Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: Emotion. 2011 May 2;11(6):1489–1494. doi: 10.1037/a0022822

Concurrent and Prospective Relations between Attention to Emotion and Affect Intensity: An Experience Sampling Study

Renee J Thompson 1, Jutta Mata 2, Susanne Jaeggi 3, Martin Buschkuehl 4, John Jonides 5, Ian H Gotlib 6
PMCID: PMC3204007  NIHMSID: NIHMS311282  PMID: 21534663

Abstract

Theorists contend that emotional awareness is vital to being able to use emotional information adaptively. The extent to which individuals attend to and value their feelings, or attention to emotion, is a facet of emotional awareness. Little research, however, has examined whether attention to emotion affects the magnitude or intensity of emotional experiences. In the present study we examined the relations between attention to emotion and levels of affect in 53 healthy adults. Participants carried hand-held electronic devices for approximately seven days and were randomly prompted eight times per day to answer a series of questions. At each prompt, participants reported attention to emotion, current negative affect (NA), and positive affect (PA). All findings presented were computed using multilevel modeling. Replicating findings obtained using trait-level measures, we found that attention to emotion was associated concurrently with higher levels of both NA and PA. We also found prospectively that attention to emotion at one prompt predicted a decrease in levels of NA, but no change in levels of PA, at the subsequent prompt. These findings suggest that emotional processes serve different functions over time and highlight the role of attention to emotion in affect regulation.

Keywords: affect intensity, attention to emotion, emotional awareness, emotion regulation, experience sampling


Several theorists have postulated that being aware of one’s feelings is vital to being able to use emotional information adaptively (e.g., Bagby, Taylor, & Parker, 1994; Clore et al., 2001; Palmieri, Boden, & Berenbaum, 2009). Attention to emotion, a facet of emotional awareness, is defined as the extent to which people attend to and value their feelings (Salovey, Mayer, Goldman, Turvey, & Palfai, 1995). Higher levels of attention to emotion have been associated with various forms of coping and emotion regulation (Gohm & Clore, 2002).

Another important aspect of emotion regulation is altering the magnitude or intensity of affective reactions (e.g., Gross & Thompson, 2007; R. A. Thompson, 1994). Affect intensity is a unique facet of emotional experience that has been differentiated from both the frequency and duration of affect (e.g., Schimmack, Oishi, Diener, & Suh, 2000). Affect intensity has been related to a wide range of social, cognitive, behavioral, and health outcomes (see Larsen, 2009, for a review).

Although some investigators have conceptualized as traits or dispositions the extent to which individuals attend to their emotions and the intensity with which they experience affect, it is likely that these characteristics vary over time and across situations. For example, individuals might attend more to their emotions when discussing their relationship with a romantic partner than when discussing a memo with coworkers. Certainly, attending to one’s emotions at all times would be cognitively taxing and maladaptive. Moreover, emotion regulation is a dynamic process (e.g., Gross & Thompson, 2007), of which attention to emotion is only one facet. No research has yet examined how levels of attention to emotion are related to the levels of affect on a moment-to-moment basis in a naturalistic setting.

Using an experience sampling methodology, we examined the association between state levels of attention to emotion and both negative affect (NA) and positive affect (PA). Investigators using questionnaires to examine the relations among trait levels of these constructs have found that higher levels of attention to emotion are associated concurrently with the tendency to experience affect intensely (e.g., Gohm & Clore, 2002; R. J. Thompson, Dizen, & Berenbaum, 2009). It may be, therefore, that stronger emotional reactions are more likely than are weaker emotional reactions to elicit individuals’ attention, and/or that the process of attending to, if not perseverating about, one’s emotions might exacerbate those feelings. Based on these findings, we predicted that higher levels of attention to emotion “moment-to-moment” (that is, at each prompt) would be associated with higher intensityof both NA and PA.

Unlike most studies of affect intensity and attention to emotion, which have typically been cross-sectional, we used an experience sampling methodology that allowed us to examine the temporal relations between attention to emotion and levels of affect. In contrast to the “concurrent” predictions described above, we hypothesized that, prospectively, higher levels of attention to emotion would predict subsequent decreases in NA and increases in PA. This hypothesis is rooted in the formulation that one function served by emotions is to provide individuals with information (e.g., Clore, et al., 2001). Essentially, we posit that when individuals report higher levels of attention to emotion, they are more engaged in processing, or even regulating, their emotions. Although there is some evidence for the adaptability of up-regulating or increasing negative emotions (e.g., Tamir & Ford, 2009; Tamir, Mitchell, & Gross, 2008), we expect that in their everyday life, individuals who are attending to their emotions will be generally trying to down-regulate or decrease their level of NA. Indeed, Vassilopoulos (2008) found in an experimental study with high socially anxious individuals that increasing self-focused attention led to a decrease in self-reported anxiety seven minutes later. Consistent with this position, individuals who are less aware of their emotions (e.g., who pay less attention to their emotions, who have less clarity of their emotions) have been found to benefit more from interventions that focus on expressive writing and emotional approach coping than have individuals who are more aware of, or better able to describe, their emotions (e.g., Baker & Berenbaum, 2007, 2008). Similarly, we expect that when people are attending to their emotions they will try to up-regulate or increase their levels of PA.

In this study we assessed the relation between attention to emotion and levels of NA and PA in a group of psychologically healthy individuals (i.e., who did not meet criteria for any current or past psychopathology). Understanding emotion regulation processes in a group of healthy individuals is important in providing a normative reference while excluding individuals with known difficulties in emotion regulation (e.g., individuals with Major Depressive Disorder; Ehring, Tuschen-Caffier, Schnulle, Fischer, & Gross, 2010). As we noted above, we predicted that higher levels of attention to emotion would be associated concurrently with higher intensity of NA and PA, and prospectively, with subsequent reductions in levels of NA and increases in levels of PA.

Method

Participants

A total of 53 participants between the ages of 18 and 39 (M =25.4 years; SD = 6.4 years) were recruited for this study.1 All participants were native English speakers. The majority of participants were women (67.9%). Ethnic/racial make-up of the sample was primarily Caucasian (62.3%), with 17.0% Asian American, 9.4% multiracial, 9.4% African American, and 1.9% Latino/a. With respect to educational attainment, 47.2% indicated completing “some” college; 43.4% had a bachelor’s degree, and 9.4% had an advanced degree. Individuals were eligible to participate if they experienced no current/past history of any mental health disorders as assessed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 2001). Additional eligibility requirements included a Beck Depression Inventory-II (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996) score of 9 or less.

Procedure

Individuals were recruited from the surrounding communities of the University of Michigan in Ann Arbor, Michigan, and Stanford University in Stanford, California. Advertisements were posted online (e.g., Craigslist) and at local agencies and businesses (e.g., bulletin boards). Each site recruited approximately 50% of the sample. Participants recruited by the two sites varied in their distribution of gender, χ2(1) = 8.75, p < .01, with the Michigan sample being composed of a greater proportion of men than was the California sample. Participant recruitment by site also differed significantly by age, t(51)= 3.89, p < .01, with the Michigan sample being younger than the California sample, Michigan sample: M = 22.5 years, SD = 4.8 years; California sample: M = 28.6 years, SD = 6.6 years. This age difference was also reflected in significant educational differences, χ2(2) = 10.55, p < .01: the Michigan sample mostly (67.9%) reported “some college,” whereas the California sample mostly (60%) reported bachelor’s degrees. Because the samples did not differ on the central variables of interest (i.e., mean levels of NA, PA, and attention to emotion over the week), ts(51) < 1.66, ns, we combined the two samples for the remaining analyses.

Participants completed the SCID-I and, if eligible, returned to the laboratory to complete a series of self-report questionnaires and computer tasks.2 Participants were then individually instructed on the experience sampling protocol and completed a full practice trial (see below). Participants carried a hand-held electronic device (Palm Pilot Z22) that was programmed using the Experience Sampling Program 4.0 (Barrett & Feldman Barrett, 2000). Participants were prompted (via a tone signal) eight times per day between 10am and 10pm. The majority of participants carried the device for seven to eight days in order to be prompted 56 times. Prompts occurred at random times within eight 90 minute-windows per day; thus, prompts could occur between a few minutes and almost three hours apart (M = 93 minutes). If participants did not respond to a prompt within three minutes, data for that trial were recorded as missing. Up to 56 trials of data were recorded for each participant. Participants completed on average 42.4 trials (SD = 7.8). Participants provided informed consent and were compensated for their participation in the study, receiving an extra incentive for responding to more than 90 percent of the prompts. The protocol was approved by both universities’ Institutional Review Boards.

Experience sampling items

Affect

At each trial, participants reported their current intensity of NA and PA. Using a 4-point scale (not at all=0, little=1, much=2, a great deal=3), participants indicated the intensity with which they were currently feeling each of 11 emotions. For each prompt, NA was computed as the mean of seven negative emotions (sad, anxious, angry, frustrated, ashamed, disgusted, and guilty; Cronbach’s α across experience sampling period = .81), and PA was computed as the mean of four positive emotions (happy, excited, alert and active; Cronbach’s α across experience sampling period = .82). The affect words were drawn from various sources, including the Positive Affect Negative Affect Scale (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) and Ekman’s basic emotions (e.g., Ekman, Friesen, & Ellsworth, 1972).

Attention to emotion

At each trial, participants reported the extent to which they were attending to their emotions at the time of the prompt by responding to the item, “I am paying a lot of attention to how I feel right now.” This item was always presented before any affect items and was adapted from the Attention to Feelings subscale of the Trait Meta-Mood Scale (TMMS; Salovey, et al., 1995). The item was selected because it had the highest factor loading on this subscale (Salovey, et al., 1995). Using a 4-point scale (not at all=0, little=1, much=2, a great deal=3), participants indicated at each trial the extent to which they were attending to their emotions. As part of the questionnaire/computer task session, participants completed the Attention to Feelings subscale of the TMMS, Cronbach’s α = .87, which provides a trait measure of attention to emotion. The correlation between participants’ averaged scores on the attention to emotion item across the entire sampling protocol (i.e., up to 56 trials) and their scores on the TMMS attention to emotion subscale was r = .32, p < .05.

Results

Statistical overview

First, we examined mean levels of NA and PA over the week. Second, we tested our “concurrent” hypotheses that, within prompts, attention to emotion would be positively associated with NA and PA. Finally, we tested our “prospective” hypotheses that, within days, attention to emotion at one prompt would predict decreases in NA and increases in PA at the subsequent prompt. Because of the nested data structure, we conducted multilevel modeling procedures. We included prompt (within-person) and between-person levels in our analyses. We used Hierarchical Linear Modeling (HLM 6.06; Raudenbush, Bryk, & Congdon, 2008), which simultaneously estimates within- and between-person effects (Krull & MacKinnon, 2001) while handling missing data inherent to multilevel data (Snijders & Bosker, 1999).3 NA, PA, and attention to emotion served as the within-participant or Level 1 variables. Predictors were centered for each individual.

Descriptive Analyses

To examine mean of NA and PA, we conducted two hierarchical linear models, one predicting NA and one predicting PA. The models we tested are below:

Level 1 Model:

affectij=β0j+rij

Level 2 Model:

β0j=γ00+u0j

At Level 1 affectij represents NA or PA for participant j at prompt i; β0j represents each participant’s mean affect (i.e., NA or PA) across the sampling period, and rij represents the error term or the within-person variance. At Level 2 the γ00 represents the grand mean of affect (i.e., NA or PA) over the experience sampling week. The u0j represents the error term or the between-person variance. The mean level of NA over the experience sampling week was 1.138 (SE = .023), and the mean level of PA was 2.151 (SE = .064). To examine attention to emotion, we conducted a third model with attention to emotion as the outcome variable. The mean attention to emotion score was 1.913 (SE = .069).

Concurrent analyses

Next, we tested our hypothesis that attention to emotion would be positively associated concurrently with levels of NA and PA. To examine relations between attention to emotion and NA and PA, we conducted two hierarchical linear models, one with NA as an outcome variable and one with PA as an outcome variable. We computed within-participant associations between attention to emotion and NA, and attention to emotion and PA. Predictors were centered for each individual. The models we tested are below:

Level 1 Model:

affectij=β0j+β1j(attentiontoemotion)+rij

Level 2 Model:

β0j=γ00+u0jβ1j=γ10+u1j

At Level 1 affectij represents NA or PA for participant j at prompt i; β0j represents each participant’s mean affect (i.e., NA or PA) across the sampling period; β1j represents the linear slope between attention to emotion and affect for each participant, and rij represents the within-person variance. At Level 2, the γ00 coefficient values represent the grand mean of affect and γ10 represents the slope between attention to emotion and affect for the entire sample. The u0j and u1j are error terms, representing between-person variance unaccounted for by the included predictor variables. The coefficients from both models are presented in Table 1. Attention to emotion was significantly associated with NA, with higher levels of attention to emotion associated with higher levels of NA. Attention to emotion was also significantly and positively associated with PA. These results support our “concurrent” hypotheses that higher levels of attention to emotion would be associated with higher levels of both NA and PA “in the moment.” Finally, we tested both models again including linear and quadratic time-of-day effects (i.e., time in minutes since first prompt of that day) as additional predictors at Level 1 to control for potential time-of-day fluctuations in affect that are independent of attention to emotion. As expected, after controlling for potential time-of-day effects, all γ00 and γ10 coefficients remained statistically significant and comparable in magnitude (NA: γ00 = 1.14;γ10 = .07; PA: γ00 = 2.19;γ10 = .15, ps < .001).4

Table 1.

Concurrent Findings: Relations between Attention to Emotion and Negative and Positive Affect

Fixed effect Unstandardized Coefficient SE t(52) p
Outcome Variable: Negative Affect
Mean Level, Intercept, β0 γ00 1.138 .023 49.494 <.001
Attention to emotion slope, β1 γ10 .069 .014 4.875 < .001

Outcome Variable: Positive Affect
Mean Level, Intercept, β0 γ00 2.151 .064 33.368 < .001
Attention to emotion slope, β1 γ10 .148 .027 5.385 < .001

Note. SE = standard error

Prospective analyses

Finally, we tested our hypothesis that, prospectively, attention to emotion would predict decreases in NA and increases in PA. We examined whether, after controlling for initial levels of NA (PA), attention to emotion at the same prompt, t, would predict NA (PA) at the subsequent prompt, t + 1, within days. We conducted two hierarchical linear models: (1) attention to emotiont and NAt were simultaneously regressed onto NAt+1; and (2) attention to emotiont and PAt were simultaneously regressed onto PA t+1. Predictors were centered for each individual. The models we tested are below:

Level 1 Model:

affect(t+1)ij=β0j+β1j(attentiontoemotiont)+β1j(affectt)+rij

Level 2 Model:

β0j=γ00+u0jβ1j=γ10+u1jβ2j=γ20+u2j

The coefficients from both models are presented in Table 2. First, we conducted the hierarchical model predicting NAt+1 scores. Both attention to emotiont and NAt were significantly associated with NAt+1, with attention to emotiont showing an inverse relation with NAt+1,and NAt showing a positive relation with NAt+1. These results indicate that, consistent with our hypothesis, higher levels of attention to emotion predicted lower levels of NA over time, even after controlling for initial levels of NA. We conducted the prospective NA model again including linear and quadratic time-of-day effects as predictors. All γ00, γ10, γ20 coefficients remained statistically significant and comparable in magnitude (γ00 =1.14, γ10= −.02, γ20 = .19, ps < .05). Linear and quadratic time-of-day effects were not significant. Next, we conducted the hierarchical model predicting PAt+1. PAt was positively and significantly associated with PAt+1. Contrary to our hypotheses, attention to emotiont was not significantly associated with PAt+1 after controlling for PAt. Instead, attention to emotion showed weak prospective relations with PA. Again, linear and quadratic time-of-day effects were not significant.

Table 2.

Prospective Findings: Attention to Emotion Predicting Changes in Negative and Positive Affect

Fixed Effects Unstandardized Coefficient SE t(52) p
Outcome variable: Negative Affectt+1
Mean Level, Intercept, β γ00 1.137 .023 48.800 < .001
Attention to Emotiont slope, β1 γ10 −.022 .010 −2.199 .032
Negative Affectt slope, β2 γ20 .193 .046 4.205 < .001

Outcome variable: Positive Affectt+1
Mean Level, Intercept, β0 γ00 2.156 .066 32.720 < .001
Attention to Emotiont slope, β1 γ10 .003 .022 .135 .893
Positive Affectt slope, β2 γ20 .285 .036 7.864 < .001

Note. SE = standard error; t = is any given experience sampling prompt, t + 1 = the experience sampling prompt that is directly following the t prompt within day.

The results of our analyses indicate that attention to emotion predicts decreases in NA over time. To examine whether the reverse direction of effect is also significant, we conducted two additional hierarchical linear models examining whether NA (PA) at prompt t is associated with changes in attention to emotion at prompt t + 1, controlling for attention to emotion at prompt t. Again, predictors were centered for each individual. The models we tested are below:

Level 1 Model:

attentiontoemotion(t+1)ij=β0j+β1j(affectt)+β1j(attentiontoemotiont)+rij

Level 2 Model:

β0j=γ00+u0jβ1j=γ10+u1jβ2j=γ20+u2j

In both models, attention to emotiont was positively and significantly associated with attention to emotiont+1 (NA model: γ10 = .127, p = .001; PA model:γ10 = .138, p < .001), indicating that reports of attention to emotion at one point in time are positively associated with reports of attention to emotion up to three hours later. In neither model was affect significantly associated with attention to emotiont+1 over time,after controlling for attention to emotiont (NA model: γ20 = .083, p = .319; PA model:γ20 = −.014, p = .657).

Discussion

Previous research has found that the disposition to experience intense emotions is positively associated with the disposition to pay attention to one’s emotions (Gohm & Clore, 2002; R. J. Thompson, et al., 2009). The present study is the first to examine these constructs in a naturalistic setting using experience sampling. Replicating patterns found in studies examining relations among trait versions of these constructs, the present results suggest that paying attention to one’s emotions is associated concurrently with the experience of stronger magnitude of affect. In fact, higher levels of attention to emotion co-occurred with reports of experiencing more intense NA and PA. Importantly, our concurrent findings remained significant when controlling for linear and quadratic time-of-day effects of PA.

By using an experience sampling method, we were also able to examine the relation between attention to emotion and NA and PA prospectively. As we hypothesized, attention to emotion prospectively predicted a decrease in levels of NA, even after controlling for previous levels of NA. The results of our prospective analyses suggest that the association between attention to emotion and levels of NA over a period of a few minutes to a few hours is unidirectional. That is, we found that higher levels of attention to emotion were associated with decreases in NA over time, but did not find a significant relation between these two constructs in the opposite direction: higher levels of NA were not associated with changes in attention to emotion.

Importantly, our prospective findings cannot be explained by a “regression to the mean” interpretation because regression to the mean operates both to increase and to decrease extreme scores over time. For example, according to regression to the mean, individuals who have unusually high values at prompt t would have lower values at prompt t + 1, and that individuals with unusually low values at prompt t would have higher values at prompt t + 1. These two cases, however, would yield comparable grand means of all prompts; the only values that would differ are the individual data points. Thus, regression to the mean would actually yield data supporting a null hypothesis in this study.

Attention to emotion was not related prospectively to PA. This finding stands in contrast to our hypothesis that attention to emotion would predict increases in PA. One interpretation of this null finding is that attention to emotion plays a more central role in the regulation of NA than of PA. Individuals may have differential emotion regulatory goals with PA than they do with NA; for example, while they may try to maintain (rather than increase or decrease) their levels of PA in everyday life, they may attempt to reduce their levels of NA. Future research is needed both to replicate these findings and to test these explanations.

We should note three limitations of the present study. First, we assessed the extent to which participants were thinking about their emotions. Attention to emotion, however, is generally defined as both attending to and valuing one’s emotional states (e.g., Salovey, et al., 1995). Future research could profitably use a more nuanced approach to measuring attention to emotion by assessing whether individuals report being guided by or placing importance on their emotions. Second, the measures of NA and PA used in this study did not fully represent the affective circumplex assessed in other experience sampling research (e.g., Pietromonaco & Barrett, 2009). Consequently, it remains for future research to examine more explicitly whether the relation between attention to emotion and NA and PA varies as a function of arousal. Finally, despite the positive concurrent relation between attention to emotion and affect intensity, we cannot determine whether, as we proposed earlier, the association between these two constructs in-the-moment (i.e., within a timeframe of seconds) is bi-directional.

Despite these limitations, our findings represent an important contribution to the emotion regulation literature by examining emotional processes as they occur relatively naturally over time. The results of this study provide support for the formulation that attention to emotion is an important component of emotion regulation. Further, the different pattern of findings we obtained depending on whether the relation between attention to emotion and NA was assessed concurrently or prospectively highlights the importance of examining individuals’ emotional experiences over time. Indeed, our results suggest that emotional processes serve different purposes as a function of the temporal nature of the relation between attention to emotion and levels of NA and PA.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by NIMH grant MH59259 to IHG, NIMH grant MH60655 to JJ, DFG fellowship Wi3496/41 to JM, SNF fellowship PA001/117473 to SMJ, and NIMH supplement MH74849 to RJT. The authors thank Patricia J. Deldin for her assistance in the study design, Howard Berenbaum, Krishna Savani, Susanne Scheibe, and Christian Waugh for their feedback and assistance with analyses, and Sarah Victor and Courtney Behnke for their assistance in project management.

Footnotes

1

Ten additional participants were excluded due to their Beck Depression Inventory-II scores being outside the range of eligibility (n = 3) or to equipment failure (n = 7).

2

These data are not presented in this report.

3

We report parameter estimates with robust standard errors.

4

Neither the linear nor the quadratic time-of-day effects was significant for the NA model; both of these effects were significant for the PA model, indicating lower levels of PA in the evening and higher levels of PA in the middle of the day than in the morning or evening.

5

We also conducted a hierarchical linear model in which attention to emotiont, NAt, and attention to emotiont+1 were simultaneously regressed onto NAt+1. All coefficient values were significant, γs: −.031 to .217, ps < .004, with NAt and attention to emotiont+1 positively associated with NAt+1, and attention to emotiont inversely associated with NAt+1. These findings clearly indicate that greater attention to emotion at time t predicts lower NA over time above and beyond the significant relation between attention to emotion and NA at time t+1.

Publisher's Disclaimer: The following manuscript is the final accepted manuscript. It has not been subjected to the final copyediting, fact-checking, and proofreading required for formal publication. It is not the definitive, publisher-authenticated version. The American Psychological Association and its Council of Editors disclaim any responsibility or liabilities for errors or omissions of this manuscript version, any version derived from this manuscript by NIH, or other third parties. The published version is available at www.apa.org/pubs/journals/emo

Contributor Information

Renee J. Thompson, Department of Psychology, Stanford University

Jutta Mata, Department of Psychology, Stanford University.

Susanne Jaeggi, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

Martin Buschkuehl, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

John Jonides, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

Ian H. Gotlib, Department of Psychology, Stanford University

References

  1. Bagby RM, Taylor GJ, Parker JDA. The 20-Item Toronto-Alexithymia-Scale--2. Convergent, Discriminant, and Concurrent Validity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1994;38(1):33–40. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90006-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baker JP, Berenbaum H. Emotional approach and problem-focused coping: A comparison of potentially adaptive strategies. Cognition and Emotion. 2007;21(1):95–118. doi: 10.1080/02699930600562276. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baker JP, Berenbaum H. The efficacy of problem-focused and emotional approach interventions varies as a function of emotional processing style. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2008;32(1):66–82. doi: 10.1007/s10608-007-9129-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barrett DJ, Feldman Barrett L. The Experience Sampling Program (ESP) 2000 Available at: http://www.experience-sampling.org/
  5. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) 2. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  6. Clore GL, Wyer RS, Dienes B, Gasper K, Gohm C, Isbell L. Affective feelings as feedback: Some cognitive consequences. In: Martin LL, Clore GL, editors. Theories of mood and cognition: A user’s guidebook. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 2001. pp. 27–64. [Google Scholar]
  7. Ehring T, Tuschen-Caffier B, Schnulle J, Fischer S, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation and vulnerability to depression: spontaneous versus instructed use of emotion suppression and reappraisal. Emotion. 2010;10(4):563–572. doi: 10.1037/a0019010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Ekman P, Friesen WV, Ellsworth P. Emotion in the human face: Guidelines for research and an integration of findings. New York, NY: Pergamon Press; 1972. [Google Scholar]
  9. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I disorders. New York, NY: NY State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  10. Gohm CL, Clore GL. Four latent traits of emotional experience and their involvement in well-being, coping, and attributional style. Cognition and Emotion. 2002;16(4):495–518. doi: 10.1080/02699930143000374. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gross JJ, Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 3–24. [Google Scholar]
  12. Krull JL, MacKinnon DP. Multilevel modeling of individual and group level mediated effects. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 2001;36(2):249–277. doi: 10.1207/S15327906MBR3602_06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Larsen RJ. Affect intensity. In: Leary MR, Hoyle RH, editors. Handbook of individual difference in social behavior. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2009. pp. 241–254. [Google Scholar]
  14. Palmieri PA, Boden MT, Berenbaum H. Measuring clarity of and attention to emotions. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2009;91(6):560–567. doi: 10.1080/00223890903228539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Pietromonaco PR, Barrett LF. Valence focus and self-esteem lability: Reacting to hedonic cues in the social environment. Emotion. 2009;9(3):406–418. doi: 10.1037/A0015691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Raudenbush S, Bryk A, Congdon R. Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling (HLM) 6 (Version 6.06) Chicago: Scientific Software International; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  17. Salovey P, Mayer JD, Goldman SL, Turvey C, Palfai TP. Emotional attention, clarity and repair: Exporing emotional intelligence using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. In: Pennebaker JW, editor. Emotion, disclosure and health. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association; 1995. pp. 125–154. [Google Scholar]
  18. Schimmack U, Oishi S, Diener E, Suh E. Facets of affective experiences: A framework for investigations of trait affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2000;26(6):655–668. doi: 10.1177/0146167200268002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Snijders TAB, Bosker RJ. Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advance multilevel modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  20. Tamir M, Ford BQ. Choosing to be afraid: Preferences for fear as a function of goal pursuit. Emotion. 2009;9(4):488–497. doi: 10.1037/a0015882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Tamir M, Mitchell C, Gross JJ. Hedonic and instrumental motives in anger regulation. Psychological Science. 2008;19(4):324–328. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02088.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs of the society for research in child development. 1994;59:25–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Thompson RJ, Dizen M, Berenbaum H. The unique relations between emotional awareness and facets of affective instability. Journal of Research in Personality. 2009;43(5):875–879. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Vassilopoulos SP. Social anxiety and ruminative self-focus. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2008;22(5):860–867. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;54(6):1063–1070. doi: 10.1037/a0012532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES