Abstract
Objective:
Parental alcoholism is generally found to be a strong predictor of alcohol misuse. Although the majority of siblings agree on the presence of parental alcohol issues, there is a significant minority who do not.
Method:
The current study analyzed sibling data from the 1979 National Longitudinal Survey of Youth using multilevel modeling, which accounts for the nested structure of the data. These analyses permitted a test of whether (a) identifying one's father as an alcoholic predicted greater risk of alcohol problems, (b) being from a family whose siblings did not all agree on the presence of paternal alcoholism increased the likelihood of alcohol problems, and (c) risk of alcohol misuse significantly differed among individuals from families in which there was familial disagreement about paternal alcoholism.
Results:
Results show that individuals who identified their father as an alcoholic were themselves more likely to have alcohol issues as compared with individuals both within and between families who did not identify their father as an alcoholic. Risk was similar for individuals in families in which there was disagreement about paternal alcoholism compared with individuals from families in which everyone agreed on the presence of paternal alcoholism. Moreover, there was not a significant interaction between paternal alcoholism attributions and familial disagreement.
Conclusions:
Findings indicate that in the case of child reports of paternal alcoholism, the increased risk of alcohol problems holds true regardless of the accuracy of an individual's assessment. These results may be not only because of the impact of paternal alcoholism on a person's alcohol misuse but also because of a person's alcohol problems potentially influencing his or her perceptions of familial alcohol-related behaviors.
Alcohol misuse is associated with myriad negative health outcomes, including markedly increased rates of accidental death (Rehm et al., 2001; Smith et al., 1999), cancer (Boffetta and Hashibe, 2006; Pöschl and Seitz, 2004), and suicide (Andreasson et al., 1988; Sher, 2006). Additionally, alcoholism tends to run in families, with both maternal and paternal alcoholism being significant predictors of children's substance use, even after accounting for other related factors such as the child's stress, sociability, and peer substance use (Chassin et al., 1993). Research on alcoholism frequently uses family member reports when determining rates of alcoholism in a family; however, there is not always consensus among family members as to whether a family member exhibits problematic drinking behavior (Andreasen et al., 1977; Crews and Sher, 1992; Prescott et al., 2005; Slutske et al., 1996).
Our goal in the present research was to examine whether attributions of paternal alcohol use were predictive of differential risk of alcohol problems within and between families and whether this effect was moderated by familial disagreement about paternal alcoholism. Real disagreement among family members is of interest clinically because it may result in problems mounting or sustaining a family-based alcohol intervention effort. Moreover, disagreement about parental alcoholism among siblings is meaningful beyond considerations of accuracy and intervention motivations because there may also be differential levels of risk for siblings identifying a parent as an alcoholic compared with those who do not. Given the ubiquity of studies administering measures of retrospective parental alcoholism and current alcohol use, it is important to understand the biases inherent in self-reported familial alcoholism. It is possible that the reported link between parental alcoholism and a person's own alcohol issues is not entirely a function of genetic and behavioral influences but is also influenced by an individual's psychological need to explain the etiology of current issues with alcohol.
Originally proposed by Heider (1958), attribution theory is a highly relevant theoretical perspective for why and how individuals draw inferences for explaining health-related behaviors and conditions. Specifically, when individuals are injured, fall ill, or struggle with some sort of chronic disease such as alcoholism, they will often attempt to regain a sense of control over their situation by developing a story or theory as to why the event has occurred (e.g., Schulz and Decker, 1985; Taylor, 1983), with one common strategy being to place the blame on others (Tennen and Affleck, 1990). When faced with one's own problematic drinking, a person may be more likely to identify other family members as alcoholics, thereby providing an explanation for his or her own drinking. Indeed, there is evidence supporting this possibility; one study analyzing reports of alcoholism of family members found that the alcoholic informants were much more likely to incorrectly indicate that a target family member had alcohol problems (Roy et al., 1994).
Why is it that, of the many reasons people might be inclined to attribute as a source of their problematic drinking, they would be more likely to identify familial influence? Freud (1896) noted a tendency for people to project their own biases, schemas, and motives onto other people and situations in his theorizing on projection, which is a defense mechanism in which individuals tend to see their faults in others rather than themselves. More modern treatments of this phenomenon argue that individuals are highly protective of their positive self-image (Hepper et al., 2010) and therefore would likely find a designation of “alcoholic” to be exceedingly aversive, leading to the suppression of the thought. Because suppressed thoughts tend to be highly accessible (Wegner and Erber, 1992; Wegner et al., 1987), individuals may cope by denying possession of the negative trait and instead project it onto close others. Laboratory evidence confirms this possibility; when participants attempt to suppress thoughts associated with a negative trait imputed to them, they tend to interpret the behavior of others as reflective of that trait (Newman et al., 1997). Hence, individuals who have issues with alcohol may cope by deflecting their perceived responsibility and projecting their own alcohol issues onto influential close others.
Examining sibling reports of familial alcoholism provides a useful test of whether individuals with alcohol problems are in fact more likely to perceive a parent as an alcoholic. If it is the case that individuals with alcohol problems are inclined to project their alcohol problems onto their parents, then risk will be greater for those who identify their parents as an alcoholic and whose siblings do not. Alternatively, if those who do not identify a parent as an alcoholic and who have siblings who do are at greater risk for alcohol issues, then the cause of the disagreement may be more likely because of underreporting of parental alcoholism than projection. In brief, the overreporting and underreporting of parental alcoholism are both possible attribution errors, although we hypothesize the causes are somewhat different (i.e., overreporting is likely a result of projection, whereas underreporting is likely because of denial and/or a lack of full knowledge of the parent's behavior).
The current analyses were conducted using the 1988 wave of the 1979 National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY79). Given the nested nature of the data (i.e., siblings nested within family), multilevel modeling was used, which simultaneously accounts for both individual-level (Level 1) and between-family (Level 2) differences (Raudenbush and Bryk, 2002). This approach therefore allows us to simultaneously examine individual-level variables (e.g., identifying one's father as an alcoholic), family-level variables (e.g., familial disagreement about the presence of paternal alcoholism), and cross-level interactions (e.g., attribution of paternal alcoholism by familial disagreement). In a simulation study of sibling data on parental alcohol use, Krull (2007) determined that multilevel modeling achieved a power value of .80 with approximately 227 families of 410 nonindependent individuals. The NLSY79 data set contained 1,331 sibling sets comprising 3,125 respondents, indicating more than adequate statistical power to test the effects of interest.
Data from families in which there were two or more sibling reports on paternal alcoholism were analyzed to determine whether there were significant differences in alcohol misuse between those who identified their father as an alcoholic and those who did not (Level 1). In addition, we examined whether individuals from families in which there was disagreement about paternal alcoholism were more at risk for alcohol misuse (Level 2) as well as whether risk was greater for those who identified their father as an alcoholic and who had one or more siblings who did not (Level 1 × Level 2 interaction). Demographic variables such as age, race, and gender (all Level 1) were also tested to control for their potential effects in the model.
Method
The NLSY79 is a longitudinal study of a nationally representative sample of men and women who were 14–22 years old at the time of their first interview in 1979. The overall data set includes all youths living in surveyed households who were born between 1957 and 1964, yielding an initial sample of 12,686 individuals in 2,826 families. The sampling procedure was designed to include a disproportionate number of racial minorities and poverty households (Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2009). The data set used for the current analyses comprised families for whom two or more siblings reported on familial alcoholism in 1988 and whose data could be linked (N = 1,331). Seventy-two percent (n = 952) of the families had two siblings, 23% (n = 309) had three siblings, and the remainder (n = 70) had four to six siblings. Seventy-nine percent of the sibling sets agreed that their father was not an alcoholic. Of the families in which one or more siblings indicated paternal alcoholism, approximately 40% (n = 114) were in agreement, whereas the remainder (n = 168) had some level of disagreement. Only 2% of the total sample identified their mother as an alcoholic; therefore, further analyses comparing sibling reports for maternal alcoholism were not possible.
Alcohol misuse was assessed using a version of the well-validated CAGE questionnaire (Ewing, 1984; O'Brien, 2008), which some research indicates is a more reliable measure of alcoholism than even some laboratory-administered biochemical tests (Girela et al., 1994). The 1988 NLSY79 survey asked 12 CAGE-type items (see Table 1). Concordant with CAGE scoring practices, answering positively to two or more of these items resulted in a positive alcoholism score for an individual. Directly after answering the CAGE items, respondents were asked to identify any close relatives who have “been alcoholics or problem drinkers at any time in their lives” (National Opinion Research Center, 1988, p. 173).
Table 1.
Cut down drinking |
Once you started drinking, was it difficult for you to stop before you became completely intoxicated? |
Have you sometimes kept on drinking after promising yourself not to? |
Have you deliberately tried to cut down or quit drinking, but didn't manage to do so? |
Annoyed by criticism of your drinking |
Have you gotten into a fight while drinking? |
Have you felt aggressive or cross while drinking? |
Have you gotten into a heated argument while drinking? |
Guilty feelings about drinking |
Were you afraid you might be an alcoholic or that you might become one? |
Have you awakened the next day not being able to remember things you had done while drinking? |
Have you lost a job, or nearly lost one, because of drinking? |
Morning Eye opener |
Have your hands shaken a lot the morning after drinking? |
Have you often taken a drink the first thing when you got up in the morning? |
Have you stayed away from work because of a hangover? |
Note: All items were in reference to the previous year, and the response options were yes/no, with ≥2 considered indicative of alcohol misuse.
Because the outcome of interest (likely alcohol misuse) was dichotomous, models were estimated using the PROC GLIMMIX procedure in SAS 9.2 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC). Given that the data were primarily dyadic, only the intercept was treated as a random effect (see Kenny et al., 2002). All continuous variables were grand mean centered to facilitate interpretation of the estimates. Dichotomous variables were dummy-coded using 0 and 1. Because multilevel generalized linear models use a quasi-likelihood estimation strategy, only the Wald statistic was used to determine whether an effect should be kept in the final model. The model-building approach used a modified version of the bottom-up approach advocated by Hox (2010).
Results
The intraclass correlation, computed assuming a threshold model (Snijders and Bosker, 1999), was .098. This indicates that 9.8% of the variance for alcohol misuse was at Level 2, suggesting a substantial degree of family-level effects. Being male (b = 0.52, p < .001), unmarried (b = 0.55, p < .0001), and having lower educational attainment (b = 0.11, p < .001) were all significantly associated with an increased risk of alcohol problems. Racial category and urban/rural environment were not significant predictors of alcohol misuse (ps > .05). Younger age and lower income were individually predictive of greater alcohol misuse, although the former became marginally significant (p = .07) and the latter became nonsignificant (p = .16) when entered with the other significant predictors. Both age and income were therefore dropped from the final model.
The hypothesis of interest, which was based on projection and attribution theories, posits that individuals who identify their father as an alcoholic, regardless of the accuracy of that attribution, will have higher rates of alcohol problems. Results from the multilevel modeling supported this supposition (b = 0.89, p < .0001). That is, after controlling for significant demographic factors, those who identified their father as an alcoholic, regardless of familial agreement, had an alcohol misuse rate of 27.3%, which was more than double the rate of 13.4% for those not identifying their father as an alcoholic. Disagreement about paternal alcoholism was not significantly predictive of alcohol misuse, nor was the interaction between identifying one's father as an alcoholic and familial agreement (ps > .05).
Discussion
The purpose of the analyses was to determine whether identifying one's father as an alcoholic, regardless of accuracy or familial consensus, can be taken prima facie as a risk indicator of alcohol misuse. The results support this view: Those who identified their father as an alcoholic, regardless of familial consensus, were more likely to exhibit problematic drinking. Familial disagreement was not predictive of alcohol issues, nor was the interaction between familial disagreement and identifying one's father as an alcoholic. These findings indicate that the risk of alcohol misuse for individuals who identified their father as an alcoholic and who had siblings who did not identify their father as an alcoholic did not differ from the risk of alcohol misuse for individuals whose siblings all agreed that their father was an alcoholic. The results suggest that the association between the attribution of paternal alcoholism and alcohol problems may be in part because of problem drinkers seeking to explain the genesis of their own alcohol issues.
A limitation of this data set, and therefore of our analysis, is that objective measures of paternal alcohol use were not available. This means that it cannot be definitively determined that cases of familial disagreement are mainly because of overreporting of paternal alcoholism. Underreporting seems less likely given that there was neither a main effect for familial disagreement nor an interaction between familial disagreement and a paternal alcoholism attribution. Also, it is possible that when siblings disagree about a parent's alcoholism, in some cases they each may be correct in that they have different memories and experiences because of changes in the parent's drinking behavior over time. Future research would benefit from a comparison of the predictive utility of objective measures of familial alcoholism with family member reports. These data would allow a direct examination of whether familial estimates are more likely to be overestimates, underestimates, or differentially accurate as a function of age and experiential differences. In the meantime, the research outlined herein clearly demonstrates that, from a clinical perspective, an attribution of parental alcoholism is a fundamentally important risk indicator of alcohol problems, regardless of its accuracy.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Phil Costanzo and Phil Cook for their input on the study concept; David A. Kenny, Madeline Carrig, and Krista Ranby for advice on the analyses; and Rose Wilson for her assistance with the reference citations.
Footnotes
The preparation of this article was facilitated in part by National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) Grant P30 DA023026. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of NIDA.
References
- Andreasen NC, Endicott J, Spitzer RL, Winokur G. The family history method using diagnostic criteria. Reliability and validity. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1977;34:1229–1235. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1977.01770220111013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreasson S, Allebeck P, Romelsjö A. Alcohol and mortality among young men: longitudinal study of Swedish conscripts. British Medical Journal (Clinical Research Ed.) 1988;296:1021–1025. doi: 10.1136/bmj.296.6628.1021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boffetta P, Hashibe M. Alcohol and cancer. The Lancet Oncology. 2006;7:149–156. doi: 10.1016/S1470-2045(06)70577-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bureau of Labor Statistics. National Longitudinal Surveys: The NLSY79. 2009. Retrieved from http://www.bls.gov/nls/nlsy79.htm. [Google Scholar]
- Chassin L, Pillow DR, Curran PJ, Molina BSG, Barrera M., Jr Relation of parental alcoholism to early adolescent substance use: A test of three mediating mechanisms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1993;102:3–19. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.102.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crews TM, Sher KJ. Using adapted short MASTs for assessing parental alcoholism: Reliability and validity. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1992;16:576–584. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1992.tb01420.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ewing JA. Detecting alcoholism. The CAGE questionnaire. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1984;252:1905–1907. doi: 10.1001/jama.252.14.1905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freud S. Weitere Bemerkungen Über Die Abwehr-neuropsychosen (Further Remarks on the Neuro-Psychoses of Defense.) Early Psycho-Analytic Publications; 1896. The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Volume III, 1893–1899) [Google Scholar]
- Girela E, Villanueva E, Hernandez-Cueto C, Luna JD. Comparison of the CAGE questionnaire versus some biochemical markers in the diagnosis of alcoholism. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 1994;29:337–343. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heider F. The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1958. [Google Scholar]
- Hepper EG, Gramzow RH, Sedikides C. Individual differences in self-enhancement and self-protection strategies: An integrative analysis. Journal of Personality. 2010;78:781–814. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00633.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hox JJ. Multilevel analysis. Techniques and applications. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Routledge; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Kenny DA, Kashy DA, Cook WL. Dyadic data analysis. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Krull JL. Using multilevel analyses with sibling data to increase analytic power: An illustration and simulation study. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43:602–619. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.3.602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Opinion Research Center. National Longitudinal Survey of Labor Market Experience, Round Ten Youth Survey. 1988. Accessed at http://www.nlsinfo.org/ordering/display_db.php3. [Google Scholar]
- O'Brien CP. The CAGE questionnaire for detection of alcoholism: A remarkably useful but simple tool. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2008;300:2054–2056. doi: 10.1001/jama.2008.570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman LS, Duff KJ, Baumeister RF. A new look at defensive projection: thought suppression, accessibility, and biased person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1997;72:980–1001. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.72.5.980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pöschl G, Seitz HK. Alcohol and cancer. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 2004;39:155–165. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agh057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prescott CA, Sullivan PF, Myers JM, Patterson DG, Devitt M, Halberstadt LJ, Kendler KS. The Irish Affected Sib Pair Study of Alcohol Dependence: study methodology and validation of diagnosis by interview and family history. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2005;29:417–429. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000156085.50418.07. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS. Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. 2nd ed. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Rehm J, Greenfield TK, Rogers JD. Average volume of alcohol consumption, patterns of drinking, and all-cause mortality: Results from the US National Alcohol Survey. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2001;153:64–71. doi: 10.1093/aje/153.1.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roy MA, Walsh D, Prescott CA, Kendler KS. Biases in the diagnosis of alcoholism by the family history method. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1994;18:845–851. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1994.tb00049.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulz R, Decker S. Long-term adjustment to physical disability: The role of social support, perceived control, and self-blame. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1985;48:1162–1172. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.48.5.1162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sher L. Alcohol consumption and suicide. QJM. 2006;99:57–61. doi: 10.1093/qjmed/hci146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slutske WS, Heath AC, Madden PAF, Bucholz KK, Dinwiddie SH, Dunne MP, Martin NG. Reliability and reporting biases for perceived parental history of alcohol-related problems: Agreement between twins and differences between discordant pairs. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1996;57:387–395. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1996.57.387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith GS, Branas CC, Miller TR. Fatal nontraffic injuries involving alcohol: A metaanalysis. Annals of Emergency Medicine. 1999;33:659–668. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snijders T, Bosker R. Multilevel analysis. London, England: Sage; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SE. Adjustment to threatening events: A theory of cognitive adaptation. American Psychologist. 1983;38:1161–1173. [Google Scholar]
- Tennen H, Affleck G. Blaming others for threatening events. Psychological Bulletin. 1990;108:209–232. [Google Scholar]
- Wegner DM, Erber R. The hyperaccessibility of suppressed thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1992;63:903–912. [Google Scholar]
- Wegner DM, Schneider DJ, Carter S, White T. Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987;53:5–13. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.53.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]