Skip to main content
Journal of Thyroid Research logoLink to Journal of Thyroid Research
. 2011 Oct 24;2011:426427. doi: 10.4061/2011/426427

Maternal Thyroid Function during the Second Half of Pregnancy and Child Neurodevelopment at 6, 12, 24, and 60 Months of Age

Jonathan Chevrier 1,*, Kim G Harley 1, Katherine Kogut 1, Nina Holland 1, Caroline Johnson 1, Brenda Eskenazi 1
PMCID: PMC3202128  PMID: 22132346

Abstract

Although evidence suggests that maternal hypothyroidism and mild hypothyroxinemia during the first half of pregnancy alters fetal neurodevelopment among euthyroid offspring, little data are available from later in gestation. In this study, we measured free T4 using direct equilibrium dialysis, as well as total T4 and TSH in 287 pregnant women at 27 weeks' gestation. We also assessed cognition, memory, language, motor functioning, and behavior in their children at 6, 12, 24, and 60 months of age. Increasing maternal TSH was related to better performance on tests of cognition and language at 12 months but not at later ages. At 60 months, there was inconsistent evidence that higher TSH was related to improved attention. We found no convincing evidence that maternal TH during the second half of pregnancy was related to impaired child neurodevelopment.

1. Introduction

The profound deleterious neurodevelopmental effect of maternal and fetal hypothyroidism caused by iodine deficiency has been recognized for more than a century [1]. More recent evidence from experimental and observational studies suggests that even among euthyroid offspring, maternal hypothyroidism and hypothyroxinemia (low thyroxine (T4) with normal thyroid-stimulating hormone (TSH) levels) during early pregnancy may be associated with impaired brain development. Man and colleagues, for instance, reported associations between maternal hypothyroxinemia in early pregnancy and lower scores on neurodevelopmental scales at 8 months, 4 years, and 7 years of age [24]. A more recent study by Haddow et al. found reduced scores on tests of intelligence, attention, and visual-motor performance at 8 years of age among children of 48 mothers with untreated clinical hypothyroidism (defined as TSH levels >99.7th percentile or TSH between the 98th and the 99.6th percentile and total T4 < 7.75 μg/dL) at the 17th week of gestation relative to 124 controls [5]. A Chinese study (n = 1,268) also found that children of 19 women with hypothyroxinemia (defined as total T4 below the reference range but normal TSH and free T4) and 18 women with subclinical hypothyroidism (high TSH and normal free and total T4) in the first half of pregnancy scored 7.6–10.0 point lower than controls on the mental (MDI) and psychomotor (PDI) development indices of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development [6].

Studies have reported reduced scores on cognitive, motor, and language scales even among children of mothers with mild hypothyroxinemia. For instance, in a large population-based cohort study conducted in The Netherlands (n = 3,659), Henrichs et al. found 80% increased odds of expressive language delays at 18 and 30 months of age among children whose mother had free T4 levels <10th percentile at 13 weeks' gestation [7]. Pop and colleagues also reported lower scores on the orientation cluster of the Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale three weeks after birth (n = 204) [8] and a 7.4 point decrease on the PDI at 10 months of age (n = 220) [9] in children of mothers with lower free T4 at 12 weeks' gestation. They also found 8–10 point reductions on the MDI and the PDI at 12 and 24 months of age in children of 57 mothers with low-normal free T4 relative to 50 controls [10]. The one study to contradict the above findings did not measure free T4 [11]. Thus, the bulk of the literature points to an association between adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes in offspring and maternal hypothyroidism, hypothroxinemia, and low-normal free T4 levels during the first half of pregnancy.

Evidence suggests that TH of maternal origin may also play a role in fetal development later in pregnancy. This hypothesis was supported by early studies which demonstrated that transfer of radiolabeled T4 and T3 through the placenta continues to occur after the onset of fetal thyroid function [12]. Maternal T4 appears to reach the fetus in significant amounts up until birth, as evidenced by a study conducted by Vulsma et al. [13]. In that study, T4 was detected in the cord blood of 25 neonates with a complete iodide organification defect, a genetic condition that prevents the iodination of tyrosine and therefore inhibits T4 synthesis. T4 measured in cord blood reached concentrations equivalent to 30–60% of the mean values found in full-term fetuses without this condition [14]. Given that a substantial proportion of thyroid hormone reaching the fetus is of maternal origin in the latter part of gestation, it is conceivable that maternal thyroid hormone may continue to affect fetal neurodevelopment during this period. To date, only the studies by Pop and colleagues examined this question in humans and found no relationship between low-normal maternal free T4 (<10th percentile) measured at 24 and 32 weeks' gestation and child neurodevelopment [810]. To our knowledge, these results have not been replicated by other groups.

Most studies investigating the association of maternal thyroid function during pregnancy and child cognitive development have focused on hypothyroidism/hypothyroxinemia, perhaps because this condition is more common than hyperthyroidism. Maternal hyperthyroidism is nevertheless a significant condition that affects 0.05–0.2% of pregnancies in the form of Graves' disease; an additional 2-3% of pregnant women are also believed to experience gestational transient thyrotoxicosis [15]. In rats, fetal/neonatal hyperthyroidism causes decreased brain and cerebellar weight as well as abnormal brain development, including an acceleration of neuronal differentiation, a delay in glial cell differentiation and early termination of cell proliferation, resulting in a smaller number of granular and basket cells [16]. In humans, maternal hyperthyroidism during pregnancy has been linked to preeclampsia, fetal loss, premature births, growth restriction, and low birth weight [1721]. Subclinical hyperthyroidism (defined as TSH values below the reference range with normal free T4 levels [22]), on the other hand, was not found to be associated with low birth weight, major malformations, or fetal, neonatal, or perinatal mortality in infants of 433 women with TSH levels ≤2.5th percentile and normal free T4 (≤1.75 ng/dL) relative to 23,124 women with normal TSH levels [23]. However, we are aware of no studies that investigated associations between high-normal T4 or subclinical hyperthyroidism and neurodevelopment.

The current study thus aims to examine whether maternal TH levels in the second half of pregnancy are associated with child neurodevelopment at 6, 12, 24, and 60 months of age. Prior studies used immunoassays to determine free T4 levels, but these measurements are influenced by T4-bound protein concentrations which increase during pregnancy [24]. In the present study, we used direct equilibrium dialysis to measure free T4 [25], a method that yields valid results in samples with normal or elevated T4-bound protein levels [26].

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Pregnant women were recruited through the Center for the Health Assessment of Mothers and Children of Salinas (CHAMACOS), a birth cohort study of primarily Latino children born in the Salinas Valley, California. Women were eligible for inclusion in the study if they were ≥18 years old, had completed <20 weeks' gestation, spoke English or Spanish, were Medi-Cal eligible (state-sponsored health care for low-income families), were planning to deliver at the Monterey county hospital (Natividad Medical Center), and received prenatal care in this hospital or at one of five clinics of Clinica de Salud del Valle de Salinas. Screening and enrollment occurred between October 1999 and October 2000. We obtained informed consent from the 601 women who agreed to participate. Out of the 526 singleton live births (there were 20 miscarriages, 3 stillbirths, 2 neonatal deaths, 5 twin births, and 45 women were lost to follow-up), we excluded children with conditions that may impact scores on neurodevelopmental tests such as hydrocephaly (n = 1), autism (n = 1), and a history of seizures (n = 7). Children whose neurodevelopment was never assessed (n = 139) or whose mother's banked serum volume was insufficient for TH analysis (n = 91) were also excluded, leaving a final sample of 287 mother-child pairs. A total of 271 children were included at 6 months, 258 at 12 months, 240 at 24 months, and 207 at 60 months of age.

Women who were excluded or who dropped out at one or more time-point were more likely to be employed, to have been born in the US, to be depressed, and had lived longer in the US compared to those who were included in analyses. Excluded children were more likely to be firstborns and to have had lower birth weights compared to those who were included in the analyses. This study was approved by the University of California, Berkeley Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects.

2.2. Interviews

Women were interviewed in English or Spanish by bilingual, bicultural staff during pregnancy (at 13 and 27 weeks' gestation on average), at delivery, and when their children were 6, 12, 24, and 60 months of age. We obtained information about sociodemographic and lifestyle characteristics at each interview, including smoking, alcohol consumption, drug use, and diet during pregnancy; and on childcare, breastfeeding, number of children in the home, and housing density (number of people per room) after birth. The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT; at the 6 month visit) [27] and the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; at the 12 month visits) [28] were also administered to mothers. In addition, the Infant-Toddler Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) [29] was completed at 6 and 12 months; some subscales of the HOME were completed at 24 months. We also administered the Kotelchuck Adequacy of Prenatal Care Utilization Index [30], the Duke-UNC Functional Social Support Questionnaire [31], and the Diet Quality Index proposed by Bodnar and Siega-Riz [32] and modified by Harley and Eskenazi [33]. Mothers' (during pregnancy) and children's (up to age 24 months) medical records were abstracted by a registered nurse. We obtained data on delivery complications including vacuum extraction, placental abruption, amnionitis, and hemorrhage, or other bleeding; and on neonatal TSH levels (see below).

2.3. Neurodevelopmental Evaluations

Children were evaluated at the ages of 6, 12, 24, and 60 months. We selected for analyses those tests that assessed the same constructs examined in previous studies of maternal thyroid hormone and child neurodevelopment [27, 911]. Children were assessed at 6, 12, and 24 months of age on the Mental Development Index (MDI) and Psychomotor Development Index (PDI) of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development-Second Edition [34], and at 6 and 12 months on the auditory and expressive comprehension subscales of the Preschool Language Scale (PLS). At 60 months of age, the Performance Intellectual Quotient (IQ) was evaluated using the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence 3rd edition (WPPSI-III) [35]. We also administered the Vocabulary subtest of the WPPSI-III. Motor and language development, memory, attention, and school readiness were assessed using the McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities (Digit Span Forward and Backward, Words and Sentences, Draw-a-Child, and Gross Motor Leg and Arm) [36], the Woodcock-Johnson Test of Cognitive Ability (Letter-Word and Applied Problems) and its Spanish-validated version (the Woodcock-Muñoz Test [37, 38]), the Pegboard subtest of the Wide Range Assessment of Visual-Motor Abilities (WRAVMA) for both dominant and non-dominant hands [39], the Conner's Kiddie Continuous Performance Test (KCPT) [40], and the PPVT [27]. The mother was queried about her child's behavior using the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL). The Bayley Scales, the WPPSI performance IQ, the WRAVMA, the Woodcock-Johnson/Muñoz test, and the PPVT are age-standardized to a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15. Standardized scores on the McCarthy Scales are only available for full subscales but not for individual subtests. Raw scores were thus used for gross motor subtests. Scores on other McCarthy subtests were determined by subtracting children's chronological age from their developmental age (in months), as determined using methods published by Kaufman and Kaufman [41]. Positive scores show accelerated development while negative scores represent a delay. Finally, raw scores were used for the CBCL following recommendations from the test manual [42].

Neurodevelopmental evaluations were conducted in Spanish and/or English by psychometricians blind to mothers' TH levels in the study office or in a recreational vehicle (RV) modified for this purpose. Psychometricians were trained and supervised by a child neuropsychologist (CJ) and were videotaped and evaluated on a regular basis to ensure consistency across psychometricians and over time. All tests were reviewed by graduate students trained by the child neuropsychologist to ensure accurate scoring.

2.4. Thyroid Hormone Measurements

We measured TSH, free T4 and total T4 in serum collected by venipuncture from pregnant women at the time of the second interview (Mean ±  SD  =  26.9  ±  3.4 weeks' gestation). Samples were processed immediately at Natividad Medical Center and stored at −80°C at the UC Berkeley School of Public Health Biorepository until shipment to Quest Diagnostic's Nichols Institute (San Juan Capistrano, CA) where they were analyzed on a Bayer ADVIA Centaur system (Siemens Healthcare Diagnostics, Deerfield, IL). A pilot experiment revealed that every freeze-thaw cycle was associated with a 0.1 ng/dL increase in free T4 levels (P < 0.001); this variable accounted for 33% of the variance (unpublished results). Samples were thus thawed only once for aliquoting, shipped refrigerated, and analyzed within 48 hours. TSH was measured by ultrasensitive third generation immunochemiluminometric assay (ICMA; functional sensitivity (FS): 0.01 mIU/L, intra-assay coefficients of variation (CV) = 2.3–6.0%); total T4 was determined by solid-phase ICMA (FS: 0.1 μg/dL, CV: 4.5–5.7%); free T4 was analyzed by direct equilibrium dialysis (ED) followed by radioimmunoassay (RIA; FS: 0.1 ng/dL, CV: 2.4–6.2%) [25]. Serum protein-bound T4 levels usually increase during pregnancy [15], which may bias results obtained by immunoassays not preceded by ED [24]. ED uses a semipermeable membrane to physically separate the bound hormone from the free portion, which is then measured using a highly sensitive RIA. This method measures free T4 accurately in samples with normal or elevated protein-bound T4 levels [26]. Previous studies used butanol-extractable iodine [24], which estimates T4 levels by measuring protein-bound iodine [43] or immunoassays [510]. Trimester-specific reference ranges for TH levels were provided by the analytical laboratory. Neonatal TSH was also measured in dried blood spots by the Genetics Disease Branch of the California Department of Health Services as part of the State's Newborn Screening Program. Hospital staff collected blood spots by heel stick on average 24.8 hours after birth (SD = 15.5); samples were analyzed by solid-phase, time-resolved sandwich fluoroimmunoassay (AutoDELPHIA; PerkinElmer, Wellesley, MA).

2.5. Statistical Analyses

Multiple linear regression models were used to evaluate associations between TH and neurodevelopmental outcomes. Models were first run with TH expressed continuously. We also ran models with TSH categorized as low (n = 43) versus normal based on trimester-specific reference ranges provided by the analytical laboratory. There were however not enough women with high TSH or with other TH measurements outside of the reference range to conduct such analyses. Therefore, to obtain sufficient sample size and to replicate methods used in prior studies [710], we dichotomized TH at the 10th and the 90th percentile based on distributions in our sample and at 0.8 ng/dL. Neurodevelopmental scores were expressed continuously. We used generalized additive models with a 3-degrees-of-freedom cubic spline function to evaluate the shape of the relationship between continuously expressed TH and scores on neurodevelopmental assessments and to test for linearity [44]. Since altered neurodevelopment was hypothesized to occur at both ends of the distribution of TH values (i.e., following an inverse U-shaped association), scores with P values for digression from linearity <0.10 were fit using a quadratic term while scores with a P value ≥0.10 were fit linearly in multiple regression models. Conclusions were similar when using quadratic or linear terms. We therefore only present results using linear terms.

We removed outliers as identified by the Generalized Extreme Studentized Deviates Many-Outlier procedure at an α = 0.01 [45]. Covariates considered for inclusion in models were identified based on prior reports suggesting that they influenced neurodevelopment (see Appendix A for a complete list). They included (categorized as shown in Table 1 or as indicated below): maternal age at enrollment (continuously), race, education, income, parity (continuously), depression (yes versus no), maternal PPVT score (continuously), Diet Quality Index (continuously), Kotelchuck Adequacy of Prenatal Care Utilization Index (adequate plus, adequate, inadequate), Composite Social Support Index (continuously), employment status at the time of and prior to assessments (yes versus no), smoking (yes versus no), alcohol (yes versus no) and illegal drug (yes versus no) consumption during pregnancy, delivery type (natural versus cesarean section), pregnancy complications (any versus none), infant sex, premature birth (yes versus no), months of breastfeeding (continuously), HOME score at the time of and prior to assessments, and psychometrician administering assessments.

Table 1.

Demographic characteristics of study participants (n = 287).

No. (%)
Mothers
Age (years)
 18–24 130 (45.3)
 25–29 95 (33.1)
 30–34 42 (14.6)
 35–45 20 (7.0)

Race/Ethnicity
 White 5 (1.7)
 Latino 278 (96.9)
 Other 4 (1.4)

Education
 ≤6th grade 121 (42.2)
 7–12th grade 105 (36.6)
 ≥High School 61 (21.3)

Income (% poverty)
 <100 171 (59.6)
 100–200 105 (36.6)
 >200 11 (3.8)

Country of birth
 United States 32 (11.1)
 Mexico 251 (87.5)
 Other 4 (1.4)

Time in the USA (years)
 ≤5 156 (54.4)
 6–10 69 (24.0)
 ≥11 62 (21.6)

Parity
 0 91 (31.7)
 ≥1 196 (68.3)

Smoking during pregnancy
 No 271 (94.4)
 Yes 16 (5.6)

Alcohol during pregnancy (≥one serving)
 No 282 (98.3)
 Yes 5 (1.7)

Children
Sex
 Boy 140 (48.8)
 Girl 147 (51.2)

Birthweight (g)
 <2500 g 10 (3.5)
 2500–3500 g 149 (51.9)
 >3500 g 128 (44.6)

Gestational duration (weeks)
 <37 21 (7.3)
 37–42 266 (92.7)
 >42 0 (0.0)

To ensure that neurodevelopment was not affected by neonatal hypothyroidism, we also considered neonatal TSH levels as a covariate. In addition, we considered the potential confounding effect of some known neurotoxicants. Lead was measured in maternal and cord blood samples using graphite furnace atomic absorption spectrophotometry. As exposure to organophosphate insecticides has been associated with altered neurodevelopment in this cohort of farmworker families [46, 47], this variable was also considered. Organophosphate insecticide exposure was assessed by measuring dialkyl phosphate metabolites in maternal urine collected at approximately 13 and 26 weeks' gestation by high-resolution gas chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry (HRGC/MS-MS) with isotope dilution quantification [48]. Measurements at the two time points were averaged and log10-transformed. For each time point, covariates were included in final models if they were associated with any of the TH measurements at P < 0.10 based on analysis of variance (ANOVA) or Pearson's correlations.

In order to control for potential selection bias due to exclusion from analyses and/or loss to followup, we ran all models with and without weights determined as the inverse probability of inclusion in our samples at each time-point [49]. Probability of inclusion was determined based on multiple logistic regression models using covariates listed in the Statistical Analyses section as potential predictors. Model selection was performed using a Deletion-Substitution-Addition (DSA) algorithm, which finds the combination of variables (including interactions and polynomials) that minimizes cross-validated risk [50]. Results were similar with and without this adjustment; we present results without the adjustment. Missing covariates were imputed. In addition, two free T4 and two TSH values below the limit of detection (LOD) (0.1 ng/dL and 0.01 mIU/L, resp.) were imputed as half the LOD. Statistical significance was defined as P < 0.05 on two-tailed tests. TSH values were log2-transformed for all statistical analyses. Analyses were performed using Intercooled STATA, version 10.0 (StataCorp, College Station, TX) and R, version 2.6.1 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria).

3. Results

Mothers were mostly low-income, Mexican-born, Spanish-speaking Latinas with a low level of education, and many were recent immigrants to the United States (Table 1). A large proportion of women (73.7%) lived in farmworker families. During pregnancy, smoking was rare in this population (5.2%), and only 2.6% of women had ≥1 serving of alcohol per week. Mothers' mean PPVT score was 88.0 (SD = 21.2).

Mean free and total T4 levels were 0.8 ng/dL (SD = 0.2) and 10.5 μg/dL (SD = 1.5), respectively; the geometric mean for TSH was 1.2 mIU/L (GSD = 1.7). Nine (2.7%) women had low free T4 (<0.5 ng/dL) and 13 (3.9%) had low total T4 (<8.0 μg/dL) levels. None of the women were hypothyroidic based on the reference range for TSH provided by the analytical laboratory (TSH > 5.2 mIU/L), but 16 were hypothyroidic using the criteria proposed by the National Academy of Clinical Biochemistry (TSH > 2.5 mIU/L) [51, 52]. Five women had high free T4 (>1.6 ng/dL), none had elevated total T4 (>17.8 and >20.1 μg/dL in the second and third trimesters, resp.), and 43 had low TSH (<0.5 and <0.8 mIU/L in the second and third trimesters, resp.). All children had normal TSH levels at birth (<25 mIU/L).

Scores on neurodevelopmental scales when children were aged 6, 12, 24, and 60 months are shown in Tables 2 and 3. Except for a low MDI score at 24 months (mean = 86.1; SD = 12.0), Bayley and PLS scores were close to the expected mean (i.e., 100) at all time points. At 60 months of age, children performed well on fine motor tests but scored relatively low on cognitive (verbal and nonverbal), language, and memory tests.

Table 2.

Mean scores on neurodevelopmental scales at 6, 12, and 24 months of age.

6 Months 12 Months 24 Months
N Mean (SD) N Mean (SD) N Mean (SD)
Bayley
 Mental development index 271 95.0 (7.9) 258 101.3 (9.1) 240 86.4 (11.7)
 Psychomotor development index 271 96.2 (11.1) 257 107.0 (12.8) 240 98.2 (10.4)

Preschool language scale
 Auditory comprehension 270 104.6 (12.9) 257 99.1 (12.9)
 Expressive comprehension 270 91.1 (13.5) 257 94.5 (13.8)
 Total score 270 97.7 (11.3) 257 96.6 (13.5)

Table 3.

Mean scores on neurodevelopmental scales at 5 years of age.

N Mean (SD)
Intelligence
WPPSI1
 Performance IQ 207 94.7 (14.7)

Motor
WRAVMA2
 Pegboard-Dominant 205 110.7 (17.4)
 Pegboard-Nondominant 204 110.3 (17.2)
McCarthy
 Draw-a-Child 206 3.9 (16.1)
 Gross Motor-Leg 194 11.0 (2.2)
 Gross Motor-Arm 202 4.1 (2.4)

Language Development
WPPSI1 Vocabulary 207 8.8 (2.6)
PPVT3 205 94.8 (17.5)

Memory
McCarthy
 Words and Sentences 205 −4.5 (16.6)
 Digit Span Forward 204 −15.2 (13.0)
 Digit Span Backward 199 −15.3 (10.6)

School Readiness
Woodcock-Johnson/Muñoz
 Letter-Word 199 92.4 (12.1)
 Applied Problems 206 87.0 (15.8)

Attention
CBCL4
 ADHD5 200 4.7 (2.8)
KCPT6
 ADHD Confidence Index5 188 45.7 (17.5)

1Weschler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence.

2Wide Range Assessment of Visual Motor Ability.

3Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test.

4Child Behavior Checklist.

5Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder.

6Kiddie Continuous Performance Test.

Note: We report differences between chronological and developmental ages for the McCarthy Draw-a-Child, Words and Sentences, and Digit Span Forward and Backward subtests (in months). Raw scores are reported for the gross motor tasks of the McCarthy scales (no developmental ages are available for these subtests) and for the CBCL as recommended by the test manual [42]. Standardized scores are used for other tests.

Table 4 shows associations between maternal thyroid hormone levels and child scores on the Bayley and Preschool Language scales at 6, 12, and 24 months of age. Associations between maternal free T4 and scores on the Bayley scales were consistently negative but none were statistically significant either in unadjusted (results not shown) or adjusted models. Associations between Bayley scores and total T4 were also generally negative but not statistically significant. Increasing maternal TSH was related to better performance on the Bayley MDI and on the auditory comprehension subscale of the PLS at 12 months but maternal thyroid hormone was not related to these constructs at later points. Maternal free and total T4 levels were not significantly associated with scores on the PLS.

Table 4.

Associations between maternal thyroid hormone levels during pregnancy (27 weeks' gestation) and child neurodevelopment at 6, 12 and 24 months of age.

Bayley scales Preschool language scale
Mental development index Psychomotor development index Auditory comprehension Expressive comprehension Total score
6 Months1 Free T4 β −3.14 −3.27 −2.03 1.65 −0.10
(95% CI) (−7.65, 1.36) (−9.36, 2.82) (−9.80, 5.75) (−5.48, 8.79) (−6.45, 6.25)
Total T4 β 0.03 −0.06 −0.24 0.58 0.19
(95% CI) (−0.57, 0.62) (−0.86, 0.74) (−1.28, 0.80) (−0.37, 1.53) (−0.66, 1.05)
TSH β 0.85 1.46 1.51 0.24 1.06
(95% CI) (−0.49, 2.18) (−0.33, 3.26) (−0.81, 3.84) (−1.91, 2.38) (−0.87, 2.98)

12 Months2 Free T4 β −0.48 −6.40 4.39 1.26 3.45
(95% CI) (−6.22, 5.26) (−13.89, 1.08) (−3.56, 12.34) (−7.20, 9.72) (−4.76, 11.67)
Total T4 β −0.12 −0.45 0.33 −0.15 0.12
(95% CI) (−0.92, 0.68) (−1.53, 0.63) (−0.79, 1.46) (−1.34, 1.04) (−1.05, 1.28)
TSH β 1.71 0.10 2.92 0.46 1.91
(95% CI) (0.05, 3.37)* (−2.16, 2.35) (0.59, 5.25)* (−2.04, 2.95) (−0.51, 4.33)

24 Months3 Free T4 β −4.29 −2.67
(95% CI) (−11.60, 3.02) (−8.95, 3.62)
Total T4 β −0.17 0.43
(95% CI) (−1.20, 0.85) (−0.43, 1.30)
TSH β 0.33 −1.31
(95% CI) (−1.97, 2.63) (−3.25, 0.63)

*P < 0.05.

1Models adjusted for maternal age, employment status at enrollment and at the 6-months visit, country of birth, time lived in the US, Diet Quality Index, blood lead levels and delivery complications; child hospitalization before 6 months, season of assessment and psychometrician.

2Models adjusted for maternal age, employment status at enrollment and at the 6 months visit, country of birth, time lived in the US, Diet Quality Index, Kotelchuck Adequacy of Prenatal Care Utilization Index, blood lead levels, delivery complications and PPVT score; child age, preterm birth, hospitalization at 6 months and 1 year, family structure at 1 year; season, and language spoken at the time of assessment.

3Models adjusted for maternal age, income, employment status at enrollment, 6 months and 1 year, country of birth, Diet Quality Index, Kotelchuck Adequacy of Prenatal Care Utilization Index, blood lead levels, delivery complications, PPVT score; child hospitalization at 1 year; number of children in the home at 6 months, home density at 2 years, family structure at 1 year; season, psychometrician and language of assessment.

4Since TSH was expressed on a log2 basis, β are equal to the change in neurodevelopmental outcomes for a doubling in TSH levels.

Maternal free T4, total T4, and TSH were not associated with performance on any tests of neurodevelopment in 60-month-old children (Table 5) with one exception: every doubling in TSH levels was associated with a 0.65 point decrease (95% CI = −1.26, −0.04) on the Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) subscale of the CBCL, although there was no significant association between maternal TH levels and CBCL Attention Problems and Pervasive Developmental Problems scales nor on child's performance on the KCPT (results not shown). Categorizing each measure of TH at the 10th or 90th percentiles yielded no significant association; subclinical hyperthyroidism was also not related with outcomes.

Table 5.

Associations between maternal thyroid hormone levels during pregnancy (27 weeks' gestation) and child neurodevelopment at 5 years of age.1

Free T4 Total T4 TSH2
β (95% CI) β (95% CI) β (95% CI)
Performance IQ
WPPSI3 −4.12 (−13.73, 5.49) 0.03 (−1.35, 1.41) −2.26 (−5.27, 0.74)

Motor Development
WRAVMA4
 Pegboard-Dominant −3.76 (−15.45, 7.93) −0.97 (−2.64, 0.70) 0.51 (−3.12, 4.15)
 Pegboard-Nondominant −4.21 (−16.03, 7.61) −1.55 (−3.23, 0.13) 0.02 (−3.65, 3.68)
McCarthy
 Draw-a-Child −5.98 (−16.74, 4.77) 0.10 (−1.44, 1.63) 0.06 (−3.27, 3.39)
 Gross Motor-Leg −0.14 (−1.60, 1.32) 0.00 (−0.22, 0.22) 0.16 (−0.31, 0.63)
 Gross Motor-Arm 0.08 (−1.51, 1.66) −0.04 (−0.27, 0.19) 0.04 (−0.45, 0.53)

Language Development
WPPSI3 Vocabulary −0.21 (−1.98, 1.57) −0.22 (−0.47, 0.03) −0.37 (−0.92, 0.19)
PPVT5 −2.71 (−14.18, 8.77) −0.89 (−2.54, 0.76) −1.05 (−4.66, 2.57)

Memory
McCarthy
 Words and Sentences 0.10 (−11.32, 11.52) −0.55 (−2.17, 1.07) −1.44 (−4.97, 2.09)
 Digit Span Forward 4.06 (−4.72, 12.83) 0.77 (−0.46, 2.00) −1.83 (−4.51, 0.86)
 Digit Span Backward 4.43 (−2.55, 11.42) −0.10 (−1.13, 0.94) −0.38 (−2.63, 1.87)

School Readiness
Woodcock-Johnson/Muñoz
 Letter-Word −1.86 (−9.33, 5.60) 0.21 (−0.88, 1.31) 1.43 (−0.94, 3.80)
 Applied Problems −3.82 (−14.06, 6.42) −0.51 (−2.01, 1.00) −1.48 (−4.74, 1.78)

Attention
CBCL6
 ADHD7 −0.10 (−2.03, 1.82) 0.00 (−0.28, 0.27) −0.65 (−1.26, −0.04)*
KCPT8
 ADHD7 Confidence Index 7.52 (−4.86, 19.91) 0.09 (−1.70, 1.87) −0.75 (−4.61, 3.12)

*P < 0.05.

1Models adjusted for maternal age, income, employment status at 6 months, country of birth, Diet Quality Index, delivery complications, PPVT score; child 5-minute APGAR, hospitalization at 1 year; number of children in home at 1 and 2 years, home density at 2 years, family structure at 1 year; season of assessment.

2Since TSH was expressed on a log2 basis, β are equal to the change in neurodevelopmental outcomes for a doubling in TSH levels.

3Weschler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence.

4Wide Range Assessment of Visual Motor Ability.

5Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test.

6Child Behavior Checklist.

7Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder.

8Kiddie Continuous Performance Test.

4. Discussion

We found little evidence that TH levels measured around the 27th week of gestation in mothers of euthyroid infants living in an iodine-sufficient area [53] were associated with child neurodevelopment. Although increasing maternal TSH levels were associated with better performance on the Bayley MDI at 12 months, these results did not persist at 24 months. Similarly, a reduction in ADHD symptoms, as reported by mothers in 60-month-old children, was not supported by other measures of hyperactivity and/or inattention at this age (i.e., maternal report on the Attention Problems scale of the CBCL or child performance on the KCPT). Better Auditory Comprehension also was noted at 12 months but not on other tests of language (WPPSI Vocabulary and PPVT) at 60 months.

Our results are in agreement with those reported by Pop and colleagues, the only other group that examined associations between maternal TH levels during the second half of gestation and child neurodevelopment [810]. In these studies, authors reported no associations between free T4 levels measured at 24 and 32 weeks' gestation and infant and toddler development, but did find relations with maternal thyroid hormone measured earlier in pregnancy. Other studies that measured TH during the first half of pregnancy have also reported associations with child neurodevelopment [210] with a notable exception in the study by Oken et al., which found no association between maternal TSH and total T4 at 10 weeks' gestation and child cognition at 6 months and 3 years of age in a large study of 500 mothers and children dyads [11]. TH of maternal origin may thus be of particular importance to brain development before the onset of fetal thyroid function, which occurs around midgestation [54]. Evidence for the potential role of maternal TH before the onset of fetal thyroid function includes the detection of T4 in coelomic fluid as early as 6 weeks' gestation [55], the fact that nuclear T3 receptors were identified in the brain of 10 week old fetuses [56], and that T3 binding to these receptors was detected between 9 and 13 weeks' gestation [57].

This study has some limitations. Women who were excluded from analyses were more likely to be depressed and to give birth to children of lower birth weight. This may have introduced bias since these variables are related to both thyroid hormone levels and neurodevelopment. However, our results were not substantially altered after applying inverse probability of inclusion weights, suggesting that this potential bias may not explain our null finding. In addition, in our study, as well as in those of Pop and colleagues [810], most women were euthyroid. Hence, our findings do not preclude the possibility that more extreme maternal thyroid hormone levels in the latter half of pregnancy may influence fetal neurodevelopment.

This study has a number of strengths. We examined a wide range of domains of behavior and neurodevelopment at multiple ages and examined maternal thyroid hormone using direct equilibrium dialysis, which is currently considered the gold standard method to measure free T4. Prior studies exclusively used immunoassays, which, according to the National Academy of Clinical Biochemistry, may only be considered as free T4 “estimates” [52]. Another strength of the present study is that we were able to consider, and control for, a large number of potential confounders, including exposure to neurotoxicants such as lead, cigarette smoke, and organophosphate insecticides [58]. In addition, our population is demographically homogenous, further reducing the potential for confounding. Finally, Zoeller and Rovet proposed that maternal hypothyroxinemia and hypothyroidism at the beginning of the third trimester (when we determined thyroid function in CHAMACOS women) primarily affects gross and fine motor skills, memory, and visuospatial skills [59]. In this study, we evaluated these constructs using well-validated and widely used instruments and yet found no clear evidence of a relationship between maternal thyroid hormone levels and child neurodevelopment.

In summary, this is the first study of maternal thyroid hormone and child neurodevelopment to use direct equilibrium dialysis to measure free T4. Although prior studies did report associations between maternal clinical hypothyroidism and mild hypothyroxinemia during the first half of pregnancy and cognitive impairments in children, we find no convincing evidence that TH measured during later gestation is associated with neurodevelopment in euthyroid children living in an iodine-sufficient area.

Acknowledgments

This publication was supported by grant numbers: RD 83171001 from the US Environmental Protection Agency and P01 ES009605 from the National Institute for Environmental Health Sciences. Additional funding was provided by the University of California Institute for Mexico and the United States (UC MEXUS), the Fonds de la Recherche en Santé du Québec and the Canadian Institutes for Health Research. The contents of this publication are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent funders' official views. The authors gratefully acknowledge study participants as well as Jon Nakamoto, MD, Ph.D., Michelle Vedar, MPH and Kristin M. Tyler, BA, for their invaluable contributions. Authors declare that they have no competing financial interests.

Appendices

A. Maternal Covariates

Baseline visit
Age at enrollment (years), No. (%)
 18–24 130 (45.3)
 25–29 95 (33.1)
 30–34 42 (14.6)
 34–45 20 (7.0)
Age at enrollment (years),
Mean (SD)
25.8 (5.0)
Race, No. (%)
 White 5 (1.7)
 Latino 278 (96.9)
 Other 4 (1.4)
Education, No. (%)
 ≤6th grade 121 (42.2)
 7–12th grade 105 (36.6)
 ≥High School 61 (21.3)
Income (% poverty), No. (%)
 <100 171 (59.6)
 100–200 105 (36.6)
 >200 11 (3.8)
Average income per person per month ($), Mean (SD) 413.8 (255.7)
Employment status, No. (%)
 No 209 (72.8)
 Yes 78 (27.2)
Country of birth, No. (%)
 United States 32 (11.1)
 Mexico 251 (87.5)
 Other 4 (1.4)
Time in the USA (years), No. (%)
 ≤5 156 (54.4)
 6 to 10 69 (24.0)
 ≥11 62 (21.6)
Parity, Mean (SD) 1.3 (1.2)
Smoking during pregnancy,
No. (%)
 No 271 (94.4)
 Yes 16 (5.6)
Smokers in household during pregnancy, No. (%)
 No 258 (89.9)
 Yes 29 (10.1)
Any second-hand smoke exposure during pregnancy, No. (%)
 No 179 (62.4)
 Yes 108 (37.6)
More than one alcoholic drink per week during pregnancy, No. (%)
 No 282 (98.3)
 Yes 5 (1.7)
Any drug consumption during pregnancy, No. (%)
 No 282 (98.3)
 Yes 5 (1.7)
Kotelchuck Adequacy of Prenatal Care Utilization Index, No. (%)
 Inadequate 62 (21.6)
 Adequate 93 (32.4)
 Adequate Plus 132 (46.0)
Diet Quality Index during pregnancy, Mean (SD) 45.3 (9.7)
Composite Social Support Index, Mean (SD) 3.7 (0.9)
Urinary DAP1metabolites during pregnancy (nmol/L), Mean (SD) 2.1 (0.4)
Lead levels during pregnancy (ug/dL), Mean (SD) 1.5 (2.1)
6-Month Visit
Income (% poverty), No. (%)
 <100 199 (69.3)
 100–200 86 (30.0)
 >200 2 (0.7)
Employment status, No. (%)
 No 199 (69.3)
 Yes 88 (30.7)
PPVT2 score, Mean (SD) 88.2 (21.1)
WAIS3 score, Mean (SD) 6.3 (2.6)
12-Month Visit
Income (% poverty), No. (%)
 <100 179 (62.4)
 100–200 99 (34.5)
 >200 9 (3.1)
Employment status, No. (%)
 No 198 (69.0)
 Yes 89 (31.0)
Composite Social Support Index, Mean (SD) 3.8 (1.0)
Depression, No. (%)
 No 140 (48.8)
 Yes 147 (51.2)
24-Month Visit
Income (% poverty), No. (%)
 <100 167 (58.2)
 100–200 107 (37.3)
 >200 13 (4.5)
Employment status, No. (%)
 No 174 (60.6)
 Yes 113 (39.4)
Composite Social Support Index, Mean (SD) 3.9 (1.0)
Three-Year Visit
Income (% poverty), No. (%)
 <100 178 (62.0)
 100–200 103 (35.9)
 >200 6 (2.1)
Employment status, No. (%)
 No 175 (61.0)
 Yes 112 (39.0)
Depression, No. (%)
 No 161 (56.1)
 Yes 126 (43.9)
60-Month visit
Income (% poverty), No. (%)
 <100 182 (63.4)
 100–200 93 (32.4)
 >200 12 (4.2)
Employment status, No. (%)
 No 158 (55.1)
 Yes 129 (44.9)
Composite Social Support Index, Mean (SD) 3.9 (1.0)

1Dialkyl phosphates (DAPs) measured in maternal urine (organophosphate pesticide metabolites).

2Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT).

3Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS).

B. Child Covariates

Baseline visit
Sex, No. (%)
 Boy 140 (48.8)
 Girl 147 (51.2)
Birthweight (g), Mean (%)
 <2500 10 (3.5)
 2500–3500 149 (51.9)
 >3500 128 (44.6)
Gestational duration (weeks),
No. (%)
 ≥37 266 (92.7)
 <37 21 (7.3)
Cesarean section, No. (%)
 No 220 (76.7)
 Yes 67 (23.3)
Pregnancy complications, No. (%)
 No 284 (99.0)
 Yes 3 (1.0)
5-minute APGAR score, Mean (SD) 8.9 (0.4)
Months child breastfed, Mean (SD) 8.6 (8.2)
Neonatal TSH (mIU/L), Mean (SD) 6.5 (3.5)
6-Month Visit
Number of children in household, Mean (SD) 2.1 (0.4)
Housing density (people per room), No. (%)
 ≤0.5 4 (1.4)
 0.51–1.00 52 (18.1)
 1.01–1.50 93 (32.4)
  ≥1.51 138 (48.1)
Lived with father, No. (%)
 All the time 242 (84.3)
 Most of the time 11 (3.8)
 Some of the time 14 (4.9)
 Not at all 20 (7.0)
HOME1 score, Mean (SD) 32.0 (4.1)
Hospitalized overnight, No. (%)
 No 253 (88.2)
 Yes 34 (11.8)
Age at assessment (months),
Mean (SD)
6.6 (1.1)
Medication/herbal intake within 24 hours of assessment, No. (%)
 No 279 (97.2)
 Yes 8 (2.8)
Location assessment performed, No. (%)
 Office 187 (65.2)
 Other 100 (34.8)
Season assessment performed,
No. (%)
 January–March 68 (23.7)
 April–June 71 (24.7)
 July–September 77 (26.8)
 October–December 71 (24.7)
Psychometrician at assessment, No. (%)
 01 117 (40.8)
 07 32 (11.1)
 13 42 (14.6)
 16 4 (1.4)
 23 92 (32.1)
12-Month Visit
Number of children in household, Mean (SD) 2.0 (1.7)
Housing density (people per room), No. (%)
 ≤0.5 1 (0.3)
 0.51–1.00 55 (19.2)
 1.01–1.50 105 (36.6)
  ≥1.51 126 (43.9)
Lived with father, No. (%)
 All the time 239 (83.3)
 Most of the time 11 (3.8)
 Some of the time 13 (4.5)
 Not at all 24 (8.4)
HOME1 score, Mean (SD) 35.9 (3.1)
Hospitalized overnight, No. (%)
 No 276 (96.2)
 Yes 11 (3.8)
Age at assessment (months), Mean (SD) 12.7 (1.3)
Medication/herbal intake within 24 hours of assessment, No. (%)
 No 255 (88.9)
 Yes 32 (11.1)
Location assessment performed, No. (%)
 Office 190 (66.2)
 Other 97 (33.8)
Season assessment performed,
No. (%)
 January–March 73 (25.4)
 April–June 67 (23.3)
 July–September 74 (25.8)
 October–December 73 (25.4)
Psychometrician at assessment, No. (%)
 01 109 (38.0)
 07 83 (28.9)
 23 95 (33.1)
24-Month visit
Number of children in household, Mean (SD) 1.8 (1.5)
Housing density (people per room), No. (%)
 ≤0.5 2 (0.7)
 0.51–1.00 51 (17.8)
 1.01–1.50 94 (32.8)
 ≥1.51 140 (48.8)
Lived with father, No. (%)
 All the time 236 (82.2)
 Most of the time 23 (8.0)
 Some of the time 5 (1.7)
Not at all 23 (8.0)
HOME1 score, Mean (SD) 26.1 (2.5)
Hospitalized overnight, No. (%)
 No 275 (95.8)
 Yes 12 (4.2)
Age at assessment (months), Mean (SD) 24.7 (1.2)
Medication/herbal intake within 24 hours of assessment, No. (%)
 No 205 (71.4)
 Yes 82 (28.6)
Location assessment performed, No. (%)
 Office 194 (67.6)
 Other 93 (32.4)
Season assessment performed,
No. (%)
 January–March 73 (25.4)
 April–June 85 (29.6)
 July–September 71 (24.7)
 October–December 58 (20.2)
Psychometrician at assessment, No. (%)
 01 122 (42.5)
 07 31 (10.8)
 23 134 (46.7)
60-Month visit
Number of children in household, Mean (SD) 1.9 (1.3)
Housing density (people per room), No. (%)
 ≤0.5 1 (0.3)
 0.51–1.00 87 (30.3)
 1.01–1.50 126 (43.9)
  ≥1.51 73 (25.4)
Lived with father, No. (%)
 All the time 221 (77.0)
 Most of the time 16 (5.6)
 Some of the time 11 (3.8)
 Not at all 39 (13.6)
Hospitalized overnight, No. (%)
 No 279 (97.2)
 Yes 8 (2.8)
Attended preschool, No. (%)
 No 124 (43.2)
 Yes 163 (56.8)
Attended kindergarten, No. (%)
 No 68 (23.7)
 Yes 219 (76.3)
Age at assessment (months), Mean (SD) 60.7 (2.2)
Medication/herbal intake within 24 hours of assessment, No. (%)
 No 240 (83.6)
 Yes 47 (16.4)
Location assessment performed, No. (%)
 Office 245 (85.4)
 Other 42 (14.6)
Season assessment performed,
No. (%)
 January–March 76 (26.5)
 April–June 72 (25.1)
 July–September 86 (30.0)
 October–December 53 (18.5)
Psychometrician at assessment, No. (%)
 01 117 (40.8)
 21 23 (8.0)
 23 32 (11.1)
 43 115 (40.1)

1Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (H.O.M.E).

References

  • 1.Dunn JT. Iodine supplementation and the prevention of cretinism. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1993;678:158–168. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1993.tb26119.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Man EB, Holden RH, Jones WS. Thyroid function in human pregnancy. VII. Development and retardation of 4-year-old progeny of euthyroid and of hypothyroxinemic women. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology. 1971;109(1):12–19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Man EB, Jones WS. Thyroid function in human pregnancy. V. Incidence of maternal serum low butanol-extractable iodines and of normal gestational TBG and TBPA capacities; Retardation of 8-month-old infants. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology. 1969;104(6):898–908. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Man EB, Serunian SA. Thyroid function in human pregnancy. IX. Development or retardation of 7 year old progeny of hypothyroxinemic women. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology. 1976;125(7):949–957. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Haddow JE, Palomaki GE, Allan WC, et al. Maternal thyroid deficiency during pregnancy and subsequent neuropsychological development of the child. The New England Journal of Medicine. 1999;341(8):549–555. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199908193410801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Li Y, Shan Z, Teng W, et al. Abnormalities of maternal thyroid function during pregnancy affect neuropsychological development of their children at 25-30 months. Clinical Endocrinology. 2010;72(6):825–829. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2265.2009.03743.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Henrichs J, Bongers-Schokking JJ, Schenk JJ, et al. Maternal thyroid function during early pregnancy and cognitive functioning in early childhood: the generation R study. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 2010;95(9):4227–4234. doi: 10.1210/jc.2010-0415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kooistra L, Crawford S, Van Baar AL, Brouwers EP, Pop VJ. Neonatal effects of maternal hypothyroxinemia during early pregnancy. Pediatrics. 2006;117(1):161–167. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Pop VJ, Kuijpens JL, Van Baar AL, et al. Low maternal free thyroxine concentrations during early pregnancy are associated with impaired psychomotor development in infancy. Clinical Endocrinology. 1999;50(2):147–155. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2265.1999.00639.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Pop VJ, Brouwers EP, Vader HL, Vulsma T, Van Baar AL, De Vijlder JJ. Maternal hypothyroxinaemia during early pregnancy and subsequent child development: a 3-year follow-up study. Clinical Endocrinology. 2003;59(3):282–288. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2265.2003.01822.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Oken E, Braverman LE, Platek D, Mitchell ML, Lee SL, Pearce EN. Neonatal thyroxine, maternal thyroid function, and child cognition. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 2009;94(2):497–503. doi: 10.1210/jc.2008-0936. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Myant NB. Passage of thyroxine and tri-iodol-thyronine from mother to foetus in pregnant women. Clinical Science. 1958;17(1):75–79. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Vulsma T, Gons MH, De Vijlder JJM. Maternal-fetal transfer of thyroxine in congenital hypothyroidism due to a total organification defect or thyroid agenesis. The New England Journal of Medicine. 1989;321(1):13–16. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198907063210103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Thorpe-Beeston JG, Nicolaides KH, Feelton CV, Butler J, McGregor AM. Maturation of the secretion of thyroid hormone and thyroid-stimulating hormone in the fetus. The New England Journal of Medicine. 1991;324(8):532–536. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199102213240805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Glinoer D. The regulation of thyroid function in pregnancy: pathways of endocrine adaptation from physiology to pathology. Endocrine Reviews. 1997;18(3):404–433. doi: 10.1210/edrv.18.3.0300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Evans IM, Pickard MR, Sinha AK, Leonard AJ, Sampson DC, Ekins RP. Influence of maternal hyperthyroidism in the rat on the expression of neuronal and astrocytic cytoskeletal proteins in fetal brain. Journal of Endocrinology. 2002;175(3):597–604. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.1750597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Anselmo J, Cao D, Karrison T, Weiss RE, Refetoff S. Fetal loss associated with excess thyroid hormone exposure. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2004;292(6):691–695. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.6.691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Phoojaroenchanachai M, Sriussadaporn S, Peerapatdit T, et al. Effect of maternal hyperthyroidism during late pregnancy on the risk of neonatal low birth weight. Clinical Endocrinology. 2001;54(3):365–370. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2265.2001.01224.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Millar LK, Wing DA, Leung AS, Koonings PP, Montoro MN, Mestman JH. Low birth weight and preeclampsia in pregnancies complicated by hyperthyroidism. Obstetrics and Gynecology. 1994;84(6):946–949. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lazarus JH. Thyroid disease in pregnancy and childhood. Minerva Endocrinologica. 2005;30(2):71–87. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Luewan S, Chakkabut P, Tongsong T. Outcomes of pregnancy complicated with hyperthyroidism: a cohort study. Archives of Gynecology and Obstetrics. 2011;283(2):243–247. doi: 10.1007/s00404-010-1362-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Surks MI, Ortiz E, Daniels GH, et al. Subclinical thyroid disease: scientific review and guidelines for diagnosis and management. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2004;291(2):228–238. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.2.228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Casey BM, Dashe JS, Wells CE, McIntire DD, Leveno KJ, Cunningham FG. Subclinical hyperthyroidism and pregnancy outcomes. Obstetrics and Gynecology. 2006;107(2 I):337–341. doi: 10.1097/01.AOG.0000197991.64246.9a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wang R, Nelson JC, Weiss RM, Wilcox RB. Accuracy of free thyroxine measurements across natural ranges of thyroxine binding to serum proteins. Thyroid. 2000;10(1):31–39. doi: 10.1089/thy.2000.10.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Nelson JC, Tomei RT. Direct determination of free thyroxin in undiluted serum by equilibrium dialysis/radioimmunoassay. Clinical Chemistry. 1988;34(9):1737–1744. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Nelson JC, Weiss RM, Wilcox RB. Underestimates of serum free thyroxine (T4) concentrations by free T4 immunoassays. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 1994;79(1):76–79. doi: 10.1210/jcem.79.1.8027258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Dunn L, Dunn L. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines, Minn, USA: American Guidance Service; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1(3):385–401. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Caldwell B, Bradley R. Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment. Little Rock, Ark, USA: University of Arkansas; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Kotelchuck M. An evaluation of the Kessner Adequacy of Prenatal Care Index and a proposed Adequacy of Prenatal Care Utilization Index. American Journal of Public Health. 1994;84(9):1414–1420. doi: 10.2105/ajph.84.9.1414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Broadhead WE, Gehlbach SH, de Gruy FV, Kaplan BH. The Duke-UNC Functional Social Support Questionnaire. Measurement of social support in family medicine patients. Medical Care. 1988;26(7):709–723. doi: 10.1097/00005650-198807000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bodnar LM, Siega-Riz AM. A diet quality index for pregnancy detects variation in diet and differences by sociodemographic factors. Public Health Nutrition. 2002;5(6):801–809. doi: 10.1079/PHN2002348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Harley K, Eskenazi B. Time in the United States, social support and health behaviors during pregnancy among women of Mexican descent. Social Science and Medicine. 2006;62(12):3048–3061. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.11.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2nd edition. San Antonio, Tex, USA: The Psychological Corporation; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Wechsler D. WPPSI-III Administration and Scoring Manual. San Antonio, Tex, USA: The Psychological Corporation; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.McCarthy D. Manual for the McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities. New York, NY, USA: The Psychological Corporation; 1972. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Woodcock RW, Munoz-Sandoval AF. Bateria Woodcock-Munoz: Pruebas de Habilidad Cognitiva-Revisada. Itasca, III, USA: The Riverside Publishing Company; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Woodcock RW, Johnson MB. Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability. Itasca, III, USA: The Riverside Publishing Company; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Adams W, Sheslow D. WRAVMA: Wide Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities. Wilmington, Del, USA: Wide Range; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Conners CK. Conners’ K-CPT: Kiddie Continuous Performance Test. North Tonawanda, NY, USA: Multi-Health Systems; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. Clinical Evaluation of Young Children with the McCarthy Scales. New York, NY, USA: Grune & Stratton; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms & Profiles. Burlington, Vt, USA: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth & Families; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Man EB, Kudd DM, Peters JP. Butanol-extractable iodine of serum. The Journal of Clinical Investigation. 1951;30(5):531–538. doi: 10.1172/JCI102469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hastie T, Tibshirani R. Generalized additive models for medical research. Statistical Methods in Medical Research. 1995;4(3):187–196. doi: 10.1177/096228029500400302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Rosner B. Percentage points for a generalized ESD many-outlier procedure. Technometrics. 1983;25(2):165–172. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Young JG, Eskenazi B, Gladstone EA, et al. Association between in utero organophosphate pesticide exposure and abnormal reflexes in neonates. NeuroToxicology. 2005;26(2):199–209. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2004.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Eskenazi B, Marks AR, Bradman A, et al. Organophosphate pesticide exposure and neurodevelopment in young Mexican-American children. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2007;115(5):792–798. doi: 10.1289/ehp.9828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Bravo R, Caltabiano LM, Weerasekera G, et al. Measurement of dialkyl phosphate metabolites of organophosphorus pesticides in human urine using lyophilization with gas chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry and isotope dilution quantification. Journal of Exposure Analysis and Environmental Epidemiology. 2004;14(3):249–259. doi: 10.1038/sj.jea.7500322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Hogan JW, Roy J, Korkontzelou C. Tutorial in biostatistics. Handling drop-out in longitudinal studies. Statistics in Medicine. 2004;23(9):1455–1497. doi: 10.1002/sim.1728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Sinisi SE, van der Laan MJ. Loss-based cross-validated deletion/substitution/addition algorithms in estimation. UC Berkeley Division of Biostatistics Working Paper Series, Working Paper 143, 2004, http://www.bepress.com/ucbbiostat/paper143.
  • 51.Mandel SJ, Spencer CA, Hollowell JG. Are detection and treatment of thyroid insufficiency in pregnancy feasible? Thyroid. 2005;15(1):44–53. doi: 10.1089/thy.2005.15.44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.National Academy of Clinical Biochemistry. Laboratory support for the diagnosis of thyroid disease. In: Demers LM, Spencer CA, editors. Laboratory Medicine Practice Guidelines. Washington, DC, USA: NACB; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Caldwell KL, Miller GA, Wang RY, Jain RB, Jones RL. Iodine status of the U.S. population, National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2003-2004. Thyroid. 2008;18(11):1207–1214. doi: 10.1089/thy.2008.0161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Morreale De Escobar G, Obregon MJ, Escobar Del Rey F. Clinical perspective: is neuropsychological development related to maternal hypothyroidism or to maternal hypothyroxinemia? Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 2000;85(11):3975–3987. doi: 10.1210/jcem.85.11.6961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Contempre B, Jauniaux E, Calvo R, Jurkovic D, Campbell S, De Escobar GM. Detection of thyroid hormones in human embryonic cavities during the first trimester of pregnancy. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 1993;77(6):1719–1722. doi: 10.1210/jcem.77.6.8263162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Bernal J, Pekonen F. Ontogenesis of the nuclear 3,5,3’-triiodothyronine receptor in the human fetal brain. Endocrinology. 1984;114(2):677–679. doi: 10.1210/endo-114-2-677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Ferreiro B, Bernal J, Goodyer CG, Branchard CL. Estimation of nuclear thyroid hormone receptor saturation in human fetal brain and lung during early gestation. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism. 1988;67(4):853–856. doi: 10.1210/jcem-67-4-853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Chevrier J, Eskenazi B, Bradman A, Fenster L, Barr DB. Associations between prenatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and neonatal thyroid-stimulating hormone levels in a Mexican-American population, Salinas Valley, California. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2007;115(10):1490–1496. doi: 10.1289/ehp.9843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Zoeller RT, Rovet J. Timing of thyroid hormone action in the developing brain: clinical observations and experimental findings. Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 2004;16(10):809–818. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2004.01243.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Thyroid Research are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES