Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Oct 21.
Published in final edited form as: J Affect Disord. 2010 Apr 14;126(1-2):272–277. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.03.004

Subregional hippocampal deformations in major depressive disorder

James Cole 1, Arthur W Toga 2, Cornelius Hojatkashani 2, Paul Thompson 2, Sergi G Costafreda 1, Anthony J Cleare 1, Steven CR Williams 1, Edward T Bullmore 3, Jan L Scott 4, Martina T Mitterschiffthaler 1, Nicholas D Walsh 5, Catherine Donaldson 1, Mubeena Mirza 2, Andre Marquand 1, Chiara Nosarti 1, Peter McGuffin 1, Cynthia HY Fu 1
PMCID: PMC3197834  NIHMSID: NIHMS190981  PMID: 20392498

Abstract

Background

Hippocampal atrophy is a well reported feature of major depressive disorder, although the evidence has been mixed. The present study sought to examine hippocampal volume and subregional morphology in patients with major depressive disorder, who were all medication-free and in an acute depressive episode of moderate severity.

Methods

Structural magnetic resonance imaging scans were acquired in 37 patients (mean age 42 years) and 37 age, gender and IQ-matched healthy individuals. Hippocampal volume and subregional structural differences were measured by manual tracings and identification of homologous surface points to the central core of each hippocampus.

Results

Both right (P = 0.001) and left (P = 0.005) hippocampal volumes were reduced in patients relative to healthy controls (n = 37 patients and n = 37 controls), while only the right hippocampus (p = 0.016) showed a reduced volume in a subgroup of first episode depression patients (n = 13) relative to healthy controls. Shape analysis localised the subregional deformations to the subiculum and CA1 subfield extending into the CA2-3 subfields predominantly in the tail regions in the right (p = 0.017) and left (p = 0.011) hippocampi.

Limitations

As all patients were in an acute depressive episode, effects associated with depressive state cannot be distinguished from trait effects.

Conclusions

Subregional hippocampal deficits are present early in the course of major depression. The deformations may reflect structural correlates underlying functional memory impairments and distinguish depression from other psychiatric disorders.

Keywords: depression, hippocampus, morphology, shape, MRI

Introduction

Reduced hippocampal volume is the most consistently observed structural abnormality in major depressive disorder (MDD) (reviewed in: Campbell et al., 2004; Videbech and Ravnkilde, 2004; McKinnon et al., 2009), with some conflicting reports (Rusch et al., 2001; Posener et al., 2003; Hastings et al., 2004; Vythilingam et al., 2004). Subregional deformations though may be discernible in the hippocampus, which are not captured by volumetric measures alone (Posener et al., 2003). Measures of regional brain structural morphology quantify the dimensions of the structure in its entirety as well as subregional features, providing an output of both the structural volume and morphological shape (Thompson et al., 2004). Analysis of hippocampal morphology have identified distinct and potentially characteristic subregional deformations in schizophrenia (Narr et al., 2004), late-life depression (Ballmaier et al., 2008) and adolescent bipolar disorder (Bearden et al., 2008).

In the present study, we sought to examine hippocampal volume and shape in mid-life depression. We applied manual tracing of each hippocampus for each subject and point-by-point statistical comparisons at each surface location (Thompson et al., 2004). In late-life depression, Ballmaier et al. (2008) observed significant hippocampal atrophy and localised deficits to the anterior CA1-CA3 regions and subiculum bilaterally. However, in mid-life depression, reports of hippocampal atrophy have been mixed (Rusch et al., 2001; Posener et al., 2003; Zou et al., 2010), perhaps reflecting a greater number of episodes or duration of illness in late-life depression (Campbell et al., 2004; Videbech and Ravnkilde, 2004; McKinnon et al., 2009). The present sample consisted of mid-life, adult MDD patients with a history of 2 or fewer episodes, all were suffering from an acute depressive episode and were medication-free (Costafreda et al., 2009). We expected to find similar deformations in the subiculum and adjacent hippocampal subfields, although perhaps less extensive than that observed in late-life depression.

Methods

Participants

Thirty-seven right-handed patients were recruited meeting DSM-IV criteria for major depressive disorder (APA, 1994) by Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) (First et al., 1995) and clinical interview with a consultant psychiatrist (Costafreda et al., 2009). Inclusion criteria were an acute episode of unipolar major depression and a minimum score of 18 on the 17-item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD) (Hamilton, 1960). Exclusion criteria were a history of neurological trauma resulting in loss of consciousness, current neurological disorder, current co-morbid Axis I disorder including bipolar disorder or an anxiety disorder, history of substance abuse within 2 months of study participation, and a lifetime history of substance dependence. All patients were medication-free for a minimum of 2 weeks prior to the scan (4 weeks if the previous treatment had been fluoxetine). Eight patients had received previous antidepressant medications, which included citalopram, fluoxetine and paroxetine. Thirty-seven age, sex, and IQ matched, right-handed healthy controls were recruited with no history of psychiatric disorder, neurological disorder, or head injury resulting in a loss of consciousness and an HRSD score < 7 (Table 1). All subjects were recruited by advertisement from the local community, and all patients were outpatients. All participants provided written informed consent to participate in this study in accordance with the guidelines of the Institute of Psychiatry and South London and Maudsley NHS Trust Ethics (Research) Committee.

Table 1.

Demographic features and hippocampal volumetric data

Healthy Controls Depressed Patients

All Patients First Episode Recurrent
Number of subjects 37 37 13 24
Mean age (years) 42.2 (9.0) 41.9 (8.9) 38.1 (7.9) 44.0 (9.4)
Sex (male/female) 9/28 9/28 2/11 7/17
HRSD 0.2 (0.6) 20.7 (2.2)* 21.5 (2.7)* 20.2 (1.7)*
Verbal IQ 114.5 (13.2) 108.7 (17.1) 103.1 (20.3) 111.6 (15.0)
L hippocampus 1.98 (0.29) 1.77 (0.26)* 1.81 (0.29) 1.75 (0.24)*
R hippocampus 2.08 (0.28) 1.86 (0.22)* 1.85 (0.21)* 1.84 (0.24)*
Intracranial volume 1501.57 (137.73) 1459.45 (125.30) 1464.24 (105.54) 1456.86 (136.91)

Data presented as mean and standard deviation in parenthesis. Volume measures are in mL units. HRSD = Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; L = left, R = right.

*

Indicates data showing a significant difference from healthy controls

Image acquisition

Structural MRI brain scans were acquired using a 1.5 T GE NV/i Signa system (General Electric, Milwaukee WI, USA) at the Maudsley Hospital, London. Head movement was limited by foam padding within the head coil and a restraining band across the forehead. 3D spoiled gradient recalled (SPGR) T1-weighted scans were obtained with the acquisition parameters: TE = 8, TR = 24ms, flip angle = 30°, field of view = 25cm × 25cm, slice thickness = 1.3 mm, number of partitions (slices) = 124, image matrix = 256 × 256 × 124, voxel size = 0.97 mm × 0.97 mm × 1.3 mm. Image requisition parameters are consistent with most MRI acquisition protocols that have been used for defining the hippocampus (reviewed in: Konrad et al, 2009) and is a standard resolution for many structural studies (http://www.loni.ucla.edu/ADNI/Research/Cores/). Image contrast for datasets was chosen with the aid of a software tool for optimising image contrast (Simmons et al, 1996).

Image pre-processing and analysis

Pre-processing steps consisted of removal of non-cortical tissue, linear alignment to standard space, and reslicing into the anterior-posterior orientation. Each hippocampus was outlined manually in MultiTracer (Woods, 2003) by a trained investigator (JC) blind to diagnosis. Inter and intra-rater reliability (intra-class correlation coefficient = 0.91) was calculated based on established protocols (Narr et al., 2004; Thompson et al., 2004). Outlines were traced in the coronal orientation, from anterior to posterior along contiguous image slices, and included all hippocampal grey matter including the dentate gyrus and subiculum. Volumetric measures were calculated from the centre of the first slice to the centre of the last, 1 mm sampling along the axis, with the square root of areas varying linearly from slice to slice. The sum of these areas generated the hippocampal volume. Intra-cranial volume (ICV) was calculated with the Brain Extraction Tool (BET) (Smith, 2002) which removes all non-brain matter from MR images. The original unprocessed images were BET-segmented, then manually checked to ensure accurate identification of grey matter, white matter and cerebrospinal fluid. Measures of volume, in mm3, were generated from the extracted images. Subgroup analyses of hippocampal volume were also conducted with the patient group divided into patients in their first episode of depression (n = 13) and those with recurrent episodes of depression (n = 24) (Table 1).

Morphological analysis

Surface mesh modelling analysis was performed as described in Thompson et al. (2004). Briefly, parametric surface meshes were computed from the manually-derived hippocampal outlines. Surface meshes were combined to form a mean shape representing each group in three dimensions. In each individual, the medial core, a central 3D curve threading down the long axis of the structure, was computed. From each point on the hippocampal surface, a radial distance measure was derived to the medial core. As the same surface grid was imposed on all subjects’ hippocampi in the same coordinate space, statistical comparisons were made at each hippocampal surface point between the groups to index contrasts on a local scale. Probability values from these statistical comparisons were mapped onto an average hippocampal shape for the entire sample to generate a 3D representation of the structural differences between the groups. Permutation testing was performed in order to correct for the 30,000 simultaneous comparisons made for each hippocampus. Each subject was randomly assigned to a diagnostic group 150,000 times with a statistical threshold of p < 0.05, and the area of the hippocampus with suprathreshold statistics in each group comparison was compared with its null distribution, thus deriving an overall corrected significance value for the pattern of effects for each hippocampus

Results

There was an expected significant difference in HRSD ratings between healthy controls and patients (F = 1611.34, df = 2, 71, P < 0.001), in which post-hoc Tukey’s test showed a significant difference between healthy controls and patients with recurrent episodes of depression (p < 0.001), healthy controls and patients in their first episode (p < 0.001), and a trend towards significance between patients with recurrent episodes and those in their first episode (p = 0.055).

There was no significant difference in intracranial volume (ICV) between healthy controls and patients (F = 0.95, df = 2, 71, P = 0.39). Left hippocampal volume showed a significant difference between patients and healthy controls (F = 5.64, df = 2, 71, P = 0.005), and post-hoc Tukey’s test revealed a significant difference between healthy controls and patients with recurrent episodes of depression (p = 0.005), but not between healthy controls and patients in their first episode or between the patient subgroups. Right hippocampal volume also showed a significant difference between patients and healthy controls (F = 7.58, df = 2, 71, P = 0.001), and post-hoc Tukey’s test revealed significant differences between healthy controls and both patient subgroups: patients with recurrent episodes of depression (p = 0.003) and patients in their first episode (p = 0.016), but there were no differences between the patient subgroups. There were no significant correlations with right or left hippocampal volume and the number of previous episodes, duration of illness, or severity of illness as measured by the HRSD score. There were no significant differences between males and females in terms of age, IQ, right or left hippocampal volumes, or corrected hippocampal volumes, all p-values > 0.3.

Morphological analysis revealed distinct hippocampal deformations in the subiculum and CA1 extending into the CA2-3 subfields predominantly in the tail regions in both the left (corrected p = 0.011) and right (corrected p = 0.017) hippocampi in patients relative to healthy controls (Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Morphological analysis of bilateral hippocampi in major depressive disorder. Top panel presents a schematic figure of the hippocampi in-situ. In the middle panel, regional subfield deformations are depicted on hippocampal surface maps. Smaller p-values reflect greater medial distance reductions in patients with depression (n = 37) relative to healthy controls (n = 37). Reference shapes are presented in the lower panel depicting the hippocampal subfields based on the subfield definitions used by Bearden et al. (2008). Hippocampi are presented left to right in the following order: Right hippocampus, Superior view; Left hippocampus, Superior view; Left hippocampus, Inferior view; Right hippocampus, Inferior view. CA = cornu ammonis.

Discussion

We found significant bilateral hippocampal atrophy in medication-free patients with major depression of a moderate severity. Morphological analysis localised the atrophy to distinct subregions within the hippocampi. Marked deformations were evident in the subiculum and CA1 subfield extending into the CA2-3 subfields largely in the tail regions of both hippocampi in depressed patients relative to healthy controls. The findings indicate that subregional hippocampal deficits are not confined to late-life depression (Ballmaier et al., 2008), but are evident early in the illness.

The hippocampal deformations observed from MRI scans are supported by neuropathological findings (Rosoklija et al., 2000). Neumister et al. (2004) reported decreased hippocampal volume in its whole and more pronounced in the posterior region, and Maller et al. (2007) found localised volume reductions in the most posterior region of the tail of the hippocampus in patients with treatment-resistant depression. Posener et al. (2003) noted specific abnormalities in the subiculum in patients with similar demographic features to those in the present study, but half the patient group was receiving psychiatric medications. In the present study, all patients were medication-free, right-handed, and had 2 or fewer episodes of depression. In late-life depression, Ballmaier et al. (2008) observed extensive morphological abnormalities in the subiculum and CA1 subregions which extended into the CA2-3 subfields. In the late-life depression group, those patients with a comparable age of onset to our sample had an average of 5 previous episodes of depression at the time of their scan (Ballmaier et al., 2008). Our findings revealed that the main hippocampal body was relatively intact while deformations were evident particularly in the tail region within the subiculum and CA1 subfield but as well in the CA2-3 subfields. Together, these data suggest that the subregional hippocampal abnormalities present at early stages in the illness may become more extensive with recurrent episodes, contributing to the hippocampal atrophy that is particularly evident in recurrent and treatment-resistant depression (reviewed in: Campbell et al., 2004; Videbech and Ravnkilde, 2004; McKinnon et al., 2009).

The posterior portion of the hippocampus is engaged by memory retrieval (Lepage et al., 1998), and the subiculum has been specifically implicated in the retrieval of episodic memories (Gabrieli et al., 1997; Zeineh et al., 2003; Eldridge et al., 2005). Memory impairments are associated with hippocampal atrophy in depression (Hickie et al., 2005). In particular, deficits in episodic memory are a commonly reported feature (Golinkoff and Sweeney, 1989; Ilsley et al., 2005), which may be greater in the retrieval, as opposed to the encoding, aspects of the memory trace (Fossati et al., 2002). These deficits are not evident in the first episode of depression, but become more prominent in recurrent depression (Fossati et al., 2004). A main component of autobiographical memory is the recall of episodes in one’s life (Brewer, 1986). It is widely recognised that individuals with depression have difficulty in recalling specific autobiographical events (Williams and Broadbent, 1986; Williams and Scott, 1988), instead they tend to produce an overgeneralised memory, which may be more pronounced with negative incidents (Williams and Scott, 1988).

Although we observed a right lateralised deficit in hippocampal volume in first-episode patients, bilateral hippocampal atrophy has been reported in medication-naïve, first-episode patients with depression (Zhou et al.. 2010), in medication-free patients with recurrent depression (Neumister et al., 2004), and a recent meta-analysis indicates a 4% aggregate loss in hippocampal volume in depression with no significant lateralisation effects (McKinnon et al., 2009). A left lateralised reduction in hippocampal volume has been noted in first-episode male patients (Frodl et al., 2002) and in late-life depression of a mean age of onset of 35 years (Ballmaier et al., 2008). The reason for the right lateralised effect that we have observed is unclear, but we did not find any significant correlations with right or left hippocampal volume and the number of previous episodes, duration of illness, or severity of illness.

However, hippocampal atrophy is not specific to depression as it has been observed in other psychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia (Sumich et al., 2002), obsessive-compulsive disorder (Atmaca et al., 2008), and borderline personality disorder (Driessen et al., 2000). The present study may help to distinguish between hippocampal abnormalities found in depression and other psychiatric disorders. For example, both depression and schizophrenia have been associated with global hippocampal volume reductions (Sumich et al., 2002). Morphological analysis revealed localised deformations in the anterior regions in schizophrenia (Narr et al., 2004), while we found greater alterations in posterior tail regions. Furthermore, in schizophrenia the deficits were generally bilateral, more extensive, and clearly evident in patients in their first-episode (Narr et al., 2004). In depression, the hippocampal deformations were more circumscribed in mid-life depression and volumetric atrophy was only found in the right hippocampus in first-episode depression. This dissociation between psychiatric disorders may have important aetiological and diagnostic implications, and the specificity of the morphological changes will require further investigation.

A caveat to the interpretation of morphological analysis though is limited anatomical accuracy. Hence, local deficits identified with shape mapping and MRI would be characterized with greater precision by neuropathological examination. Furthermore, the present study did not address possible causative mechanisms, such as hippocampal apoptosis and decreased neurogenesis, which may be driven by hypercortisolemia or diminished neurotrophin levels (Czeh and Lucassen, 2007). Posener et al. (2003) examined hippocampal morphology by delineating the boundaries of the hippocampus based on specific landmarks, reporting no difference in hippocampal volume but deficits in the subiculum region. The method though consisted of a limited number of anatomical landmarks to define hippocampal morphology which may have minimised gross and local irregularities. Moreover, we did not observe a correlation between hippocampal volume and the number of depressive episodes. The present group though consisted of patients with none or few previous episodes of depression, which may have limited the observation of any correlations as they may become evident in a larger sample with a greater range of episodes.

In summary, hippocampal deformations localised to the subiculum and CA1 subregion extending into the CA2-3 subregions were observed in patients with major depression. Subregional hippocampal deformations may distinguish depression from other psychiatric disorders.

Acknowledgments

We thank all the patients who participated in the study and the radiographers for their assistance in data collection.

Role of Funding Source This work was funded by a National Alliance for Research in Schizophrenia and Depression Young Investigator Award (CF) and the National Institutes of Health through the National Center for Research Resources (P41 RR13642) and the NIH Roadmap for Medical Research, Grant U54 RR021813. Information on the National Centers for Biomedical Computing can be obtained from http://nihroadmap.nih.gov/bioinformatics. The Funders had no further role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Footnotes

Contributors Author CF designed the study and wrote the protocol. Authors SC, JC, CH, and PT undertook the statistical analysis, and author JC wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4. APA; Washington, DC: 1994. DSM-IV. [Google Scholar]
  2. Atmaca M, Yildirim H, Ozdemir H, Ozler S, Kara B, Ozler Z, et al. Hippocampus and amygdalar volumes in patients with refractory obsessive-compulsive disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharm Biol Psychiatry. 2008;32:1283–1286. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2008.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ballmaier M, Narr KL, Toga AW, Elderkin-Thompson V, Thompson PM, Hamilton L, et al. Hippocampal morphology and distinguishing late-onset from early-onset elderly depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:229–237. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bearden CE, Soares JC, Klunder AD, Nicoletti M, Dierschke N, Hayashi KM, et al. Three-dimensional mapping of hippocampal anatomy in adolescents with bipolar disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47:515–525. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31816765ab. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brewer WF. What is autobiographical memory? In: Rubin D, editor. Autobiographical memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1986. pp. 25–49. [Google Scholar]
  6. Campbell S, Marriott M, Nahmias C, MacQueen GM. Lower hippocampal volume in patients suffering from depression: a meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:598–607. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.4.598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Costafreda SG, Chu C, Ashburner J, Fu CHY. Prognostic and diagnostic potential of the structural neuroanatomy of depression. PLOS One. 2009;4:e6353. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Czeh B, Lucassen PJ. What causes the hippocampal volume decrease in depression? Are neurogenesis, glial changes and apoptosis implicated? Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2007;257:250–260. doi: 10.1007/s00406-007-0728-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Driessen M, Herrmann J, Stahl K, Zwaan M, Meier S, Hill A, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging volumes of the hippocampus and the amygdala in women with borderline personality disorder and early traumatization. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:1115–1122. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.12.1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Eldridge LL, Engel SA, Zeineh MM, Bookheimer SY, Knowlton BJ. A dissociation of encoding and retrieval processes in the human hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2005;25:3280–3286. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3420-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Fossati P, Harvey PO, Le Bastard G, Ergis AM, Jouvent R, Allilaire JF. Verbal memory performance of patients with a first depressive episode and patients with unipolar and bipolar recurrent depression. J Psychiatr Res. 2004;38:137–144. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2003.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders. New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  13. Fossati P, Coyette F, Ergis AM, Allilaire JF. Influence of age and executive functioning on verbal memory of inpatients with depression. J Affect Disord. 2002;68:261–271. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00362-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Frodl T, Meisenzahl EM, Zetzsche T, Born C, Groll C, Jäger M, et al. Hippocampal Changes in Patients With a First Episode of Major Depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:1112–1118. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.7.1112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gabrieli JD, Brewer JB, Desmond JE, Glover GH. Separate neural bases of two fundamental memory processes in the human medial temporal lobe. Science. 1997;276:264–266. doi: 10.1126/science.276.5310.264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Golinkoff M, Sweeney JA. Cognitive impairments in depression. J Affect Disord. 1989;17:105–112. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(89)90032-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;23:56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hastings RS, Parsey RV, Oquendo MA, Arango V, Mann JJ. Volumetric analysis of the prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and hippocampus in major depression. Neuropsychopharm. 2004;29:952–959. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hickie I, Naismith S, Ward PB, Turner K, Scott E, Mitchell P, et al. Reduced hippocampal volumes and memory loss in patients with early- and late-onset depression. Br J Psychiatry. 2005;186:197–202. doi: 10.1192/bjp.186.3.197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ilsley JE, Moffoot AP, O’Carroll RE. An analysis of memory dysfunction in major depression. J Affect Disord. 1995;35:1–9. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(95)00032-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Konrad C, Ukas T, Nebel C, Arolt V, Toga AW, Narr KL. Defining the human hippocampus in cerebral magnetic resonance images: An overview of current segmentation protocols. NeuroImage. 2009;47:1185–1195. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Lepage M, Habib R, Tulving E. Hippocampal PET activations of memory encoding and retrieval: the HIPER model. Hippocampus. 1998;8:313–322. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:4<313::AID-HIPO1>3.0.CO;2-I. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Maller JJ, Daskalakis ZJ, Fitzgerald PB. Hippocampal volumetrics in depression: the importance of the posterior tail. Hippocampus. 2007;17:1023–1027. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. McKinnon MC, Yucel K, Nazarov A, MacQueen GM. A meta-analysis examining clinical predictors of hippocampal volume in patients with major depressive disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2009;34:41–54. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Narr KL, Thompson PM, Szeszko P, Robinson D, Jang S, Woods RP, et al. Regional specificity of hippocampal volume reductions in first-episode schizophrenia. Neuroimage. 2004;21:1563–1575. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Neumeister A, Wood S, Bonne O, Nugent AC, Luckenbaugh DA, Young T, et al. Reduced hippocampal volume in unmedicated, remitted patients with major depression versus control subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:935–937. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Posener JA, Wang L, Price JL, Gado MH, Province MA, Miller MI, et al. High-dimensional mapping of the hippocampus in depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:83–89. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Rosoklija G, Toomayan G, Ellis SP, Keilp J, Mann JJ, Latov N, et al. Structural abnormalities of subicular dendrites in subjects with schizophrenia and mood disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:349–356. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.4.349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Rusch BD, Abercrombie HC, Oakes TR, Schaefer SM, Davidson RJ. Hippocampal morphometry in depressed patients and control subjects: relations to anxiety symptoms. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:960–964. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01248-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Simmons A, Arridge SR, Barker GJ, Williams SCR. Simulation of MRI cluster plots and application to neurological segmentation. Magn Res Imaging. 1996;14:73–92. doi: 10.1016/0730-725x(95)02040-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Smith SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp. 2002;17:143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Sumich A, Chitnis XA, Fannon DG, O’Ceallaigh S, Doku VC, et al. Temporal lobe abnormalities in first-episode psychosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:1232–1235. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.7.1232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Thompson PM, Hayashi KM, de Zubicaray G, Janke AL, Rose SE, Semple J, et al. Mapping Hippocampal and Ventricular Change in Alzheimer’s Disease. Neuroimage. 2004;22:1754–1766. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Videbech P, Ravnkilde B. Hippocampal volume and depression: a meta-analysis of MRI studies. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1957–1966. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.11.1957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Vythilingam M, Vermetten E, Anderson GM, Luckenbaugh D, Anderson ER, Snow J, et al. Hippocampal volume, memory, and cortisol status in major depressive disorder: effects of treatment. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56:101–112. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Williams JMG, Broadbent K. Autobiographical memory in suicide attempters. J Abn Psychol. 1986;95:144–149. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.95.2.144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Williams JMG, Scott J. Autobiographical memory in depression. Psychological Med. 1988;18:689–654. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700008370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Woods RP. Multitracer: a Java-based tool for anatomic delineation of grayscale volumetric images. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1829–1834. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00243-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Zeineh MM, Engel SA, Thompson PM, Bookheimer SY. Dynamics of the hippocampus during encoding and retrieval of face-name pairs. Science. 2003;299:577–580. doi: 10.1126/science.1077775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Zou K, Deng W, Li T, Zhang B, Jiang L, Huang C, Sun X, Sun X. Changes of brain morphometry in first-episode, drug-naïve, non–late-life adult patients with major depression: an optimized voxel-based morphometry study. Biol Psych. 2010;67:186–188. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES