, 1995; Jordan & Deluty, 1998; Wright & Perry, 2006). Differences in sexual identity (i.e., lesbian/gay vs. bisexual), sexual orientation (i.e., gender of their sexual attractions and fantasies), and gender of their sexual partners also may be associated with greater identity integration (Morris, Waldo, & Rothblum, 2001). Furthermore, potential differences by gender and ethnicity/race may exist, given the hypothesis in the literature that girls are more variable (i.e., “fluid”) in their development (e.g., Peplau, 2003), and ethnic/racial minority youths may experience lower integration, given greater contextual barriers (e.g., Dubé & Savin-Williams, 1999; Rosario et al., 2004).

The current report examined diversity in sexual identity development among LGB youths by retrospectively investigating potential patterns of identity formation and prospectively investigating over one year potential patterns of identity integration. We did not impose an arbitrary a priori classification scheme on the data, but allowed the data to dictate the naturally occurring patterns of identity formation and integration. Rather than focusing just on changes in identity (e.g., Diamond, 2000; Rosario et al., 2006) or just on identity formation (Maguen et al., 2002; Savin-Williams & Diamond, 2000), which has already attracted substantial attention, the current report is the first to examine changes in identity integration over time. Finally, the current report goes beyond describing potential developmental patterns to examining hypothesized individual and social contexts that may serve as barriers and facilitators of the patterns in identity formation and integration.

Method

Participants

One-hundred and sixty-four youths, ages 14 to 21 years, were recruited from three LGB-focused community-based organizations (CBOs, 85%) and two LGB college student organizations (15%) in New York City. Eight youths were excluded because they did not meet eligibility criteria,1 resulting in a final sample of 156 youths (49% female), mean age of 18.3 years (SD = 1.65). The youths identified as lesbian or gay (66%), bisexual (31%), or other (3%). They were Latino (37%), Black (35%), White (22%), or Asian and other ethnic backgrounds (7%). Of the youths, 34% reported having a parent who received welfare, food stamps, or Medicaid; these youths were classified as low socioeconomic status (SES) and the remainder as high SES.

Procedure

Youths provided voluntary and signed informed consent. The Commissioner of Mental Health for New York State waived parental consent for youths under age 18. Instead, an adult at each CBO served in loco parentis to safeguard the rights of every minor in the study. The university’s Institutional Review Board and recruitment sites approved the study.

A 2- to 3-hour structured interview was conducted at recruitment (Time 1) with follow-up interviews occurring 6 and 12 months later. Youths were interviewed between October 1993 and June 1994, with follow-up interviews conducted through August 1995. The retention rates were 92% (n = 143) for the 6-month assessment and 90% (n = 140) for the 12-month assessment. Youths received $30 at each interview.

Measures of Sexual Identity Formation

All sex-related variables were assessed with the Sexual Risk Behavior Assessment – Youth (SERBAS-Y) for LGB youths (Meyer-Bahlburg, Ehrhardt, Exner, & Gruen, 1994). The SERBAS-Y has demonstrated moderate to high test-retest reliability over two weeks among our sample (Schrimshaw, Rosario, Meyer-Bahlburg, & Scharf-Matlick, 2006).

For sexual identity formation, the SERBAS-Y assessed the ages when youths experienced each of six psychosexual developmental milestones. Youths were asked the ages when they were first 1) erotically attracted to the same sex, 2) fantasized about the same sex, and 3) were aroused by erotica focusing on the same sex. A factor analysis of the ages at which each of these three milestones were achieved revealed a single factor (Cronbach’s α = .88), suggesting that the milestones occurred at approximately the same age. As such, the mean of these three milestones was computed to create a single composite milestone indicating the mean age of first awareness of same-sex sexual orientation. The subsequent three milestones were retained as single items. Specifically, youths were asked about 1) the age when they first thought they “might be” lesbian/gay or bisexual and 2) when they first thought they “really were” lesbian/gay or bisexual. Finally, they were asked about 3) the age when they first experienced any of several sexual activities with the same sex. In toto, we had four identity formation milestones for analysis. Although nearly all youths (91%) had experienced all four milestones, there were some who had not (i.e., 8% had not yet had any sexual activity with the same sex, 1 [0.6%] did not believe he or she “really was” LGB). Data were coded as missing for youths who had not experienced a particular milestone.

We agree with others (e.g., Diamond & Savin-Williams, 2000) who have critiqued sexual milestone research suggesting that the ages, per se, do not tell us much about the coming-out process, as individuals may first experience these milestones at any time in life. However, because the coming-out process necessarily takes time to work through and integrate, we have argued that more important than age when milestones occur is the length of time between first experiencing these milestones and the present (Rosario et al., 2006). Two young women who first experienced attractions to women at age 15 may differ, if one is currently 16 (one year later) and the other is 21 (six years later). Thus, for all of the developmental milestones, we computed the number of years since the youth first experienced the various milestones by subtracting the age at each milestone from the youth’s age at Time 1.

Measures of Sexual Identity Integration

Involvement in LGB-Related Social Activities

We developed a 28-item checklist to assess lifetime involvement in gay-related social and recreational activities at Time 1 (Rosario et al., 2001). At follow-up assessments, youths were asked about their activity involvement during the past 6 months (i.e., since their last assessment). Factor analysis of the Time 1 data generated 11 items that loaded on one factor (e.g., going to a gay bookstore, coffee house). We computed how many of the 11 items were endorsed by the youths (Cronbach’s α = .64 to .77 across the three assessments).

Positive Attitudes Toward Homosexuality/Bisexuality

We administered a modified version of the Nungesser Homosexual Attitudes Inventory (NHAI; Nungesser, 1983) at all assessments, using a 4-point response scale ranging from “disagree strongly” (1) through “agree strongly” (4). A factor analysis of the Time 1 data resulted in two factors. The first factor, composed of 11 items, assessed attitudes toward homosexuality [e.g., “My (homosexuality/bisexuality) does not make me unhappy”]. We computed the mean of these items at each assessment, with high scores indicating more positive attitudes toward homosexuality (Cronbach’s α = .83 to .85 across the three assessments). Because these data were negatively skewed at all assessments, we transformed the data using the exponential e to stretch the positive end of the distribution.

Comfort with Others Knowing about Your Homosexuality/Bisexuality

As noted above, a factor analysis of the Time 1 data from the NHAI (Nungesser, 1983) identified two factors. The second factor, composed of 12 items, assessed comfort with other individuals potentially knowing about the youth’s sexuality [e.g., “If my straight friends knew of my (homosexuality/bisexuality), I would feel uncomfortable”]. We computed the mean of these items at each time period, with a high score indicating more comfort with homosexuality (Cronbach’s α = .89 to .91 across the assessments).

Disclosure of Homosexuality/Bisexuality to Others

We asked youths at Time 1 to enumerate “all the people in your life who are important or were important to you and whom you told that you are (lesbian/gay/bisexual)” (Rosario et al., 2001). Subsequently, youths were asked about the number of new individuals to whom the youth had disclosed during the past six months (i.e., since their last assessment). Because disclosure cannot be undone and because it is carried into the future, the indicator of disclosure is cumulative over time. Therefore, we summed the disclosure data at Time 1 with new disclosures at the 6- and 12-months assessments as our self-disclosure indicator at Time 2. We imposed a logarithmic transformation on the disclosure at Time 2 because these data were positively skewed.

Measures of Hypothesized Individual and Social Context Factors

Current Sexual Orientation

The SERBAS-Y (Meyer-Bahlburg et al., 1994) assessed current sexual orientation at every assessment. Youths were asked to indicate the extent to which their sexual attractions, thoughts, and fantasies focused on the same or other sex: (1) when in the presence of other individuals in a public setting, (2) when masturbating, dreaming, or day dreaming, and (3) when viewing erotic material in films, magazines, or books. A 7-point, Kinsey-type response scale was used ranging from always focused on the other sex (0) to always focused on the same sex (6). Youths were allowed to indicate whether they had not experienced the assessed event. We computed current sexual orientation as the mean of the three items (Cronbach’s α = .91 to .92 across the assessments).

Sexual Identity

An item from the SERBAS-Y (Meyer-Bahlburg et al., 1994) at every assessment inquired, “When you think about sex, do you think of yourself as lesbian/gay, bisexual, or straight?” Youths who rejected these identities were coded as “other.”

Sexual Behaviors

The SERBAS-Y (Meyer-Bahlburg et al., 1994) was used to assess number of lifetime same-sex partners at Time 1. These data were logarithmically transformed because they were positively skewed. The SERBAS-Y also assessed prevalence of recent sex (in the 3 months prior to Time 1) with the same sex.

Childhood Sexual Abuse

A single item from the SERBAS-Y (Meyer-Bahlburg et al., 1994) assessed at Time 1 whether the youth had ever, before age 13 years, had sexual activity with an adult or with another youth more than five years older than him or her. Sexual activity was defined as oral, vaginal or anal sex, touching or being touched on the breasts or genitals, exposing oneself or someone exposing themselves, or someone rubbing their body against them in a sexual way.

Social Support from Family and Friends

Procidano and Heller’s (1983) measures of perceived social support from family and from friends were adapted, deleting items that might be confounded with psychological health. The two resulting 12-item measures, administered at Time 1 using a yes (1) or no (0) response format, assessed the extent to which needs for support, information, and feedback were met by family and by friends (e.g., “I rely on my [family/friends] for emotional support”). A count of the items endorsed was the index of social support from family (Cronbach’s α = .90) and friends (Cronbach’s α = .80). The measure was originally validated on late adolescents (Procidano & Heller, 1983).

Negative Social Relationships

The 12-item Social Obstruction Scale (Gurley, 1990) was administered at Time 1 to assess the presence of negative social relationships with others, including being treated poorly, being ignored, and being manipulated by others (e.g., “Somebody treats me as if I were nobody”). Items use a response scale ranging from “definitely false” (1) to “definitely true” (4). The responses were internally consistent among Gurley’s (1990) heterosexual youths and our LGB youths (Cronbach’s α = .85). The mean score was computed, with higher scores indicating greater levels of negative social relationships.

Gay-Related Stressful Life Events

A 12-item checklist of stressful events related to homosexuality was administered at Time 1 (e.g., “Losing a close friend because of your [homosexuality/bisexuality]”; Rosario et al., 2002). The youths indicated whether they had experienced any of the events within the past 3 months. The number of events experienced was computed. Because the responses were skewed, we computed a response scale of zero (0), or one or more (1) stressful events.

Measures of Potential Covariates

In addition to sex, age, SES, ethnicity/race, and recruitment site (all assessed at Time 1), we assessed the tendency to provide socially desirable responses at Time 1 by means of the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability scale (Crowne & Marlowe, 1964). We used its original true-false response format, but deleted 2 of 31 items we considered inappropriate for youths. A factor analysis generated 12 items that loaded on a single factor. The number of these items endorsed was computed as our measure of social desirability (Cronbach’s α = .74). A similarly reduced version of this measure has been previously used with LGB youths (Safren & Heimberg, 1999).

Data Analysis

We used cluster analysis to identify subgroups of LGB youths with respect to sexual identity formation and integration. Cluster analysis is an iterative, two-stage procedure to determine whether groups exist based on inherent pattern of associations among the variables of interest (e.g., Everitt, Landau, & Leese, 2001; Henry, Tolan, & Gorman-Smith, 2005; Rapkin & Luke, 1993). For the first stage, we used hierarchical clustering, a procedure that utilizes Euclidian distances among cases on the standardized variables of interest, to determine the number of groups or clusters by means of dendograms or trees showing how the individuals group together. In this case, several large unambiguous groups were identified for both identity formation and identity integration. No outliers were noted. In the second stage, we aimed to validate and interpret (or define) the profiles of the original cluster solution by the K-means cluster analytic procedure. We specified the number of clusters to be identified (based on the hierarchical cluster results) and the iterative analysis attempted to fit the cases on the standardized variables to those numbers of clusters. Comparison of the hierarchical cluster findings and the K-means cluster findings (on size of each cluster, individual membership in each cluster) were highly similar, suggesting a validity of the findings. We then interpreted the clusters that were revealed and investigated the relations among them. This procedure was followed for identity formation and identity integration clusters at Time 1. To identify if identity integration cluster membership was consistent or changed over time, we examined identity integration at the 12-month assessment (here designated as Time 2) using a similar K-means procedure. However, to ensure the clusters were comparable (and thus allowing us to assess change between clusters), we used the cluster centers (i.e., means) identified at Time 1 to assign youths to equivalent clusters based on their Time 2 data.

We examined whether differences existed among our clusters by our hypothesized correlates and potential covariates, using ANOVA for continuous variables and chi-square for categorical variables. We then conducted logistic regression analyses, in which all significant variables at the previous, bivariate level of analysis were examined simultaneously in order to assess which of the hypothesized correlates uniquely provided significant information about our clusters or groups of youths. In order to protect against Type I error, only those logistic regression models with a significant omnibus chi-square were interpreted; this procedure is analogous to Fisher’s protected t-test in ANOVA. Next, we examined whether each group changed over time (group-level change) using repeated measures ANOVA. Then, we examined individual-level change in identity integration group membership. For this level of analysis we tracked whether individual youths changed in identity integration from recruitment (Time 1) to the 12-month assessment (Time 2), determining whether they remained in the same cluster or changed cluster membership (e.g., changed to a higher or lower level of integration) over the year. We examined change between the Time 1 and 12-month assessment (leaving out the 6-month assessment) in all of our analyses in order to maximize variability. Finally, we examined the correlates of these individual-level changes at both the bivariate (using ANOVA and chi-square) and multivariate levels (using multiple logistic regression). As noted before, only logistic regression models with significant omnibus chi-square tests were interpreted. For all these analyses, we present effect sizes in the form of the proportion of explained variance, including η2 for ANOVA and its equivalent τ for chi-square analysis (Goodman & Kruskal, 1979), and odds ratios (OR) for the results from logistic regression analyses.

Results

Patterns of LGB Youth Identity Development

To identify potential patterns of LGB identity formation and patterns of identity integration (at Time 1 and Time 2), three separate cluster analyses were conducted. First, the length of time since first experiencing each of four identity formation milestones (i.e., time since first same-sex attraction, first same-sex behavior, first thought might be LGB, and first thought really were LGB) were entered into a hierarchical cluster analysis. Two identity formation clusters were identified, and subsequently re-examined using K-means cluster analysis. The clusters for identity formation denoted a group of youths who, relatively speaking, recently formed their LGB identity (67%) as compared with a smaller group of youths who underwent this process at an earlier time (33%). Indeed, the two groups of youths differed significantly on the time since experiencing each of the four psychosexual milestones examined (see Table I).

Table I.

Differences on Variables Composing the Groups of Identity Formation and Integration at Time 1 and Time 2.

Identity Formation Groups: Time 1
Test Statistic Effect Size Total Sample (n = 156)
Recent Development (n = 105) Early Development (n = 51)
Time 1: Years since first: M M F η2 M

 --Same-sex attractions, fantasies, and arousal 5.55 9.58 73.21*** .32 6.87
 --Thought might be LGB 4.64 9.73 95.25*** .38 6.30
 --Same-sex sexual activity 2.56 8.68 153.67*** .52 4.70
 --Thought really was LGB 2.70 5.73 63.66*** .29 3.70
Identity Integration Groups: Time 1
Test Statistic Effect Size Total Sample (n = 156)
High (n = 64) Middling (n = 59) Low (n = 33)
Time 1: M M M F η2 M

 --Involvement in LGB-related social activities 7.27 a 6.56 a 5.21 b 6.83*** .08 6.56
 --Positive attitudes toward homo/bisexuality 52.80 a 38.24 b 17.85 c 635.52*** .89 39.81
 --Comfort with others knowing about your homo/bisexuality 3.19 a 2.78 b 2.26 c 24.88*** .25 2.83
 --Disclosure to others 8.72 a 5.42 b 5.06 b 10.50*** .12 6.70
Identity Integration Groups: Time 2
Test Statistic Effect Size Total Sample (n = 140)
High (n = 57) Middling (n = 55) Low (n = 28)
Time 2: M M M F η2 M

 --Involvement in LGB-related social activities 5.33 5.69 4.64 1.24 .02 5.34
 --Positive attitudes toward homo/bisexuality 53.60 a 37.93 b 19.36 c 519.56*** .88 40.60
 --Comfort with others knowing about your homo/bisexuality 3.29 a 3.00 b 2.42 c 19.15*** .22 3.00
 --Disclosure to others 2.80 2.72 2.38 1.22 .02 2.69

Note. For identity integration groups, means with differing superscripts differed at p < .05. The means for attitudes toward homosexual/bisexuality and for disclosure at Time 2 are based on transformed data due to skewness. Thus, the values for disclosure at Time 1 and Time 2 are not comparable.

***

p < .001.

In order to identify potential patterns of LGB identity integration, we cluster analyzed the four indicators of identity integration (i.e., involvement in LGB-related social activities, positive attitudes toward homosexuality/bisexuality, comfort with others knowing about his/her homosexuality/bisexuality, and disclosure of sexuality to others). The cluster analyses of the Time 1 data suggested the existence of three groups of youths that were low, middling, or high on LGB identity integration (see the means in Table I). At Time 1, all four markers of identity integration significantly differed among the groups. Post-hoc tests between the three groups found that while the high integration and low integration groups differed, the middling group differed from both the low and high groups on attitudes toward homosexuality/bisexuality and comfort with others knowing. However, the middling group was similar to the high integration group on involvement in LGB social activities, but more similar to the low integration group on self-disclosure.

Another cluster analysis was conducted of the identity integration data at Time 2. The cluster analysis of the Time 2 integration data assigned youths to identity integration clusters based on the cluster centroids (i.e., means) obtained from the Time 1 cluster analysis. This provides information on whether individual youths changed identity integration group membership between Time 1 and Time 2. Unlike Time 1, the three identity integration groups differed significantly on attitudes toward homosexuality and comfort with homosexuality (see Table I), but did not differ in involvement in LGB activities or disclosure to others.

Given our hypothesis that identity formation and integration are related, we examined the associations between the groups of sexual identity formation and sexual identity integration. No significant associations were found between identity formation and identity integration either at Time 1 or Time 2, suggesting that those who began identity formation earlier were not more likely to report a greater level of identity integration. However, identity integration at Time 1 was associated with identity integration at Time 2, χ2 (4, N = 140) = 58.8, p < .001. Consistency over time was apparent: 64% of the low identity integration youths were consistent, 51% of the middling integration youths were consistent, and 68% of the high integration youths were consistent. Change over time also existed, with much of the change to an adjacent cluster (e.g., 20% of low integration youths transitioned to middling, but only 2% of low integration youths skipped to high).

As a further analysis of potential change, the three identity integration groups at Time 1 were examined to determine whether the groups had implications for subsequent development of identity integration. We used repeated measures ANOVA to examine whether the three identity integration groups exhibited change in the four components of identity integration between Time 1 and Time 2. Indeed, significant changes were found in 8 of 12 analyses (see Figure 1). Groups differed on whether or not they exhibited significant change. Overall, the pattern indicates that each of the three groups become more similar over the one-year period examined, such that the high integration group decreased on 3 of 4 aspects of identity integration and the low integration group increased on 2 of 4 aspects of identity integration.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Prospective changes in each of the four aspects of identity integration over one year for each of the three identity integration groups (from Time 1). * indicates that the change was significant at p < .05 for that identity group.

Correlates of Identity Development Among Groups of LGB Youths

Bivariate analyses

We examined potential predictors of sexual identity formation group membership (see Table II). The LGB youths who underwent identity formation earlier were more than twice as likely to have been sexually abused (OR = 2.3, 95% CI = 1.1 – 4.8). Further, early developing youths were older and less likely to be recruited from the college organizations than the more recent developing youths. Gender, social desirability, ethnicity/race, and SES were not significantly associated with identity formation groups.

Table II.

Bivariate Predictors of Group Membership at Time 1 and Time 2.

Identity Formation Groups: Time 1
Test Statistic Effect Size
Recent Development (n = 105) Early Development (n = 51)
Time 1: M M F η2

 --Age 17.9 19.0 16.35*** .10

% % χ2 τ

 --Sexual abuse 56% 75% 4.90* .03
 --Recruitment site (colleges) 24% 11% 4.65* .03
Identity Integration Groups: Time 1
Test Statistic Effect Size
High (n = 64) Middling (n = 59) Low (n = 33)
Time 1: M M M F η2

 --Current sexual orientation 5.4 a 5.0 4.6 b 5.01** .06
 --Social support: family 7.5 a 5.8 b 4.9 b 5.60** .07
 --Social support: friends 10.4 a 10.3a 9.3 b 2.90 .04
 --Negative social relationships 1.9 a 2.2 b 2.4 b 6.24** .08
 --Age 18.5 a 18.4 a 17.5 b 4.53* .06
 --Social desirability 6.8 a 5.4 b 5.5 b 4.48* .06

% % % χ2 τ

 --Gay-related stress 31% a 41% 58% b 6.27* .02
 --Identify as lesbian/gay v. bi 75% a 72% a 45% b 9.17** .02
 --Female sex 64% a 46% b 24% c 14.2*** .04
Identity Integration Groups: Time 2
Test Statistic Effect Size
High (n = 57) Middling (n = 55) Low (n = 28)
Time 1: M M M F η2

 --Current sexual orientation 5.3 a 5.2 a 4.3 b 7.78*** .10
 --# same-sex partners-lifetime 1.4 1.7 a 1.1 b 2.71 .04
 --Social support: family 6.7 a 6.8 a 4.3 b 4.02* .06
 --Social support: friends 10.1 10.7 a 9.2 b 3.71* .05
 --Negative social relationships 2.1 a 2.1 a 2.5 b 3.82* .05
 --Social desirability 6.7 a 5.5 b 5.6 3.17* .04

% % % χ2 τ

 --Identify as lesbian/gay v. bi 78% a 73% a 37% b 15.05*** .04
 --Female sex 58% a 49% a 25% b 8.20* .02
 --Ethnicity/race:
  Black 46% a 18% b 50% a 12.53* .05
  Latino 32% a 47% b 25% a
  White/Other 23% 35% 25%

Note. For identity integration groups, means or percents with differing superscripts differ at p < .05.

p < .06.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

Similarly, examination of the bivariate predictors of identity integration groups at Time 1 found nine factors significantly associated with identity integration group membership (see Table II). Follow-up pairwise comparisons indicated that youths high or low on integration always differed from each other, with the middling group either differing from one or the other group, or not differing significantly from either group. As compared with the low integration group, the high group of youths possessed a cognitive sexual orientation that was more centered on the same sex, perceived more support from family and friends, had fewer negative social relationships, was less likely to experience gay-related stress, was older, provided more socially desirable responses, was more likely to identify as gay/lesbian than bisexual, and was more likely to be female than male. Factors not significantly related to identity integration at Time 1 were lifetime number of sexual partners, prevalence of recent sex with the same sex, ethnicity/race, SES, and recruitment site.

For the identity integration groups at Time 2, several significant associations were identified (see Table II). Follow-up pairwise comparisons found the greatest number of differences between the high and low groups, with the middling group now more similar to the high group than to the low group. In addition, many correlates identified at Time 1 were still related to integration a year later. The high as compared with the low integration group had a cognitive sexual orientation that was more same-sex centered at Time 1, perceived more family support at Time 1, experienced fewer negative social relationships at Time 1, was more likely to identify as gay/lesbian than bisexual at Time 1, was more likely to be female than male, and differed on ethnicity/race. No significant differences were found among the groups on Time 1 prevalence of recent sex with the same sex, gay-related stress, age, SES, and recruitment site.

Multivariate analyses

To determine which of the significant bivariate associations would predict the groups of sexual identity formation and integration after controlling for all other significant bivariate associations, multivariate logistic regression analysis was performed. For sexual identity formation, although sexual abuse was only marginally significant in multivariate analyses (p = .065), the consistently high magnitude of the association, OR = 2.1, 95% CI = 0.96 – 4.5, and the fact that this effect hardly decreased from the bivariate analyses suggests that abuse remains an important predictor of subsequent of identity formation. Age was significantly associated with identity formation, OR = 1.5, 95% CI = 1.1 – 1.9, p = .01.

For the three identity-integration groups at Time 1 and Time 2, pairwise comparisons were made using logistic regression (see Table III). The overall model was found to distinguish (i.e., the omnibus chi-square was significant) between each of the identity integration groups at both Time 1 and Time 2. At Time 1, the high group as compared with the low group of youths had fewer negative social relationships, was less likely to experience gay-related stress, and was older and more likely to be female than male. In contrast, the high group as compared to the middling group of youths had a cognitive sexual orientation that was more same-sex centered, experienced more family support, and was more likely to be female.

Table III.

Multivariate Predictors of Group Comparisons at Time 1 and Time 2.

Identity Integration Group Comparisons: Time 1
High vs. Middling High vs. Low Middling vs. Low
Time 1: OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)

--Current sexual orientation 1.8* (1.1, 2.9) -- --
--Social support: family 1.1 (1.0, 1.3) -- --
--Social support: friends -- -- --
--Negative social relationships -- 0.4* (0.1, 1.0) --
--Gay-related stress -- 0.2* (0.1, 0.7) --
--Age -- 1.7* (1.1, 2.6) 1.4 (1.0, 1.9)
--Social desirability -- -- --
--Identify as lesbian/gay v. bi -- -- --
--Female sex 3.6** (1.5, 8.9) 14.4*** (3.2, 64.2) 3.2 (1.0, 10.3)
Identity Integration Group Comparisons: Time 2
High vs. Middling High vs. Low Middling vs. Low
Time 1: OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)

--Current sexual orientation -- -- 4.1** (1.6, 10.9)
--Social support: family -- 1.2* (1.0, 1.5) --
--Social support: friends 0.8 (0.7, 1.0) -- --
--Negative social relationships -- 0.4 (0.2, 1.1) 0.3* (0.1, 0.9)
--# same-sex partners-lifetime -- -- --
--Social desirability -- -- --
--Identify as lesbian/gay v. bi -- -- --
--Female sex -- 11.3** (2.4, 53.2) 13.9** (2.1, 91.2)
--Ethnicity/race: Black 3.6* (1.2, 11.0) -- 0.1* (0.0, 0.7)
--Ethnicity/race: Latino -- -- --

Note: The full model was found to distinguish (i.e., significant omnibus chi-square) between each of the identity integration groups. The odds ratio for individual variables that significantly distinguished between these groups are reported, and nonsignificant variables are marked with a dash.

p < .08.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

Comparisons of the identity integration groups at the Time 2 (see Table III) indicated that, as compared with the low group, the high group perceived more family support at Time 1, had fewer negative social relationships at Time 1, and was more likely to be female. The high as compared with the middling group reported marginally less friend support at Time 1 and was nearly four times more likely to be Black. The middling as compared with the low group reported a cognitive sexual orientation that was more same-sex focused at Time 1, reported fewer negative social relationships at Time 1, and its members were more likely to be female, but less likely to be Black.

Correlates of Individual-Level Change in Identity Integration

Bivariate analyses

As valuable as the previous information may be, those changes and their correlates are assessed at the level of the group, and thus do not provide any information on patterns or predictors specific to individual changes. Therefore, we examined if youths changed in their identity integration groups between Time 1 and Time 2 at the level of the individual. As indicated in Table IV, nearly two-thirds of youths were consistent over time, but the remainder changed. Given the small number of youths in some categories, we were only able to focus subsequent analyses on youths who were consistently high, increased from low or middling to high, decreased from high to middling, were consistently middling, and were consistently low.

Table IV.

Individual-level Consistency and Change in Identity Integration Groups Over One Year.

From Time 1 to Time 2: n %
Consistently High Integration 39 28%
Increased from Middling to High Integration 17 12%
Consistently Middling Integration 28 20%
Consistently Low Integration 18 13%
Decreased from High to Middling Integration 16 11%
Increased from Low to Middling Integration 11 8%
Decreased from Middling to Low Integration 8 6%
Increased from Low to High Integration 1 1%
Decreased from High to Low Integration 2 1%

Note. Percentages do not sum to 100% due to rounding error.

Table V contains the significant bivariate associations along with pairwise comparisons. Youths who were consistently high differed from those who were consistently low on many factors, including being more same-sex focused in their cognitive sexual orientation at Time 1, reporting more support from both family and friends at Time 1, having fewer negative social relationships at Time 1, experiencing less gay-related stress at Time 1, as well as being more likely to identify as lesbian/gay at Time 1, being more likely to be female, and providing more socially desirable responses at Time 1. Youths who decreased from high to middling and youths who were consistently middling were often similar to the consistently high integration group and significantly different from the consistently low group on these same variables. In contrast, youths who increased from low or middling to high integration often did not differ from the consistently high integration group, the consistently middling group, or the consistently low group. Consistently low integration youths were uniformly the lowest group in terms of family and friend support, and highest in negative relationships and experiences of gay-related stress. They also reported the lowest levels of same-sex sexual orientation, the fewest same-sex sexual partners, and they were least likely to identify as lesbian/gay or to be female. Prevalence of same-sex behavior and ethnicity/race were not associated with identity integration change.

Table V.

Bivariate Associations Predicting Individual-Level Consistency and Change in Identity Integration Groups.

Consistency or Change in Identity Integration Groups
Test Statistic Effect Size
Consistently High Increased from Low/Middling to High Decreased from High to Middling Consistently Middling Consistently Low
Time 1: M M M M M F η2

--Current sexual orientation 5.5 a 4.9 5.3 a 5.2 a 4.2 b 3.85** .12
--Social support: family 7.0 a 6.1 8.6 a 6.4 4.6 b 2.31 .08
--Social support: friends 10.4 a 9.4 10.8 a 10.7 a 8.7 b 3.21* .10
--Negative social relationships 2.0 a 2.3 1.9 a 2.2 a 2.6 b 3.25* .10
--# same-sex partners-lifetime 1.2 ab 1.8 ac 1.9 c 1.5 0.9 b 2.51* .08
--Social desirability 7.2 a 5.7 6.1 5.0 b 5.4 b 3.12* .10

% % % % % χ2 τ

--Gay-related stress 28% a 44% 31% a 32% a 67% b 8.92 .02
--Identify as lesbian/gay v. bi 76% a 82% a 75% a 75% a 29% b 15.76** .03
--Female sex 64% a 44% 63% a 46% a 17% b 12.46* .03

Note. Means or percents with differing superscripts differ at p < .05.

p < .07.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

Multivariate analyses

Each of the factors identified in the bivariate analyses as significantly associated with identity integration change groups were subsequently re-examined in multivariate logistic regression analyses in which each group was contrasted with the others; controls for gender differences and social desirability were imposed in all analyses. These multivariate findings largely replicated the bivariate findings, with the consistently low group differing from most other identity integration change groups (see Table VI). Indeed, the full model was found to significantly distinguish (i.e., significant omnibus chi-square tests) between the consistently low group and each of the other identity change groups. Youths who were consistently high in identity integration, as compared with youths consistently low in identity integration, had a cognitive sexual orientation that was more same-sex centered at Time 1, perceived more support from both friends and family at Time 1, had fewer negative social relationships at Time 1, experienced less gay-related stress at Time 1, and were more likely to identify as lesbian/gay at Time 1. Youths who increased from low or middling to high integration during the year, relative to youths who remained consistently low in integration, were more same-sex centered in their sexual orientation at Time 1, had more same-sex partners at Time 1, and were more likely to be lesbian/gay self-identified at Time 1. Youths who decreased from high to middling also differed significantly from the consistently low integration group on all the Time 1 variables, reporting a more same-sex centered cognitive sexual orientation, more family and friend support, fewer negative social relationships, less gay-related stress, more same-sex partners, and a greater tendency to identify as lesbian/gay. Finally, consistently middling youths as compared with consistently low youths were more same-sex centered in their orientation at Time 1 and reported at Time 1 more friend support, fewer negative social relationships, less gay-related stress, more same-sex partners, and a greater likelihood of identifying as lesbian or gay. The overall model did not significantly distinguish between the consistently high youths, youths who increased from low or middling to high, youths who decreased from high to middling, and consistently middling youths as evidenced by the nonsignficant omnibus chi-square tests for the logistic regression models.

Table VI.

Multivariate Associations Predicting Comparisons of Individual-level Consistency and Change in Identity Integration

Comparison of Change in Identity Integration Groups
Consistently High vs. Increased from Low/Mid to High Consistently High vs. Consistently Low Increased from Low/Mid to High vs. Consistently Low Decreased from High to Middling vs. Consistently Low Consistently Middling vs. Consistently Low

OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)
--Current sexual orientation 1.5 (0.9, 1.5) 4.5** (1.7, 11.9) 1.6 (1.0, 2.7) 2.9* (1.2, 7.3) 5.1** (1.8, 14.7)
--Social support: family -- 1.2 (1.0, 1.4) -- 1.2* (1.0, 1.5) --
--Social support: friends -- 1.3 (1.0, 1.7) -- 1.7* (1.1, 2.6) 1.4* (1.0, 1.9)
--Negative social relationships -- 0.2** (0.1, 0.7) -- 0.3* (0.1, 1.0) 0.2** (0.1, 0.7)
--Gay-related stress -- 0.2* (0.1, 0.9) -- 0.1* (0.0, 0.8) 0.2* (0.1, 0.8)
--# same-sex partners-lifetime -- -- 3.4** (1.4, 8.6) 3.8* (1.3, 11.3) 2.1* (1.1, 3.9)
--Identify as lesbian/gay v. bi -- 7.8** (1.8, 33.9) 13.0** (2.2, 79.1) 6.7* (1.2, 38.6) 14.9** (2.6, 87.1)

Note: Controls were imposed for sex and social desirability in all contrasts. Although tested, the full model did not distinguish (i.e., the omnibus chi-square test was nonsignificant) between the consistently high vs. decreased group, the consistently high vs. consistently middling group, the increasing vs. decreasing group, the increasing vs. consistently middling group, or the decreasing vs. consistently middling groups. Therefore, these nonsignificant logistic regression models are not reported here. The odds ratios of individual variables that significantly distinguished between identity change groups are reported, whereas nonsignificant variables are indicated with a dash.

p < .08.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

Discussion

Given the discrepancies between various models in the order of sexual identity development stages and the different patterns of development empirically identified, the current report sought to examine different patterns of identity formation and identity integration among LGB youths and the implications the patterns might have for subsequent identity development. In contrast to much past work that examined a specific theoretically-based pattern of development, the current study did not impose an a priori structure on the data because we had no basis for such a structure. Instead, we assessed various theoretically important dimensions of sexual identity formation and integration and allowed the data to suggest the naturally occurring patterns that might characterize and differentiate groups of youths. Also of critical concern in this report were the potential individual and social contextual factors hypothesized to predict the different patterns that might emerge, and the contextual factors that might predict changes in identity development over time.

Different Patterns of Identity Integration and Formation

The sexual identity formation of the LGB youths was found to develop in one of two naturally occurring patterns. One group consisted of a small number of youths (n = 51) who had undergone various milestones of sexual identity formation much earlier than the second and larger group of youths (n = 105). The early developing group was found to be older and to be more likely to have experienced childhood sexual abuse (p < .07). These findings are consistent with past research that suggests that sexual abuse may facilitate an earlier sensitivity to sexuality in general that may lead to an earlier awareness of sexual feelings, earlier (voluntary) sexual behavior, and an earlier questioning of sexual identity (e.g., Bartholow et al., 1994).

Three patterns of sexual identity integration were identified at Time 1, and the stability of these three patterns was examined at Time 2. One group reported high levels on all aspects of integration, a second group reported low levels on all aspects, and a middling group that was moderately high (like the high group) on some aspects and moderately low (like the low group) on other aspects. The groups differed on all four dimensions of identity integration at Time 1. They differed only on attitudes toward homosexuality/bisexuality and comfort with homosexuality/bisexuality at Time 2. Because these two factors are the internal aspects of identity integration, one reason for this finding at Time 2 is that it may take time to work through one’s biases. The other (nonsignificant) dimensions of integration at Time 2, involvement in LGB-related social activities and self-disclosure to others, are more external because they involve other individuals in the social networks of LGB persons. The lack of significant difference on these two dimensions at Time 2 may indicate a ceiling effect that is reached after someone has been integrating his or her sexual identity for some time. A flurry of activity involvement and disclosure to others may occur within a certain window of sexual identity development, followed by a relative flattening or decrease in the frequency of both dimensions. Future research must confirm the patterns or profiles of sexual identity formation and integration that were found here.

Our hypothesis that youths who began identity development earlier would be further along in the identity integration process was not supported. The identity formation groups were unrelated to the groups of sexual identity integration, suggesting that time since undergoing identity formation had little relation to identity integration. Sexual identity formation logically sets sexual identity integration in motion, but like a catalyst, it seems to initiate a process, but fails to chart the unfolding of that process. We suggest that sexual identity formation is necessary for sexual identity integration, but that the formation of an LGB identity is not sufficient for the continued unfolding of sexual identity integration. The nonsignificant association may indicate that some youths who experienced early identity formation experienced barriers that stalled subsequent integration of their new LGB identity, while other youths did not.

The current study is the first to identify potential individual and social context factors that serve as barriers to and facilitators of identity integration, both cross-sectionally and longitudinally. Specifically, multivariate analyses demonstrated that the group of youths high on integration differed from the middling and low integration groups in that they had a cognitive sexual orientation that was more same-sex focused, perceived receiving more family support, had fewer negative social relationships, and was less likely to experience gay-related stress. These findings suggest the critical role of positive factors, such as a supportive family, for optimal development of LGB youths. The findings also suggest that negative social interactions (e.g., gay-related stress, negative relationships) may not only have negative mental health consequences (e.g., Bontempo & D’Augelli, 2002), but may also impede further identity development. Furthermore, the fact that many of these findings were identified longitudinally (Time 1 factors associated with identity integration a year later at Time 2) suggests that these associations are potential causal predictors of identity integration. These data not only indicate why different groups of LGB youths may exist, but they also may suggest that LGB youths, just like their heterosexual peers, need supportive social relationships in order to develop and thrive.

Individual Changes in Identity Integration Over Time

In addition to group level differences, we also examined whether individual youths changed over the course of the year. These individual-level analyses found that many youths remained relatively consistent over the year, meaning they varied little over time. Changes in identity integration also were apparent. These transitions were both toward increasing integration (i.e., middling to high integration, 12%) and toward decreasing integration (i.e., high to middling integration, 11%). Other transitions occurred, but fewer numbers of youths experienced them (8% or less). However, the presence of decreasing integration suggests that identity integration is an ongoing process, and that changes in external or contextual factors (e.g., stress, support) may lead some youths to fluctuate in their integration over time. Also, transitions representing large changes, such as leaps from low to high integration over one year, were rare (1%), suggesting that the process of identity integration takes time and that profound changes of this magnitude are not likely to occur in the short time frame examined here.

As hypothesized, the individual and social context factors identified as cross-sectionally and longitudinally associated with identity integration groups were also found to predict individual-level changes in identity integration over time. The comparison of youths who were consistently high with those who where consistently low found that the former were more same-sex focused in their cognitive orientation, reported more support from family and friends, experienced fewer negative social relationships, and were less likely to have had gay-related stress. Similarly, the youths who decreased from high to middling integration and youths who were consistently middling also significantly differed from the consistently low integration group on these same factors. However, the psychosocial factors examined were generally unable to distinguish among the youths who were consistently high, those who increased from low or middling to high, those who decreased from high to middling, and those who were consistently middling in their LGB identity integration. Future research with larger samples than available here may wish to examine these predictors of change in identity integration.

These findings offer a number of important implications for those who design or deliver interventions or services to LGB youths in community settings. First, the findings of diverse patterns of identity formation and integration suggest that a single program to promote a healthy LGB identity may not serve all youths well. Rather, the findings suggest that programs must take into account and be tailored to each youth’s identity integration at that specific moment in time. At particular risk, and perhaps in most need of supportive assistance, are those youths who remain consistently low in their identity integration (e.g., relatively poorer attitudes toward homosexuality, lower participation in LGB social activities, less comfort with others potentially knowing about their sexuality, and less disclosure to others relative to other LGB youths). However, the finding that some youths decreased in identity integration suggests that all youths may need support to maintain their current level of integration, especially in times of stress or conflict. Fortunately, the findings also suggest potential ways that individuals and community-based organizations may promote LGB identity integration. Efforts to reduce the experiences of rejection, ridicule, and victimization among LGB youths, as well as negative social relationships more generally, clearly are important in assisting LGB identity integration. Further, the provision of emotionally supportive relationships, the development of supportive friendships, and efforts to facilitate supportive parent-child relationships would promote a healthy LGB identity for these youths. Although interventions to reduce experiences of gay-related stress and to promote support may be undertaken with the LGB youths individually, these findings also suggest the need to address the larger social context in which the youths find themselves. For example, efforts in the schools may both reduce victimization and provide safe supportive spaces (e.g., gay-straight alliances). Similarly, working with the families of LGB youths may help family members learn ways in which they can provide nurturance and support to their child particularly as he/she undergoes the process of identity development. Indeed, our findings suggest that efforts, such as those noted above, to address the larger social context of LGB youths are likely to facilitate healthy LGB identity development.

Limitations

Our study has a number of limitations. First, our sample was modest in size, although we did have sufficient numbers of cases to detect medium effect sizes. Second, we only had a one-year longitudinal period of analysis. Despite the relatively narrow assessment window and the likelihood that sexual identity development takes time, we did find important changes. Nevertheless, longer follow-up periods should be proposed to examine the degree of consistency and change in sexual identity development; our study provides the rationale for such studies. Lastly, because our youths were recruited from New York City and from gay-focused programs and community organizations, generalizability may be limited. As we encourage attempts to identify and understand diversity among LGB youths, we also encourage diversity in the samples of such youths.

Conclusions

The current report found that LGB youths experience a diverse set of coming-out experiences, and no single pattern was found to be typical. We found multiple patterns of identity formation, identity integration, and change in identity integration over time. Further, the findings of both increasing and decreasing identity integration suggest that this may be an ongoing developmental process. In addition, we found that several hypothesized factors predicted the sexual identity development patterns of LGB youths. Therefore, future research should be sensitive to the diversity that exists among LGB youths before data are aggregated across the youths. In addition, future research should consider the implications of any diversity for the youths’ continuing development and their mental and physical health. The higher prevalence of poor mental and physical health found among LGB youths relative to heterosexual peers (e.g., Bontempo & D’Augelli, 2002; Fergusson, Horwood, & Beautrais, 1999; Russell & Joyner, 2001; Valleroy et al., 2000) may be attributed to a subgroup(s) of LGB youths, rather than all LGB youths. If true, the identification of that subgroup(s) would advance our scientific understanding and hone our intervention efforts.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Center Grant P50-MH43520 from the National Institute of Mental Health (Margaret Rosario, Project PI; Anke Ehrhardt, Center PI). Earlier versions of this articles were presented at the inaugural conference of the University Consortium for Sexuality Research and Training at the Kinsey Institute, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, April 2007; and, at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco, CA, August 2007.

Footnotes

1

Specifically, one youth was excluded because she was older than the 21-year age criterion. Four female youths were excluded because they identified as heterosexual and never engaged in sexual activity with the same sex. Two male youths were interviewed twice; their second interviews were discarded. Finally, one youth was excluded because the interviewer believed him to have provided unreliable and invalid responses.

Contributor Information

Margaret Rosario, Department of Psychology, The City University of New York – City College and Graduate Center.

Eric W. Schrimshaw, Doctoral Program in Psychology, The City University of New York – Graduate Center

Joyce Hunter, HIV Center for Clinical and Behavioral Studies, New York State Psychiatric Institute.

References

  1. Bartholow BN, Doll LS, Joy D, Douglas JM, Bolan G, Harrison JS, Moss PM, McKirnan D. Emotional, behavioral, and HIV risks associated with sexual abuse among adult homosexual and bisexual men. Child Abuse & Neglect. 1994;18:747–761. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(94)00042-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bontempo DE, D’Augelli AR. Effects of at-school victimization and sexual orientation on lesbian, gay, or bisexual youths’ health risk behavior. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2002;30:364–374. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(01)00415-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Boxer AM, Cohler BJ. The life course of gay and lesbian youth: An immodest proposal for the study of lives. Journal of Homosexuality. 1989;17:315–355. doi: 10.1300/J082v17n03_07. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Cass VC. Homosexual identity formation: A theoretical model. Journal of Homosexuality. 1979;4:219–235. doi: 10.1300/J082v04n03_01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Chapman BE, Brannock JC. Proposed model of lesbian identity development: An empirical examination. Journal of Homosexuality. 1987;14:69–80. doi: 10.1300/J082v14n03_05. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Crowne DP, Marlowe D. The approval motive: Studies in evaluative dependence. Westport, CT: Greenwood; 1964. [Google Scholar]
  7. D’Augelli AR. Lesbian and gay male development: Steps toward an analysis of lesbians’ and gay men’s lives. In: Greene B, Herek GM, editors. Lesbian and gay psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1994. pp. 118–132. [Google Scholar]
  8. D’Augelli AR, Hershberger SL. Lesbian, gay, and bisexual youth in community settings: Personal challenges and mental health problems. American Journal of Community Psychology. 1993;21:421–448. doi: 10.1007/BF00942151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. D’Augelli AR, Hershberger SL, Pilkington NW. Lesbian, gay, and bisexual youth and their families: Disclosure of sexual orientation and its consequences. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 1998;68:361–371. doi: 10.1037/h0080345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Diamond LM. Sexual identity, attractions, and behavior among young sexual-minority women over a 2-year period. Developmental Psychology. 2000;36:241–250. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.36.2.241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Diamond LM. New paradigms for research on heterosexual and sexual-minority development. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2003;32:490–498. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3204_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Diamond LM. A new view of lesbian subtypes: Stable versus fluid identity trajectories over an 8-year period. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2005;29:119–128. [Google Scholar]
  13. Diamond LM, Savin-Williams RC. Explaining diversity in the development of same-sex sexuality among young women. Journal of Social Issues. 2000;56:297–313. [Google Scholar]
  14. Dickson N, Paul C, Herbison P. Same-sex attraction in a birth cohort: Prevalence and persistence in early adulthood. Social Science and Medicine. 2003;56:1607–1615. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(02)00161-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dubé EM. The role of sexual behavior in the identification process of gay and bisexual males. The Journal of Sex Research. 2000;37:123–132. [Google Scholar]
  16. Dubé EM, Savin-Williams RC. Sexual identity development among ethnic sexual-minority male youths. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35:1389–1398. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.35.6.1389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Eliason MJ. Identity formation for lesbian, bisexual, and gay persons: Beyond a “minoritizing” view. Journal of Homosexuality. 1996;30:31–58. doi: 10.1300/J082v30n03_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Elizur Y, Mintzer A. A framework for the formation of gay male identity: Processes associated with adult attachment style and support from family and friends. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2001;30:143–167. doi: 10.1023/a:1002725217345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Everitt BS, Landau S, Leese M. Cluster analysis. 4. New York: Oxford; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  20. Fassinger RE, Miller BA. Validation of an inclusive model of sexual minority identity formation on a sample of gay men. Journal of Homosexuality. 1996;32:53–78. doi: 10.1300/j082v32n02_04. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Beautrais AL. Is sexual orientation related to mental health problems and suicidality in young people? Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56:876–880. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.10.876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Floyd FJ, Bakeman R. Coming-out across the life course: Implications of age and historical context. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2006;35:287–296. doi: 10.1007/s10508-006-9022-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Floyd FJ, Stein TS. Sexual orientation identity formation among gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths: Multiple patterns of milestone experiences. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2002;12:167–191. [Google Scholar]
  24. Goodman LA, Kruskal WH. Measures of association for cross classification. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1979. [Google Scholar]
  25. Grov C, Bimbi DS, Nanín JE, Parsons JT. Race, ethnicity, gender, and generational factors associated with the coming-out process among gay, lesbian, and bisexual individuals. The Journal of Sex Research. 2006;43:115–121. doi: 10.1080/00224490609552306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gurley DN. Doctoral Dissertation. University of Kentucky; Lexington, KY: 1990. The context of well-being after significant life stress: Measuring social support and obstruction. [Google Scholar]
  27. Henry B, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Langley J, Silva PA. On the “remembrance of things past”: A longitudinal evaluation of the retrospective method. Psychological Assessment. 1994;6:92–101. [Google Scholar]
  28. Henry DB, Tolan PH, Gorman-Smith D. Cluster analysis in family psychology research. Journal of Family Psychology. 2005;19:121–132. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.19.1.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hershberger SL, D’Augelli AR. The impact of victimization on the mental health and suicidality of lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths. Developmental Psychology. 1995;31:65–74. [Google Scholar]
  30. Horowitz JL, Newcomb MD. A multidimentional approach to homosexual identity. Journal of Homosexuality. 2001;42:1–19. doi: 10.1300/j082v42n02_01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Jordan KM, Deluty RH. Coming out for lesbian women: Its relation to anxiety, positive affectivity, self-esteem, and social support. Journal of Homosexuality. 1998;35:41–63. doi: 10.1300/J082v35n02_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kahn MJ. Factors affecting the coming out process for lesbians. Journal of Homosexuality. 1991;21:47–70. doi: 10.1300/J082v21n03_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kitzinger C, Wilkinson S. Transitions from heterosexuality to lesbianism: Discursive production of lesbian identities. Developmental Psychology. 1995;31:95–104. [Google Scholar]
  34. Maguen S, Floyd FJ, Bakeman R, Armistead L. Developmental milestones and disclosure of sexual orientation among gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths. Applied Developmental Psychology. 2002;23:219–233. [Google Scholar]
  35. Matthews AK, Hughes TL, Tartaro J. Sexual behavior and sexual dysfunction in a community sample of lesbian and heterosexual women. In: Omoto AM, Kurtzman HS, editors. Sexual orientation and mental health: Examining identity and development in lesbian, gay, and bisexual people. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2006. pp. 185–205. [Google Scholar]
  36. Meyer S, Schwitzer AM. Stages of identity development among college students with minority sexual orientations. Journal of College Student Psychotherapy. 1999;13:41–65. [Google Scholar]
  37. Meyer-Bahlburg HFL, Ehrhardt AA, Exner TM, Gruen RS. Sexual risk behavior assessment schedule – youth. HIV Center for Clinical and Behavioral Studies, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  38. Minton HL, McDonald GJ. Homosexual identity formation as a developmental process. Journal of Homosexuality. 1984;9:91–104. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Morris JF. Lesbian coming out as a multidimentional process. Journal of Homosexuality. 1997;33:1–22. doi: 10.1300/J082v33n02_01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Morris JF, Balsam KF. Lesbian and bisexual women’s experiences of victimization: Mental health, revictimization, and sexual identity development. Journal of Lesbian Studies. 2003;7:67–85. doi: 10.1300/J155v07n04_05. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Morris JF, Waldo CR, Rothblum ED. A model of predictors and outcomes of outness among lesbian and bisexual women. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2001;71:61–71. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.71.1.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Nungesser L. Homosexual acts, actors, and identities. New York: Praeger; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  43. Peplau LA. Human sexuality: How do men and women differ? Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2003;12:37–40. [Google Scholar]
  44. Pilkington NW, D’Augelli AR. Victimization of lesbian, gay, and bisexual youth in community settings. Journal of Community Psychology. 1995;23:34–56. [Google Scholar]
  45. Procidano ME, Heller K. Measures of perceived social support from friends and from family: Three validation studies. American Journal of Community Psychology. 1983;11:1–24. doi: 10.1007/BF00898416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rabkin BD, Luke DA. Cluster analysis in community research: Epistemology and practice. American Journal of Community Psychology. 1993;21:247–277. [Google Scholar]
  47. Robohm JS, Litzenberger BW, Pearlman LA. Sexual abuse in lesbian and bisexual young women: Associations with emotional/behavioral difficulties, feelings about sexuality, and the “coming out” process. Journal of Lesbian Studies. 2003;7:31–47. doi: 10.1300/J155v07n04_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Rosario M, Hunter J, Maguen S, Gwadz M, Smith R. The coming-out process and its adaptational and health-related associations among gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths: Stipulation and exploration of a model. American Journal of Community Psychology. 2001;29:133–160. doi: 10.1023/A:1005205630978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Rosario M, Meyer-Bahlburg HFL, Hunter J, Exner TM, Gwadz M, Keller AM. The psychosexual development of urban lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths. The Journal of Sex Research. 1996;33:113–126. [Google Scholar]
  50. Rosario M, Schrimshaw EW, Hunter J. Ethnic/racial differences in the coming-out process of lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths: A comparison of sexual identity development over time. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2004;10:215–228. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.10.3.215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Rosario M, Schrimshaw EW, Hunter J, Braun L. Sexual identity development among lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths: Consistency and change over time. The Journal of Sex Research. 2006;43:46–58. doi: 10.1080/00224490609552298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Rosario M, Schrimshaw EW, Hunter J, Gwadz M. Gay-related stress and emotional distress among gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths: A longitudinal examination. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002;70:967–975. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.70.4.967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Ross M. Relation of implicit theories to construction of personal histories. Psychological Review. 1989;96:341–257. [Google Scholar]
  54. Rotheram-Borus MJ, Fernandez MI. Sexual orientation and developmental challenges experienced by gay and lesbian youths. Suicide & Life Threatening Behavior. 1995;25(Suppl):26–34. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Russell ST, Joyner K. Adolescent sexual orientation and suicide risk: Evidence from a national study. American Journal of Public Health. 2001;91:1276–1281. doi: 10.2105/ajph.91.8.1276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Rust PC. “Coming-out” in the age of social constructionism: Sexual identity formation and lesbian and bisexual women. Gender & Society. 1993;7:50–77. [Google Scholar]
  57. Saewyc EM. Pregnancy among lesbian, gay, and bisexual adolescents: Influences of stigma, sexual abuse, and sexual orientation. In: Omoto AM, Kurtzman HS, editors. Sexual orientation and mental health: Examining identity and development in lesbian, gay, and bisexual people. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2006. pp. 95–116. [Google Scholar]
  58. Safren SA, Heimberg RG. Depression, hopelessness, suicidality, and related factors in sexual minority and heterosexual adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1999;67:859–866. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.67.6.859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Savin-Williams RC. A critique of research on sexual-minority youths. Journal of Adolescence. 2001;24:5–13. doi: 10.1006/jado.2000.0369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Savin-Williams RC, Diamond LM. Sexual identity trajectories among sexual-minority youths: Gender comparisons. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2000;29:607–627. doi: 10.1023/a:1002058505138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Schindhelm RK, Hospers HJ. Sex with men before coming-out: Relation to sexual activity and sexual risk-taking behavior. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2004;33:585–591. doi: 10.1023/B:ASEB.0000044742.83096.b9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Schneider MS. Toward a reconceptualization of the coming-out process for adolescent females. In: D’Augelli AR, Patterson CJ, editors. Lesbian, gay and bisexual identities and youth: Psychological perspectives. New York: Oxford; 2001. pp. 71–96. [Google Scholar]
  63. Schrimshaw EW, Rosario M, Meyer-Bahlburg HFL, Scharf-Matlick AA. Test-retest reliability of self-reported sexual risk behavior, sexual orientation, and sexual development among gay, lesbian, and bisexual youths. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2006;35:225–234. doi: 10.1007/s10508-005-9006-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Sophie J. A critical examination of stage theories of lesbian identity development. Journal of Homosexuality. 1985/86;12:39–51. doi: 10.1300/j082v12n02_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Stokes JP, Damon W, McKirnan DJ. Predictors of movement toward homosexuality: A longitudinal study of bisexual men. The Journal of Sex Research. 1997;34:304–312. [Google Scholar]
  66. Stokes JP, McKirnan DJ, Burzette R. Sexual behavior, condom use, disclosure of sexuality, and stability of sexual orientation in bisexual men. The Journal of Sex Research. 1993;30:203–213. [Google Scholar]
  67. Troiden RR. The formation of homosexual identities. Journal of Homosexuality. 1989;17:43–73. doi: 10.1300/J082v17n01_02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Valleroy LA, MacKellar DA, Karon JM, Rosen DH, McFarland W, Shehan DA, et al. HIV prevalence and associated risks in young men who have sex with men. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2000;284:198–204. doi: 10.1001/jama.284.2.198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Wright ER, Perry BL. Sexual identity distress, social support, and the health of gay, lesbian, and bisexual youth. Journal of Homosexuality. 2006;51:81–110. doi: 10.1300/J082v51n01_05. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES