Abstract
The role of working memory (WM) in sentence comprehension has received considerable interest, but little work has investigated how sentence production relies on memory mechanisms. These three experiments investigated speakers’ tendency to produce syntactic structures that allow for early production of material that is accessible in memory. In Experiment 1, speakers produced accessible information early less often when under a verbal WM load than when under no load. Experiment 2 found the same pattern for given-new ordering, i.e., when accessibility was manipulated by making information given. Experiment 3 addressed the possibility that these effects do not reflect WM mechanisms but rather increased task difficulty by relying on the distinction between verbal and spatial WM: Speakers’ tendency to produce sentences respecting given-new ordering was reduced more by a verbal than by a spatial WM load. These patterns show that accessibility effects do in fact reflect accessibility in verbal WM, and that representations in sentence production are vulnerable to interference from other information in memory.
Keywords: language production, grammatical encoding, working memory, accessibility effects, given-new ordering, dative alternation, similarity-based interference
A fundamental difficulty with producing language is that non-linear conceptual information must be expressed as a linear order of words. This transformation happens at the level of grammatical encoding, where selection and retrieval of content words interfaces with structure building processes. By most models of language production, grammatical encoding involves two sub-processes: content processing involves mapping the semantic aspects of a to-be-expressed message onto syntactically based lexical representations (termed lemmas), which then point to metrical and phonological information (word forms or lexemes). Structure processing involves mapping the relational aspects of the message (i.e., who did what to whom) onto grammatical functions (e.g., subject and direct object) and the sequential ordering of constituents (for further detail on this consensus model of grammatical encoding, see Bock, 1995; V. Ferreira & Slevc, 2007; Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999). Because content retrieval proceeds relatively independently of structural processing and because lexical retrieval is a variable process, there will often be a discrepancy between the order in which lexical content is retrieved and the order in which that content must be produced (Bock, 1982). Consequently, speakers often must temporarily maintain lemmas and word forms in working memory (WM) until they can be grammatically produced.
WM is generally assumed to be a limited capacity system, so there is pressure on speakers to minimize demand on WM by not maintaining to-be-produced lexical representations any longer than necessary. Thus speakers could benefit by choosing structures that allow them to produce retrieved lexical representations as soon as they are grammatically able, which motivates the otherwise puzzling phenomenon that languages often offer more than one way to say the same thing (V. Ferreira, 1996). For example, imagine that a speaker wanted to convey information about a pirate-agent, a monk-patient, and a book being given (setting aside for now exactly why someone would want to convey this information). If the speaker retrieved “book” before “monk,” she might benefit by using a prepositional dative structure (The pirate gave the book to the monk) whereas if she retrieved “monk” before “book,” she might benefit by using a double-object dative structure (The pirate gave the monk the book).
In fact, speakers do tend to choose structures allowing for earlier production of easier-to-retrieve information. Evidence for these accessibility effects comes from experiments showing that speakers tend to mis-recall sentences as having syntactic structures where more accessible items occur early (Bock & Irwin, 1980; Davidson, Zacks, & F. Ferreira, 2003; V. Ferreira & Yoshita, 2003). This is true when the items are easier to access both conceptually and lexically, but other work has shown that conceptual accessibility alone can affect syntactic choice. For example, speakers tend to choose syntactic structures allowing for early production of information that is more imageable (Bock & Warren, 1985), animate (McDonald, Bock, & Kelly, 1993), or that is semantically primed (Bock, 1986). The few studies that have directly addressed the effect of formal accessibility on speakers’ choice of syntactic structures have found either little or no effect (Bock, 1987; Levelt & Maassen, 1981; McDonald et al., 1993), suggesting that form-based accessibility is not a very relevant factor at the point when a speaker must commit to a particular syntactic structure. Thus the accessibility of message level information and of lemmas appears to influence syntactic choice, but the accessibility of phonological word forms does not.
Of course, accessibility is only one of many factors contributing to variations in syntactic structure (see, e.g., Bresnan, Cueni, Nikitina, & Baayen, 2007). Another important factor in syntactic choice is the information structure of a sentence (e.g., Halliday, 1970). Most relevant to the current discussion, things that are given (exact definitions vary, but given information uncontroversially includes what has been previously mentioned in the discourse) tend to be mentioned earlier than things that are new, a phenomenon termed given-new ordering (Bock, 1977; Chafe, 1976; Gundel, 1988; Halliday, 1970; Smyth, Prideaux, & Hogan, 1979). Given information tends to also be relatively accessible, and so given-new ordering can, at least in some circumstances, result from the same factors that lead to accessibility effects (Arnold, Wasow, Losongco, & Ginstrom, 2000; Bock & Irwin, 1980; Braingan, McLean, & Reeve, 2003).1
As suggested above, a plausible explanation for these accessibility effects (and at least some instances of given-new ordering) is that they result from pressures on WM. However there is, as of yet, no evidence that actually shows this to be the case. In fact, there is relatively little work investigating the role of WM in any aspect of language production. There is a small body of research showing that WM processes are involved in lexical access (Belke, 2008; Daneman, 1991; Daneman & Green, 1986; see also Kellogg, 2004 for evidence from written production), and other work linking short-term memory processes (including WM) to the scope of planning in production (R. Martin & Freedman, 2001; R. Martin, Miller, & Vu, 2004; Wagner, Jescheniak, & Schriefers, 2010), to syntactic agreement processes (Badecker & Kuminiak, 2007; Bock & Cutting, 1992; Hartsuiker & Barkhuysen, 2006), and to aspects of discourse processing and audience design (Arnold, 2010; Arnold & Griffin, 2007; Horton & Gerrig, 2005a; 2005b; Roßnagel, 2000, 2004).
The goal of the present experiments is to investigate if and how accessibility effects reflect WM processes. All experiments share the same basic design, in which participants described dative-eliciting pictures after either the theme (the direct object) or the goal argument (the indirect object) was made accessible or given. More specifically, participants main task was to describe pictures (e.g., of a pirate, a monk, and a book being given) from a poster on the wall. Participants knew which picture to describe on each trial because the experimenter first asked a question about an agent (e.g., What’s going on with the pirate?), thus limiting responses to a subset of pictures containing that agent, and the participant was given a cue, which was the theme or goal argument (book or monk). This uniquely identified a single picture that the participant then described. The presentation of the cue word was also intended to increase the accessibility of one of the post-verbal NPs, and so participants were expected to show accessibility effects by tending to produce structures where the cued item was produced relatively early. To determine how accessibility effects depend on WM, participants performed this task as described and also while under a concurrent verbal WM load.
One way to think of accessibility effects is as a metaphorical “release valve” for WM. By this account, when a speaker is describing a picture of a pirate, a monk, and a book being given and the theme concept and lemma (book) are active in WM by virtue of having been previously seen or mentioned, the speaker can reduce demand on her limited-capacity WM system by producing the theme relatively early in a prepositional dative structure (The pirate is giving the book to the monk) rather than maintaining the theme in WM for relatively longer in order to produce a double-object dative structure (The pirate is giving the monk the book). (It should be noted that this is presumably an unconscious effect, not a conscious strategy.) This account fits with models of WM as a limited capacity workspace or buffer (e.g., Baddeley, 1986; Jackendoff, 2002; Just & Carpenter, 1992; R. Martin, Lesch, & Bartha, 1999) in which the accessible information is produced early to free WM capacity that could otherwise be devoted to other processes. This logic is similar to the idea that the recency effect in list recall reflects the utility of quickly producing those items still active in short-term or primary memory (Davelaar, Goshen-Gottstein, Ashkenazi, Haarmann, & Usher, 2005; Glanzer & Cunitz, 1966, but see Crowder, 1982, 1993). Accessibility effects should thus be particularly helpful when WM resources are low, so a concurrent load on verbal WM should lead to an increase in accessibility effects.
A different way to conceptualize accessibility effects comes from work looking at the intrinsic properties of information such as imageability or animacy that make that information easy to retrieve (e.g., Bock & Warren, 1985; McDonald et al., 1993). These findings suggest that accessibility effects might reflect ease of lexical retrieval by WM mechanisms rather than reflecting a pressure to free WM capacity. A focus on retrieval fits with recent models of WM that conceptualize WM as having an extremely limited capacity for active maintenance in focused attention (perhaps even as limited as one-item; McElree, 2001) but an important role in managing the retrieval of information from a set of activated (non-focused) information in long term memory (LTM) (e.g., Cowan, 2001; Lewis, 1996; Oberauer, 2002; Unsworth & Engle, 2007). By these accounts, the activated set of LTM is not capacity limited per se; instead, access to this non-focused information is subject to retrieval interference from other active cue-relevant information. Given this conception of WM, accessibility effects reflect the utility of quickly producing easily retrieved information that could otherwise be subject to interference at retrieval.
According to this retrieval-based account of accessibility effects, maintaining a concurrent verbal WM load places additional potentially interfering information in the activated subset of LTM (or perhaps even in the focus of attention as a verbal load is being rehearsed). Thus when a speaker is describing a picture (again, of a pirate agent, a monk patient, and a book being given) and one of the post-verbal arguments (monk or book) is easier to retrieve by virtue of having been previously seen or mentioned, she should tend to choose a structure allowing that information to be produced early. However, when the speaker is also maintaining a verbal WM load, the accessibility of the previously seen or mentioned item (monk or book) is likely to be moderated by interference from this WM load, thus she should show a lesser tendency to produce that item early. That is, a verbal WM load should lead to a reduction in accessibility effects if the relevant WM limitation is interference, but should show an increase in accessibility effects if the relevant limitation is capacity.
There are at least two additional possibilities. In light of arguments that syntactic parsing relies on a syntax-specific form of WM (a claim most strongly associated with Caplan and Waters, 1999), speakers’ choices between syntactic alternatives in production might also rely on syntax-specific WM. If so, a concurrent verbal WM load should have no effect on accessibility effects. A quite different claim comes from arguments that many purportedly WM-based effects actually reflect very general processing constraints such as the ability to actively maintain task goals and/or to control attention (e.g., Kane et al., 2004; McCabe, Roediger, McDaniel, Balota, & Hambrick, 2010; Unsworth & Spillers, 2010; Vergauwe, Barrouillet, & Camos, 2010). These arguments suggest that a verbal WM load might affect accessibility effects not because of WM processes per se, but rather because of general attentional demands. Thus this account predicts that any type of concurrent cognitive load (even a non-verbal load) would interact with accessibility effects.
Experiment 1
The goal of Experiment 1 was to test the idea that accessibility effects on syntactic choice result from WM processes. To this end, participants described dative-eliciting pictures where either the name of the theme or goal constituent was made accessible, and did so with or without a concurrent WM-load. More specifically, participants’ primary task was to describe pictures from a poster on the wall (e.g., of a pirate-agent, a monk-patient, and a book being given). On each trial, participants heard a question that indicated the agent in the to-be-described picture (What’s going on with the pirate?) and then saw a visually presented cue on a computer screen that indicated another piece of information in the picture (here, book or monk). Together these pieces of information uniquely identified one picture and the visual cue additionally increased the accessibility of the theme (if the cue was book) or the goal (if the cue was monk). On WM load trials, participants read two unrelated words at the beginning of the trial and had to recall those words after producing their sentence.
Method
Participants
49 students at UCSD participated in exchange for class credit. Data were excluded from one participant because of an equipment malfunction. All participants reported English as their native language.
Materials and design
Experimental materials for the dative-eliciting task consisted of 24 pictures of dative actions with dative verbs printed underneath. Twelve dative verbs were used, each with two pictures. An additional 12 pictures of transitive actions with transitive verbs printed underneath were used as filler items. These 36 pictures were printed on an approximately 3.5 by 3.5 foot poster in six rows of six pictures each. The pictures in each row had the same agent, but different themes and goals (and patients, in the case of the two fillers in each row). Half of the pictures in each row were oriented with the subject on the right, and half with the subject on the left. Two additional filler pictures were printed on a separate sheet of paper and used for examples and practice during the instructions.
Each picture was paired with a cue word (the name of the theme or goal argument), and both with and without a WM load of two unrelated words. This led to four versions of each item, which were counterbalanced across four lists such that each item appeared an equal number of times in each condition across the experiment, and such that each participant was presented with each item only once.
Procedure
The experiment was administered with PsyScope 1.2.5 (Cohen, MacWhinney, Flatt, & Provost, 1993). Participants sat facing the computer screen, and the experimenter sat beside the participant such that the computer screen was obscured from the experimenter’s view by a cardboard divider. Both the participants and the experimenter could easily see the poster of line-drawn pictures.
Each experimental session lasted about 30 minutes, beginning with instructions and administration of two practice items, which were not used in the experimental trials. In the experimental trials, participants described dative-eliciting pictures from a poster on the wall (e.g., of a pirate giving a monk a book) in response to spoken questions and visually presented cues. Specifically, on each trial the experimenter would ask a question about the agent in a subset of the pictures (e.g., What’s going on with the pirate?). Participants would then read a word corresponding to one of the post-verbal NPs in a specific picture (e.g., BOOK or MONK) on the computer screen (which was not visible to the experimenter), thereby making that NP more accessible and also allowing participants to find the appropriate picture to describe (i.e., the picture that includes both the subject from the question and the object named on the computer screen). Participants then described the appropriate picture to the experimenter by saying a sentence that started with the same noun phrase that the experimenter used in the question (participants were asked to avoid using pronouns) and included the verb printed under the picture. On half of the trials, participants were first presented with a WM load of two unrelated words in boldfaced, blue, 14-point font, which they were instructed to remember and recall aloud in any order when prompted at the end of the trial. Trials occurred in a fixed pseudo-random order, constrained such that no more than two consecutive questions were about the same agent and no more than three consecutive trials were of the same WM load condition.
Analysis
Participants’ sentences from the picture-description task were transcribed and coded as prepositional datives or double-object datives. Trials in which participants did not produce a dative sentence, did not produce a full subject noun phrase, did not produce both post-verbal noun phrases, or did not produce the appropriate verb were excluded from the analysis, causing 9.7% of all critical trials to be excluded (10.2% of trials in the WM load condition and 7.8% of trials in the no load condition). Additionally, trials in which participants did not correctly recall at least one word of the WM-load were excluded, eliminating another 2.4% of trials in the WM load condition.
In this and in all experiments reported here, data were analyzed using orthogonal contrast coding in logistic generalized linear mixed-effects models as implemented in the lme4 package (Bates & Maechler, 2009) in the statistical software R (version 2.9.1; R Development Core Team, 2009). Cue-type (theme or goal) and WM-load were included as fixed effects and random intercepts were included for both participants and items, however random effects are not reported as only the fixed effects tests were of interest. Parameter estimates were calculated with maximum likelihood modeling using Laplace approximations, and the statistical significance of individual fixed-effect estimates were determined with the Wald z statistic. For readability and for purposes of graphical presentation, values are described as proportions rather than as log-odds ratios.
Results and discussion
Figure 1 shows the proportion of prepositional datives produced as a function of cue-type (theme or goal) and WM-load (none vs. two words). Participants were numerically more likely to produce prepositional datives following theme than a goal cues, but this main effect reached only marginal significance (b = 0.37, SE = 0.20, z = 1.82, p < .10). There was no main effect of WM-load (b = −0.09, SE = 0.10, z = −0.46), however cue-type and WM-load interacted significantly (b = −0.80, SE = 0.40, z = −2.00, p < .05). As can be seen in Figure 1, this interaction reflects participants’ tendencies to produce the cued NP early only when not under a concurrent WM-load: participants showed an 8% accessibility effect in the no-load condition (shown by a significant simple main effect of cue-type within the no-load conditions: b = 0.76, SE = 0.28, z = 2.73, p < .01), but no effect in the concurrent 2-word load condition (a non-significant −1% simple main effect: b = −0.03, SE = 0.28, z = −0.12).
Figure 1.
Proportion of prepositional dative (PD) sentences produced in Experiment 1 as a function of cue-type (i.e., which post-verbal argument was made accessible: theme or goal) and WM load (none or 2-word load). Note that the Y-axis does not start at zero and error bars represent standard errors.
One might have expected more accurate WM load recall when the cued NP was produced early (i.e., when early production presumably led to reduced interference during retrieval of the load words), however there was no effect of constituent order on WM load accuracy: b = 0.73, SE = 1.02, z = 0.71), reflecting participants very accurate recall of load words both when the cued NP was produced early (98.8% correct) and when the cued NP was produced late (97.6% correct).
These data show an interaction between accessibility effects and verbal WM load, and thus provide clear evidence that accessibility effects rely on WM. Based on the motivation of accessibility effects as a WM ‘release valve’ that eases production, it is surprising that accessibility effects disappeared when under WM load when production was presumably most difficult. Instead, this pattern suggests that accessibility effects are susceptible to interference from other items in memory, which is consistent with models of WM that posit only a very limited ability for active maintenance plus an interference-susceptible cue-based retrieval process (e.g., Cowen, 2001; Lewis, 1996; Unsworth & Engle, 2007). That is, the WM load presumably led to increased interference during lexical retrieval, thus reducing or eliminating the differential accessibility of the cued item. Experiment 2 aimed to replicate this effect using a more ecologically valid form of accessibility; specifically, manipulating whether information was given (by virtue of having been previously mentioned) or new.
Experiment 2
As mentioned in the introduction, speakers not only tend to mention more accessible information early, but also tend to order given information before new information. Given-new ordering is, by some accounts, a manifestation of audience design; that is, speakers use given-new ordering in order to facilitate comprehension by their listener (e.g., Ariel, 1991; Clark & Haviland, 1977). Others argue that given-new ordering is not necessarily based on a cognitive model of the listeners’ knowledge, but occurs simply because information that is given (by, for example, previous mention or perceptual salience) is often also relatively more accessible than new information (Bock & Irwin, 1980; Branigan, McLean, & Reeve, 2003). In either case, when something is given it is likely to also be highly accessible. Based on the results of Experiment 1, this predicts that speakers’ tendency to produce given-new ordering should also be reduced when under a WM load.
Method
Participants
48 students at UCSD participated in exchange for class credit. All participants reported English as their native language
Materials, design, and procedure
The experimental design and materials were identical to those used in Experiment 1. The procedure was similar to that of Experiment 1, with one major exception: instead of manipulating the accessibility of a post-verbal NP by showing that word on a computer screen that was privileged to the participant, one post-verbal argument was made given by the experimenter. Specifically, participants described pictures (e.g., of a pirate giving a book to a monk), in response to questions asked by the experimenter, who manipulated the given argument by mentioning one of the post-verbal NPs (underlined in the examples below; contrast sentences (1) and (2).)
What’s going on with the pirate and the book?
What’s going on with the pirate and the monk?
The experimenter sat facing away from the poster (unlike in Experiment 1) so that the pictures themselves would not be construed as being given information, and the given argument was always referred to with the definite determiner the, which is consistently related to givenness (Grieve, 1973). On WM load trials, the experimenter first said aloud two unrelated words for participants to maintain throughout the question and picture description. Finally, participants read instructions from a sheet of paper instead of from the computer screen.
Results and Discussion
Following the same criteria used in Experiment 1, trials in which participants did not produce a dative sentence, did not produce both post-verbal noun phrases, or did not produce the appropriate verb were excluded (accounting for 5.9% of trials in each WM-load condition), as were trials in which participants did not recall at least one word of the WM-load (resulting in exclusion of an additional 8.3% of trials in the WM load condition).
Figure 2 shows the proportion of prepositional datives produced as a function of WM-load (none vs. two words) and cue-givenness (theme or goal). As can be seen, results were similar to those of Experiment 1 but with bigger differences overall. A main effect of cue-givenness (b = 1.62, SE = 0.21, z = 7.72, p < .001) reflected participants’ tendency to produce given information before new information. There was no main effect of WM load (b = −0.10, SE = 0.20, z = −0.50), but importantly, the effect of cue-givenness was reduced when participants were under a concurrent WM-load, as shown by a significant cue-givenness by WM-load interaction (b = −0.84, SE = 0.41, z = −2.07, p < .05).
Figure 2.
Proportion of prepositional dative (PD) sentences produced in Experiment 2 as a function of cue-givenness (i.e., which post-verbal argument was given information: theme or goal) and WM load (no load or 2-word load). Note that the Y-axis does not start at zero and error bars represent standard errors.
These results show that a verbal WM load influences given-new ordering in essentially the same way that it influences accessibility effects (in Experiment 1): by leading to a reduction in speakers’ tendency to produce given information early. This pattern is as predicted by retrieval models of WM, but is inconsistent with syntax-specific or workspace models of WM. A statistical comparison of Experiments 1 and 2 using cue-manipulation (experiment) as a between-subjects factor (i.e., whether cues were made accessible or made given) revealed only one effect involving cue-manipulation: an interaction with cue-type (b = 1.16, SE = 0.29, z = 4.04, p < .001). This shows that making a cue given had a greater effect on constituent ordering than did making a cue accessible (a 16.9% effect of cue-givenness in Experiment 2 vs. a 3.4% effect of cue-accessibility in Experiment 1), but the way in which a WM load affected accessibility effects and given-new ordering did not differ. That cue-manipulation was not involved in any other effects or interactions suggests that the manipulation of givenness used in Experiment 2 was essentially acting as a strong manipulation of accessibility.
Unlike in Experiment 1, participants’ memory for the WM load in Experiment 2 was affected by constituent order: Participants accurately recalled at least one of the two words on every trial where they had produced a sentence with given information before new, but only on 81.6% of the trials where they produced new information before given (χ2 = 106.5, p < .001).2 This provides further support for the idea that choosing a syntactic structure that allows for early mention of accessible information can, in fact, reduce demand on WM. When speakers were able to produce the cued item early, this presumably allowed for active maintenance of other items and also prevented that cued item from interfering with the retrieval of the WM load words. When the cued item was not produced early, it was presumably either maintained for a longer time in the small window of active maintenance or had to be retrieved for later production; in either case, the cue word had more opportunity to interfere with retrieval or maintenance of the WM load.
In addition, these results suggest that the modality of WM load presentation was not important in these experiments as the auditorally-presented WM load in Experiment 2 produced the same pattern of effects as did the visually-presented WM load in Experiment 1.
Experiment 3
Although Experiments 1 and 2 found a WM load to reduce speakers’ use of accessibility effects and given-new ordering, it is possible that these results arise not from a load on WM per se but simply because the experimental task is more complicated or demanding on the WM load trials. For example, perhaps participants were less affected by manipulations of accessibility and givenness in the WM load conditions simply because the load trials effectively imposed a dual task (cf. Pashler, 1994; see also Caplan & Waters, 1999, for a similar alternative explanation applied to other studies). This explanation fits with evidence that WM capacity at least partially reflects the ability to control attention (e.g., Kane et al., 2004; Unsworth & Spillers, 2010), and suggests that general attentional control (not interference in verbal WM per se) might underlie these syntactic production processes.
There is, however, a straightforward way to address this possibility. WM theorists have pointed to an important distinction between verbal and spatial WM (e.g., Baddeley, 1986; Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Friedman & Miyake, 2000; Shah & Miyake, 1996, but see Vergauwe et al., 2010) and most work suggesting a role of WM in syntactic processing has, at least implicitly, claimed that language processing involves verbal but not spatial WM (e.g., Fedorenko, Gibson, & Rohde, 2007; Jackendoff, 2002; although note that some aspects of language processing may be inherently spatial, e.g., Chatterjee, Southwood, & Basilico, 1999; Richardson, Spivey, Barsalou, & McRae, 2003).3 If verbal and spatial WM are separate subsystems, then an external load on verbal WM should affect accessibility effects, but a load on spatial WM should not. Frameworks that define WM in terms of processes that act on long-term memory representations and that point to WM’s susceptibility to interference (Cowan, 1999, 2005; Lewis, 1996; McElree, 2001; Unsworth & Engle, 2007) make the same prediction, although for different reasons. Specifically, these models claim that the same system is used for both phonological and spatial maintenance, however because interference occurs between representations that are similar, a load that is quite different from the primary task materials should not interfere with that task (see also Guérard & Tremblay, 2008).
Experiment 3 had two parts. The first part of Experiment 3 was identical to Experiment 2 except that the no-load trials were replaced with spatial load trials. The second part required participants to perform a spatial task (mental rotation) under the same two WM load conditions (verbal or spatial). The spatial load was a pattern memory task matched in difficulty to the verbal load, as determined in a previous norming experiment (reported in the method section below). If the effects of accessibility and givenness shown in Experiments 1 and 2 resulted from verbal WM processes, then participants in Experiment 3 should show a reduced tendency to produce given-new ordering effects in the verbal, but not in the spatial WM load condition. The opposite pattern is predicted for the mental rotation task, in which the spatial WM load should impair performance relative to the verbal load. However, if these effects reflected the relative complexity of the load trials rather than the memory load per se, then there should be no effect of the type of WM load (on either the sentence production or the spatial task) as both types of WM load should lead to a reduction in attentional control.
Method
Participants
55 students at UCSD participated in exchange for class credit. Data were excluded from six participants who did not produce analyzable sentences on a majority of trials, and from one participant because of an experimenter error. Data were also excluded from nine additional participants who showed particularly poor performance on spatial WM load recall (these participants recalled at least part of a WM load on fewer than 40% of the spatial load trials).
Materials and design
The main part of Experiment 3 used the same experimental design and materials as used in the previous experiments. In addition, a spatial load and a spatial task (letter-rotation) were created, and thirteen additional verbal load words were selected and digitally recorded.
Spatial WM load
The spatial WM load was based on Ichikawa’s (1981, 1983) dot-in-matrix patterns (see also Miyake, Friedman, Rettinger, Shah, & Hegarty, 2001). These patterns are five-by-five grids with dots randomly assigned to spaces in the grid. Dot configurations that were duplicates or that formed systematic patterns were eliminated (Ichikawa, 1981; Miyake et al., 2001) and these patterns were presented for 750 ms on a computer screen. This duration was chosen following Miyake et al. (2001), who found 750 ms to be a brief enough presentation to discourage verbal or other idiosyncratic coding strategies (e.g., “two over, three down”) while still allowing enough exposure to encode the pattern into spatial WM.
In order to determine the appropriate difficulty of this dot memory task, a norming experiment was conducted with a separate group of ten participants. On each trial of this norming task, participants either saw a verbal (two-word) or a spatial (dot-in-matrix pattern) memory load, then saw a sentence on the computer screen, which they were instructed to say aloud. The sentence disappeared as soon as the participants started to speak (triggered by a head-worn microphone connected to a PsyScope button box), thus participants had to process the sentences sufficiently to produce them from memory. Finally, participants either said aloud the verbal load, or marked the location of the dots on an answer sheet containing several five-by-five matrix grids. The verbal load trials used the same 18 two-word verbal load stimuli used in Experiments 1 and 2, and the spatial load trials were 48 dot-in-matrix patterns containing two, three, four, or five dots (12 patterns of each difficulty). The 66 sentences were mostly intransitive or predicate-adjective structures. Two lists were created that randomly paired sentences with load stimuli, with the constraint that the verbal load stimuli were not paired with sentences to which they shared any obvious semantic or phonological relationship. Trial order was randomly ordered within each list.
On average, participants accurately recalled both words on 47.2% of the verbal-load trials and accurately recalled the dot pattern on 52% of the two-dot trials, 25% of the three-dot trials, 8% of the four-dot trials, and 7% of the five-dot trials. Matched-pairs t-tests revealed that performance on the verbal load and on the two-dot spatial load did not differ significantly (t(9) = 0.53), whereas performance on the verbal load was significantly better than performance on the three-dot (t(9) = −3.71, p < .005), four-dot (t(9) = −6.87, p < .001), and five-dot (t(9) = −5.96, p < .005) spatial load trials. Because these data suggest that a two-dot pattern is equivalently difficult to the two-word verbal load, 34 unique two-dot patterns were created (as above) to serve as the spatial load in Experiment 3.
Letter rotation task
The spatial processing task was a letter rotation task adapted from one used in Shah and Miyake (1996).4 On each trial, participants saw a series of two to five capital letters (from the set of F, J, L, P, or R) that were either normal or mirror-imaged, and that were presented in a non-canonical orientation (either rotated 45°, 90°, 135°, 180°, 225°, 270°, or 315° from upright). Participants’ task was to press the right/green button on the button-box to indicate if the letter was “normal” or the left/red button if the letter was “mirror imaged” as quickly and accurately as possible. There was a practice block of 30 upright letters (three normal and three mirrored versions of each of the six letters, presented in random order), then a second practice block with three trials of two rotated letters per trial. There were 24 experimental trials, in four blocks of six trials each, which progressed from two to five letters per trial.
Procedure
Experiment 3 consisted of two parts. The procedure of the first part was identical to that of Experiment 2 except that the no-load trials were replaced with trials requiring maintenance of a spatial WM load. Thus Experiment 3 manipulated givenness (theme or goal) and WM task (verbal or spatial). On each spatial-load trial, participants first pressed a button and saw a five-by-five grid with dots in two of the squares for 750 ms on the computer screen. The experimenter then asked a question (e.g., What’s going on with the pirate and the book?), to which the participant responded by describing the relevant picture from the poster on the wall, and then indicated the two dot locations on a five-by-five grid printed on a paper answer sheet. The procedure for the verbal load trials was the same, except that the experimenter read aloud the two unrelated load words, which the participant recalled aloud at the end of the trial (just as on the load trials in Experiment 2).
In the second part of Experiment 3, participants judged whether letters at different rotations were normal or mirror imaged while maintaining either a verbal or a spatial WM load (just as in the sentence production task). A practice block familiarized participants with the normal/mirrored distinction, and a second practice block familiarized participants with the rotation task while under verbal and spatial loads (with one verbal-load, and two spatial-load practice trials). On each trial of the second practice block and of the experimental blocks, participants either saw a two-dot pattern for 750 ms or heard two unrelated words through a speaker next to the computer, then made normal/mirror judgments for two, three, four, or five rotated letters, then recalled aloud the verbal load or marked the spatial load dot locations on an answer grid.
Analysis, results and discussion
Sentence production task
As in the previous experiments, participants’ sentences from the picture-description task were transcribed and coded as prepositional datives or double-object datives. Trials in which participants did not produce a dative sentence or did not produce both post-verbal noun phrases were excluded from the analysis, causing 8.1% of all critical trials to be excluded (9.0% of trials in the verbal-load condition and 7.3% of trials in the spatial-load condition). Trials on which participants did not correctly remember at least one word of the verbal load or one dot location of the spatial load were also excluded, leading to the exclusion of an additional 13.2% of all critical trials (6.8% of verbal load trials and 19.4% of spatial load trials).
Figure 3 shows the proportion of prepositional datives produced as a function of cue-givenness (theme or goal) and WM-task (verbal vs. spatial). A significant effect of cue-givenness reflected participants’ tendency to produce given information before new information (b = 2.92, SE = 0.33, z = 8.97, p < .001). Importantly, this effect interacted with WM-task (b = −1.29, SE = 0.57, z = −2.28, p < .05), reflecting the reduced use of given-new ordering when under verbal load (when participants produced prepositional datives 20% more often following theme than goal mention) compared to when under spatial load (when participants produced prepositional datives 31% more often following theme than goal mention).
Figure 3.
Proportion of prepositional dative (PD) sentences produced in Experiment 3 as a function of cue-type (i.e., which post-verbal argument was given information: theme or goal) and type of WM-task (spatial load vs. verbal load). Note that the Y-axis does not start at zero and error bars represent standard errors.
As in Experiment 2, participants’ were more accurate at recalling the WM load when they produced given-new ordering (recalling at least one word or dot on every given-new trial) than when they produced new-given ordering (recalling at least one word or dot on only 75% of new-given trials; χ2 = 216.19, p < .001). This was expected for the verbal load because not producing given information early presumably results in a greater chance for that information to interfere with retrieval of the WM load words. Unexpectedly, spatial WM load accuracy was also worse on trials where participants produced new-given ordering (i.e., the effect of ordering on accuracy did not interact with load-type; χ2 < 1). One might imagine that trials where speakers produced new-given ordering were trials where production was relatively more difficult and time consuming, and this took attention away from maintenance of the spatial WM load. But if given-new ordering results from verbal WM processes, then it is not clear why production of given-new ordering would reduce demand on domain-general attentional control. And if given-new ordering reflects the general ability to control attention, it is then mysterious that the spatial WM load did not appear to affect given-new ordering. Of course, without a no-load condition in Experiment 3, it is possible that the spatial load did subtly influence given-new ordering, leaving open the possibility that both verbal WM and a more general form of cognitive control play a role in accessibility effects (cf. Swets, Desmit, Hambrick, & Ferreira, 2007; Unsworth & Spillers, 2010).
Letter rotation task
Trials in which participants did not correctly remember the WM load were excluded from the analysis according to the same criteria as in the sentence production task, leading to the exclusion of 15.2% of all trials. Participants seem to have found the spatial load more difficult than the verbal load as this criteria led to the exclusion of 30.3% of trials in the spatial-load condition but no trials in the verbal load condition. Additionally, reaction times (RT) deviating more than two standard deviations from each participant’s mean RT were excluded from the analysis, leading to the exclusion of an additional 3.0% of all trials (3.4% of trials in the spatial load condition and 2.6% of trials in the verbal load condition). Reaction times were analyzed with a linear mixed-effects model treating subject as a random effect and WM-task as a fixed effect. Statistical significance was assessed with Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) sampling as implemented in the languageR package (Baayen, 2008).
Although the accuracy of letter rotation judgments did not differ as a function of load, reaction times did: participants were about 70 ms slower to make letter rotation judgments when under a spatial WM load than when under a verbal WM load, as shown by a significant effect of WM-task (b = −65.53 ms, SE = 21.61 ms, t = −3.03, p < .01).
Experiment 3 provides evidence that given-new ordering depends on verbal WM because a concurrent load on verbal WM led to decreased use of given-new ordering compared to a load on spatial WM. This shows that the effect of a verbal WM load on syntactic production found here and in the previous experiments is not attributable to the dual-task nature of the load trials, nor to the overall increased difficulty in the WM load trials. If anything, the spatial WM load seems to have been more difficult overall, as evidenced by how often participants were unable to remember the spatial load relative to the verbal load. Furthermore, the opposite pattern was found in a letter-rotation task that requires spatial processing, namely that the spatial WM load led to worse performance than the verbal WM load. This both shows that the spatial load was difficult enough to influence processing, and also adds further support to the distinction between verbal and spatial WM processes. There was an effect of chosen word order on spatial load recall, which shows that general attentional control mechanisms may play some role in accessibility effects (cf. Swets et al., 2007), however the dissociation between effects of verbal and spatial WM load on production of given-new ordering suggests that verbal WM is the primary cognitive mechanism underlying accessibility effects and given-new ordering.
General Discussion
These experiments show that accessibility effects interact with manipulations of WM load, thereby confirming the suggestion that accessibility effects can result from a pressure to avoid maintaining otherwise ready-to-produce material in WM (Bock, 1982; V. Ferreira, 1996). It should be noted that the manipulations of accessibility used here were of memory-based accessibility; future work should examine if other forms of accessibility and givenness (e.g., accessibility due to animacy (McDonald et al., 1993) or givenness due to definite reference (Grieve, 1973)) also interact with manipulations of verbal WM. These data also support the idea that given-new ordering can arise as a function of the accessibility of given information in speakers’ verbal WM (Arnold et al., 2000; Bock & Irwin, 1980; Branigan et al., 2003).
These findings have a number of implications for the role of WM in language production. First, the type of WM underlying accessibility effects is not specific to syntactic production. It is still possible that syntactic aspects of production rely on a dedicated WM system as accessibility effects influence syntactic choice somewhat indirectly (i.e., by influencing the mapping of lexical content onto syntax). Nevertheless, these results, along with work showing retrieval based interference in other aspects of production (in particular, in agreement production: Badecker & Kuminiak, 2007), suggest that sentence production does not rely on a dedicated pool of memory resources as has been claimed to be the case for syntactic parsing (e.g., Caplan & Waters, 1999).
Instead, syntactic choice was influenced by non-syntactic pressure on WM from the maintenance of a concurrent verbal WM load. Interestingly, the way that a verbal WM load influenced syntactic choice was by leading to a reduction, rather than an increase, in accessibility effects and given-new ordering. This is contrary to the motivation of accessibility effects as a “release valve” for WM. By that account, a WM load should have co-existed in a limited capacity WM workspace along with the cued NP (i.e., the made-accessible information). Thus a concurrent WM load should have added pressure on WM, which would increasing the utility of freeing capacity by producing buffered information as early as possible (in this case, by choosing a syntactic structure allowing for early mention of the cued NP).
Instead, these data showed that a verbal WM load lead to a reduction in accessibility effects. This pattern suggests that the crucial limiting factor in WM is interference rather than capacity limits, and fits with models in which WM consists of a system of limited attentional focus with a maximum capacity of only 1 to 4 items plus a cue-based search mechanism for items displaced from this attentional focus (e.g., Cowan, 2001; Lewis, 1996; McElree, 2001; Unsworth & Engle, 2007). Accessibility effects, then, emerge when lexical items (i.e., lemmas) that are part of this limited attentional focus can be produced early, thereby freeing up attentional resources for other purposes. However, when a WM load displaces information from focus, that displaced lexical information must be re-retrieved at which point it is subject to interference both from items currently in focus as well as from other cue-relevant information (cf. Badecker & Kuminiak, 2007; Fedorenko, Gibson, & Rohde, 2006; Gordon, Hendrick, & Levine, 2002; Lewis & Vasishth, 2005; A. Martin & McElree, 2009; Van Dyke, 2007; Wagers, Lau, & Phillips, 2009). The fact that effects of givenness on syntactic choice were greater than effects of accessibility emerge simply from the assumption that givenness exerts a stronger pull on the focus of attention than does accessibility alone.
Still, the role WM plays in sentence production (at least as far as accessibility effects are concerned) is not so general of a mechanism as attentional control (e.g., Cowan et al., 2005; Kane et al., 2004; McCabe et al., 2010; Vergauwe et al., 2010). This conclusion comes from evidence that while a verbal WM load led to a reduction in given-new ordering, a load on spatial WM did not. In fact, the given-new ordering effects in the spatial WM load condition of Experiment 3 were numerically the largest effects across all experiments. This contrast between the verbal and spatial WM load also addresses an alternative account for many other demonstrations of domain-general WM effects on syntactic processing, namely that the load condition imposes a dual-task and this task switching actually leads to the effects purported to be WM-related (see Caplan & Waters, 1999, for critiques of this sort).
The discussion thus far has assumed that the verbal WM load interfered with either the maintenance or retrieval of the accessible lexical item, that is, that the WM load either made it difficult to keep information sufficiently active to warrant early mention or led to increased interference at the point of retrieving that otherwise accessible item. However, a plausible alternative is that the WM load interfered with the encoding of the accessible item. By this account, the WM load caused the cued information to be less thoroughly processed, causing that information to sometimes be insufficiently accessible to warrant early mention. The data presented here cannot distinguish between these accounts because, in these experiments, the WM load always preceded the manipulation of accessibility. However, under either account, grammatical encoding processes depend on verbal WM mechanisms, therefore this distinction does not detract from the main conclusions that accessibility effects and given-new ordering result from constraints on general processes of interference in verbal WM.
An additional point deserves mention, namely that speakers overwhelmingly preferred to use prepositional dative structures in these data (81% of all dative sentences produced in these three experiments were prepositional datives). This is somewhat surprising because data from corpora of naturally produced language suggest that prepositional datives and double-object datives are about equally common in English (e.g., Zhong, Stent, & Swift, 2006). While this preponderance of prepositional datives was unexpected, there is no reason to assume it is related to the interaction between accessibility effects and WM load, thus it should not detract from the main pattern of results.
Conclusions
Because speaking requires the production of non-linear conceptual information as a linear order of words, speakers often must maintain information that is otherwise ready to be produced. The experiments reported here show that this process is susceptible to general processes of interference in verbal WM: Speakers help reduce the potential for interference in WM by relying on the flexibility afforded by the grammar and choosing syntactic structures that allow information to be produced as it becomes accessible. The fact that accessibility effects and given-new ordering were reduced when under verbal WM load shows that WM processes in sentence production are vulnerable to similarity-based interference. This supports retrieval models of WM that point to limitations due not to capacity (as in, e.g., Baddeley, 1986; Ruchkin, Grafman, Cameron, & Berndt, 2003) but rather due to interference (Cowan, 2001, 2005; Lewis, 1996; Lewis, Vasishth, & Van Dyke, 2006; McElree, 2001; Oberauer, 2002; Unsworth & Engle, 2007). This claim also fits with evidence that similarity based interference is critical in other aspects of syntactic production (Badecker & Kuminiak, 2007) and parsing (e.g., Gordon et al., 2002; Wagers et al., 2009).
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by NIH grants HD-051030 and F32 DC-008723 and has benefited from the helpful comments and advice of many people, including Kay Bock, Jeremy Boyd, Tamar Gollan, Liane Wardlow Lane, and especially Vic Ferreira. Additionally, I thank Stephanie Ang, Serina Chang, Ruby Cho, Katie Doyle, Paige Heath, Ramsey Khouri, Yanny Siu, and Neil Spears for assistance with data collection.
Footnotes
This distinction between given and new is similar to the division between theme and rheme and between focus and presupposition (Jackendoff, 1972), all of which are often grouped together under the broader terms topic and comment. While at least some of these concepts are dissociable (e.g., Halliday, 1994), information that is given, theme, focused, and/or topic is likely to also be relatively accessible.
Because of participants’ perfect accuracy in the given-new ordering conditions of Experiments 2 and 3, it is not possible to estimate significance based on a Wald z test. Thus these analyses of accuracy data are instead reported with likelihood ratio tests based on single degree of freedom model comparisons (Agresti, 2002). Although this test may be anti-conservative (Pinheiro & Bates, 2000) it was deemed acceptable in these situations as the accuracy differences in these experiments are not particularly subtle.
This is also likely to be quite different for signed languages (cf. Wilson & Emmorey, 1997, 2003).
Thanks to Naomi Friedman for helpful advice regarding this task.
Publisher's Disclaimer: The following manuscript is the final accepted manuscript. It has not been subjected to the final copyediting, fact-checking, and proofreading required for formal publication. It is not the definitive, publisher-authenticated version. The American Psychological Association and its Council of Editors disclaim any responsibility or liabilities for errors or omissions of this manuscript version, any version derived from this manuscript by NIH, or other third parties. The published version is available at www.apa.org/pubs/journals/xlm
References
- Agresti A. Categorical Data Analysis. 2. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Ariel M. The function of accessibility in a theory of grammar. Journal of Pragmatics. 1991;16:443–463. [Google Scholar]
- Arnold JE. How speakers refer: The role of accessibility. Language and Linguistics Compass. 2010;4:187–203. [Google Scholar]
- Arnold JE, Griffin ZM. The effect of additional characters on choice of referring expression: Everyone counts. Journal of Memory & Language. 2007;56:521–536. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnold JE, Wasow T, Losongco A, Ginstrom R. Heavyness vs. newness: The effects of structural complexity and discourse status on constituent ordering. Language. 2000;76:28–55. [Google Scholar]
- Baayen RH. R package version 0.953. 2008. languageR: Data sets and functions with “Analyzing Linguistic Data: A practical introduction to statistics”. [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley AD. Working Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ. Working Memory. In: Bower GA, editor. Recent Advances in Learning and Motivation. Vol. 8. New York: Academic Press; 1974. pp. 47–90. [Google Scholar]
- Badecker W, Kuminiak F. Morphology, agreement and working memory retrieval in sentence production: Evidence from gender and case in Slovak. Journal of Memory & Language. 2007;56:65–85. [Google Scholar]
- Bates DM, Maechler M. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375–31. 2009 URL: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.
- Belke E. Effects of working memory load on lexical-semantic encoding in language production. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2008;15:357–363. doi: 10.3758/pbr.15.2.357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bock JK. The effect of a pragmatic presupposition on syntactic structure in question answering. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior. 1977;16:723–734. [Google Scholar]
- Bock JK. Toward a cognitive psychology of syntax: Information processing contributions to sentence formulation. Psychological Review. 1982;89:1–47. [Google Scholar]
- Bock JK. Meaning, sound, and syntax: Lexical priming in sentence production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition. 1986;12:575–586. [Google Scholar]
- Bock JK. An effect of the accessibility of word forms on sentence structures. Journal of Memory & Language. 1987;26:119–137. [Google Scholar]
- Bock JK. Sentence production: from mind to mouth. In: Miller JL, Eilmas PD, editors. Handbook of Perception and Cognition, Vol. 11: Speech, Language, and Communication. Orlando, FL: Academic Press; 1995. pp. 181–216. [Google Scholar]
- Bock JK, Cutting JC. Regulating mental energy: Performance units in language production. Journal of Memory & Language. 1992;31:99–127. [Google Scholar]
- Bock JK, Irwin DE. Syntactic effects of information availability in sentence production. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior. 1980;19:467–484. [Google Scholar]
- Bock JK, Warren RK. Conceptual accessibility and syntactic structure in sentence formulation. Cognition. 1985;21:47–67. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90023-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Branigan HP, McLean JF, Reeve HM. Something old, something new: Addressee knowledge and the given-new contract. Proceedings of the 25th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society.2003. [Google Scholar]
- Bresnan J, Cueni A, Nikitina T, Baayen RH. Predicting the dative alternation. In: Boume G, Kraemer I, Zwarts J, editors. Cognitive Foundations of Interpretation. Amsterdam: Royal Netherlands Academy of Science; 2007. pp. 69–94. [Google Scholar]
- Caplan D, Waters GS. Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1999;22:77–126. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x99001788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chafe WL. Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, and point of view. In: Li C, editor. Subject and Topic. New York: Academic Press; 1976. pp. 25–55. [Google Scholar]
- Chatterjee A, Southwood MH, Basilico D. Verbs, events and spatial representations. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37:395–402. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00108-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark HH, Haviland SE. Comprehension and the given-new contract. In: Freedle R, editor. Discourse Production and Comprehension. New York, NY: Ablex; 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen JD, MacWhinney B, Flatt M, Provost J. PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers. 1993;25:257–271. [Google Scholar]
- Cowan N. An embedded-processes model of working memory. In: Miyake A, Shah P, editors. Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 1999. pp. 62–101. [Google Scholar]
- Cowan N. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2001;24:87–114. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x01003922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowan N. Working Memory Capacity. New York: Psychology Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Cowan N, Elliott EM, Saults JS, Morey CC, Mattox S, Hismjatullina A, Conway ARA. On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes. Cognitive Psychology. 2005;51:42–100. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crowder RG. The demise of short-term memory. Acta Psychologica. 1982;50:291–323. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(82)90044-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crowder RG. Short-term memory: Where do we stand? Memory & Cognition. 1993;21:142–145. doi: 10.3758/bf03202725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daneman M. Working memory as a predictor of verbal fluency. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 1991;20:445–464. [Google Scholar]
- Daneman M, Green I. Individual differences in comprehending and producing words in context. Journal of Memory & Language. 1986;25:1–18. [Google Scholar]
- Davelaar EJ, Goshen-Gottstein Y, Ashkenazi A, Haarmann HJ, Usher M. The demise of short-term memory revisited: empirical and computational investigations of recency effects. Psychological Review. 2005;112:3–42. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson DJ, Zacks RT, Ferreira F. Age preservation of the syntactic processor in production. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 2003;32:541–566. doi: 10.1023/a:1025402517111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fedorenko E, Gibson E, Rohde D. The nature of working memory capacity in sentence comprehension: Evidence against domain-specific working memory resources. Journal of Memory & Language. 2006;54:541–553. [Google Scholar]
- Fedorenko E, Gibson E, Rohde D. The nature of working memory in linguistic, arithmetic and spatial integration processes. Journal of Memory & Language. 2007;56:246–269. [Google Scholar]
- Ferreira VS. Is it better to give than to donate? Syntactic flexibility in language production. Journal of Memory & Language. 1996;35:724–755. [Google Scholar]
- Ferreira VS, Slevc LR. Grammatical encoding. In: Gareth Gaskell M, editor. The Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics. Oxford University Press; 2007. pp. 453–469. [Google Scholar]
- Ferreira VS, Yoshita H. Given-new ordering effects on the production of scrambled sentences in Japanese. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 2003;32:669–692. doi: 10.1023/a:1026146332132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman NP, Miyake A. Differential roles for visuospatial and verbal working memory in situation model construction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2000;129:61–83. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.129.1.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glanzer M, Cunitz AR. Two storage mechanisms in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1966;5:351–360. [Google Scholar]
- Gordon PC, Hendrick R, Levine WH. Memory-load interference in syntactic processing. Psychological Science. 2002;13:425–430. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grieve R. Definiteness in discourse. Language and Speech. 1973;16:365–372. doi: 10.1177/002383097301600407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guérard K, Tremblay S. Revisiting evidence for modularity and functional equivalence across verbal and spatial domains in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition. 2008;34:556–569. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gundel JK. Universals of topic-comment structure. In: Hammond M, Moravcsik EA, Wirth JR, editors. Studies in syntactic typology. Philadelphia, PA: J. Benjamins Pub. Co; 1988. pp. 209–239. [Google Scholar]
- Halliday MAK. Language structure and language function. In: Lyons J, editor. New Horizons in Linguistics. New York: Penguin Books; 1970. pp. 140–165. [Google Scholar]
- Halliday MAK. The construction of knowledge and value in the grammar of scientific discourse: With reference to Charles Darwin’s ‘The Origin of Species’. In: Coulthard M, editor. Advances in Written Text Analysis. London: Routledge; 1994. pp. 136–156. [Google Scholar]
- Hartsuiker RJ, Barkhuysen PN. Language production and working memory: the case of subject-verb agreement. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2006;21:181–204. [Google Scholar]
- Horton WS, Gerrig RJ. Conversational common ground and memory processes in language production. Discourse Processes. 2005a;40:1–35. [Google Scholar]
- Horton WS, Gerrig RJ. The impact of memory demands on audience design during language production. Cognition. 2005b;96:127–142. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ichikawa S-i. Rated ease of memorization of dot-in-matrix patterns: Multiple regression analysis by physical variables which describe configuration of dots. Japanese Psychological Research. 1981;23:69–78. [Google Scholar]
- Ichikawa S-i. Verbal memory span, visual memory span, and their correlations with cognitive tasks. Japanese Psychological Research. 1983;25:173–180. [Google Scholar]
- Jackendoff R. Semantic interpretation in generative grammar. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press; 1972. [Google Scholar]
- Jackendoff R. Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Just MA, Carpenter PA. A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review. 1992;99:122–149. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.99.1.122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kane MJ, Hambrick DZ, Tuholski SW, Wilhelm O, Payne TW, Engle RW. The generality of working memory capacity: A latent variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2004;133:189–217. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kellogg RT. Working memory components in written sentence generation. American Journal of Psychology. 2004;117:341–361. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levelt WJM, Maassen B. Lexical search and order of mention in sentence production. In: Klein W, Levelt W, editors. Crossing the Boundaries in Linguistics. Dordrecht: Reidel; 1981. pp. 221–252. [Google Scholar]
- Levelt WJM, Roelofs A, Meyer AS. A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1999;22:1–75. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x99001776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis RL. Interference in short-term memory: the magical number two (or three) in sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 1996;25:93–115. doi: 10.1007/BF01708421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis RL, Vasishth S. An activation-based model of sentence processing as skilled memory retrieval. Cognitive Science. 2005;29:375–419. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis RL, Vasishth S, Van Dyke JA. Computational principles of working memory in sentence comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2006;10:447–454. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin AE, McElree B. Memory operations that support language comprehension: Evidence from verb-phrase ellipsis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition. 2009;35(5):1231–1239. doi: 10.1037/a0016271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin RC, Freedman ML. Short-term retention of lexical-semantic representations: Implications for speech production. Memory. 2001;9:261–280. doi: 10.1080/09658210143000173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin RC, Lesch MF, Bartha MC. Independence of input and output phonology in word processing and short-term memory. Journal of Memory & Language. 1999;41:3–29. [Google Scholar]
- Martin RC, Miller M, Vu H. Lexical-semantic retention and speech production: Further evidence from normal and brain-damaged participants for a phrasal scope of planning. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 2004;21:625–644. doi: 10.1080/02643290342000302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCabe DP, Roediger HL, McDaniel MA, Balota DA, Hambrick DZ. The Relationship Between Working Memory Capacity and Executive Functioning: Evidence for a Common Executive Attention Construct. Neuropsychology. 2010;24:222–243. doi: 10.1037/a0017619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDonald JL, Bock K, Kelly MH. Word and world order: Semantic, phonological, and metrical determinants of serial position. Cognitive Psychology. 1993;25:188–230. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1993.1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McElree B. Working memory and focal attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2001;27:817–835. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyake A, Friedman NP, Rettinger DA, Shah P, Hegarty M. How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2001;130:621–640. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.130.4.621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberauer K. Access to information in working memory: Exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2002;28:411–421. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pashler H. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;116:220–244. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinheiro JC, Bates DM. Mixed-effects models in S and S-PLUS. New York: Springer; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- R Development Core Team. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Vienna, Austria: 2009. R: A language and environment for statistical computing (Version 2.9.1) URL: http://www.R-project.org. [Google Scholar]
- Richardson DC, Spivey MJ, Barsalou LW, McRae K. Spatial representations activated during real-time comprehension of verbs. Cognitive Science. 2003;27:767–780. [Google Scholar]
- Roßnagel C. Cognitive load and perspective-taking: Applying the automatic-controlled distinction to verbal communication. European Journal of Social Psychology. 2000;30:429–445. [Google Scholar]
- Roßnagel C. Lost in Thought: Cognitive Load and the Processing of Addressees’ Feedback in Verbal Communication. Experimental Psychology. 2004;51:191–200. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.51.3.191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruchkin DS, Grafman J, Cameron K, Berndt RS. Working memory retention systems: A state of activated long-term memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2003;26:709–777. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x03000165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shah P, Miyake A. The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking and language processing: An individual differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1996;125:4–27. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.125.1.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smyth RH, Prideaux GD, Hogan JT. The effect of context on dative position. Lingua. 1979;47:27–42. [Google Scholar]
- Swets B, Desmit T, Hambrick DZ, Ferreira F. The role of working memory in syntactic ambiguity resolution: A psychometric approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2007;136:64–81. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.1.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Unsworth N, Engle RW. The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. Psychological Review. 2007;114:104–132. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Unsworth N, Spillers GJ. Working memory capacity: Attention control, secondary memory, or both? A direct test of the dual-component model. Journal of Memory and Language. 2010;62:392–406. [Google Scholar]
- Van Dyke JA. Interference effects from grammatically unavailable constituents during sentence processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2007;33:407–430. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.2.407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vergauwe E, Barrouillet P, Camos V. Do mental processes share a domain-general resource? Psychological Science. 2010;21:384–390. doi: 10.1177/0956797610361340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagers MW, Lau EF, Phillips C. Agreement attraction in comprehension: Representations and processes. Journal of Memory and Language. 2009;61:206–237. [Google Scholar]
- Wagner V, Jescheniak JD, Schriefers H. On the flexibility of grammatical advance planning during sentence production: Effects of cognitive load on multiple lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Leraning, Memory, & Cognition. 2010;36:423–440. doi: 10.1037/a0018619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson M, Emmorey K. A visuospatial “phonological loop” in working memory: Evidence from American Sign Language. Memory & Cognition. 1997;25:313–320. doi: 10.3758/bf03211287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson M, Emmorey K. The effect of irrelevant visual input on working memory for sign language. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. 2003;8:97–103. doi: 10.1093/deafed/eng010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhong H, Stent A, Swift M. Modeling the dative alternation with automatically extracted features. Proceedings of the Workshop on Statistical and Empirical Approaches for Spoken Dialog Systems; American Association for Artificial Intelligence; 2006. [Google Scholar]