Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Oct 28.
Published in final edited form as: Soc Sci Med. 2008 Mar 4;66(8):1828–1840. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.01.025

Social determinants of psychological distress in a nationally-representative sample of South African adults

Landon Myer a,b,*, Dan J Stein c,d, Anna Grimsrud a, Soraya Seedat d, David R Williams e
PMCID: PMC3203636  NIHMSID: NIHMS327461  PMID: 18299167

Abstract

There is substantial evidence from developed countries that lower socioeconomic status (SES) is associated with increased occurrence of mental illness, and growing interest in the role of social support and social capital in mental health. However, there are few data on social determinants of mental health from low- and middle-income nations. We examined the association between psychological distress and SES, social support and bonding social capital in a nationally-representative sample of South African adults. As part of a national survey of mental health, a probability sample of 4351 individuals was interviewed between 2002 and 2004. Non-specific psychological distress was measured using the Kessler K-10 scale. SES was assessed from an aggregate of household income, individual educational and employment status, and household material and financial resources. Social support, bonding social capital and traumatic life events were measured using multi-item scales. The mean age in the sample was 37 years and 76% of participants were black African. Measures of SES and social capital were inversely associated (p < 0.001). Both recent and traumatic life events were more common among individuals with low levels of SES and social support. After adjusting for participant demographic characteristics and life events, high levels of psychological distress were most common among individuals with lower levels of SES and social capital. There was no independent association between levels of social support and psychological distress. The occurrence of recent life events appeared to partially mediate the association between SES and psychological distress (p = 0.035) but not the association involving social capital (p = 0.40). These data demonstrate persistent associations between levels of SES, social capital and psychological distress in South Africa. The increased frequency of recent life events appears to only partially explain higher levels of psychological distress among individuals of lower SES. Additional research is required to understand the temporality of this association as well as mechanisms through which SES and social capital influence mental health in low- and middle-income settings where high levels of poverty and trauma may contribute to excess burden of mental illness.

Keywords: Psychological distress, Mental health, Socioeconomic status (SES), Social support, Social capital, South Africa

Introduction

The study of social factors, and socioeconomic status (SES) in particular, as determinants of mental health has a long history in psychiatric epidemiology. Throughout the 20th century, epidemiological research investigating the distribution of mental health in European and North American populations found an increased burden of disease in groups of lower SES (Faris & Dunham, 1939; Hollingshead & Redlich, 1958; Kesser, 1982). Today, there is general consensus that SES plays a significant role in the aetiology of depression, through mechanisms of both increased individual vulnerability and reduced access to protective resources (Muntaner, Eaton, Miech, & O’Campo, 2004). Prospective studies from Britain (Power, Stansfeld, Matthews, Manor, & Hope, 2002) and the United States (Johnson, Cohen, Dohrenwend, Link, & Brooks, 1999) have found the incidence of depression higher among individuals of lower SES, and meta-analysis has suggested that individuals from the lowest socioeconomic stratum of society are at 1.5–2.0 times the risk of a major depressive episode compared to individuals from the highest socioeconomic stratum (Lorant et al., 2003). There is less evidence regarding the association between SES and different anxiety disorders (Muntaner et al., 2004). Multiple studies have suggested that anxiety disorders may be more common in groups of low socioeconomic status, including post-traumatic stress disorder, generalized anxiety disorder and different phobias (Eaton, Kessler, Wittchen, & Magee, 1994; Foa, Stein, & McFarlane, 2006; Magee, Eaton, Wittchen, McGonagle, & Kessler, 1996; Wittchen, Zhao, Kessler, & Eaton, 1994). However, much of the data on this question come from cross-sectional studies and the selection-causation issue remains unresolved.

While the role of SES in shaping the population mental health is widely recognized, the possible role of other social factors is less well understood. There is evidence to suggest that individual social networks may decrease individual susceptibility to anxiety and depression (Kawachi & Berkman, 2001; Kendler, Myers, & Prescott, 2005; Kessler, Kendler, Heath, Neale, & Eaves, 1992). More recently, the concept of social capital has become popular in mental health research with several studies suggesting that there may be qualities of communities and neighborhoods that contribute to the aetiology or prevention of common mental illnesses (Almedom, 2005), although any causal relationship is likely to be highly context-specific (Caughy, O’Campo, & Muntaner, 2003). Again, insight into these issues comes almost exclusively from high-income countries. One study of women in Lusaka, Zambia and Durban, South Africa suggested that membership in community groups (in this study, primarily church groups) was associated with better self-rated mental health, although adjustments for individual socioeconomic position are not reported (Thomas, 2006). Furthermore, there have been few studies examining the influence of social networks and/or social capital on mental health after adjustment for measures of socioeconomic status; given the potential interrelationships between these constructs, failure to account for their covariance may lead to an overestimation of the association between any one social determinant and mental health outcomes.

Evidence for the association between mental health and SES or other social determinants comes largely from developed countries (Patel, 2001). Several cross-sectional studies have demonstrated associations between different measures of mental health and various markers of socioeconomic status (Patel, Araya, de Lima, Ludermir, & Todd, 1999; Patel & Kleinman, 2003), although few studies have had access to comprehensive measures of SES. One of the only prospective studies of this association conducted to date found that household income was a persistent predictor of incident common mental disorders during 12 months of follow-up of Indian women (Patel, Kirkwood, Pednekar, Weiss, & Mabey, 2006). And, while there are few data on the population prevalence of mental disorders in developing countries, their public health burden is likely to be substantial (WHO, 2001). The focus of research on the social determinants of mental health on high-income countries is part of a broader neglect of mental illness as a public health issue in many low- and middle-income countries (Saraceno et al., 2007). Compared to high-income countries, low- and middle-income countries may have different prevalences of mental disorders (Demyttenaere et al., 2004), as well as different socio-cultural systems and population levels of SES; together, these may point to differences in the major aetiologies of common mental disorders. For instance, one comparison of depression and anxiety levels among women living in Harare, Zimbabwe and London, England suggested that the strength of association between specific types of life events and mental disorders differed in the two countries (Broadhead & Abas, 1998), suggesting that the social determinants of mental health may vary between developed and developing countries.

Despite the importance of social factors, and socioeconomic status in particular, as determinants of mental health, there have been few population-based studies of the social determinants of mental health in developing country settings. To address this gap, we examined the social determinants of non-specific psychological distress in a nationally-representative sample of South African adults. We hypothesized that decreased levels of socioeconomic status, social networks and social capital would each be associated with increased levels of psychological distress, independent of individual demographic characteristics. In addition, we examined the possibility that the associations between each social determinant and psychological distress are explained by increased exposure to negative life events: that individuals of lower socioeconomic status, social networks and/or social capital would be more likely to be exposed to life events, in turn causing them increased psychological distress.

Methods

The South African Stress and Health (SASH) study is a national survey of mental health conducted between January 2002 and June 2004. The study rationale and methods have been described in detail previously (Williams et al., 2004). The survey protocol, including all recruitment, consent and field procedures, was approved by the Human Subjects Committees of the University of Michigan, Harvard Medical School, and by a single project assurance of compliance from the Medical University of South Africa that was approved by the National Institute of Mental Health (USA).

Sampling

A probability sample of South African adults was selected using three-stage design. First, the Enumerator Areas (EAs; a unit of census administration of which there are approximately 85,000 in the country) used in the 2001 national census were stratified according to province, location (rural/urban) and majority population group (African, coloured, white or Indian); 960 EAs were selected from the resulting strata, with the number of EAs selected per stratum proportional to the estimated stratum population (minimum 1; maximum 85). Second, within each enumerator area a random sample of five households was selected. Third, within each household, a single adult was selected to participate using the Kish procedure for objective respondent selection (Kish, 1949). Fieldworkers made up to three attempts to contact selected participants. The overall response rate for the study was 85.5%, and the final sample size was 4351 individuals.

Measures

Interviews were conducted by fieldworkers with special training in psychiatric interviewing working in one of six local languages. Interviews lasted approximately 3 h and sometimes took place over multiple home visits. Participant demographic characteristics, including age, gender, rural/urban location, and marital status, were investigated using standard questionnaire items; race was self-defined by participants. Both location and race are likely to capture variation in socioeconomic position in South Africa. Residents of rural areas have typically had less access to education, services, employment and formal economic structures compared to individuals living in urban centres. The construct of racial groupings has been used as a formal guide to allocate political and economic opportunity in South Africa historically, with individuals of European ancestry (whites) the most favored and black Africans the most discriminated against (here we differentiate between indigenous black Africans, coloureds, whites and Indians, on the basis of historical classification systems).

The Kessler K-10 scale was used to measure global psychological distress, including significant pathology which does not meet formal criteria for a psychiatric illness (Kessler et al., 2002; Kessler et al., 2003). This scale measures the following symptoms over the preceding 30 days by asking: “In the past 30 days, how often did you feel: nervous; so nervous that nothing could calm you down; hopeless; restless or fidgety; so restless that you could not sit still; depressed; that everything was an effort; so sad that nothing could cheer you up; worthless; tired out for no good reason?” The frequency with which each of these items was experienced was recorded using a five-point likert scale ranging from “none of the time” to “all the time”. This score was then summed with increasing scores reflecting an increasing degree of psychological distress. The K-10 has been shown to capture variability related to non-specific depression, anxiety and substance abuse, but does not measure suicidality or psychoses (Brooks, Beard, & Steel, 2006). This scale serves to identify individuals who are likely to meet formal definitions for anxiety and/or depressive disorders, as well as to identify individuals with sub-clinical illness who may not meet formal definitions for a specific disorder (Kessler et al., 2002). This scale is increasingly used in population mental health research and has been validated in multiple settings (Andrews & Slade, 2001). We examined the K-10 scale using ordinal categories for low, moderate, high and very high psychological distress (scores of 10–19, 20–24, 25–29 and ≥30, respectively) and as a binary variable comparing scores of 0–29 versus ≥30. The K-10 scale was strongly predictive of both anxiety and depressive disorders (as defined by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, Fourth Edition, and measured using the World Health Organization Composite International Diagnostic Interview Version 3.0 [Kessler & Ustun, 2004]), including agoraphobia, post-traumatic stress disorder, panic disorder, social phobia, generalized anxiety disorder, as well as minor and major depressive disorders (p < 0.0001 for all associations).

The measurement of recent life events was based on the Life Event schedule of the World Mental Health Survey. This investigated events during the previous 12 months in 13 areas based on whether the participant had experienced (1) illness or injury; (2) physical attack; (3) robbery; (4) death of someone close; (5) separation from spouse or partner; (6) end of another close relationship; (7) being fired from a job; (8) retiring from a job against the participant’s will; (9) losing a job for another reason; (10) searching for employment without success for over a month; (11) major financial crisis; (12) problems with the police; or (13) whether someone close to the participant had experienced illness, injury or physical attack. Lifetime experience of trauma was measured through 28 items used to screen for post-traumatic stress disorder in the World Mental Health survey, including experiences of accidents, illness, loss of loved ones, abuse, crime, war and natural disasters. Recent and lifetime traumatic life event were categorized into ordinal variables of none, 1–2, 3–4 and 5 or more life events.

Socioeconomic status was investigated using estimated household income, years of participant education, and participant employment. Because these traditional markers of socioeconomic status may not adequately capture variation in socioeconomic position in developing country settings where there is a substantial informal economy and resource sharing is common (particularly in rural areas where bartering of goods and services may act in the place of income-based wealth) (Myer, Ehrlich, & Susser, 2004), we used an asset index based on 17 items reflecting individual and household wealth. This was based on: household ownership of appliances (refrigerator/freezer; vacuum/floor cleaner; television; video cassette recorder; radio; microwave; and washing machine), other household resources (telephone; running water in the home; kitchen sink; flush toilet; automobile; domestic servant; and stove/hot-plate) and financial activities which participants engaged in (shopping at a supermarket; using financial services such as a bank account or credit card; and having an account at a retail store). This index demonstrated excellent reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.92). The measures of asset ownership were used to construct an aggregate asset score, which was categorized into categories for 0–5, 6–12 and 13–17 assets.

In addition to reporting measures of SES (participant income, education, employment and asset index) individually, we standardized the raw income, education and asset index values, then summed them to create an aggregate measure of SES. This standardized measure was divided into tertiles for analysis corresponding to low, moderate and high SES.

The construct of social support was based on five items in the social networks section of the World Mental Health survey schedule. These included items capturing aspects of social support (based on the frequency of contacts with family and friends other than those who are living together) as well as emotional support measures (based on the ability to rely on family or friends if the participant had a problem, the ability of participants to open up to their family or friends, and having someone with whom participants could share private feelings and concerns) (Table 1). These items were summed to form an aggregate scale in which each item received equal weighting. This scale was divided into tertiles to reflect low, moderate and high levels of social support.

Table 1.

Description of participant demographic characteristics, socioeconomic status, social support and social capital in a nationally-representative sample of South African adults

% or Mean
Demographic characteristics
 Age (mean) (years) 37.0
  18–29 39.1
  30–39 22.1
  40–49 18.1
  50+ 20.7
 Gender
  Female 53.7
  Male 46.3
 Race
  Black 76.2
  Coloured 10.4
  White 10.0
  Indian/Asian 3.4
 Currently married 50.1
 Location
  Urban 61.6
  Rural 38.4
Socioeconomic status
 Education
  None 6.8
  Grade 1–7 19.1
  Grade 8–11 35.4
  Completed high school 23.4
  Post-high school education 15.3
 Employed 31.1
 Household income (mean) R59,404
  R0 13.7
  R1–5000 29.5
  R5001–25,000 15.4
  R25,000–100,000 19.6
  R100,001+ 21.8
 Assets owned by household: 0–5 39.3
  6–12 37.4
  13–17 23.3
Social support
 Contact with family member: nearly every day* 22.7
 Contact with friends: nearly every day* 40.3
 Can rely on family/friends if in a serious problem: a lot** 57.7
 Can open up to family/friends to talk about worries: a lot** 49.3
 Someone in life to share private feelings & concerns: Yes*** 81.7
Social capital
 How often do you get together with neighbours: a lot**** 36.5
 How often are there problems with crime in area: very often***** 23.1
 Are there community groups in this area: Yes*** 59.8
 Are you involved in any community groups: Yes*** 31.9
*

These items are coded as: nearly every day (4+ times/week), 3–4 days/week, 1–2 days/week, 1–3 days/month, less than once a month, never.

**

These items are coded as: a lot, some, little, not at all.

***

These items are coded as: Yes or No.

****

This item is coded as: nearly every day (4+ times/week); 1–3 times/week; 2–3 times a month; at least once a month; a few times a year; never.

*****

This item is coded as: very often; fairly often; not too often; hardly ever; never; this item was reverse-coded, so that higher perceived levels of crime were associated with lower social capital.

We examined the bonding form of social capital at the individual level using on four items from the National Survey of American Life (Jackson et al., 2004), which were adapted to the South African context. These were: (1) how often participants spend time with their neighbours (a structural measure reflecting community cohesiveness); (2) participants’ perceptions of crime in the area (a cognitive measure of social trust); (3) whether the participant knows of local civic groups (such as social clubs or community associations) (a cognitive measure of community cohesiveness); and (4) whether the participant is a member of any such community group (a structural measure of community cohesiveness) (Table 1). These items were recoded so that higher scores (including lower levels of crime) contributed to higher levels of bonding social capital, and then summed to form an aggregate scale in which each item received equal weighting. This scale was divided into tertiles to reflect low, moderate and high levels of bonding social capital.

Analysis

Data were analysed using Stata Version 9.2 (Stata Corporation, College Station, USA). All analyses accounted for the complex survey design using person-level weights that incorporated sample selection, non-response and post-stratification factors. Calculations for estimation and inference were based on the Taylor series linearization method. We examined the crude associations between psychological distress (dependent variable) and participants’ life events, socioeconomic status, social support and social capital (independent variables) using Pearson’s chi-squared test for comparison of proportions and a corrected t-test for comparison of means. All statistical tests are two-sided at α = 0.05.

We developed a series of multiple logistic regression models to examine the independent effects of recent traumatic life events, socioeconomic status, social support and social capital on psychological distress. The first series of models examined separately the crude associations between the different social constructs (SES, social support and social capital) and psychological distress. Following this we examined each social construct separately, but adjusted for participant demographic characteristics (age, rural/urban location, race, and gender). We then included all the social constructs in a single model with demographic variables, to determine their independent associations. Finally, we added recent and lifetime negative life events to the latter model, in order to examine the potential for mediation of the association between any of social constructs and the prevalence of psychological distress (Susser, Schwartz, Morabia, & Bromet, 2006). Mediation was formally assessed using the Aroian test based on the analysis of the continuous, standardized social construct scales (MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets, 2002; Shrout & Bolger, 2002). Potential heterogeneity in associations with demographic characteristics and social constructs was examined using stratified analyses and interaction terms based on the Box–Tidwell transformation. The coefficients from all regression models are reported as odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CI).

Results

The demographic characteristics and socioeconomic characteristics of the study sample are presented in Table 1. Three-quarters of the sample was black African, and 61% of participants lived in an urban area. Slightly more than one-third of participants had completed high school, and one-fifth of individuals lived in households with more than R100,000 (approximately, US $16,000) per annum.

Strong associations were observed among measures of socioeconomic status, social support and social capital. Social support and social capital were positively associated (Pearson’s correlation coefficient for the scale values, 0.114, p < 0.001). Levels of social support and social capital were unevenly distributed across socioeconomic strata in opposing directions. Increasing social support was associated with increasing socioeconomic position (42% of individuals with high levels of social support were of high SES, compared to 26% of individuals with low levels of social support, p < 0.001). Conversely, increasing social capital was associated with decreasing socioeconomic position: 27% of individuals with high levels of social capital were also of high SES, compared to 41% of individuals with low levels of social capital (p < 0.001).

Each measure of SES was strongly patterned according to participant location, with individuals living in rural settings having significantly lower levels of education, employment, family income, and access to household assets, compared to individuals in urban settings. For example, 7% of individuals living in rural settings had access to 13 or more household assets, compared to 33% of individuals living in urban settings (p < 0.0001). In addition, living in an urban setting was associated with lower levels of social capital (40% of individuals in urban settings reported low levels of social capital compared to 28% of participants living in rural settings, p < 0.0001) but there was no difference in levels of social support between individuals living in urban and rural settings (36% of individuals in rural settings reported low levels of social support compared to 33% of those in urban settings, p = 0.094).

Overall, 73% of the study sample scored between 10 and 19 on the K-10 scale, reflecting low levels of psychological distress, 11% showed moderate levels (scores 20–24), 8% showed high levels (scores 25–29), and 8% showed very high levels of distress (scores of 30 or higher). In unadjusted analyses, increased psychological distress was associated with age over 30 years, female gender, black African racial group (relative to white racial group), and rural location (Table 2). Increasing psychological distress was strongly associated with lower SES: compared to participants of high SES, individuals of moderate SES had 1.56 times the odds of psychological distress, and individual of low SES had 2.99 times the odds of psychological distress (p-value for trend <0.001). Similarly, low levels of both social support and social capital were moderately associated with increased psychological distress compared to the highest strata of each category (OR, 95% CI: 1.62, 1.11–2.37 and 1.56, 1.05–2.32, respectively).

Table 2.

Unadjusted associations between 30-day psychological distress and socioeconomic status, social support and social capital, in a nationally-representative sample of South African adults

Category of psychological distress (total K-10 score)
Comparison of 30+ versus <30
Low (0–19) Moderate (20–24) High (25–29) Very high (30+) p-Value Odds ratio 95% CI
Overall prevalence 73.3 10.7 7.9 8.2
Demographic characteristics
 Age
  18–29 40.1 41.2 35.8 30.5 0.324 1.0 (Reference)
  30–39 21.4 23.0 25.0 24.1 1.44 1.06–1.97
  40–49 17.7 18.1 18.8 22.0 1.62 1.10–2.38
  50+ 20.8 17.7 21.5 23.4 1.49 1.04–2.14
 Gender
  Female 52.0 57.1 53.3 64.6 0.003 1.64 1.19–2.26
 Race
  Black African 73.6 81.8 82.8 85.8 0.015 1.0 (Reference)
  Coloured 11.0 7.8 10.0 9.8 0.82 0.49–1.36
  White 11.7 7.8 5.1 2.2 0.18 0.08–0.39
  Indian/Asian 3.7 2.6 2.2 2.3 0.59 0.26–1.31
 Currently married 49.7 49.4 51.1 54.1 0.583 1.18 0.94–1.51
 Urban location 62.4 64.2 58.5 54.1 0.126 0.71 0.52–0.98
Socioeconomic status
 High 39.7 30.8 27.2 21.4 <0.001 1.0 (Reference)
 Moderate 33.2 35.1 35.4 29.8 1.56 1.06–2.29
 Low 27.1 34.1 37.4 48.9 2.99 2.01–4.44
Social support
 High 35.3 33.8 28.1 26.1 0.014 1.0 (Reference)
 Moderate 34.2 30.2 31.5 34.7 1.37 0.93–2.01
 Low 30.4 36.0 40.4 39.2 1.62 1.11–2.37
Social capital
 High 33.5 29.6 27.4 24.4 0.035 1.0 (Reference)
 Moderate 31.9 35.7 36.4 34.9 1.42 1.02–1.79
 Low 34.7 34.7 36.1 40.7 1.56 1.05–2.32
Recent life events
 None 40.1 26.5 19.7 15.0 <0.001 1.0 (Reference)
 1–2 40.7 35.6 37.5 36.7 2.26 1.64–3.11
 3–4 14.5 23.7 24.8 26.9 4.02 2.60–6.21
 5 or more 4.7 14.1 18.0 21.4 7.58 4.93–11.63
Traumatic life events
 None 29.3 20.8 17.5 19.1 <0.001 1.0 (Reference)
 1–2 50.7 46.8 51.3 45.9 1.31 0.92–1.86
 3–4 9.5 14.5 15.1 13.3 1.80 1.08–3.02
 5 or more 10.5 17.9 16.1 21.7 2.62 1.71–4.01

There was a clear dose–response relationship between traumatic life events and psychological distress in the previous 30 days (Table 2) (Williams et al., 2007). Compared to individuals not experiencing any recent life events, individuals experiencing 1–2 life events in the previous 12 months had 2.3 times the odds of psychological distress; individuals experiencing 3–4 recent life events had 4 times the odds of psychological distress; and individuals reporting 5 or more recent life events had more than 7 times the odds of psychological distress. A similar gradient, with a somewhat weaker overall magnitude, was observed for traumatic life events. Experience of recent life events was strongly associated with SES, and this association was consistent across each measure of SES (Table 3). Compared to individuals in the highest tertile of SES, individuals in the lowest tertile were 1.55 times as likely to experience recent life events (95% CI: 1.26–1.90). Lower levels of social support were also associated with increased recent life events; however, there was no association between strata of social capital and recent life events.

Table 3.

Unadjusted associations between recent (12 month) life events and socioeconomic status, social support and social capital, in a nationally-representative sample of South African adults

Recent life events
Comparison of 3+ versus none
None 1–2 3–4 5+ p-Value OR 95% CI
Overall prevalence 35.0 39.5 17.3 8.1
Socioeconomic status
 Education
  None 6.4 7.8 6.5 4.4 0.001 1.0 (Reference)
  Grade 1–7 18.4 18.5 20.0 23.3 0.97 0.68–1.39
  Grade 8–11 35.6 36.3 36.9 39.1 1.04 0.71–1.52
  Completed high school 22.6 23.2 26.1 22.5 0.97 0.68–1.38
  Post-high school education 20.0 14.3 10.6 10.8 0.59 0.38–0.90
 Employed 38.3 29.6 24.0 21.3 <0.001 0.59 0.47–0.74
 Household income
  R0 12.0 13.3 16.4 16.9 <0.001 1.0 (Reference)
  R1–5000 24.7 30.8 35.5 31.8 0.94 0.76–1.16
  R5001–25,000 15.5 15.8 12.8 18.2 0.71 0.54–0.94
  R25,000–10,0000 22.5 19.0 17.6 14.6 0.62 0.46–0.81
  R100,001+ 25.4 21.2 17.7 18.5 0.59 0.43–0.81
Assets owned by household
  0–5 35.9 40.0 44.6 43.9 <0.001 1.0 (Reference)
  6–12 32.9 39.1 39.9 43.5 1.03 0.85–1.25
  13–17 31.8 21.3 15.5 12.6 0.50 0.39–0.64
Socioeconomic status
 High 43.1 34.6 30.8 27.0 <0.001 1.0 (Reference)
 Moderate 29.8 34.8 34.7 38.6 1.45 1.12–1.87
 Low 27.1 30.7 34.6 34.4 1.55 1.26–1.90
Social support
 High 35.4 34.4 32.7 26.9 0.027 1.0 (Reference)
 Moderate 35.8 32.4 33.2 31.2 1.08 0.86–1.37
 Low 28.9 33.25 34.1 41.9 1.33 1.03–1.72
Social capital
 High 31.7 33.4 31.6 34.8 0.087 1.0 (Reference)
 Moderate 31.4 32.2 3435 39.8 1.26 1.02–1.57
 Low 36.9 34.4 33.9 35.4 1.07 0.87–1.32

In multivariate analysis, the association between each social construct and psychological distress persisted after adjusting for participant demographic characteristics (Table 4, panel A). However, when all the social constructs were entered into single model, the association between social support and psychological distress was attenuated after adjustment for joint confounding by SES and social capital (panel B). When both recent and traumatic life events were added to this model as putative pathways through which social determinants influenced psychological distress, the associations involving SES and social capital both persisted (panel C). Comparing the models with and without adjustment for recent life events (panels B and C), recent life events appeared to mediate the associations between SES and psychological distress (Aroian test p-value, 0.035). However, the occurrence of recent life events was not an intermediate factor in the association between social capital and psychological distress (p = 0.400).

Table 4.

Results of logistic regression models predicting relative odds of 30-day psychological distress according to participant demographic characteristics, social determinants, and life events, in a nationally-representative sample of South African adults

(A) Model adjusted for demographic characteristics only*
(B) Model adjusted for demographic characteristics and social constructs
(C) Model adjusted for demographic characteristics, social constructs and life events
OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI
Socioeconomic status
 High 1.0 (Reference) 1.0 (Reference) 1.0 (Reference)
 Moderate 1.23 0.81–1.86 1.22 0.81–1.84 1.17 0.76–1.78
 Low 2.22 1.43–3.45 2.20 1.42–3.40 2.11 1.36–3.29
Social support
 High 1.0 (Reference) 1.0 (Reference) 1.0 (Reference)
 Moderate 1.34 0.91–1.97 1.30 0.87–1.92 1.32 0.90–1.95
 Low 1.51 1.03–2.19 1.36 0.92–2.00 1.27 0.88–1.85
Social capital
 High 1.0 (Reference) 1.0 (Reference) 1.0 (Reference)
 Moderate 1.49 1.07–2.08 1.50 1.08–2.08 1.38 1.01–1.87
 Low 1.76 1.17–2.64 1.73 1.16–2.59 1.67 1.12–2.49
Recent life events
 None 1.0 (Reference)
 1–2 1.98 1.41–2.77
 3–4 3.27 2.04–5.25
 5 or more 6.21 3.95–9.78
Traumatic life events
 None 1.0 (Reference)
 1–2 1.09 0.75–1.58
 3–4 1.26 0.73–2.16
 5 or more 1.82 1.16–2.88
*

Demographic characteristics included in these models are: age category, gender, race, and location (urban/rural).

Results presented as odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CI).

The adjusted association between decreased social capital and increased psychological distress was strongest in the lowest stratum of SES (OR, 2.06) compared to the highest SES stratum (OR, 1.54) although this interaction did not reach statistical significance (p = 0.112). There was also no evidence for modification in the association between traumatic life events and psychological distress by strata of SES (p = 0.700). When the final model was restricted to black Africans, the overall association between SES and psychological distress remained (OR comparing lowest to highest tertile of SES, 1.87; 95% CI: 1.02–3.41; p = 0.048), although the association between social capital and psychological distress was not statistically significant (OR comparing lowest to highest tertile of social capital, 1.59; 95% CI: 0.31–7.99; p = 0.171). The associations between each social determinant and psychological distress did not change appreciably when the final model was stratified by location of residence (rural or urban) or participant gender (not shown).

In a separate analysis of individual SES measures, increasing levels of education, household income and household asset ownership all showed a strong graded relationship with decreased levels of psychological distress (Table 5, panel A). These associations were only slightly attenuated when adjusted for participant demographic characteristics (panel B). However, when all measures of socioeconomic status were entered into the same model, few of the associations remained statistically significant (panel C).

Table 5.

Results of logistic regression models of the associations between 30-day psychological distress and individual measures of socioeconcomic status in a nationally-representative sample of South African adults

(A) Model of unadjusted associations involving individual measures
(B) Model of associations adjusted for participant demographic characteristics only*
(C) Model of associations adjusted for participant demographic characteristics and other measures of SES in table*
OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI
Education
 None 1.0 (Reference) 1.0 (Reference) 1.0 (Reference)
 Grade 1–7 1.10 0.65–1.86 1.09 0.65–1.82 1.12 0.66–1.89
 Grade 8–11 0.83 0.55–1.26 0.96 0.62–1.46 1.01 0.64–1.61
 Completed high school 0.39 0.22–0.67 0.50 0.28–0.88 0.56 0.30–1.03
 Post-high school education 0.32 0.14–0.71 0.46 0.19–1.11 0.66 0.28–1.54
Employed 0.58 0.43–0.78 0.62 0.45–0.86 0.70 0.50–0.99
Household income
 R0 (None) 1.0 (Reference) 1.0 (Reference) 1.0 (Reference)
 R1–5000 0.83 0.61–1.15 0.80 0.58–1.10 0.91 0.64–1.31
 R5001–25,000 0.60 0.38–0.92 0.65 0.41–1.01 0.83 0.50–1.37
 R25,000–100,000 0.52 0.34–0.79 0.60 0.40–0.89 0.75 0.49–1.14
 R100,001+ 0.41 0.27–0.63 0.49 0.31–0.77 0.60 0.35–1.01
Assets owned by household
 0–5 1.0 (Reference) 1.0 (Reference) 1.0 (Reference)
 6–12 0.72 0.53–0.98 0.74 0.55–1.01 0.88 0.63–1.21
 13–17 0.34 0.20–0.57 0.48 0.25–0.91 0.79 0.42–1.49
*

Demographic characteristics included in these models are: age category, gender, race, and location (urban/rural).

Results presented as odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CI).

Discussion

This analysis provides novel insights into the social determinants of population mental health in resource-limited settings. We found that decreased levels of SES and social capital were persistently associated with increased non-specific psychological distress experienced in the previous 30 days. The association between SES and psychological distress was mediated in part by recent life events. Levels of social support were less strongly associated with psychological distress, however, and there was no evidence for negative life events as a mediating factor.

The findings for a persistent gradient between decreased SES and increased psychological distress parallels the body of evidence for the general association observed in Europe, Australia and North America. Adding to the results of previous studies, this analysis demonstrated that the SES-psychological distress gradient operated independently of levels of social support and social capital, constructs that are often not included in analyses of SES and mental health. The data also suggest that lower levels of SES are associated with the increased occurrence of life events, which in turn is associated with increased psychological distress. However, there was still a substantial residual effect of SES on psychological distress after accounting for variation in traumatic and recent life events. Part of this residual effect may be attributed to incomplete measures of life events; although the measures employed here of recent and lifetime negative life events include a broad range of experiences, they are unlikely to capture all aspects of negative life events that may be shaped by SES, and in turn influence individual psychological distress (Turner, Wheaton, & Lloyd, 1995). It is also likely that many of the social conditions which contribute to psychological distress are not captured by measures of discrete life events, but instead are better considered as regular features of living in poverty and inequality; in this light, it may be unrealistic to expect that the influence of SES on psychological distress would be wholly mediated by any measure of life events. In addition, it is likely that there are other pathways through which reduced SES influences mental health. Such alternate pathways are likely to include environmental stressors as well as access to protective resources such as education and health services. There was no suggestion in these data that the strength of association between negative life events and psychological distress differed by levels of SES, suggesting that levels of SES are unlikely to alter vulnerability to negative life events. Additional research is required to examine the different mechanisms through which wealth and poverty influence population mental health, both to identify the most vulnerable subpopulations and to target health promotion interventions aimed at reducing the burden of mental illness.

We also found a persistent association between decreased levels of social capital and psychological distress, independent of participants’ levels of social support and SES. This association is in keeping with the results of several previous studies (Almedom, 2005; Caughy et al., 2003; De Silva, Huttly, Harpham, & Kenward, 2007) but is among the first such findings from a population-based study in a developing country. There was no association between this measure of social capital and the experience of life events, and in turn no evidence for the mediation of this association by negative life events. Taken together, these results suggest that reduced levels of social capital may be associated with increased psychological distress through other mechanisms, including increased individual psychological resources (such as self-esteem or self-efficacy), a possibility that requires additional research. The persistent associations of lower levels of both SES and social capital with increased psychological distress was observed despite the inverse association between these two measures, with higher levels of social capital observed among individuals of lower SES, on average. While such a finding may appear counterintuitive, it points to the possibility of social capital as a resource which can act to offset the negative mental health impacts of reduced socioeconomic status (Thomas, 2006).

These results should be interpreted in light of several important limitations. First, the cross-sectional nature of the study design makes the temporal order of the observed associations difficult to distinguish. Specifically, it is not possible to discern whether individual psychological distress affects social and economic conditions, whether social and economic conditions influence psychological distress, or whether the relationship is bi-directional. The K-10 measure of non-specific psychological distress refers only to symptoms in the 30 days before the interview, and the measure of recent negative life events refers to the previous 12 months; in contrast to this, measures of SES, social capital and social networks did not have a reference period, and are likely to be highly stable over a period of months to years. Thus it seems plausible that individual SES precedes recent negative life events, which in turn precede psychological distress. However, prospective studies are necessary to discern the temporality of these associations more definitively. It is important to note that we focused on participants’ current SES, and did not examine childhood socioeconomic position, which may be a separate determinant of mental health outcomes (Gilman, Kawachi, Fitzmaurice, & Buka, 2002; Power et al., 2002).

Second, the social constructs studied here are complex phenomena that can be difficult to measure. The measures of SES used here, incorporating both traditional SES constructs (employment, education, income) as well as more locally-appropriate measures of wealth (based on material wealth, household resources and access to financial institutions), is likely to capture meaningful variability across the different economic systems which operate in a middle-income country such as South Africa (Myer et al., 2004). We found that each measure of SES showed similar associations with psychological distress, even after adjusting for participant demographic characteristics; with growing evidence that different measures of SES may capture different pathways of disease causation in different populations (Braveman et al., 2005), the associations between individual measures of SES and specific mental disorders requires more attention. The measure of social support is also robust, but does not capture the negative aspects of social relations (such as conflict and interpersonal demands). There is evidence to suggest that the negative aspects of social relations may be more important than positive support in the aetiology of mental health (Lincoln, Chatters, & Taylor, 2003), a possibility that warrants more attention.

Social capital is particularly complex and context-specific, with no well-established method of measuring of this construct across populations (Harpham, Grant, & Thomas, 2002). The measures used here focus on cognitive and structural aspects of bonding social capital assessed at the individual level. Several components of bonding social capital were not included here, and these measures focused entirely on social capital measured at the individual level. Future research is required to explore how other aspects of bonding social capital, as well as bridging social capital and ecological measures, relate to mental health outcomes. Nonetheless, we found a persistent association between these measures of bonding social capital and psychological distress after adjusting for individual demographic characteristics, social support and socioeconomic status, suggesting that the items used here do capture meaningful variability in health-related social perceptions and behaviors. While these findings provide preliminary evidence for this association, the measurement of social capital in low- and middle-income settings requires more attention particularly in the context of mental health research. In this analysis, we may anticipate that any measurement error in our assessment of social capital occurred independently of the true level of social capital, leading to the observed association between social capital and mental health being attenuated; with more precise measures adapted for use in South Africa, we may expect to see a stronger link between low levels of social capital and increased psychological distress.

South Africa has a unique historical context, high levels of violence and trauma, and greater socio-cultural and economic diversity than many other middle-income countries. While this diversity provides variability in social conditions, it also presents significant challenges to the study of the social determinants of mental health. Mental health measures may have different meanings in different groups within the South African population, the most relevant types of life events may vary across settings, and the most relevant aspects of social networks and social capital may differ between communities. This potential diversity points to the need to better understand the validity of these measures and other constructs in public mental health research within South Africa.

This research focused on non-specific psychological distress as a general mental health outcome. The same measure, with appropriate translation, was applied across multiple socio-cultural and linguistic groups and the possibility that the validity of the K-10 varies in different contexts requires future research attention. And while this may be a robust measure of population mental health, it collapses together different forms of pathology which may have differing social aetiologies. Additional research is required to understand how social determinants influence specific disorders, with particular attention to common mood and anxiety disorders that are associated with the greatest population-level burden of disease. In addition, while these data come from a representative sample of the South African population, these findings should be generalized with caution. With a growing body of research on population mental health in developing countries, including the work of the World Mental Health Survey Initiative, the social determinants of mental health outcomes can be assessed in additional low- and middle-income countries (Kessler, Haro, Heeringa, Pennell, & Ustun, 2006).

These cross-sectional data demonstrate that lower levels of SES and social capital are independently associated with increased levels of psychological distress in South Africa, paralleling results from developed countries. While life events appeared to partially mediate the association between decreased SES and increased distress, further prospective research is needed to understand the temporality of this association as well as the mechanisms through which poverty contributes to mental illness in low- and middle-income countries.

Acknowledgments

The South African Stress and Health study was carried out in conjunction with the World Health Organization World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative. We thank the WMH staff for assistance with instrumentation, fieldwork, and data analysis. These activities were supported by the United States National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH070884), the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation, the Pfizer Foundation, the US Public Health Service (R13-MH066849, R01-MH069864, and R01 DA016558), the Fogarty International Center (FIRCA R01-TW006481), the Pan American Health Organization, Eli Lilly and Company, Ortho-McNeil Pharmaceutical, Inc., GlaxoSmithKline, and Bristol-Myers Squibb. A complete list of WMH publications can be found at http://www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/wmh/.

Footnotes

The South Africa Stress and Health study was funded by grant R01-MH059575 from the National Institute of Mental Health and the National Institute of Drug Abuse with supplemental funding from the South African Department of Health and the University of Michigan. Dan Stein and Soraya Seedat are also supported by the Medical Research Council (MRC) of South Africa.

References

  1. Almedom AM. Social capital and mental health: an interdisciplinary review of primary evidence. Social Science & Medicine. 2005;61(5):943–964. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.12.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Andrews G, Slade T. Interpreting scores on the Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K10) Australia & New Zealand Journal of Public Health. 2001;25(6):494–497. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-842x.2001.tb00310.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Braveman PA, Cubbin C, Egerter S, Chideya S, Marchi KS, Metzler M, et al. Socioeconomic status in health research: one size does not fit all. JAMA. 2005;294:2879–2888. doi: 10.1001/jama.294.22.2879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Broadhead JC, Abas MA. Life events, difficulties and depression among women in an urban setting in Zimbabwe. Psychological Medicine. 1998;28:29–38. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797005618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brooks RT, Beard J, Steel Z. Factor structure and interpretation of the K10. Psychological Assessment. 2006;18(1):62–70. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.18.1.62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Caughy MO, O’Campo PJ, Muntaner C. When being alone might be better: neighborhood poverty, social capital, and child mental health. Social Science & Medicine. 2003;57(2):227–237. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(02)00342-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Demyttenaere K, Bruffaerts R, Posada-Villa J, Gasquet I, Kovess V, Lepine JP, et al. Prevalence, severity, and unmet need for treatment of mental disorders in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. JAMA. 2004;291(21):2581–2590. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.21.2581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. De Silva MJ, Huttly SR, Harpham T, Kenward MG. Social capital and mental health: a comparative analysis of four low income countries. Social Science & Medicine. 2007;64:5–20. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.08.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Eaton W, Kessler RC, Wittchen H, Magee WJ. Panic and panic disorder in the United States. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1994;151:413–420. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.3.413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Faris RE, Dunham W. Mental disorders in urban areas: An ecological study of schizophrenia and other psychoses. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1939. [Google Scholar]
  11. Foa EB, Stein DJ, McFarlane AC. Symptomatology and psychopathology of mental health problems after disaster. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2006;67(Suppl. 2):15–25. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gilman SE, Kawachi I, Fitzmaurice GM, Buka SL. Socioeconomic status in childhood and the lifetime risk of adult depression. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2002;31:359–367. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Harpham T, Grant E, Thomas E. Measuring social capital within health surveys: key issues. Health Policy & Planning. 2002;17(1):106–111. doi: 10.1093/heapol/17.1.106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hollingshead AB, Redlich FC. Social class and mental illness: A community study. New York: Wiley & Sons; 1958. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Jackson JS, Torres M, Caldwell CH, Neighbors HW, Nesse RM, Taylor RJ, et al. The National Survey of American Life: a study of racial, ethnic and cultural influences on mental disorders and mental health. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research. 2004;13(4):196–207. doi: 10.1002/mpr.177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Johnson JG, Cohen P, Dohrenwend BP, Link BG, Brooks JS. A longitudinal investigation of social causation and social selection processes involved in the association between socioeconomic status and psychiatric disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1999;108:490–499. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.108.3.490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kawachi I, Berkman LF. Social ties and mental health. Journal of Urban Health. 2001;78(3):458–467. doi: 10.1093/jurban/78.3.458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kendler KS, Myers J, Prescott CA. Sex differences in the relationship between social support and risk for major depression: a longitudinal study of opposite-sex twin pairs. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162(2):250–256. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.2.250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kesser RC. A disaggregation of the relationship between socioeconomic status and psychological distress. American Sociology Review. 1982;47(6):752–764. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kessler RC, Andrews G, Colpe LJ, Hiripi E, Mroczek DK, Normand ST, et al. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in nonspecific psychological distress. Psychological Methods. 2002;32:959–976. doi: 10.1017/s0033291702006074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kessler RC, Barker PR, Colpe LJ, Epstein JF, Gfroerer JC, Hiripi E, et al. Screening for serious mental illness in the general population. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60(2):184–189. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.2.184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kessler RC, Haro JM, Heeringa SG, Pennell BE, Ustun TB. The World Health Organization World Mental Health Survey Initiative. Epidemiologia e psichiatria sociale. 2006;15(3):161–166. doi: 10.1017/s1121189x00004395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kessler RC, Kendler KS, Heath AC, Neale MC, Eaves LJ. Social support, depressed mood, and adjustment to stress: a genetic epidemiologic investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1992;62:257–272. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.62.2.257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kessler RC, Ustun TB. The World Mental Health Survey Initiative Version of the World Health Organization Composite International Diagnostic Interview. International Journal of Methods Psychiatric Research. 2004;13:93–121. doi: 10.1002/mpr.168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kish L. A procedure for objective respondent selection within the household. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1949;44:380–387. [Google Scholar]
  26. Lincoln KD, Chatters LM, Taylor RJ. Psychological distress among black and white Americans: differential effects of social support, negative interaction, and personal control. Journal of Health & Social Behavior. 2003;44(3):309–407. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lorant V, Deliege D, Eaton W, Robert A, Philippot P, Ansseau M. Socioeconomic inequalities in depression: a meta-analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2003;157(2):98–112. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Hoffman JM, West SG, Sheets V. A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods. 2002;7:83–104. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.1.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Magee WJ, Eaton W, Wittchen H, McGonagle KA, Kessler RC. Agoraphobia, simple phobia, and social phobia in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1996;53:159–168. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830020077009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Muntaner C, Eaton WW, Miech R, O’Campo P. Socioeconomic position and major mental disorders. Epidemiology Reviews. 2004;26:53–62. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxh001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Myer L, Ehrlich RI, Susser ES. Social epidemiology in South Africa. Epidemiology Reviews. 2004;26:112–123. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxh004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Patel V. Poverty, inequality and mental health in developing countries. In: Leon D, Walt G, editors. Poverty, inequality and health. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2001. pp. 247–262. [Google Scholar]
  33. Patel V, Araya R, de Lima M, Ludermir A, Todd C. Women, poverty and common mental disorders in four restructuring societies. Social Science & Medicine. 1999;49:1461–1471. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(99)00208-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Patel V, Kirkwood BR, Pednekar S, Weiss H, Mabey D. Risk factors for common mental disorders in women: population-based longitudinal study. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;189:547–555. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.022558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Patel V, Kleinman A. Poverty & common mental disorders in developing countries. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 2003;81:609–615. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Power C, Stansfeld SA, Matthews S, Manor O, Hope S. Childhood and adulthood risk factors for socio-economic differentials in psychological distress: evidence from the 1958 British birth cohort. Social Science & Medicine. 2002;55:1989–2004. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(01)00325-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Saraceno B, van Ommeren M, Batniji R, Cohen A, Gureje O, Mahoney J, et al. Barriers to improvement of mental health services in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet. 2007;370(9593):1164–1174. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61263-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Shrout PE, Bolger N. Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods. 2002;7(4):422–445. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Susser S, Schwartz S, Morabia A, Bromet E. Psychiatric epidemiology: Searching for the causes of mental disorders. New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  40. Thomas L. Social capital and mental health of women living in informal settlements in Durban, South Africa and Lusaka, Zambia. In: McKenzie K, Harpam T, editors. Social capital and mental health. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers; 2006. pp. 124–137. [Google Scholar]
  41. Turner RJ, Wheaton B, Lloyd DA. The Epidemiology of social stress. American Sociology Review. 1995;60(1):104–125. [Google Scholar]
  42. Williams DR, Herman A, Kessler RC, Sonnega J, Seedat S, Stein DJ, et al. The South Africa Stress And Health Study: rationale and design. Metabolic Brain Disease. 2004;19(1–2):135–147. doi: 10.1023/b:mebr.0000027424.86587.74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Williams SL, Williams DR, Stein DJ, Seedat S, Jackson PB, Moomal H. Multiple traumatic events and global psychological distress in South Africa. Journal of Trauma & Stress. 2007;20(5):845–855. doi: 10.1002/jts.20252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Wittchen HU, Zhao S, Kessler RC, Eaton WW. DSM-III-R generalized anxiety disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1994;51(5):355–364. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950050015002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. World Health Organization (WHO) Mental health: New understanding, new hope. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2001. The World Health Report, 2001. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES