Abstract
Emerging evidence suggests that depression is associated with executive dysfunction, particularly after committing errors or receiving negative performance feedback. To test this hypothesis, 57 participants performed two executive tasks known to elicit errors (the Simon and Stroop Tasks) during positive or negative performance feedback. Participants with elevated depressive symptoms (Beck Depression Inventory scores ≥ 13) were characterized by impaired post-error and post-conflict performance adjustments, especially during emotionally negative task-related feedback. Additionally, for both tasks, depressive symptoms were inversely related to post-conflict reaction time adjustments following negative, but not positive, feedback. These findings suggest that subclinical depression is associated with impairments in behavioral adjustments after internal (perceived failure) and external feedback about deficient task performance.
Keywords: Depression, Conflict Monitoring, Feedback, Executive Functioning
Over the years, the investigation of neuropsychological dysfunction in depression has attracted considerable interest (Rogers et al., 2004). Among the most replicated findings are reports of impaired executive functions, which typically emerge in experimental settings requiring adaptive action monitoring and flexible behavioral adjustments (Austin, Mitchell, & Goodwin, 2001; Paradiso, Lamberty, Garvey, & Robinson, 1997; Porter, Gallagher, Thompson, & Young, 2003; Siegle, Steinhauer, & Thase, 2004). Interestingly, executive dysfunctions have been observed after remission of depressive symptoms (e.g., Trichard et al., 1995), and have predicted poor response to antidepressant treatments (Dunkin et al., 2000). Although these findings indicate that executive dysfunctions confer increased vulnerability to depression, the precise mechanisms giving rise to these impairments remain largely unexplored.
Findings from recent neuropsychological studies in both clinically depressed and dysphoric subjects suggest that the observed executive deficits may be partially due to abnormal responses to negative feedback or perceived failure. Accordingly, depressed subjects have been shown to display circumscribed and specific behavioral impairments in trials immediately following errors or negative feedback concerning task performance (Elliott et al., 1996; Elliott, Sahakian, Herrod, Robbins, & Paykel, 1997; Murphy, Michael, Robbins, & Sahakian, 2003; Pizzagalli, Peccoralo, Davidson, & Cohen, 2006). These neuropsychological findings are in line with independent reports emphasizing that depressed and dysphoric participants are characterized by (1) amplification of the relative significance of their failures (Wenzlaff & Grozier, 1988); (2) difficulty in suppressing failure-related thoughts (Conway, Howell, & Giannopoulos, 1991); and (3) increased depressed mood after encountering negative social feedback (Henriques & Leitenberg, 2002). As a corpus, this research indicates that depressed patients are less able to utilize information conveyed by errors or feedback to modulate their subsequent performance. This is consistent with literature associating depression with enduring negative cognitive schemata and processing biases (Clark, Beck, & Alford, 1999) and suggests a link between deficits in executive functioning and negative affect in depression.
In parallel, research in cognitive psychology and cognitive neuroscience has demonstrated that errors and task-relevant feedback can elicit adaptive shifts in behavior (Gauggel, Wietasch, Bayer, & Rolko, 2000; Laming, 1979; Rabbitt, 1966; Tucker, Luu, Frishkoff, Quiring, & Poulsen, 2003). This is consistent with the conflict monitoring theory which suggests that the occurrence of errors and conflicts between mutually incompatible responses triggers the engagement of control processes resulting in adjustments in performance (Botvinick, Nystrom, Fissell, Carter, & Cohen, 1999). These online, or trial-to- trial, task adjustments have been traditionally measured in participants’ reactivity to errors and conflict trials (e.g., incongruent trials in a Stroop task) over the course of an experiment. For instance, studies have shown that healthy subjects typically display increased accuracy but slowed reaction time immediately following an error, a post-error adjustment known as the Rabbitt/Laming effect (Laming, 1979; Rabbitt, 1966). Similarly, participants generally exhibit decreased error rates and slowed reaction time following conflict trials, a post-conflict adjustment phenomenon known as the Gratton effect (Gratton, Coles, & Donchin, 1992).
The Present Study
In order to assess putative action monitoring dysfunctions in subjects with elevated depressive symptoms, versions of the Simon and Stroop tasks known to induce conflict monitoring and errors were adapted to include a task-relevant feedback manipulation. Based on prior findings in the literature (Gratton et al., 1992; Rabbitt, 1966; Laming, 1979), trials requiring greater levels of performance monitoring (high conflict trials, post-error trials, and post-conflict trials) were hypothesized to be associated with behavioral adjustments reflecting the recruitment of additional cognitive resources. As in previous studies, congruence (Simon/Stroop), post-error (Rabbitt/Laming), and post-conflict (Gratton) effects were expected across all participants.
Most importantly, in light of evidence highlighting executive deficits in depression, particularly in response to negative feedback or perceived failure, the present study aimed to investigate action monitoring processes (i.e., conflict monitoring, error processing, and feedback evaluation) in individuals with varying levels of sub-clinical depressive symptoms. Based on the literature associating depression with (a) dysfunctional action monitoring (Ruchsow et al., 2004; Tucker et al., 2003; Elliott et al., 1996; Elliott et al., 1997; Murphy et al., 2003; Pizzagalli et al., 2006; Siegle et al., 2004); and (b) difficulty in suppressing thoughts related to failure (Conway et al., 1991), it was hypothesized that subjects with elevated depressive symptoms would show decreased performance following high-conflict trials and errors. Moreover, because both clinical depression and dysphoria have been associated with abnormal affective, cognitive, and neural reactions to both internal (implicit) and external (explicit) feedback of poor performance (Conway et al., 1991; Elliott et al., 1996; Elliott et al., 1997; Henriques et al., 2002; Murphy et al., 2003; Ruchsow et al., 2004; Tucker et al., 2003; Wenzlaff & Grozier, 1988), dysfunctional action monitoring processes were hypothesized to be amplified in the context of negative feedback pertaining to ongoing task performance. Finally, participants reporting increased levels of cognitive-affective symptoms were expected to be particularly vulnerable to these deficits.
Methods
Participants
Seventy-four subjects were recruited from the Harvard University Study Pool, which includes both Harvard University undergraduate students and community subjects. Prior to behavioral testing, participants provided written informed consent to a protocol approved by the Committee on the Use of Human Subjects at Harvard University. Participants received course credit or monetary compensation ($10/hour) for their participation. Seventy-three subjects were right handed (one was ambidextrous), as assessed by the Chapman and Chapman handedness scale (Chapman & Chapman, 1987). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and normal color vision as assessed by the Ishihara Test for Color Blindness (Ishihara, 1973). Six subjects were not included in the analysis because following the experiment they reported that the feedback manipulation (see below) was not veridical. These participants did not differ demographically, or in their self-reported measures of mood from individuals who believed the feedback. Following prior recommendations, seven participants with a Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) score of 0 or 1 were excluded because such scores may be indicative of non-normative functioning (Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961; Kendall, Hollon, Beck, & Hammen, 1987). Two participants were excluded from all analyses because their age was greater than three standard deviations from the mean age of all subjects. Two additional participants performed worse than chance levels in the Simon Task and their data were excluded from that portion of the analysis. Two participants were excluded from the Stroop task data analysis because they were not fluent in English. The final sample consisted of 57 participants for both the Simon (61.40% Caucasian; 68.40% female; mean age = 22.33, SD: 6.68, mean education 14.54, SD: 1.71) and the Stroop (63.20% Caucasian; 66.70% female; mean age = 22.07, SD: 6.44, mean education 14.51, SD: 1.68) tasks.
Procedure
Each subject performed both behavioral (Simon and Stroop) tasks during a single session according to an order counterbalanced across subjects. Before each task, subjects were presented with a practice block to familiarize them with the paradigm. After the behavioral tasks, the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck, Steer, Ball, & Ranieri, 1996) and the trait form of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushere, 1970) were administered, among other questionnaires, to assess levels of depressive symptoms and negative affect, respectively. Following the task, participants completed a questionnaire asking whether they believed the feedback manipulation. Subjects who did not believe the manipulation were excluded from the analysis.
Apparatus
The experiment was presented on an IBM 2.4 GHz with 1GB of RAM using Eprime software version 1.1 (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA). Subject responses were collected by key press on a button box (Response Pad 200; Electrical Geodesic, Inc, Oregon).
Tasks
Based on criticisms that trial-to-trial adjustments might be due to stimulus-specific priming effects (Mayr, Awh, & Laurey, 2003), for each task, the number of previously congruent trials prior to each incongruent trial as well as the number of previously incongruent trials prior to each congruent trial was fully randomized using software developed for neuroimaging research to maximize the statistical orthogonality of experimental conditions (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/optseq/). Accordingly, any individual incongruent trial could be preceded by one to four congruent trials. Additionally, no more than three repetitions of a given stimulus type were allowed. To increase task difficulty, and thus induce more errors, there were 98 congruent trials and only 54 incongruent trials in each block (37.5% incongruent trials per block). Reaction time (RT) and accuracy measures were collected throughout the tasks. Each task consisted of four blocks, each lasting 7:36 min.
Simon Task
Participants were instructed to perform a speeded variant of the Simon task, known to elicit response conflict and a high error rate. Instructions emphasized responding as quickly as possible to each probe while still remaining accurate, and were re-administered after each block. Probes were presented for 200 ms on either the left or right of a fixation cross and participants were instructed to make spatial responses with either their left or their right index finger. Prior to the task the participants received instructions, both verbally and in writing. Next, subjects were presented with the congruent and incongruent stimuli they would encounter during the task. For a congruent probe, participants were asked to respond with the index finger that corresponded to the side the stimuli appeared. For incongruent probes, the participants were asked to respond with their index finger that corresponded to the side opposite from where the stimulus appeared. The trials consisted of one congruent probe (green circle) and three incongruent probes (red circle, red square, and green square). Multiple levels of incongruence were used to prevent participants from building a simple association between the color of the stimulus and the response set. Following the probe, the inter-trial interval was jittered between 2300 and 3300ms (2300, 2800, and 3300). Throughout the task, a fixation cross (‘+’) was presented in the center of the screen.
Stroop Task
Participants performed a speeded variant of the Stroop task. The stimuli consisted of three words (RED, GREEN, and BLUE) printed in three different colors of ink (red, green, and blue). Trials could be either congruent (when the ink matches the word) or incongruent (when the ink does not match the word). Participants were instructed to respond, as quickly as possible while still remaining accurate, to the ink color of each word. Participants responded with a button press using the middle three fingers of their right hand, with one finger representing each color. Probes were presented for 200 ms in the center of the screen. Following the probe the inter-trial interval was jittered between 2300 and 3300ms (2300, 2800, and 3300). Between probes, a fixation cross (‘+’) was presented in the center of the screen for the duration of the task.
Task-Related Performance Feedback
For both tasks, participants were presented with positive or negative feedback at the beginning of each block. The participants were informed that this feedback reflected their performance on the previous block compared to subjects who had already completed the study. For the first block, participants were told that the feedback reflected their performance on the practice block. In reality, the feedback received was not contingent upon subjects’ performance but was pre-determined to allow for a balanced design. For the positive manipulation, the following feedback was presented: “Compared to subjects who have already participated in the study your performance in the last block was ABOVE AVERAGE. Please remember to respond as quickly as possible to the stimuli while remaining accurate.” For the negative manipulation, the following feedback was presented: “Compared to subjects who have already participated in the study your performance in the last block was significantly BELOW AVERAGE. Please remember to respond as quickly as possible to the stimuli while remaining accurate.” The order of feedback was pseudo-randomized and fully counterbalanced across participants (negative-positive-positive-negative or positive-negative-negative-positive).
Data Reduction
To minimize the influence of outliers on RTs, trials with RTs (after natural log transformation) falling outside the range of mean ± 3SD for each trial type were excluded from analyses (Simon Task: 1.54%±1.88%; Stroop Task: 1.07%±0.06%). Outliers were identified separately for congruent and incongruent trial types and each subject. Only trials where the subject made a response were considered for the analyses.
As summarized above, substantial evidence suggests that the occurrence of conflict and errors triggers the engagement of control processes resulting in adjustments in performance (Botvinick et al., 1999). In cognitive studies these on-line task adjustments have been typically studied in the form of the Simon/Stroop, Laming/Rabbitt, and Gratton effects. The Simon/Stroop effect is a measurement of interference elicited by the task, and is computed as: [RTIncongruent trials – RTCongruent trials] and [AccuracyCongruent trials – AccuracyIncongruent trials]; higher scores are indicative of general impairments in cognitive control. The Laming/Rabbitt effect (Laming, 1979; Rabbitt, 1966) is a measurement of post-error adjustment, and is calculated as: [RTAfter incorrect trials – RTAfter correct trials] and [AccuracyAfter incorrect trials -AccuracyAfter correct trials]. The Gratton effect (Gratton et al., 1992) is a measure of post-conflict adjustment, and is operationalized as: [RTIncongruent trials following congruent trials – RTIncongruent trials following incongruent trials] and [AccuracyIncongruent trials following incongruent trials – AccuracyIncongruent trials following congruent trials]. For both the Laming/Rabbit and Gratton effects higher scores are indicative of increased cognitive control.
An additional, potentially more sensitive, method of examining behavioral adjustments after conflict trials includes examining whether an individual exhibits decreased RT on incongruent trials preceded by incongruent trials (iI) relative to incongruent trials preceded by congruent trials (cI), as well as decreased RT on congruent trials preceded by congruent trials (cC) relative to congruent trials preceded by incongruent trials (iC; Kerns et al., 2004). This effect can be studied through the calculation of a so-called “post-conflict RT adjustment score” [(iC – cC) + (cI – iI)]. For this effect, higher scores also indicate increased cognitive control.
Statistical Analyses
For each task, exploratory analyses revealed no significant effects of gender or form of compensation (course credit vs. payment); consequently, data were collapsed across these variables. Two sets of statistical analyses were performed. The first aimed to assess the relationship between feedback and levels of depressive symptoms on behavioral adjustments. To this end, accuracy and RT scores were separately calculated for the negative and positive feedback manipulation, and entered in a mixed 2 × 2 × 2 analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Group (low BDI vs. high BDI subjects) as a between-subject factor and Condition (e.g., post-error vs. post-correct accuracy) and Feedback (negative vs. positive) as repeated measures. Participants with a BDI-II score less ≤ 5 were assigned to the low BDI group, those with a BDI-II score ≥ 13 to the high BDI group (Beck et al., 1996). For the Simon task, the low BDI (3.50±0.88) and high BDI (16.57±3.34) included 21 and 13 subjects, respectively. For the Stroop task, the low BDI (3.43±0.87) and high BDI (16.38±3.40) included 20 and 14 subjects, respectively. The low and high BDI groups did not differ in their demographic variables for both the Simon [ethnicity χ2(1) = 0.22, p > 0.63; gender χ2(1) = 1.09, p = 0.30; age t(32) = 0.14, p > 0.88; education t(32) = 1.51, p > 0.22] and Stroop [ethnicity χ2(1) = 1.40, p > 0.23; gender χ2(1) = 1.40, p > 0.23; age t(32) = -0.25, p > 0.80; education t(32) = 1.78, p > 0.08] task.
To assess modulations on the Simon/Stroop effects, the performance for congruent vs. incongruent trials was entered for the Condition factor. For the Laming/Rabbitt effect, the performance following a correct vs. incorrect response was used. Lastly, for the Gratton effect, the performance for incongruent trials following a congruent trial was compared to the performance during incongruent trials following an incongruent trial. A significant Group x Condition interaction when considering, for example, post-correct and post-incorrect trials, indicates that the high and low BDI groups differ in their Laming/Rabbitt effect. In light of reports highlighting temporal decay of affective states (Hemenover, 2003), only the first half of the trials within each block were utilized for the ANOVAs.1 In the case of significant interactions emerging from the ANOVAs, post-hoc Newman-Keuls tests were performed.
In a second and complementary approach, hierarchical regression analyses were run to examine whether individual differences in depressive symptoms predicted unique variance in adjustment effects during the negative feedback condition. The regression analyses were conducted on all participants (n = 57). Specifically, these regressions tested whether BDI scores (entered in the second step) predicted a given behavioral adjustment effect during the negative feedback after adjusting for the same effect during the positive feedback (entered in the first step). Regression analyses were run on the Simon/Stroop, Rabbitt, and the post-conflict RT adjustment effect [(iC – cC) + (cI – iI); Kerns et al., 2004]. The latter was preferred over the Gratton effect because it was expected to provide a more sensitive measure of post-conflict behavioral adjustments.
If significant findings emerged, two follow-up regression analyses were performed to further assess their specificity. The first tested whether two BDI subscores, which have been previously identified in factor analytic studies of the BDI-II within undergraduate samples, differentially contributed to a given behavioral adjustment effect during the negative feedback. According to the BDI-II manual (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996), a cognitive-affective subscore (sadness, past failure, loss of pleasure, guilty feelings, punishment feelings, self-dislike, self-criticalness, suicidal thoughts, crying, agitation, loss of interest, indecisiveness, worthlessness, irritability) and a somatic subscore (loss of energy, changes in sleeping pattern, changes in appetite, concentration difficulty, tiredness or fatigue) were computed. The second regression analysis assessed whether BDI scores predicted a given behavioral effect during the negative feedback after adjusting for negative affect, as assessed by the trait form of the STAI.
Results
Simon Task: ANOVA Analyses
Congruence (Simon) effects
For both RT and accuracy, a main effect of Condition (congruent trials vs. incongruent trials) emerged (RT: F1,32 = 134.06, p < 0.00; partial η2 = 0.81; accuracy: F1,32 = 18.55, p < 0.00; partial η2 = 0.37). As expected, participants were faster and more accurate during congruent than incongruent trials. For accuracy, a Group x Feedback interaction also emerged (F1,32 = 4.15, p < 0.05; partial η2 = 0.12). Post-hoc Newman-Keuls tests revealed a trend for high BDI (p > 0.08), but not low BDI (p > 0.60), subjects to show decreased overall accuracy during the negative compared to the positive feedback condition (Fig. 1a).
Post-error adjustment (Laming/Rabbitt) effects
For RT, the effect of Condition (post-error trials vs. post-correct trials) revealed a trend, which was due to slower RT following an incorrect than a correct trial (F1,32 = 3.53, p < 0.07; partial η2 = 0.10). No other effects emerged for RT. For accuracy, the ANOVA revealed a Group x Feedback interaction (F1,32 = 9.75, p < 0.01; partial η2 = 0.23), which was qualified by a Group x Condition x Feedback interaction (F1,32 = 5.95, p < 0.03; partial η2 = 0.16; Fig. 1b). Post-hoc Newman-Keuls clarified that the Group x Feedback interaction was due to significantly lower accuracy during the negative compared to the positive feedback for high BDI (p < 0.03), but not low BDI (p > 0.18), subjects. To evaluate the triple interaction, follow-up Group x Condition ANOVAs were performed for the positive and negative feedback separately. For the positive feedback, no effects emerged (all Fs1,32 < 2.57, all ps > 0.11), suggesting that the two groups did not differ in the Laming/Rabbitt effect during the positive feedback condition. For the negative feedback, the only significant effect was the Group x Condition interaction, F1,32 = 5.19, p < 0.03, indicating that high and low BDI groups differed in their Laming/Rabbitt effects during the negative feedback condition (Fig. 1c). Compared to low BDI subjects, high BDI subjects had significantly lower accuracy after incorrect (p < 0.005), but not correct (p > 0.10), trials, as assessed with post-hoc Newman-Keuls tests. Moreover, low (p = 0.05), but not high (p > 0.23), subjects had higher accuracy after incorrect than correct trials. Accordingly, only low BDI subjects showed a significant Laming/Rabbitt effect in the negative feedback condition.
Conflict-adaptation (Gratton) effects
For RT, a Group x Condition (incongruent trials following congruent trials vs. incongruent trials following incongruent trials) x Feedback interaction emerged (F1,32 = 4.51, p < 0.05; partial η2 = 0.12), indicating that the high and low BDI subjects differed in their Gratton effects depending on the nature of the feedback (Fig. 1d). As above, follow-up Group x Condition ANOVAs were run separately for the positive and negative feedback. For the positive feedback, no effects emerged (all Fs1,32 < 1.58, all ps > 0.20). For the negative feedback, a trend for the interaction was observed, F1,32 = 6.08, p < 0.08. Post-hoc Newman-Keuls tests for this interaction were, however, not significant. For accuracy, a main effect for Condition was found indicating that participants responded less accurately on incongruent trials following congruent trials relative to incongruent trials following incongruent trials (F1,32 = 3.34, p < 0.02; partial η2 = 0.16). No further effects emerged.
Simon Task: Regression Analyses
Congruence (Simon) effects
For both RT (ΔR2 = 0.000, ΔF(1,54) = 0.01, p > 0.90) and accuracy (ΔR2 = 0.002, ΔF(1,54) = 0.11, p > 0.75), BDI scores did not predict the Simon effect during the negative feedback after adjusting for the effect during the positive feedback.
Post-error adjustment (Laming/Rabbitt) effects
No effects emerged when considering RT (ΔR2 = 0.005, ΔF(1,54) = 0.29, p > 0.59) or accuracy (ΔR2 = 0.025, ΔF(1,54) = 1.43, p > 0.23).
Post-conflict adjustment RT effects
BDI scores explained unique variance in post-conflict RT adjustment scores during negative feedback even after adjusting for individual differences in positive feedback effects, ΔR2 = 0.115, ΔF(1,54) = 7.32, p < 0.01 (Fig. 2a). Accordingly, the higher the BDI scores, the lower the post-conflict behavioral adjustments in reaction time during the negative feedback manipulation. Analogous regression analyses performed on the two BDI subscores revealed that the cognitive-affective dimension (ΔR2 = 0.139, ΔF(1,54) = 8.87, p < 0.01), but not the somatic dimension (ΔR2 = 0.034, ΔF(1,54) = 1.93, p > 0.15), predicted post-conflict RT adjustment scores during the negative feedback after adjusting for effects during the positive feedback. Finally, the second hierarchical linear regression analysis showed that BDI scores tended to explain unique variance in post-conflict RT adjustment scores during negative feedback even when removing variance associated with individual differences in STAI trait scores2, ΔR2 = 0.034, ΔF(1,54) = 2.89, p < 0.10.
Stroop Task: ANOVA Analyses
Congruence (Stroop) effects
For both RT and accuracy, the main effect of Condition (congruent trials vs. incongruent trials) was significant (RT: F1,32 = 108.94, p < 0.00; partial η2 = 0.77; accuracy: F1,32 = 37.44, p < 0.00; partial η2 = 0.54). Participants were quicker and more accurate during congruent than incongruent trials. Additionally, for RT, a Feedback x Condition interaction emerged, F1,32 = 7.20, p < 0.02, partial η2 = 0.18; this effect was not further explored since was irrelevant to the primary goals of the study.
Post-error adjustment (Laming/Rabbitt) effects
As expected, a main effect of Condition (post-error trials vs. post-correct trials) emerged due to slower (F1,32 = 12.61, p < 0.02; partial η2 = 0.30), but more accurate responses (F1,32 = 7.68, p = 0.10; partial η2 = 0.21) after incorrect than correct trials. No additional effects emerged for RT or accuracy scores.
Conflict-adaptation (Gratton) effects
When considering RT, a main effect of Condition (incongruent trials following congruent trials vs. incongruent trials following incongruent trials) emerged (F1,32 = 14.44, p < 0.02; partial η2 = 0.31); participants responded more slowly on incongruent trials following congruent trials than incongruent trials following incongruent trials. In addition, a reliable Group x Condition interaction indicated that the high and low BDI subjects differed in their Gratton effects irrespective of the feedback manipulation (F1,32 = 5.14, p < 0.04; partial η2 = 0.14). Post-hoc Newman-Keuls tests revealed that the low BDI (p < 0.01), but not high BDI (p > 0.50), subjects were faster for the incongruent following incongruent trials relative to the incongruent following congruent trials (Fig. 3). Accordingly, for the low BDI, but not high BDI, subjects the Gratton effect was significant. When considering accuracy, no significant ANOVA effects emerged.
Stroop Task: Regression Analyses
Congruence (Stroop) effects
For both RT (ΔR2 = 0.033, ΔF(1,54) = 1.82, p > 0.18) and accuracy (ΔR2 = 0.027, ΔF(1,54) = 1.53, p > 0.22), BDI scores did not predict the Stroop effect during negative feedback after adjusting for the Stroop effect during the positive feedback condition.
Post-error adjustment (Rabbitt) effects
No effects emerged for either RT (ΔR2 = 0.007, ΔF(1,54) = 0.35, p > 0.56) or accuracy (ΔR2 = 0.000, ΔF(1,54) = 0.00, p > 0.97).
Post-conflict adjustment RT effects
As for the Simon task, BDI scores explained unique variance in post-conflict RT effect scores during negative feedback after adjusting for positive feedback, ΔR2 = 0.093, ΔF(1,54) = 5.60, p < 0.03 (Fig. 2b). Likewise, the follow-up regression analyses indicated that the cognitive-affective BDI subscore (ΔR2 = 0.121, ΔF(1,54) = 7.60, p < 0.01), but not the somatic BDI subscore (ΔR2 = 0.010, ΔF(1,54) = 0.55, p > 0.46), uniquely predicted post-conflict RT adjustment scores during negative feedback. Further highlighting the specificity of these findings, a final regression analysis indicated that BDI scores explained unique variance in post-conflict RT adjustment scores during negative feedback even when considering individual differences in STAI trait scores3, ΔR2 = 0.076, ΔF(1,54) = 8.12, p < 0.01.
Discussion
Consistent with the cognitive conflict theory (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001), the present results provide direct empirical evidence for trial-to-trial behavioral adjustments during conflict trials as well as immediately following both conflict and error trials. Specifically, for both the Simon and Stroop tasks, participants showed significantly reduced accuracy and increased reaction time during incongruent relative to congruent trials. Similarly, during both tasks, participants were significantly more accurate and faster for incongruent trials following an incongruent trial compared to incongruent trials following a congruent trial, thus exhibiting reliable post-conflict accuracy and RT adjustments. Additionally, as expected, participants showed significant post-error adjustments, which were manifested in higher accuracy (Stroop task) and RT (Stroop and Simon task) scores after incorrect relative to correct trials. Collectively, these results indicate that the intended behavioral task adjustments were achieved using the present versions of the Simon and Stroop task. More generally, the current findings support the hypothesis that response conflict and errors act as a signal to allocate increased levels of cognitive control on subsequent trials, resulting in adaptive performance adjustments (Botvinick, Cohen, & Carter, 2004; Botvinick et al., 2001).
Motivated by growing evidence implicating dysfunctional executive functioning in depression, in particular after perceived failures or negative feedbacks (Elliott et al., 1996; Elliott et al., 1997; Murphy et al., 2003; Ruchsow et al., 2004; Tucker et al., 2003; Pizzagalli et al., 2006), the primary aim of the present study was to examine action monitoring processes in individuals with elevated levels of depressive symptoms in the presence of emotionally positive or negative feedback concerning task performance. Evidence pointing to dysfunctional action monitoring in high, but not low, BDI subjects emerged in various forms, particularly during the negative feedback condition.
In the Simon task, participants with elevated depressive symptoms showed significantly lower overall accuracy during negative than positive feedback, a pattern not seen in low BDI subjects. This effect was qualified by a significant Group x Condition (post-error trials vs. post-correct trials) x Feedback interaction, and further analyses revealed significant group differences in post-error behavioral adjustments (Laming/Rabbitt effect) for the negative, but not positive, feedback manipulation. Post-hoc analyses further clarified that high BDI subjects had lower post-error accuracy than low BDI subjects during the negative feedback condition. Highlighting the specificity of these findings, no differences emerged between the groups for trials following correct responses. Moreover, the two groups did not differ in their post-error adjustments during the positive feedback.4
In the Stroop task, evidence emerged indicating that high and low BDI subjects differed in their post-conflict adjustments regardless of the feedback manipulation. Unlike low BDI subjects, participants with elevated depressive symptoms failed to show significantly faster RT for incongruent trials following incongruent trials relative to incongruent trials following congruent trials. Accordingly, for the low BDI, but not high BDI, subjects the Gratton effect was significant. Complementing these findings, the regression analyses for both the Simon and Stroop tasks revealed that the more severe the depressive symptoms, the lower the subjects’ ability to adjust performance after high-conflict trials during emotionally negative task performance feedback. This effect was observed in relation to the cognitive-affective, but not somatic, dimension of the BDI. Moreover, this effect remained significant even after adjusting for individual differences in STAI trait scores, suggesting that it was not due to general distress or negative affect.
Overall, these results highlight impairments in behavioral adjustments after internal (perceived failure) and external feedback indicating deficient task performance in subjects with elevated depressive symptoms. The observed dysfunctions in action monitoring as shown by the Laming/Rabbit and Gratton effects contrasted with the lack of group differences in the congruence (Simon/Stroop) effect. Although this null finding was not hypothesized, it is possible that more global top-down cognitive control, which is captured by the congruence effect, is not affected in subjects with elevated depressive symptoms. Whether this pattern of findings will extend to a clinical sample remains an empirical question that should be investigated in future studies.
Critically, although the findings emerging from the Simon task highlight performance impairments in the high BDI subjects that were specific to trials following errors and the negative feedback manipulation, it is important to note that they could not be replicated in the Stroop task. The reasons for this discrepancy between the Simon and Stroop tasks are not entirely clear and represent one of the main limitations of the present study. One possibility is that the two tasks have dissimilar cognitive demands. Consistent with this speculation, a recent neuroimaging study found that the two tasks recruited common, but also unique, brain regions (Liu, Banich, Jacobson, & Tanabe, 2004). In general, whereas both tasks commonly recruited regions involved in attentional control (e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal cortex), the Simon task elicited stronger activation than the Stroop task in regions critically implicated in the detection of response conflict, response selection, and planning (e.g., anterior cingulate cortex). Future studies will be needed to test whether behavioral deficits emerging from the present study are restricted to experimental settings involving stimulus-response conflict, as in the Simon task, and/or whether a clinically depressed sample may be characterized by more general (i.e., task-independent) action monitoring dysfunctions.
Before discussing the present findings in further detail, some comments pertaining to the feedback manipulation utilized in this study are warranted. First, in an attempt to maximize the feedback effects on task performance, the analyses evaluating the effects of the feedback manipulation considered only trials from the first half of each block. This choice was motivated by prior findings suggesting that effects associated with experimentally induced affective states can degrade over time (Hemenover, 2003). Consistent with this view, the main findings of the present study involving the feedback manipulation could not be replicated when the entire block of trials was considered. Future studies assessing the effects of blocked, affectively laden feedback in depression might thus profit from evaluating task performance at different stages of the experiment. Second, unlike paradigms utilizing trial-to- trial feedback, it was not possible to examine the effect of trials immediately following positive or negative feedback. Moreover, it is possible that the blocked task performance feedback utilized in the present study resulted in a transient mood manipulation. Although the primary goal of the current study was to assess the sustained effects of blocked feedback on task performance and analyses indicated a successful feedback manipulation5, paradigms incorporating trial-by-trial feedback may reveal further information about behavioral adjustment in depression (e.g., Tucker et al., 2004; Ruschow et al., 2004).
Conflict Monitoring and Error Processing in Depression
Relative to healthy controls, patients with depression have been found to display dysfunctional conflict monitoring processes (George et al., 1997; Lemelin, Baruch, Vincent, Everett, & Vincent, 1997; Moritz et al., 2002) and abnormal reactions to negative feedback (Elliott et al., 1997; Elliott, Sahakian, Michael, Paykel, & Dolan, 1998). In addition, after committing an error, clinically depressed subjects as well as dysphoric subjects were more likely to make mistakes on subsequent trials relative to comparison subjects (Elliott et al., 1996; Pizzagalli et al., 2006). The present findings indicating that these action monitoring dysfunctions followed a negative feedback manipulation are intriguing, especially in light of cognitive theories of depression postulating that vulnerability to depression is associated with enduring negative cognitive schemata and processing biases (Clark, Beck, & Alford, 1999). Notably, decreased post-conflict behavioral adjustments were observed only with increasing cognitive-affective, but not somatic, depressive symptoms. Collectively, the present findings suggest that depressive symptoms are associated with difficulty adjusting behavior after internal (perceived failure) and external (overt) feedback about deficient task performance, particularly within the context of a psychosocial “stressor” indicating that participant’s performance was significantly lower than that of peers who had previously participated in the study. These findings are consistent with and extend prior findings indicating that depressed and dysphoric subjects, specifically those with dysfunctional cognitive attitudes, are characterized by amplification of the significance of failure (Wenzlaff & Grozier, 1988), difficulty suppressing failure-related thoughts (Conway et al., 1991), and increased depressed mood after negative social feedback (Henriques et al., 2002). Further, the effects of feedback observed in the behavioral tasks could help explain the variable nature of the behavioral findings in depression (Lemelin et al., 1997; Moritz et al., 2002; Paradiso et al., 1997; Austin et al., 1999; Degl’Innocenti, Agren, & Backman, 1998).
In terms of potential mechanisms, the conflict monitoring theory suggests that the occurrence of conflict and errors triggers the engagement of control processes, subserved by the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and prefrontal cortex (PFC) regions, resulting in adjustments in performance (Botvinick et al., 1999). Interestingly, feedback stimuli notifying participants of error responses have been shown to activate dorsal ACC regions similar to those implicated in conflict monitoring (Holroyd et al., 2004; Holroyd, Larsen, & Cohen, 2004). Of note, recent findings implicate the dorsal ACC in learning the predicted likelihood of an error, highlighting a more fundamental role of this region in negative reinforcement learning (Brown & Braver, 2005). It is important to note that functional and structural cingulate abnormalities have been repeatedly observed in patients with depression (Bench et al., 1992; Davidson, Pizzagalli, Nitschke, & Putnam, 2002; George et al., 1997). Accordingly, dysfunction within various ACC regions may be linked to impairments in the recruitment of additional cognitive control during high-conflict trials and following perceived failure (errors) or overt negative performance feedback. The present findings raise the possibility that, even in a non-clinical sample, adaptive affective-cognitive processes subserved by the ACC may be dysfunctional in subjects with elevated depressive symptoms, who have been found to be at-risk for later depressive disorders (Haavisto et al., 2004; Pine, Cohen, Cohen, & Brook, 1999).
Conclusion
In summary, the present findings confirm that individuals utilize errors and negative feedback to monitor their performance and adjust behavior accordingly. These results are consistent with the theory that response conflict and errors act as a signal to allocate increased levels of cognitive control on subsequent trials. In subjects with elevated depressive, particularly cognitive-affective symptoms, some of these behavioral adjustments were impaired. Specifically, compared to subjects with low BDI scores, subjects with elevated depressive symptoms showed decreased post-error and post-conflict performance, emerging mainly within the context of emotionally negative feedback relating to task performance. These findings indicate that depressive symptoms are associated with dysfunctional action monitoring after perceived failures or negative feedback. Whether these behavioral impairments are associated with dysfunctions in frontocingulate pathways subserving conflict monitoring, error processing, and feedback evaluation should be tested in future neuroimaging studies.
Acknowledgments
The authors wish to thank Allison Jahn, Avgusta Shestyuk, Pearl Chiu, Christen Deveney, James O’Shea, and Kyle Ratner for their contributions and assistance with various aspects of this research and Ryan Bogdan for invaluable comments on early versions of this manuscript. This research was supported by Ditmars Restricted Fund Research Grant, Allport Fund Research Grant, and Stimson Fund Research Grant, Harvard University, awarded to AJH, and by NIMH Research Grant R01MH68376 awarded to DAP.
Footnotes
Consistent with likely time-limited and decaying effects of the current feedback manipulation, exploratory analyses using all available trials failed to reveal reliable findings.
When considering the conflict-adaptation (Gratton) reaction time (RT) effect, a similar pattern of findings emerged from the three regression analyses (findings available upon request). No findings emerged when considering accuracy.
The significant findings emerging from these three regression analyses were confirmed when considering the conflict-adaptation (Gratton) reaction time (RT) effect. As with the Simon task, no reliable findings emerged with the accuracy scores.
Although there were no significant group differences in task performance following positive feedback, a close look at Fig. 1b suggests that high BDI subjects had higher post-error accuracy during the positive feedback compared to the negative feedback. An exploratory paired t-test confirmed that this difference was significant, t12 = 2.27, p < 0.05. Further, for the positive feedback manipulation, high BDI subjects showed significantly higher accuracy following error relative to correct responses, t12 = 3.03, p < 0.02. Thus, whereas the low BDI group significantly improved their performance following negative feedback, subjects with elevated depressive symptoms displayed more adaptive behavioral adjustments in the positive feedback condition. In light of the fact that (1) there were no significant group differences for the positive feedback condition; and (2) post-hoc tests were not warranted given the null ANOVA findings, future studies with larger sample size are needed to evaluate the reliability of this apparent dissociation.
In the present study, task performance was significantly faster during the negative relative to the positive feedback block, indicating that the feedback manipulation successfully elicited changes in performance.
Reference List
- Austin MP, Mitchell P, Goodwin GM. Cognitive deficits in depression: possible implications for functional neuropathology. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;178:200–206. doi: 10.1192/bjp.178.3.200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Austin MP, Mitchell P, Wilhelm K, Parker G, Hickie I, Brodaty H, et al. Cognitive function in depression: a distinct pattern of frontal impairment in melancholia? Psychological Medicine. 1999;29:73–85. doi: 10.1017/s0033291798007788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri W. Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1996;67:588–597. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck Depression Inventory Manual. 2 ed. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio: 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1961;4:53–63. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bench CJ, Friston KJ, Brown RG, Scott LC, Frackowiak RS, Dolan RJ. The anatomy of melancholia--focal abnormalities of cerebral blood flow in major depression. Psychological Medicine. 1992;22:607–615. doi: 10.1017/s003329170003806x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick M, Nystrom LE, Fissell K, Carter CS, Cohen JD. Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature. 1999;402:179–181. doi: 10.1038/46035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review. 2001;108:624–652. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.3.624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick MM, Cohen JD, Carter CS. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004;8:539–546. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown JW, Braver TS. Learned predictions of error likelihood in the anterior cingulate cortex. Science. 2005;307:1118–1121. doi: 10.1126/science.1105783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman LJ, Chapman JP. The measurement of handedness. Brain Cognition. 1987;6:175–183. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(87)90118-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark D, Beck AT, Alford B. Scientific Foundations of Cognitive Theory and Therapy of Depression. John Wiley and Sons; New York: 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Conway M, Howell A, Giannopoulos C. Dysphoria and thought suppression. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1991;15:153–166. [Google Scholar]
- Davidson RJ, Pizzagalli D, Nitschke JB, Putnam K. Depression: perspectives from affective neuroscience. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53:545–574. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Degl’Innocenti A, Agren H, Backman L. Executive deficits in major depression. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 1998;97:182–188. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1998.tb09985.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunkin JJ, Leuchter AF, Cook IA, Kasl-Godley JE, Abrams M, Rosenberg-Thompson S. Executive dysfunction predicts nonresponse to fluoxetine in major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2000;60:13–23. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(99)00157-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott R, Sahakian BJ, Herrod JJ, Robbins TW, Paykel ES. Abnormal response to negative feedback in unipolar depression: evidence for a diagnosis specific impairment. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry. 1997;63:74–82. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.63.1.74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott R, Sahakian BJ, McKay AP, Herrod JJ, Robbins TW, Paykel ES. Neuropsychological impairments in unipolar depression: the influence of perceived failure on subsequent performance. Psychological Medicine. 1996;26:975–989. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700035303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott R, Sahakian BJ, Michael A, Paykel ES, Dolan RJ. Abnormal neural response to feedback on planning and guessing tasks in patients with unipolar depression. Psychological Medicine. 1998;28:559–571. doi: 10.1017/s0033291798006709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gauggel S, Wietasch A, Bayer C, Rolko C. The impact of positive and negative feedback on reaction time in brain-damaged patients. Neuropsychology. 2000;14:125–133. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.1.125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- George MS, Ketter TA, Parekh PI, Rosinsky N, Ring HA, Pazzaglia PJ, et al. Blunted left cingulate activation in mood disorder subjects during a response interference task (the Stroop) Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 1997;9:55–63. doi: 10.1176/jnp.9.1.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gratton G, Coles MG, Donchin E. Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1992;121:480–506. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.121.4.480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haavisto A, Sourander A, Multimaki P, Parkkola K, Santalahti P, Helenius H, et al. Factors associated with depressive symptoms among 18-year-old boys: a prospective 10-year follow-up study. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2004;83:143–154. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2004.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hemenover SH. Individual differences in rate of affect change: studies in affective chronometry. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;85:121–131. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henriques G, Leitenberg H. An experimental analysis of the role of cognitive errors in the development of depressed mood following negative social feedback. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2002;26:245–260. [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd CB, Larsen JT, Cohen JD. Context dependence of the event-related brain potential associated with reward and punishment. Psychophysiology. 2004;41:245–253. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00152.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd CB, Nieuwenhuis S, Yeung N, Nystrom L, Mars RB, Coles MG, et al. Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex shows fMRI response to internal and external error signals. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:497–498. doi: 10.1038/nn1238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ishihara S. Test for color blindness. Kanehara Shuppan; Tokyo: 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Kendall PC, Hollon SD, Beck AT, Hammen CL. Issues and recommendations regarding use of the Beck Depression Inventory. Cognitive Therapy & Research. 1987;11:289–299. [Google Scholar]
- Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW, III, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science. 2004;303:1023–1026. doi: 10.1126/science.1089910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laming D. Autocorrelation of choice-reaction times. Acta Psychologica. 1979;43:381–412. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(79)90032-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lemelin S, Baruch P, Vincent A, Everett J, Vincent P. Distractibility and processing resource deficit in major depression. Evidence for two deficient attentional processing models. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders. 1997;185:542–548. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199709000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu X, Banich MT, Jacobson BL, Tanabe JL. Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI. Neuroimage. 2004;22:1097–1106. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.02.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayr U, Awh E, Laurey P. Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6:450–452. doi: 10.1038/nn1051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moritz S, Birkner C, Kloss M, Jahn H, Hand I, Haasen C, et al. Executive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder, unipolar depression, and schizophrenia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2002;17:477–483. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy FC, Michael A, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Neuropsychological impairment in patients with major depressive disorder: the effects of feedback on task performance. Psychological Medicine. 2003;33:455–467. doi: 10.1017/s0033291702007018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paradiso S, Lamberty GJ, Garvey MJ, Robinson RG. Cognitive impairment in the euthymic phase of chronic unipolar depression. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders. 1997;185:748–754. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199712000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pine DS, Cohen E, Cohen P, Brook J. Adolescent depressive symptoms as predictors of adult depression: moodiness or mood disorder? American Journal of Psychiatry. 1999;156:133–135. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.1.133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pizzagalli DA, Peccoralo LA, Davidson RJ, Cohen JD. Resting anterior cingulate activity and abnormal responses to errors in subjects with elevated depressive symptoms: A 128-channel EEG study. Human Brain Mapping. 2006;27:185–201. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porter RJ, Gallagher P, Thompson JM, Young AH. Neurocognitive impairment in drug-free patients with major depressive disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;182:214–220. doi: 10.1192/bjp.182.3.214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rabbitt PM. Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1966a;71:264–272. doi: 10.1037/h0022853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers MA, Kasai K, Koji M, Fukuda R, Iwanami A, Nakagome K, et al. Executive and prefrontal dysfunction in unipolar depression: a review of neuropsychological and imaging evidence. Neuroscience Research. 2004;50:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2004.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruchsow M, Herrnberger B, Wiesend C, Gron G, Spitzer M, Kiefer M. The effect of erroneous responses on response monitoring in patients with major depressive disorder: A study with event-related potentials. Psychophysiology. 2004;41:833–840. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00237.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegle GJ, Steinhauer SR, Thase ME. Pupillary assessment and computational modeling of the Stroop task in depression. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2004;52:63–76. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushere RE. Manual of the State- Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press; Palo Alto, CA: 1970. [Google Scholar]
- Trichard C, Martinot JL, Alagille M, Masure MC, Hardy P, Ginestet D, et al. Time course of prefrontal lobe dysfunction in severely depressed in-patients: a longitudinal neuropsychological study. Psychological Medicine. 1995;25:79–85. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700028105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tucker DM, Luu P, Frishkoff G, Quiring J, Poulsen C. Frontolimbic response to negative feedback in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112:667–678. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wenzlaff RM, Grozier SA. Depression and the magnification of failure. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1988b;97:90–93. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.97.1.90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]