Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Oct 30.
Published in final edited form as: Epidemiology. 2010 May;21(3):284–290. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181d61f53

From midlife to early old age: Health trajectories associated with retirement

Markus Jokela 1,2, Jane E Ferrie 1, David Gimeno 1,3,4, Tarani Chandola 1, Martin J Shipley 1, Jenny Head 1, Jussi Vahtera 5,6, Hugo Westerlund 7, Michael G Marmot 1, Mika Kivimäki 1,6
PMCID: PMC3204317  NIHMSID: NIHMS319322  PMID: 20220519

Abstract

Background

Previous studies report contradictory findings regarding health effects of retirement. This study examines longitudinally the associations of retirement with mental health and physical functioning.

Methods

The participants were 7584 civil servants from the Whitehall II cohort study aged 39-64 years at baseline and 54-76 years at the last follow-up. Self-reported mental health and physical functioning were assessed using the Short Form Medical Outcomes Survey questionnaire (SF-36), and the scales were scored as T-scores (Mean [SD] =50 [10]). Retirement status and health were assessed with six repeated measurements over a 15-year period.

Results

The associations between retirement and health were dependent on age at retirement, reason for retirement, and length of time spent in retirement. Compared with continued employment, statutory retirement at age 60 and early voluntary retirement, respectively, were associated with 2.2 (95% confidence interval = 1.7 to 2.8) and 2.2 (1.7 to 2.7) points higher mental health and with 1.0 (0.6 to 1.5) and 1.1 (0.8 to 1.4) points higher physical functioning. Retirement due to ill health was associated with poorer mental health (-0.7 points [-1.62 to 0.2]) and physical functioning (-4.5 points [-5.1 to -3.9]). Within-subject analyses suggested a causal interpretation for statutory and voluntary retirement, but health selection for retirement due to ill health.

Conclusions

Longitudinal analyses of repeat data suggest that health status improves after statutory and voluntarily retirement, although the improvement appears to attenuate over time. By contrast, the association between retirement due to ill health and subsequent poor health seems to reflect selection rather than causation.


Retirement is one of the most important social transitions of late adulthood. Previous studies on the potential health consequences of retirement have produced conflicting results.1-13 Retirement has been associated both with improving1-4 and deteriorating mental health,5-8 while some studies have found retirement to be unrelated to mental8,9 and physical health.10,11 An association between early retirement and increased mortality risk has also been reported,14,15 but it is unclear whether this association is causal or explained by the pre-existing poor health among people retiring early on health grounds. Depending on the evidence considered, retirement has been characterized either as an additional stressor or a relief occurring in late adulthood.13

Many of the previous studies have been cross-sectional.2,5,7-9 Reverse causality may confound such studies because poor physical health is a strong predictor of early retirement.16,17 The few prospective studies available are based on relatively short follow-up periods1,3,4,11 leading to a limited perspective on retirement and health, as the short-term and long-term effects of retirement may differ. Moreover, it is important to take into account the reason for retirement,4 i.e., whether individuals are forced to retire due to illness, whether they retire voluntarily, or whether they retire at the statutory retirement age for their occupation; different reasons for retirement may be differently related to health consequences.

The Whitehall II study18,19 of British civil servants provides an opportunity to examine the associations of retirement with mental health and physical functioning using repeated measurements over a 15-year period to determine mental health and physical functioning before and after retirement. A large sample size and an age range of 39 to 76 years provided sufficient power to examine whether the effects of retirement on mental health and physical functioning varied depending on reason for retirement, age at retirement, and length of time spent in retirement. The longitudinal setting also afforded the opportunity to assess potential confounding due to reverse causality.

Methods

Participants

Participants and measurements from the ongoing longitudinal Whitehall II study have been described previously in detail.18,19 Briefly, the study has followed a sample of 10,308 men and women who were London-based civil servants. Here we used data from 6 study phases (phases 3 through 8, collected between 1991 and 2006); all of these phases included measures of retirement, mental health and physical functioning. The participants were 39 to 64 years of age at baseline (phase 3) and 54 to 76 years of age at the most recent follow-up. We included all participants with data on retirement status and health from at least one study phase, but excluded those who were already retired at baseline (n = 722). For participants who left the civil service for reasons other than retirement, detailed data on subsequent retirement decisions outside the civil service were not available. Therefore, these participants were included in the sample at phases when they still worked in the civil service but were excluded from the sample after they left the civil service (n = 881, 398, 206, 122, and 105 participants leaving civil service at phases 4, 5, 6, 7, and 8). These selection criteria resulted in a sample of 7584 participants (5248 men, 2336 women; 74% of the original cohort). At phases 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, and 8, data were available for 7584, 5931, 5133, 4689, 4815, and 5433 participants, respectively. Ethical approval for the Whitehall II study was obtained from the University College London Medical School committee on the ethics of human research.

Measures

At all phases, mental health and physical functioning were assessed using the self-administered Short Form Medical Outcomes Survey (SF-36) questionnaire.20,21 We used two of the subscales, mental health and physical functioning, which represent the main SF-36 subscales for mental and physical health.20 The mental health subscale includes 5 items assessing aspects of mental wellbeing (e.g., feeling happy, feeling nervous), and the physical functioning subscale includes 10 items assessing the ability to carry out daily activities (e.g., difficulties in carrying groceries, difficulties in walking long distances). Both subscales were negatively skewed, and were corrected by cubic transformations (XT=X3/10,000). Following common practice for using the SF-36, the subscales were then transformed into T-scores so that the overall mean scores across participants and study phases was 50 (standard deviation=10) for both mental health and physical functioning. High values indicated better health/functioning.

At each study phase, the participants reported whether they were still working in the civil service, had retired from the civil service, or had left the civil service for a reason other than retirement (e.g., employment outside civil service, privatization or redundancy). They also reported the reason for their retirement: early voluntary retirement, retirement due to ill health, or statutory retirement at age 60 years. Based on these data, we created a four-category retirement status variable (0 = working in the civil service, 1 = statutory retirement, 2 = ill-health retirement, 3 = voluntary early retirement). Retirement was coded as a time-varying predictor, such that a participant’s retirement status could change across study phases. Although there is a statutory retirement age of 60 in the civil service, some workers may continue longer if their job is regarded as essential or if they have insufficient years of service to be eligible for a full pension.

Socioeconomic status (SES) was measured by the participant’s civil service employment grade assessed on a 6-point scale at baseline (Mean [SD] =3.4 [1.6]). Grade of employment was determined by asking all participants for their civil service grade title. Based on salary the civil service identified 12 non-industrial grades that, in order of increasing salary, comprise clerical assistant, clerical officer, executive officer, higher executive officer, senior executive officer, and seven “unified grades.” Other professional and technical staff were assigned to these grades on the basis of salary. As in previous reports from the Whitehall II cohort,18,19 unified grades 1–6 were combined into one group and the bottom two clerical grades into another, producing six categories; here we coded the variable so that category 1 represents the lowest status jobs and category 6 the highest. In 1995 the annual salary in the two lowest grades was £4000 to £10 999, for the two intermediate grades was £5500 to £26 000, and for the two highest grades was £28 975 to £150 000.1,19 For retired participants and participants no longer working in the civil service, the assigned SES was the final grade before leaving the civil service. The civil service pension is based upon years of service and employment grade on departure; civil servants with 40 years of service should receive a pension equivalent to about half their final salary. The pension of an employee who is medically retired may be higher than an ordinary pension (to allow for medical care costs).22 Voluntary early retirement has previously been shown to have a positive effect on personal income,4 reflecting the financial package offered to people retiring early.

Statistical analysis

Age-dependent mental health and physical functioning trajectories associated with retirement status were assessed using random-intercept multilevel modelling.23,24 In multilevel longitudinal models the data are structured so that measurement times (observations) are nested within participants. On average, participants provided data at 4 of the possible 6 study phases, resulting in a total of 33,585 observations for the 7584 participants. In a random-intercept model, the average level of the outcome (i.e., the intercept) is allowed to vary across individuals. The regression coefficients for the covariates are calculated as weighted averages of between-participant effects (comparison between different participants having different values for the covariates) and within-participant effects (comparison between repeated measurements for the same participant).

Although physical functioning is known to decline with age, there have been conflicting findings regarding aging and mental health.25,26 We therefore began our analysis by plotting the mean mental health and physical functioning scores by age, allowing the association between age and health to take any shape. Based on these preliminary analyses, we chose to model the effect of age using a piecewise approach27 where the age range was divided into two parts – before and after the age of 60 – by creating one variable for ages 39 to 59 (Age before 60y) and another variable for ages 60 to 76 (Age after 60y). This offered a flexible model to evaluate whether retirement had different effects before and after the age of 60. In addition to taking into account the effect of age, all models were adjusted for the effect of measurement period, i.e., study phase.28

In order to assess whether the association between retirement status and health was dependent on age at retirement, we tested retirement status × age interaction effects. These interactions were assessed by first testing interaction terms with linear terms of age and then with quadratic terms of age. Only statistically significant terms were retained in the models. The findings were illustrated by growth curve models showing the predicted trajectories by age and retirement status. All models were adjusted for sex and SES, and the aforementioned procedure of testing interaction effects with age was applied to these covariates. In addition to including sex and SES as covariates in the main analyses, we ran subsidiary analyses stratified by sex and SES to examine potential moderating effects of these covariates. These analyses yielded results substantially similar to the main analyses (eAppendix [http://links.lww.com]).

The association between retirement status and health could change depending on how long a person has been retired. In order to assess this possibility, we created cumulative indicators of retirement status at each phase, summing the number of phases at which the participant had been retired up to and including that phase. Separate cumulative indicators were created for statutory retirement, ill-health retirement, and voluntary early retirement. For example, a participant who took voluntary early retirement (say, between phases 4 and 5) and remained retired at phases 6, 7, and 8 was assigned the values of 0, 0, 1, 2, 3, and 4 on the cumulative voluntary-early-retirement indicator at phases 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, and 8, respectively. The cumulative variables ranged from 0 (being retired in none of the phases) to 5 (being retired in all the phases after baseline). In this analysis, the participants contributed observations from all phases in which they had complete data between baseline and the phase being analyzed (n = 6714 participants, 31,104 observations). For example, a participant with data available at phases 3, 4, 5, 7, and 8 contributed 3 observations (at phases 3, 4, and 5) because missing data at phase 6 prevented the calculation of the cumulative retirement variable after phase 5.

The associations between cumulative retirement indicators and health outcomes were assessed with random-intercept multilevel modelling. As we had time-varying measures of retirement status and health outcomes at 6 phases, it was possible to examine within-participant effects, i.e., comparing post-retirement health with pre-retirement health within the same person (in contrast to comparing retired and non-retired participants).23,24 For the analysis of cumulative retirement time, we therefore fitted multilevel models estimating only within-participant effects of retirement. If the association between retirement and health is causal rather than a reflection of selection effects, one would expect there to be within-participant effects.

Finally, in order to assess reverse causality, we applied discrete-time survival analysis23,29 to assess whether mental health and physical functioning of non-retired participants predicted the likelihood of becoming retired. Only participants who were not retired at phase 3 and who had complete data at each phase were included in this analysis (n=4837). We fit separate survival-analysis models for each reason for retirement. In each analysis, participants retiring for reasons other than the reason of interest were censored when they retired. Time-varying mental health and physical functioning scores assessed at a given phase were used to predict retirement status at the next phase. We adjusted survival analysis models for sex, SES, and age. All models were fitted using STATA 9.2 statistical program (StataCorp, College Station, TX).

Results

Mental health

On average, mental health increased in a curvilinear fashion up to age 60, after which it remained relatively stable (Figure 1). Preliminary analyses showed that a piecewise model including linear and quadratic terms for both age variables fitted the mental-health trajectory well. In sex- and SES-adjusted analyses, the association between retirement status and mental health was dependent on reason for retirement and age at retirement (Figure 2; eTable 2 [http://links.lww.com]). Voluntary early retirement and statutory retirement were associated with better mental health compared to being in the workforce. By the age of about 65, these differences had disappeared due to improvement in mental health of non-retired participants. Ill-health retirement was associated with poor mental health, but the shape of the trajectory closely followed the trajectory for those still in work. This suggests that ill-health retirement is associated with poor but not deteriorating mental health over time. The size of the mental health benefit associated with statutory/early retirement when the participant was aged 60 was approximately three times the size of the mental health deficit associated with ill health retirement at the same age (Figure 2; eTable 2 [http://links.lww.com]).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Average scores (and 95% confidence intervals) of mental health and physical functioning by age.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Trajectories of mental health and physical functioning as a function of retirement status and age. Mental health and physical functioning are scored as T-scores (mean [SD] = 50 [10]). The vertical lines are 95% confidence intervals. See eTable 1 (http://links.lww.com) for the percentages of observations belonging to the various retirement status groups by age group. See eTable 2 (http://links.lww.com) for statistical details of the multilevel model.

We then assessed whether the association between retirement and mental health was dependent on the number of phases at which the participant had been retired (Figure 3; eTable 3 [http://links.lww.com]). Statutory retirement was associated with improved mental health from the first through fourth phase after retirement but not at the fifth phase. Voluntary early retirement was associated with greater improvement in mental health, although this association attenuated over time. Ill-health retirement was not associated with poor or declining mental health over time.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Associations between length of retirement (number of follow-up phases being retired) and differences in mental health and physical functioning. The values are within-participants regression coefficients from separate multilevel models for each reason for retirement and indicate the within-participant difference in health associated with retirement compared to not being retired. Mental health and physical functioning are scored as T-scores (mean [SD] = 50 [10]). The vertical lines are 95% confidence intervals. See eTable 3 (http://links.lww.com) for statistical details.

Physical functioning

Physical functioning declined with age, the pace accelerating slightly after age 60 (Figure 1). A piecewise model with linear term for age before 60 and linear and quadratic terms for age after 60 fitted the data well. Statutory and voluntary early retirement were associated with better physical functioning compared with being in the workforce. The magnitude of these differences was approximately half of those observed for mental health (Figure 2; eTable 2 [http://links.lww.com]), and retired participants at age 60 had the same level of physical functioning as non-retired participants at age 58. Ill-health retirement was associated with substantially poorer physical functioning, such that a 60-year-old person who had retired on health grounds had the physical functioning level of a 71-year-old person who had not retired on health grounds. The trajectories for ill-health retirement and other categories began to converge after age 60.

We then examined whether the association between retirement and physical functioning was dependent on the number of study phases spent in retirement (Figure 3; eTable 3 [http://links.lww.com]). Statutory retirement was associated with higher physical functioning across study phases, but this association attenuated after retirement. Voluntary early retirement exhibited a similar pattern. Ill-health retirement was associated with poor physical functioning at the beginning, but this improved over time spent in ill-health retirement.

Sensitivity analyses

As a sensitivity analysis, we repeated the analysis of retirement-related health trajectories and within-participant analyses separately by sex and SES. With few exceptions, the shapes of health trajectories were qualitatively similar across the subgroups, suggesting that the findings in the main analysis are robust (eFigures 1-4 [http://links.lww.com]).

Reverse causality

Finally, we assessed whether time-varying mental health and physical functioning prospectively predicted the likelihood of retiring at the next phase (Table). Poor mental health was associated with increased odds of subsequent voluntary early retirement, and poor physical functioning was associated with increased odds of statutory retirement (compared with continuing in the civil service or having left the civil service for reasons other than retirement). Both poor mental health and physical functioning increased the odds of ill-health retirement.

Table.

Assessing the selection effect of health on the likelihood of retiring for different reasons compared with staying in work. Three discrete-time survival analysis models.

Model 1
Statutory retirement
Model 2
Ill-health retirement
Model 3
Early voluntary retirement

OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)
Mental health 0.99 (0.92 to 1.08) 0.64 (0.56 to 0.73) 0.89 (0.84 to 0.94)
Physical functioning 0.91 (0.84 to 0.99) 0.55 (0.49 to 0.62) 0.98 (0.92 to 1.05)
Female sex 1.02 (0.84 to 1.23) 0.71 (0.53 to 0.95) 1.11 (0.98 to 1.26)
Baseline SES 1.05 (0.99 to 1.08) 0.88 (0.81 to 0.95) 1.18 (1.14 to 1.22)

Values are odds ratios (and 95% confidence intervals) of discrete-time survival analysis models. Odds ratios for mental health and physical functioning are presented for standardized scales (SD=1), and higher scores indicate better health (i.e., odds ratios below 1.00 indicate that good health decreases the odds of retirement). All models adjusted for period effects and age at baseline.

Discussion

In a British occupational cohort, statutory retirement and voluntary early retirement were associated with better mental health and physical functioning compared with being in the workforce. Retirement on health grounds was associated with poorer mental health and physical functioning but not with an accelerated decline in health over time. On average, mental health improved after midlife and reached a plateau around the age of 60. Such improvement in mental health after the midlife years has been observed in several previous studies, 25,30 although the explanation for this general pattern remains unknown. The present findings show that people retiring early may enjoy a head start in this post-midlife mental health improvement.

The improved mental health and physical functioning of retirees tended to attenuate over time since retirement, particularly in voluntary early retirement. This may reflect a process in which people adapt to changes in their life circumstances – a process observed in several studies of life events and subjective wellbeing.31 However, it bears emphasis that the level of health in those who took early/statutory retirement did not drop below their pre-retirement health level even in participants who were retired for most of the study period. Despite the possible attenuation of the health benefits, being retired for several years appears to have no adverse impact on health. Participants taking ill-health retirement, in turn, recovered from their initially low physical functioning while their mental health did not change over the time.

The sex- and SES-specific multilevel models suggest that the associations between retirement and health were qualitatively similar across sexes and SES groups, although statutory retirement and voluntary early retirement were slightly more strongly associated with improved health in men than women and in participants with high than low SES (eFigures 1-4 [http://links.lww.com]). There was one exception to these general patterns; participants retiring due to ill health from a high SES job did not have low physical functioning at the beginning of their ill-health retirement, but their physical functioning did improve over time and their mental health deteriorated over time. This stands in contrast to the effect pattern observed in the total sample (or in participants with low SES), i.e., a recovery from an initially poor physical functioning and no change in mental health. Reasons for these SES-related differences are unknown. Perhaps having to retire due to ill health is mentally more stressful to employees with high SES because they lose a high-status occupation. Despite declining mental health, however, they appear to enjoy the improving physical functioning associated with ill-health retirement.

Health causation and selection

Survival analyses indicate that all three forms of retirement were predicted by poor rather than good health; the specific associations varied according to reason for retirement. Poor mental health increased the probability of opting for voluntary early retirement, while poor physical functioning increased the probability of leaving the workforce at statutory retirement age. It is possible that people with mental health problems are more troubled by work-related stressors and therefore more likely to retire early. The decision to continue working beyond statutory retirement age, in turn, might be affected more by individuals’ physical capabilities than their mental health. Both poor mental health and physical functioning contributed to the probability of ill health retirement, which is in agreement with the fact that ill health retirement is granted because of mental and physical illnesses.

Our study shares the limitations inherent to all observational studies, including the difficulty of establishing causal relations. Nevertheless, two arguments support a causal interpretation for the association of statutory and early retirement with improved health. First, our analyses of reverse causality indicate that the probability of retirement was increased by poor health, and so reverse causality is unlikely to account for the observed association between retirement and improved health. If anything, such a selection effect might have led to a conservative estimation of the health improvements associated with retirement. Second, longitudinal within-participant analyses suggested that, for a given individual, mental health and physical functioning were better after retirement than before.

Not surprisingly, and in contrast to patterns observed for statutory and early voluntary retirement, our analyses suggest that the association between ill-health retirement and poor health reflects health selection rather than causation. First, possible selection and causation effects were both in the same direction, i.e., ill-health retirement was associated with poorer health both pre- and post-retirement. Second, within-participant analyses indicated no deterioration in mental health after ill-health retirement. There was an initial decline in physical functioning associated with ill-health retirement, which may reflect an onset of a disease between the study phases, i.e., the cause rather than the effect of ill-health retirement. However, physical functioning of ill-health retirees improved over time and tended to reach its pre-retirement level in approximately 15 years. Together these findings suggest that, with the exception of participants with high SES, ill-health retirement has generally no adverse health effects. If anything, it may be related to recovering of physical functioning.

Given the inherent limits of observational studies in establishing causal relationships, the causal role of retirement remains uncertain and should be interpreted cautiously. The changes in health after retirement were most pronounced in early retirees, suggesting that removal of work-related mental strain32-34 could underlie the beneficial effect of retirement. Increased leisure time and opportunity to pursue personal interests and hobbies could also be contributing factors. One may argue that it is actually these factors, rather than retirement per se, that improve health. However, we believe that such life changes following retirement are best interpreted as mediating mechanisms that bring about the beneficial effects of retirement, rather than as co-occurring events confounding the independent effects of retirement. If retirement had not taken place, the life changes and opportunities following retirement would not have taken place either. From this perspective, it seems justifiable to consider retirement as a potentially causal social transition that influences health.

Strengths and limitations of the study

The main strength of the study is the prospective longitudinal design, with 6 follow-up phases covering the ages between 39 and 76 years (during which most people leave the workforce). These data allowed us to model health trajectories across the adult life-course while taking into account age at retirement and the length of time spent in retirement. Multiple repeated measurements of retirement and health outcomes strengthened the evidence supporting causal interpretations, thus contributing to previous evidence based on cross-sectional studies and longitudinal studies with only one follow-up, including two earlier studies1,4 of retirement in the Whitehall II cohort. The main limitation of the study was the homogeneity of the study sample, as the participants consisted mainly of white-collar men and women working in public sector workplaces. Further research is needed to assess whether our results are generalizable across all occupational settings and across different countries with different retirement policies. For instance, recent evidence from the GAZEL study35,36 suggests that the beneficial effects of retirement may not be limited to UK settings, as retirement in this French occupational cohort, including also blue-collar workers, was associated with a substantial decrease in the prevalence of sleep disturbances and improved self-rated health.

In conclusion, data from British civil servants provide clear support for the hypothesis that mental health and physical functioning improve after statutory and voluntarily retirement, although the improvement appears to attenuate over time. The magnitude of observed improvements was relatively modest, particularly in the within-participant analyses and for statutory retirement, and thus the health consequences of retirement should not be overemphasized. Our findings suggest that these health consequences are mostly positive and that much of the assumed negative health effects of retirement14,15 are attributable to health selection rather than causation.

Acknowledgments

Funding: The Whitehall II study has been supported by grants from the British Medical Research Council (MRC); the British Heart Foundation; the British Health and Safety Executive; the British Department of Health; the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (grant HL36310); the National Institute on Aging (grant AG13196); the Agency for Health Care Policy and Research (grant HS06516); and the John D. and Catherine T. Mac Arthur Foundation Research Networks on Successful Midlife Development and Socioeconomic Status and Health. JV and MK were supported by the Academy of Finland (grants no: 117604, 124322, and 124271). JEF was supported by the Medical Research Council (grant G8802774), MJS by the British Heart Foundation and MGM by an MRC Research Professorship. HW was supported by the Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research (FAS, grant #2004-2021).

Footnotes

SDC

Supplemental digital content is available through direct URL citations in the HTML and PDF versions of this article (www.epidem.com).

References

  • 1.Mein G, Martikainen P, Hemingway H, Stansfeld S, Marmot M. Is retirement good or bad for mental and physical health functioning? Whitehall II longitudinal study of civil servants. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57:46–49. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.1.46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Drentea P. Retirement and mental health. J Aging Health. 2002;14:167–194. doi: 10.1177/089826430201400201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Mojon-Azzi S, Sousa-Poza A, Widmer R. The effect of retirement on health: a panel analysis using data from the Swiss Household Panel. Swiss Med Wkly. 2007;137:581–585. doi: 10.4414/smw.2007.11841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hyde M, Ferrie J, Higgs P, Mein G, Nazroo J. The effects of pre-retirement factors and retirement route on circumstances in retirement: findings from the Whitehall II study. Ageing & Society. 2004;24:279–296. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Buxton JW, Singleton N, Melzer D. The mental health of early retirees-- national interview survey in Britain. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2005;40:99–105. doi: 10.1007/s00127-005-0866-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bossé R, Aldwin CM, Levenson MR, Ekerdt DJ. Mental health differences among retirees and workers: findings from the Normative Aging Study. Psychol Aging. 1987;2:383–389. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.2.4.383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Alavinia SM, Burdorf A. Unemployment and retirement and ill-health: a cross-sectional analysis across European countries. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 2008;82:39–45. doi: 10.1007/s00420-008-0304-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Villamil E, Huppert FA, Melzer D. Low prevalence of depression and anxiety is linked to statutory retirement ages rather than personal work exit: a national survey. Psychol Med. 2006;36:999–1009. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706007719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Butterworth P, Gill SC, Rodgers B, Anstey KJ, Villamil E, Melzer D. Retirement and mental health: analysis of the Australian national survey of mental health and well-being. Soc Sci Med. 2006;62:1179–1191. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Van Solinge H. Health change in retirement: A longitudinal study among older workers in the Netherlands. Res Aging. 2007;29:225–256. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ekerdt DJ, Baden L, Bossé R, Dibbs E. The effect of retirement on physical health. Am J Public Health. 1983;73:779–783. doi: 10.2105/ajph.73.7.779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kim JE, Moen P. Retirement transitions, sex, and psychological well-being: a life-course, ecological model. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2002;57:P212–222. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.3.p212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kim JE, Moen P. Is retirement good or bad for subjective well-being? Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2001;10:83–86. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Tsai SP, Wendt JK, Donnelly RP, de Jong G, Ahmed FS. Age at retirement and long term survival of an industrial population: prospective cohort study. BMJ. 2005;331:995. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38586.448704.E0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bamia C, Trichopoulou A, Trichopoulos D. Age at retirement and mortality in a general population sample: the Greek EPIC study. Am J Epidemiol. 2008;167:561–9. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Mein G, Martikainen P, Stansfeld SA, Brunner EJ, Fuhrer R, Marmot MG. Predictors of early retirement in British civil servants. Age Ageing. 2000;29:529–36. doi: 10.1093/ageing/29.6.529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Heponiemi T, Kouvonen A, Vänskä J, Halila H, Sinervo T, Kivimäki M, Elovainio M. Health, psychosocial factors and retirement intentions among Finnish physicians. Occup Med. 2008;58:406–12. doi: 10.1093/occmed/kqn064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Marmot MG, Davey Smith G, Stansfeld S, et al. Health inequalities among British civil servants: the Whitehall II study. Lancet. 1991;337:1387–1393. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(91)93068-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Marmot M, Brunner E. Cohort Profile: the Whitehall II study. Int J Epidemiol. 2005;34:251–256. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyh372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ware JE, Snow KK, Kosinski M, Gandek B. SF-36 Health survey manual and interpretation guide. Boston: New England Medical Center; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ware JE., Jr SF-36 health survey update. Spine. 2000;25:3130–3139. doi: 10.1097/00007632-200012150-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Higgs P, Mein G, Ferrie J, Hyde M, Nazroo J. Pathways to early retirement: structure and agency in decision-making among British civil servants. Ageing Soc. 2003;23:761–778. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Singer JB, Willett JB. Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Gelman A, Hill J. Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical model. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Blanchflower DG, Oswald AJ. Is well-being U-shaped over the life cycle? Soc Sci Med. 2008;66:1733–1749. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.01.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Easterlin RA. Life cycle happiness and its sources: Intersections of psychology, economics and demography. J Econ Psychol. 2006;27:463–482. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Naumova EN, Must A, Laird NM. Tutorial in biostatistics: Evaluating the impact of ‘critical periods’ in longitudinal studies of growth using piecewise mixed effects models. Int J Epidemiol. 2001;30:1332–1341. doi: 10.1093/ije/30.6.1332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Chandola T, Ferrie J, Sacker A, Marmot M. Social inequalities in self reported health in early old age: follow-up of prospective cohort study. BMJ. 2007;334:990–993. doi: 10.1136/bmj.39167.439792.55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Singer JD, Willett JB. Modeling the days of our lives: Using survival analysis when designing and analyzing longitudinal studies of duration and the timing of events. Psychol Bull. 1991;110:268–290. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Jorm AF. Does old age reduce the risk of anxiety and depression? A review of epidemiological studies across the adult life span. Psychol Med. 2000;30:11–22. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799001452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Diener E, Lucas RE, Scollon CN. Beyond the hedonic treadmill: revising the adaptation theory of well-being. Am Psychol. 2006;61:305–14. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.61.4.305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Stansfeld S, Candy B. Psychosocial work environment and mental health--a meta-analytic review. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2006;32:443–62. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.1050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Bonde JP. Psychosocial factors at work and risk of depression: a systematic review of the epidemiological evidence. Occup Environ Med. 2008;65:438–45. doi: 10.1136/oem.2007.038430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Laine S, Gimeno D, Virtanen M, Oksanen T, Vahtera J, Elovainio M, Koskinen A, Pentti J, Kivimäki M. Job strain as a predictor of disability pension: the Finnish Public Sector Study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2008;63:24–30. doi: 10.1136/jech.2007.071407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Vahtera J, Westerlund H, Hall M, Sjösten N, Kivimäki M, Salo P, Ferrie JE, Jokela M, Pentti J, Singh-Manoux A, Goldberg M, Zins M. Effect of retirement on sleep disturbances: the GAZEL prospective cohort study. Sleep. 2009;32:1459–66. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.11.1459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Westerlund H, Kivimäki M, Singh-Manoux A, Melchior M, Ferrie JE, Pentti J, Jokela M, Leineweber C, Goldberg M, Zins M, Vahtera J. Self-rated health before and after retirement: findings from the French GAZEL cohort study. Lancet. 2009;374:1889–96. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61570-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES