Skip to main content
Pain Research and Treatment logoLink to Pain Research and Treatment
. 2011 Oct 27;2012:585419. doi: 10.1155/2012/585419

Neurobiology Underlying Fibromyalgia Symptoms

Marta Ceko 1, 2,2,*, M Catherine Bushnell 1, 2, 3,2,3, Richard H Gracely 4
PMCID: PMC3205654  PMID: 22135739

Abstract

Fibromyalgia is characterized by chronic widespread pain, clinical symptoms that include cognitive and sleep disturbances, and other abnormalities such as increased sensitivity to painful stimuli, increased sensitivity to multiple sensory modalities, and altered pain modulatory mechanisms. Here we relate experimental findings of fibromyalgia symptoms to anatomical and functional brain changes. Neuroimaging studies show augmented sensory processing in pain-related areas, which, together with gray matter decreases and neurochemical abnormalities in areas related to pain modulation, supports the psychophysical evidence of altered pain perception and inhibition. Gray matter decreases in areas related to emotional decision making and working memory suggest that cognitive disturbances could be related to brain alterations. Altered levels of neurotransmitters involved in sleep regulation link disordered sleep to neurochemical abnormalities. Thus, current evidence supports the view that at least some fibromyalgia symptoms are associated with brain dysfunctions or alterations, giving the long-held “it is all in your head” view of the disorder a new meaning.

1. Introduction

In order to examine the neurobiology underlying the symptoms of fibromyalgia, we must first determine what those symptoms are. Until recently, fibromyalgia (FM) was diagnosed based on the ARC1990 criteria [1], which were widespread pain in combination with tenderness at 11 or more of 18 specific tender point sites. The provisional ACR 2010 FM diagnostic criteria [2], suggested as an alternative method of diagnosing FM, do not require the presence of tenderness, but rather include a list of several other symptoms, including fatigue, unrefreshing sleep, and cognitive symptoms, as well as a mix of some other symptoms that could include headache, depression, and lower abdominal pain/cramping. The hallmark symptom is still widespread pain, and a diagnosis of fibromyalgia requires this symptom. However, a patient must also have some of the other symptoms that are common among FM patients in order to reach a composite score that would lead to a diagnosis of FM. In addition to clinical symptoms that make up the diagnosis of FM, experimental studies have identified a number of other abnormalities in FM patients, including increased sensitivity to multiple types of painful stimuli, increased sensitivity to other sensory modalities, and alterations in pain modulatory mechanisms. Further, neuroimaging studies have found functional, anatomical, and neurochemical differences in the brains of FM patients compared to healthy control subjects. Most of the clinical symptoms associated with FM have not been systematically studied in the experimental setting, but there are a number of studies that have provided an objective evaluation of the altered cognitive functioning and sleep disturbances reported in FM patients. Thus, this paper will focus on the experimental evidence related to FM symptoms and connect these perceptual and cognitive signs to abnormalities observed in the brains of FM patients.

1.1. Altered Pain Perception in FM Patients

The hallmark symptom of FM is widespread ongoing musculoskeletal pain. In addition, FM patients have been distinguished from other patients with widespread pain syndromes primarily by the presence of tenderness that has been assessed clinically by finding pain evoked by 4 kg manual pressure in at least 11 of 18 defined tender points. This tender point concept was not based on an understanding of the underlying pathophysiology, but rather on empirical observation. Thus, although the ARC-90 diagnostic criteria provided an important uniform tool for defining the FM syndrome, they did not validate the tender point concept, due to the circular evidence on which the criteria were based [3]. In fact, much evidence indicates that tender points are just sites normally more sensitive to pressure pain in all individuals [47] and that FM patients have an increased pressure sensitivity at non-tender-point sites as well [8]. Accumulating evidence now shows that FM patients have increased sensitivity to many types of painful stimulation, including pressure at non-tender-point sites [9], heat and cold pain [6, 1014], electrical stimulation [6], and intramuscular hypertonic saline injection [15]. Despite the plethora of evidence for hypersensitivity to painful stimuli, there is less evidence that FM patients are more sensitive to innocuous somatosensory stimuli. Detection thresholds for tactile and electrical stimuli are not altered in FM [6, 12, 13], but Hollins et al. [16] found that FM patients rated innocuous pressure as more intense than did healthy controls, although the effects in the innocuous range were weaker than in the noxious range. The evidence for changes in cool or warm detection also is mixed, with most investigators finding no differences between FM and controls for heat [6, 10] or cold [10, 12], whereas one study found FM patients to have reduced heat detection thresholds [12], and one study found patients to have reduced cold detection thresholds [6]. Thus, it appears that the altered sensitivity within the somatosensory system is more profound in the noxious range than in the innocuous range.

1.2. Evidence for Generalized Hypersensitivity to Unpleasant Stimuli

The hypersensitivity of FM patients to painful stimuli has led some investigators to propose that fibromyalgia involves a hypervigilance to pain and pain-associated information [1719]. However, there is now evidence that the hypersensitivity to unpleasant stimuli extends beyond the somatosensory system, which has led to the hypothesis that there is a generalized hypervigilance for sensory stimuli in FM [16, 20, 21]. A few studies have examined the sensitivity of FM patients in modalities other than pain and found perceptual amplification. FM patients have been shown to have decreased tolerance of unpleasant noise [20] and increased sensitivity to loud unpleasant auditory stimuli that parallels their increased pressure pain sensitivity [22]. Similarly, FM patients perceive unpleasant olfactory stimuli to be more intense and more unpleasant than do matched control subjects [23]. On the other hand, when pleasant odors were tested, FM patients and controls perceived the odors as equally intense, consistent with another evidence that the hypersensitivity across perceptual modalities may be confined to stimuli in the unpleasant range [24]. Nevertheless, for pleasant odors, although FM patients did not rate them as more intense, they did evaluate the pleasant odors as less pleasant than did control subjects. Further, a range of auditory stimuli were rated as more intense by FM patients than by controls, and auditory stimuli rated as mildly pleasant by healthy subjects were rated as somewhat unpleasant by FM patients [16]. The finding of hypersensitivity in multiple modalities of stimulation, particularly for unpleasant stimuli, suggests that the evoked pain sensitivity of FM may be related to an altered hedonic appreciation for sensory stimuli, rather than to peripheral tissue abnormalities.

1.3. Other Phenomena Related to Altered Pain Perception

Other types of evidence from experimental pain studies in FM patients support the idea of a centrally mediated up-regulation of nociceptive activity in the CNS. A central pathophysiological process that appears to be disturbed in FM patients is the “windup” of central nociceptive processing of C-fibre input to the spinal cord, resulting in the perceptual phenomenon of temporal summation of pain. Windup of nociceptive activity is dependent on activation of the NMDA receptor complex in the spinal cord by input from C-nociceptors [25, 26]. Some FM patients show increased temporal summation of pain and increased aftersensations at the termination of noxious stimulation [27]. These enhanced responses could be related to one or more of several possible factors: (1) an ongoing peripheral source of input from C nociceptors other than the applied stimulus; (2) sensitized NMDA receptors on central nociceptive neurons; (3) abnormalities in descending modulation; (4) abnormal processing at supraspinal levels. Evidence of increased sensitivity in multiple sensory modalities suggests that ongoing C-nociceptor input cannot alone account for FM symptoms, indicating that there probably also are either sensitized NMDA receptors, abnormalities in modulatory systems in the brain, or abnormal sensory processing at spinal or supraspinal levels. Increased sensitivity has been demonstrated at the spinal level in FM [11]. Staud et al. [28] showed that an NMDA inhibitor reduced temporal summation in both healthy people and FM patients, suggesting that NMDA receptors probably are not sensitized in FM. On the other hand, experimental evidence shows that there are abnormalities in pain modulatory systems in FM patients that could account for altered temporal summation and other putative spinal effects.

1.4. Altered Pain Inhibition in FM Patients

For hundreds of years, clinicians have known that pain inhibits pain, a phenomenon termed “counterirritation.” More recently, a physiological basis of this phenomenon has been identified; the application of noxious stimulation activates an endogenous analgesic system involving supraspinal descending control of dorsal horn nociceptive activity. This system is termed “diffuse noxious inhibitory control” or DNIC and its physiological basis in the spinal cord has been studied extensively in anesthetized animals [29, 30]. Nevertheless, when competing noxious stimuli are presented in conscious humans, other systems that modulate pain, such as distraction, also are probably in effect, so that care must be taken in inferring that perceptual effects are due to DNIC. Accordingly, a group of interested researchers has suggested that the term “conditioned pain modulation” be used in humans studies to avoid the mechanistic implication [31]. Studies that have examined conditioned pain modulation in FM patients show that conditioning stimuli that produce an analgesic response to experimental pain stimuli in healthy control subjects fail to have an effect on FM patients [13, 3234]. One of these studies controlled for the effects of distraction and habituation and found a similar lack of conditioned pain modulation in FM patients [33], suggesting the possibility that the DNIC system is in fact impaired in these individuals. Alternatively, DNIC and other descending inhibitory systems could be activated by the widespread pain of FM, and the failure to demonstrate DNIC in FM could represent a ceiling effect in which these activated systems cannot be further engaged by the experimental manipulations [8]. In addition, distraction can have a powerful pain-inhibiting effect [3539], and some researchers have suggested that FM patients have altered attentional focusing, with a hypervigilance to unpleasant stimuli (see discussion above).

2. Other Symptoms of FM

2.1. Altered Cognitive Function in FM Patients

In addition to pain, many patients with fibromyalgia complain of problems with memory and concentration, often referred to as “fibrofog” [4043]. This clinical symptom has received a large amount of experimental study, and studies using objective cognitive tests substantiate patients' subjective reports of cognitive dysfunctions, most commonly related to speed of information processing, attention, and memory [4356]. The most robust deficits in tests of memory and attention have so far been observed in paradigms involving a prominent distraction from a competing source of information, wherein FM patients are less capable than healthy controls to retain new information when rehearsal is prevented by a distraction [49, 50, 57]. Milder deficits have been observed in memory free of distraction at encoding [43, 44, 48, 49, 51, 58, 59]. FM patients frequently display greater impairments in the ability to actively retrieve past episodic events in the absence of a cue (free recall) than on recognition tests, which serve to evaluate the retrieval of remembered information and are more resistant to the effects of impaired attention and concentration [43, 44, 48, 51]. It has thus been proposed that memory impairments in FM are more highly related to attentional factors that modulate the efficiency of memory functioning than to primary memory processes per se [48, 60, 61]. Thus, the inability to manage distraction seems to be a particular problem in fibromyalgia patients and is reflected in patients' reports of difficulty concentrating and dealing with complex, rapidly changing environments [61] and by memory tests showing performance decrements in the presence of distraction. Impaired cognitive performance is evident even after controlling for anxiety and depression and the influence of medications that might affect cognitive functioning [43, 50, 52, 58]. Another area of cognitive functioning that has been shown to be abnormal in FM is that of emotional decision making [62, 63]. A similar deficit has been shown in chronic back pain patients, suggesting that this is not unique to FM [64].

2.2. Sleep Disturbances in FM Patients

Many FM patients complain of unrefreshed sleep. Several laboratory studies using objective measures of sleep physiology such as EEG substantiate these reports by showing disordered sleep architecture in FM patients, including delayed onset to sleep, altered sleep stage dynamics, and reduced slow wave sleep (deep sleep) and rapid-eye movement (REM) sleep [6568]. The intrusion of EEG frequencies characteristic of wakefulness (alpha waves) in the deep non-REM sleep (delta waves) seems to be a prominent feature of the nonrestorative sleep of FM patients [65, 6971]. Further, patients with FM often have fragmented sleep resulting from periodic intrusions such as involuntary limb movements (restless legs), sleep apnea, and arousal disturbances [68, 7274]. Although FM patients tend to report greater disturbances in sleep duration and quality than shown in laboratory studies, and their subjective reports correlate better with the severity of clinical symptoms [75], objectively measured sleep disturbances have been associated with pain and subjective daily sleepiness in several studies [67, 68, 71, 73].

3. Brain Changes That Could Underlie Symptoms

3.1. Neural Basis of Pain Amplification and Altered Pain Modulation

Functional brain imaging studies support psychophysical findings of increased pain perception in FM, in that there is an augmentation of sensory processing throughout pain-related brain regions [9, 7681]. This is important, since laboratory findings of increased sensitivity could be interpreted as a reporting bias, rather than evidence of increased activation in pain pathways. The functional imaging studies have found that fibromyalgia patients show significantly more activity in response to pressure and thermal stimuli compared to controls in a number of brain regions. Increased activations were observed not only in limbic structures, but also in brain regions involved in sensory-discriminative processing, such as primary and secondary somatosensory cortices, which supports the view that neural responses to afferent signals are amplified in fibromyalgia.

Although the increased pain-evoked brain activations corroborate patients' reports, the correlation between increased brain activity and increased pain perception does not explain how the afferent signal is amplified. As discussed above, there is psychophysical evidence of dysfunctions in pain modulation as well as pain perception. There is now much evidence that the activation of descending control circuitry is involved in pain modulation and that this circuitry includes parts of prefrontal, cingulate, and insular cortices [23, 36, 37, 82, 83]. A number of anatomical imaging studies in FM patients reveal decreased brain gray matter in these regions [8490]. Although the cellular basis of decreased gray matter in FM patients is not known, it is possible that due to neuronal loss, decreased dendritic arborisation, or changes in glial activation, pain inhibitory systems do not work in FM patients as well as in healthy individuals.

Consistent with the idea that pain modulatory systems may be disturbed in fibromyalgia are data showing that some FM patients have abnormalities in neurochemical systems involved in pain control, including the forebrain opioid and dopamine systems. A positron emission tomography (PET) competitive binding study using the D2/D3 receptor antagonist [11C] raclopride showed that striatal dopamine is released in response to painful muscle stimulation in healthy subjects, but not in FM patients [15, 91], which might partially explain the increased sensitivity of FM patients to the painful muscle stimulation. For the opioid system, investigators using PET found that FM patients had decreased binding potentials at rest for the exogenously administered μ-opioid receptor agonist carfentanil in several brain areas, including the ventral striatum, the anterior cingulate cortex, and the amygdala [92]. These areas are implicated in pain and its emotional modulation, and correspondingly, the binding potentials showed a negative relationship with the magnitude of affective pain scores relative to the sensory scores. Although results of this study do not tell us whether levels of endogenous opioids were increased or whether receptor availability was decreased, the findings support the notion that disturbances in the opioidergic system might be related to the increased pain sensitivity in fibromyalgia. For both dopamine and opioids, the ongoing widespread pain of FM could lead to a tonic activation within these systems and thus be a main factor in altering receptor availability and associated responsiveness to externally applied painful stimuli.

3.2. Neural Basis of Cognitive Symptoms

It is well known that cognitive capabilities such as attention and memory functions decline continuously across the adult lifespan [93], which, together with findings of accelerated age-related decline of brain gray matter observed in FM patients [84], suggests that there may be a relationship between gray matter reductions in FM and cognitive deficits in these patients. Two recent studies have linked FM to impaired emotional decision making [62, 63]. Anatomical imaging studies have reported that FM patients have decreased gray matter in the medial prefrontal and insular cortices [84, 85, 89], areas implicated in emotional decision making [9499]. Together, these data suggest a possible association between gray matter loss and emotional decision making in FM. One study has directly examined the relationship between performance on working memory tasks and gray matter in FM patients and found that an individual's performance was positively correlated with gray matter values in medial frontal and anterior cingulate cortices, thereby providing direct evidence for an association between altered working memory and gray matter morphology in fibromyalgia [51]. Both of these brain regions, together with lateral premotor cortex, lateral prefrontal cortex, frontal poles, and posterior parietal cortex, are areas known to be related to working memory processes [100105]. In terms of the neurochemical abnormalities in FM discussed above, dopamine plays an important role for cognitive functioning. Multiple lines of evidence demonstrate the importance of mesocortical and striatal dopaminergic pathways in memory tasks, perceptual speed, and response inhibition (see [106] for review). Thus, there is an overlap between tasks in which fibromyalgia patients perform poorly and tasks that are related to dopamine functioning, suggesting that a dysfunctional dopamine system could contribute to the cognitive symptoms of fibromyalgia.

3.3. Neural Basis of Sleep Disturbances

While many studies have used EEG and related methods to show various aspects of disordered sleep physiology in FM patients, little is known about the neurobiology underlying these disturbances. Several neurotransmitters have been proposed to influence CNS hypersensitivity associated with sleep alterations. For example, inhibition of the CNS serotonin synthesis has been linked to insomnia and increased pain sensitivity [107]. Accordingly, in FM there is evidence for low serum and cerebrospinal fluid serotonin levels [108, 109]. Injecting amounts of substance P into the CNS of rats has been shown to reduce sleep efficiency, increasing latency to onset to sleep and provoking awakenings from sleep [110], and there is evidence for elevated cerebrospinal fluid levels of substance P in FM patients [111, 112].

3.4. What Do the Psychophysical, Cognitive, and Neuroimaging Studies Tell Us about the Neurobiology Underlying FM Symptoms?

The wealth of experimental evidence showing that FM patients are hypersensitive to painful stimuli, as well as unpleasant stimuli from other sensory modalities, in conjunction with functional brain imaging data showing increased stimulus-evoked activation throughout nociceptive pathways, shows that the defining symptom of FM—increased pain—is in fact real and not just a response bias of the patients. The finding that perception is increased in multiple modalities speaks against the hypothesis that FM pain is due to an upregulation of peripheral nociceptive processes. Further, psychophysical evidence that descending modulatory systems are altered in FM patients supports the opposing idea that FM symptoms are at least in part caused by alterations in CNS processing of the pain signal, including a dysregulation of pain modulatory systems. Nevertheless, the apparent dysregulation within these systems could be caused and/or perpetuated by a tonic activation related to the presence of ongoing widespread pain, so that the systems are saturated and cannot regulate further in response to external stimuli.

Since similar descending control systems, including attentional and emotional regulatory circuitry, affect multiple sensory modalities [113119], a dysfunction (or saturation) in these systems could lead to the hypersensitivity in multiple sensory modalities. FM patients show reduced habituation to nonpainful tactile stimuli and increased cortical response to intense auditory stimuli, both of which have been linked to deficient inhibition of incoming sensory stimuli [120, 121]. Also in support of the idea of a central dysregulation or saturation of pain modulation are changes in the opioid and dopamine neurotransmitter systems, both known to be involved in hedonic regulation [122].

Finally, the findings that FM patients not only perceive themselves to have altered memory and concentration (“fibrofog”), but also in fact perform poorly on multiple cognitive tests, even when depression is excluded as a contributing factor, suggest that there are alterations in brain function. The anatomical brain imaging studies that show reductions in gray matter in frontal regions important for cognitive function further indicate that this common symptom of FM is based on altered brain function. Together, the experimental evidence provides strong support for the idea that FM symptoms are related to dysfunctions in the central nervous system. The cause of these changes cannot be deduced from the available evidence, as it is correlational in nature. Did long-term ongoing pain cause the changes or did the changes cause the pain? Without a relevant animal model or long-term longitudinal studies, we cannot answer these questions. Nevertheless, we can at least say that fibromyalgia is real and that it is associated with multiple changes in the brain.

References

  • 1.Wolfe F, Smythe HA, Yunus MB, et al. The American College of Rheumatology 1990. Criteria for the classification of fibromyalgia. Report of the Multicenter Criteria Committee. Arthritis and Rheumatism. 1990;33(2):160–172. doi: 10.1002/art.1780330203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Wolfe F, Clauw DJ, Fitzcharles M-A, et al. Fibromyalgia criteria and severity scales for clinical and epidemiological studies: a modification of the ACR preliminary diagnostic criteria for fibromyalgia. Journal of Rheumatology. 2011;38(6):1113–1122. doi: 10.3899/jrheum.100594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Cohen ML, Quintner JL. Fibromyalgia syndrome, a problem of tautology. Lancet. 1993;342(8876):906–909. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(93)91950-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Granges G, Littlejohn G. Pressure pain threshold in pain-free subjects, in patients with chronic regional pain syndromes, and in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. Arthritis and Rheumatism. 1993;36(5):642–646. doi: 10.1002/art.1780360510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lautenbacher S, Krieg JC. Pain perception in psychiatric disorders: a review of the literature. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1994;28(2):109–122. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(94)90023-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lautenbacher S, Rollman GB, McCain GA. Multi-method assessment of experimental and clinical pain in patients with fibromyalgia. Pain. 1994;59(1):45–53. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(94)90046-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Tunks E, Crook J, Norman G, Kalaher S. Tender points in fibromyalgia. Pain. 1988;34(1):11–19. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(88)90176-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gracely RH, Grant MAB, Giesecke T. Evoked pain measures in fibromyalgia. Best Practice and Research. 2003;17(4):593–609. doi: 10.1016/s1521-6942(03)00036-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gracely RH, Petzke F, Wolf JM, Clauw DJ. Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence of augmented pain processing in fibromyalgia. Arthritis and Rheumatism. 2002;46(5):1333–1343. doi: 10.1002/art.10225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Berglund B, Harju EL, Kosek E, Lindblom U. Quantitative and qualitative perceptual analysis of cold dysesthesia and hyperalgesia in fibromyalgia. Pain. 2002;96(1-2):177–187. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(01)00443-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Desmeules JA, Cedraschi C, Rapiti E, et al. Neurophysiologic evidence for a central sensitization in patients with fibromyalgia. Arthritis and Rheumatism. 2003;48(5):1420–1429. doi: 10.1002/art.10893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kosek E, Ekholm J, Hansson P. Sensory dysfunction in fibromyalgia patients with implications for pathogenic mechanisms. Pain. 1996;68(2-3):375–383. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(96)03188-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lautenbacher S, Rollman GB. Possible deficiencies of pain modulation in fibromyalgia. Clinical Journal of Pain. 1997;13(3):189–196. doi: 10.1097/00002508-199709000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Petzke F, Clauw DJ, Ambrose K, Khine A, Gracely RH. Increased pain sensitivity in fibromyalgia: effects of stimulus type and mode of presentation. Pain. 2003;105(3):403–413. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00204-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wood PB, Schweinhardt P, Jaeger E, et al. Fibromyalgia patients show an abnormal dopamine response to pain. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;25(12):3576–3582. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05623.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hollins M, Harper D, Gallagher S, et al. Perceived intensity and unpleasantness of cutaneous and auditory stimuli: an evaluation of the generalized hypervigilance hypothesis. Pain. 2009;141(3):215–221. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Asmundson GJG, Kuperos JL, Norton GR. Do patients with chronic pain selectively attend to pain-related information? Preliminary evidence for the mediating role of fear. Pain. 1997;72(1-2):27–32. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(97)00010-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Keogh E, Ellery D, Hunt C, Hannent I. Selective attentional bias for pain-related stimuli amongst pain fearful individuals. Pain. 2001;91(1-2):91–100. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(00)00422-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Crombez G, Eccleston C, Den Broeck AV, Goubert L, Van Houdenhove B. Hypervigilance to pain in fibromyalgia: the mediating role of pain intensity and catastrophic thinking about pain. Clinical Journal of Pain. 2004;20(2):98–102. doi: 10.1097/00002508-200403000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.McDermid AJ, Rollman GB, McCain GA. Generalized hypervigilance in fibromyalgia: evidence of perceptual amplification. Pain. 1996;66(2-3):133–144. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(96)03059-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.González JL, Mercado F, Barjola P, et al. Generalized hypervigilance in fibromyalgia patients: an experimental analysis with the emotional Stroop paradigm. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2010;69(3):279–287. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2010.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Geisser ME, Glass JM, Rajcevska LD, et al. A psychophysical study of auditory and pressure sensitivity in patients with fibromyalgia and healthy controls. Journal of Pain. 2008;9(5):417–422. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2007.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Schweinhardt P, Sauro KM, Bushnell MC. Fibromyalgia: a disorder of the brain? Neuroscientist. 2008;14(5):415–421. doi: 10.1177/1073858407312521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bartley EJ, Rhudy JL, Williams AE. Experimental assessment of affective processing in fibromyalgia. Journal of Pain. 2009;10(11):1151–1160. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Dickenson AH. A cure for wind up: NMDA receptor antagonists as potential analgesics. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences. 1990;11(8):307–309. doi: 10.1016/0165-6147(90)90228-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Woolf CJ, Thompson SWN. The induction and maintenance of central sensitization is dependent on N-methyl-D-aspartic acid receptor activation; implications for the treatment of post-injury pain hypersensitivity states. Pain. 1991;44(3):293–299. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(91)90100-C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Staud R, Vierck CJ, Cannon RL, Mauderli AP, Price DD. Abnormal sensitization and temporal summation of second pain (wind-up) in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. Pain. 2001;91(1-2):165–175. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(00)00432-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Staud R, Vierck CJ, Robinson ME, Price DD. Effects of the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist dextromethorphan on temporal summation of pain are similar in fibromyalgia patients and normal control subjects. Journal of Pain. 2005;6(5):323–332. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2005.01.357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Le Bars D, Dickenson AH, Besson JM. Diffuse noxious inhibitory controls (DNIC). I. Effects on dorsal horn convergent neurones in the rat. Pain. 1979;6(3):283–304. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(79)90049-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Le Bars D, Dickenson AH, Besson JM. Diffuse noxious inhibitory controls (DNIC). II. Lack of effect on non-convergent neurones, supraspinal involvement and theoretical implications. Pain. 1979;6(3):305–327. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(79)90050-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Yarnitsky D, Arendt-Nielsen L, Bouhassira D, et al. Recommendations on terminology and practice of psychophysical DNIC testing. European Journal of Pain. 2010;14(4):p. 339. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2010.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kosek E, Hansson P. Modulatory influence on somatosensory perception from vibration and heterotopic noxious conditioning stimulation (HNCS) in fibromyalgia patients and healthy subjects. Pain. 1997;70(1):41–51. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(96)03295-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Julien N, Goffaux P, Arsenault P, Marchand S. Widespread pain in fibromyalgia is related to a deficit of endogenous pain inhibition. Pain. 2005;114(1-2):295–302. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.12.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Normand E, Potvin S, Gaumond I, Cloutier G, Corbin J-F, Marchand S. Pain inhibition is deficient in chronic widespread pain but normal in major depressive disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2011;72(2):219–224. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04969blu. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Tracey I, Ploghaus A, Gati JS, et al. Imaging attentional modulation of pain in the periaqueductal gray in humans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22(7):2748–2752. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-07-02748.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Wiech K, Ploner M, Tracey I. Neurocognitive aspects of pain perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2008;12(8):306–313. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Villemure C, Bushnell MC. Mood influences supraspinal pain processing separately from attention. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(3):705–715. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3822-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Villemure C, Schweinhardt P. Supraspinal pain processing: distinct roles of emotion and attention. Neuroscientist. 2010;16(3):276–284. doi: 10.1177/1073858409359200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Villemure C, Slotnick BM, Bushnell MC. Effects of odors on pain perception: deciphering the roles of emotion and attention. Pain. 2003;106(1-2):101–108. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(03)00297-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Bennett RM, Jones J, Turk DC, Russell IJ, Matallana L. An internet survey of 2,596 people with fibromyalgia. BMC Musculoskeletal Disorders. 2007;8, article 27 doi: 10.1186/1471-2474-8-27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Katz RS, Heard AR, Mills M, Leavitt F. The prevalence and clinical impact of reported cognitive difficulties (Fibrofog) in patients with rheumatic disease with and without fibromyalgia. Journal of Clinical Rheumatology. 2004;10(2):53–58. doi: 10.1097/01.rhu.0000120895.20623.9f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Leavitt F, Katz RS, Mills M, Heard AR. Cognitive and dissociative manifestations in fibromyalgia. Journal of Clinical Rheumatology. 2002;8(2):77–84. doi: 10.1097/00124743-200204000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Glass JM, Park DC, Minear M, Crofford LJ. Memory beliefs and function in fibromyalgia patients. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2005;58(3):263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2004.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Landrø NI, Stiles TC, Sletvold H. Memory functioning in patients with primary fibromyalgia and major depression and healthy controls. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1997;42(3):297–306. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(96)00301-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Sletvold H, Stiles TC, Landro NI. Information processing in primary fibromyalgia, major depression and healthy controls. Journal of Rheumatology. 1995;22(1):137–142. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Suhr JA. Neuropsychological impairment in fibromyalgia: relation to depression, fatigue, and pain. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2003;55(4):321–329. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(02)00628-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Cote KA, Moldofsky H. Sleep, daytime symptoms, and cognitive performance in patients with fibromyalgia. Journal of Rheumatology. 1997;24(10):2014–2023. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Grace GM, Nielson WR, Hopkins M, Berg MA. Concentration and memory deficits in patients with Fibromyalgia Syndrome. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1999;21(4):477–487. doi: 10.1076/jcen.21.4.477.876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Leavitt F, Katz RS. Distraction as a key determinant of impaired memory in patients with fibromyalgia. Journal of Rheumatology. 2006;33(1):127–132. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Dick BD, Verrier MJ, Harker KT, Rashiq S. Disruption of cognitive function in Fibromyalgia Syndrome. Pain. 2008;139(3):610–616. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Luerding R, Weigand T, Bogdahn U, Schmidt-Wilcke T. Working memory performance is correlated with local brain morphology in the medial frontal and anterior cingulate cortex in fibromyalgia patients: structural correlates of pain-cognition interaction. Brain. 2008;131(12):3222–3231. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Dick B, Eccleston C, Crombez G. Attentional functioning in fibromyalgia, rheumatoid arthritis, and musculoskeletal pain patients. Arthritis Care and Research. 2002;47(6):639–644. doi: 10.1002/art.10800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Leavitt F, Katz RS. Speed of mental operations in fibromyalgia a selective naming speed deficit. Journal of Clinical Rheumatology. 2008;14(4):214–218. doi: 10.1097/RHU.0b013e31817a2472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Correa A, Miró E, Martínez MP, Sánchez AI, Lupiáñez J. Temporal preparation and inhibitory deficit in fibromyalgia syndrome. Brain and Cognition. 2011;75: 211–216. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Miro E, Lupianez J, Hita E, Martinez MP, Sanchez AI, Buela-Casal G. Attentional deficits in fibromyalgia and its relationships with pain, emotional distress and sleep dysfunction complaints. Psychology and Health. 2011;26(6):765–780. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2010.493611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Kim SH, Kim SH, Kim SK, Nam EJ, Han SW, Lee SJ. Spatial versus verbal memory impairments in patients with fibromyalgia. doi: 10.1007/s00296-010-1762-1. Rheumatology International. In press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Munguía-Izquierdo D, Legaz-Arrese A. Assessment of the effects of aquatic therapy on global symptomatology in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome: a randomized controlled trial. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2008;89(12):2250–2257. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2008.03.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Park DC, Glass JM, Minear M, Crofford LJ. Cognitive function in fibromyalgia patients. Arthritis and Rheumatism. 2001;44(9):2125–2133. doi: 10.1002/1529-0131(200109)44:9<2125::AID-ART365>3.0.CO;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Leavitt F, Katz RS. Normalizing memory recall in fibromyalgia with rehearsal: a distraction-counteracting effect. Arthritis Care and Research. 2009;61(6):740–744. doi: 10.1002/art.24559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Schmidt-Wilcke T, Wood P, Lürding R. Cognitive impairment in patients suffering from fibromyalgia: an underestimated problem. Schmerz. 2010;24(1):46–53. doi: 10.1007/s00482-009-0872-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Glass JM. Review of cognitive dysfunction in fibromyalgia: a convergence on working memory and attentional control impairments. Rheumatic Disease Clinics of North America. 2009;35(2):299–311. doi: 10.1016/j.rdc.2009.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Verdejo-García A, López-Torrecillas F, Calandre EP, Delgado-Rodríguez A, Bechara A. Executive function and decision-making in women with fibromyalgia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2009;24(1):113–122. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acp014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Walteros C, Sánchez-Navarro JP, Muñoz MA, Martínez-Selva JM, Chialvo D, Montoya P. Altered associative learning and emotional decision making in fibromyalgia. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2011;70:294–301. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2010.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Apkarian AV, Sosa Y, Krauss BR, et al. Chronic pain patients are impaired on an emotional decision-making task. Pain. 2004;108(1):129–136. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2003.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Horne JA, Shackell BS. Alpha-like EEG activity in non-REM sleep and the fibromyalgia (fibrositis) syndrome. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1991;79(4):271–276. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(91)90122-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Drewes AM, Nielsen KD, Taagholt SJ, Bjerregard L, Svendsen L, Gade J. Sleep intensity in fibromyalgia: focus on the microstructure of the sleep process. British Journal of Rheumatology. 1995;34(7):629–635. doi: 10.1093/rheumatology/34.7.629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Burns JW, Crofford LJ, Chervin RD. Sleep stage dynamics in fibromyalgia patients and controls. Sleep Medicine. 2008;9(6):689–696. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2007.10.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Sarzi-Puttini P, Rizzi M, Andreoli A, et al. Hypersomnolence in fibromyalgia syndrome. Clinical and Experimental Rheumatology. 2002;20(1):69–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Moldofsky H, Lue FA. The relationship of alpha and delta EEG frequencies to pain and mood in “fibrositis” patients treated with chlorpromazine and L-tryptophan. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1980;50(1-2):71–80. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(80)90324-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Perlis ML, Giles DE, Bootzin RR, et al. Alpha sleep and information processing, perception of sleep, pain, and arousability in fibromyalgia. International Journal of Neuroscience. 1997;89(3-4):265–280. doi: 10.3109/00207459708988479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Roizenblatt S, Moldofsky H, Benedito-Silva AA, Tufik S. Alpha sleep characteristics in fibromyalgia. Arthritis and Rheumatism. 2001;44(1):222–230. doi: 10.1002/1529-0131(200101)44:1<222::AID-ANR29>3.0.CO;2-K. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Jennum P, Drewes AM, Andreasen A, Nielsen KD. Sleep and other symptoms in primary fibromyalgia and in healthy controls. Journal of Rheumatology. 1993;20(10):1756–1759. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Rizzi M, Sarzi-Puttini P, Atzeni F, et al. Cyclic alternating pattern: a new marker of sleep alteration in patients with fibromyalgia? Journal of Rheumatology. 2004;31(6):1193–1199. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Viola-Saltzman M, Watson NF, Bogart A, Goldberg J, Buchwald D. High prevalence of restless legs syndrome among patients with fibromyalgia: a controlled cross-sectional study. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine. 2010;6(5):423–427. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Okifuji A, Hare BD. Nightly analyses of subjective and objective (actigraphy) measures of sleep in fibromyalgia syndrome: what accounts for the discrepancy? The Clinical Journal of Pain. 2010;27: 289–296. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0b013e31820485db. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Cook DB, Lange G, Ciccone DS, Liu WC, Steffener J, Natelson BH. Functional imaging of pain in patients with primary fibromyalgia. Journal of Rheumatology. 2004;31(2):364–378. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Burgmer M, Pogatzki-Zahn E, Gaubitz M, Wessoleck E, Heuft G, Pfleiderer B. Altered brain activity during pain processing in fibromyalgia. NeuroImage. 2009;44(2):502–508. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Diers M, Schley MT, Rance M, et al. Differential central pain processing following repetitive intramuscular proton/prostaglandin E2 injections in female fibromyalgia patients and healthy controls. European Journal of Pain. 2011;15(7):716–723. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2010.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Diers M, Koeppe C, Yilmaz P, et al. Pain ratings and somatosensory evoked responses to repetitive intramuscular and intracutaneous stimulation in fibromyalgia syndrome. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology. 2008;25(3):153–160. doi: 10.1097/WNP.0b013e31817759c5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Pujol J, López-Solà M, Ortiz H, et al. Mapping brain response to pain in fibromyalgia patients using temporal analysis of fMRI. PLoS ONE. 2009;4(4) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005224. Article ID e5224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Staud R, Craggs JG, Perlstein WM, Robinson ME, Price DD. Brain activity associated with slow temporal summation of C-fiber evoked pain in fibromyalgia patients and healthy controls. European Journal of Pain. 2008;12(8):1078–1089. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2008.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Schweinhardt P, Bushnell MC. Pain imaging in health and disease—how far have we come? Journal of Clinical Investigation. 2010;120(11):3788–3797. doi: 10.1172/JCI43498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Wager TD, Rilling JK, Smith EE, et al. Placebo-induced changes in FMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science. 2004;303(5661):1162–1167. doi: 10.1126/science.1093065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Kuchinad A, Schweinhardt P, Seminowicz DA, Wood PB, Chizh BA, Bushnell MC. Accelerated brain gray matter loss in fibromyalgia patients: premature aging of the brain? Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(15):4004–4007. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0098-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Burgmer M, Gaubitz M, Konrad C, et al. Decreased gray matter volumes in the cingulo-frontal cortex and the amygdala in patients with fibromyalgia. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2009;71(5):566–573. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181a32da0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Wood PB, Glabus MF, Simpson R, Patterson JC. Changes in gray matter density in fibromyalgia: correlation with dopamine metabolism. Journal of Pain. 2009;10(6):609–618. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Lutz J, Jäger L, De Quervain D, et al. White and gray matter abnormalities in the brain of patients with fibromyalgia: a diffusion-tensor and volumetric imaging study. Arthritis and Rheumatism. 2008;58(12):3960–3969. doi: 10.1002/art.24070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Schmidt-Wilcke T, Luerding R, Weigand T, et al. Striatal grey matter increase in patients suffering from fibromyalgia—A voxel-based morphometry study. Pain. 2007;132(1):S109–S116. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Robinson ME, Craggs JG, Price DD, Perlstein WM, Staud R. Gray matter volumes of pain-related brain areas are decreased in fibromyalgia syndrome. Journal of Pain. 2011;12(4):436–443. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.10.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Puri BK, Agour M, Gunatilake KDR, Fernando KAC, Gurusinghe AI, Treasaden IH. Reduction in left supplementary motor area grey matter in adult female fibromyalgia sufferers with marked fatigue and without affective disorder: a pilot controlled 3-T magnetic resonance imaging voxel-based morphometry study. Journal of International Medical Research. 2010;38(4):1468–1472. doi: 10.1177/147323001003800429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Scott DJ, Heitzeg MM, Koeppe RA, Stohler CS, Zubieta JK. Variations in the human pain stress experience mediated by ventral and dorsal basal ganglia dopamine activity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26(42):10789–10795. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2577-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Harris RE, Clauw DJ, Scott DJ, McLean SA, Gracely RH, Zubieta JK. Decreased central mu-opioid receptor availability in fibromyalgia. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(37):10000–10006. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2849-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Park DC, Gutchess AH. Aging, cognition, and culture: a neuroscientific perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2002;26(7):859–867. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(02)00072-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H. Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain. 2000;123(11):2189–2202. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Clark L, Bechara A, Damasio H, Aitken MRF, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Differential effects of insular and ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions on risky decision-making. Brain. 2008;131(5):1311–1322. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Northoff G, Grimm S, Boeker H, et al. Affective judgment and beneficial decision making: ventromedial prefrontal activity correlates with performance in the Iowa Gambling Task. Human Brain Mapping. 2006;27(7):572–587. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Fukui H, Murai T, Fukuyama H, Hayashi T, Hanakawa T. Functional activity related to risk anticipation during performance of the Iowa gambling task. NeuroImage. 2005;24(1):253–259. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Bar-On R, Tranel D, Denburg NL, Bechara A. Exploring the neurological substrate of emotional and social intelligence. Brain. 2003;126(8):1790–1800. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Paulus MP, Rogalsky C, Simmons A, Feinstein JS, Stein MB. Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism. NeuroImage. 2003;19(4):1439–1448. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00251-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E. N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Human Brain Mapping. 2005;25(1):46–59. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Müller NG, Knight RT. The functional neuroanatomy of working memory: contributions of human brain lesion studies. Neuroscience. 2006;139(1):51–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Champod AS, Petrides M. Dissociable roles of the posterior parietal and the prefrontal cortex in manipulation and monitoring processes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2007;104(37):14837–14842. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0607101104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Ragland JD, Turetsky BI, Gur RC, et al. Working memory for complex figures: an fMRI comparison of letter and fractal n-back tasks. Neuropsychology. 2002;16(3):370–379. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Veltman DJ, Rombouts SARB, Dolan RJ. Maintenance versus manipulation in verbal working memory revisited: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2003;18(2):247–256. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00049-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Cohen JD, Perlstein WM, Braver TS, et al. Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature. 1997;386(6625):604–611. doi: 10.1038/386604a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Bäckman L, Nyberg L, Lindenberger U, Li SC, Farde L. The correlative triad among aging, dopamine, and cognition: current status and future prospects. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2006;30(6):791–807. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Moldofsky H. Rheumatic pain modulation syndrome: the interrelationships between sleep, central nervous system serotonin, and pain. Advances in neurology. 1982;33:51–57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Russell IJ, Michalek JE, Vipraio GA, Fletcher EM, Javors MA, Bowden CA. Platelet 3H-imipramine uptake receptor density and serum serotonin levels in patients with fibromyalgia/fibrositis syndrome. Journal of Rheumatology. 1992;19(1):104–109. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Russell IJ, Vaeroy H, Javors M, Nyberg F. Cerebrospinal fluid biogenic amine metabolites in fibromyalgia/fibrositis syndrome and rheumatoid arthritis. Arthritis and Rheumatism. 1992;35(5):550–556. doi: 10.1002/art.1780350509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Andersen ML, Nascimento DC, MacHado RB, Roizenblatt S, Moldofsky H, Tufik S. Sleep disturbance induced by substance P in mice. Behavioural Brain Research. 2006;167(2):212–218. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Russell IJ, Orr MD, Littman B, et al. Elevated cerebrospinal fluid levels of substance P in patients with the fibromyalgia syndrome. Arthritis and Rheumatism. 1994;37(11):1593–1601. doi: 10.1002/art.1780371106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Vaeroy H, Helle R, Forre O, Kass E, Terenius L. Elevated CSF levels of substance P and high incidence of Raynaud phenomenon in patients with fibromalgia: new features for diagnosis. Pain. 1988;32(1):21–26. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(88)90019-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Mayer AR, Hanlon FM, Franco AR, et al. The neural networks underlying auditory sensory gating. NeuroImage. 2009;44(1):182–189. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Posner MI, Driver J. The neurobiology of selective attention. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1992;2(2):165–169. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(92)90006-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.McMains S, Kastner S. Interactions of top-down and bottom-up mechanisms in human visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31(2):587–597. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3766-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Grabenhorst F, Rolls ET. Attentional modulation of affective versus sensory processing: functional connectivity and a top-down biased activation theory of selective attention. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2010;104(3):1649–1660. doi: 10.1152/jn.00352.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Staines WR, Graham SJ, Black SE, McIlroy WE. Task-relevant modulation of contralateral and ipsilateral primary somatosensory cortex and the role of a prefrontal-cortical sensory gating system. NeuroImage. 2002;15(1):190–199. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Bardouille T, Picton TW, Ross B. Attention modulates beta oscillations during prolonged tactile stimulation. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;31(4):761–769. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07094.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Montoya P, Sitges C. Affective modulation of somatosensory-evoked potentials elicited by tactile stimulation. Brain Research. 2006;1068(1):205–212. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Montoya P, Sitges C, García-Herrera M, et al. Reduced brain habituation to somatosensory stimulation in patients with fibromyalgia. Arthritis and Rheumatism. 2006;54(6):1995–2003. doi: 10.1002/art.21910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Carrillo-de-la-Peña MT, Vallet M, Pérez MI, Gómez-Perretta C. Intensity dependence of auditory-evoked cortical potentials in fibromyalgia patients: a test of the generalized hypervigilance hypothesis. Journal of Pain. 2006;7(7):480–487. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2006.01.452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Koob GF. Neural mechanisms of drug reinforcement. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1992;654:171–191. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb25966.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Pain Research and Treatment are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES