Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Adolesc. 2011 Jun 22;35(2):451–454. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.05.005

Brief Report: How Anxiously Withdrawn Preadolescents Think about Friendship

Bridget K Fredstrom 1, Linda Rose-Krasnor 2, Kelly Campbell 2, Kenneth H Rubin 3, Cathryn Booth-LaForce 4, Kim B Burgess 5
PMCID: PMC3213280  NIHMSID: NIHMS303369  PMID: 21696816

Abstract

Previous research suggests that anxiously withdrawn preadolescents demonstrate success in forming friendships, yet these friendships tend to be of lesser quality. Drawing on Selman’s (1980) theory of interpersonal understanding, we compared levels of friendship understanding between anxiously withdrawn preadolescents and a sample of non-withdrawn age mates. Fifth graders (N=116; 58% girls; mean age = 10.33 yrs) completed same-sex friendship and social behavior nominations, as well as a semi-structured clinical interview assessing understanding of various friendship issues in response to a hypothetical friendship dilemma. Results suggest that anxiously withdrawn preadolescents demonstrated lower levels of friendship understanding for some, but not all, friendship issues that may be related to friendship quality. The findings suggest that social cognitive assessments of friendship may be useful in understanding the friendship successes and difficulties of anxiously withdrawn preadolescents.

Keywords: Friendship, Social withdrawal, Friendship reasoning, Perspective taking, Social cognition, Preadolescents


Involvement in successful friendships has been associated with overall positive adjustment (see Rubin, Bukowski, & Parker, 2006 for review). Not all youth, however, are able to form and maintain these healthy relationships. Socially withdrawn youth, defined as those who consistently display anxious solitary behaviors when among peers (Rubin, Coplan, & Bowker, 2009), may be at particular risk for friendship difficulties. They frequently lack the social skills and cognitions that underlie friendship relationships (e.g., Hodges, Malone, & Perry, 1997), perhaps due to their restricted peer interactions (e.g., Rubin & Krasnor, 1986; Schneider, 2009). In this study, we focus on a subset of socially withdrawn preadolescents who show anxious withdrawal in the presence of familiar peers. We examined the friendship-related cognitions of these anxiously withdrawn youth in an attempt to explain their friendship successes and difficulties.

Selman (1980) suggested that adolescents’ understanding of friendship issues (e.g., formation, closeness/intimacy) could be categorized into one of five invariant developmental stages, which reflect increasingly complex perspective taking skills and coordination of multiple viewpoints. Adolescents with clinically diagnosed aggressive or emotional difficulties show less mature friendship understanding than do nonclinical youth (e.g., Gurucharri, Phelps, & Selman, 1984). Withdrawn youth, who also exhibit peer difficulties (e.g., Ladd, 2006), may show similarly immature friendship understanding. Indeed, some researchers have found negative relations between social withdrawal and friendship understanding (e.g. Hart, Keller, Edelstein, & Hoffmann, 1998; Schneider & Tessier, 2007); others, however, have failed to find such a connection (Cohen, Kershner, & Wherspann, 1985).

There are several potential reasons for this inconsistency. First, findings may vary with specific friendship issues. Although socially withdrawn youth are as likely to have and maintain a mutual best friendship as are their more socially competent peers (Rubin, Wojslawowicz, Rose-Krasnor, Booth-LaForce, & Burgess, 2006; Schneider 1999), their friendships tend to be of lesser quality (Fordham & Stevenson-Hinde, 1999; Rubin, Wojslawowicz et al., 2006). Thus, we hypothesized that anxiously withdrawn preadolescents would not differ from their typical age mates in their understanding of friendship formation, but would show lower levels of friendship understanding for closeness, trust, conflict resolution, and friendship termination issues. Second, researchers have failed to differentiate between youth with and without friends, introducing a potential confound. Therefore, we focused on a sample of anxiously withdrawn and typical preadolescents for whom a mutually best friend could be identified. Finally, methods used to identify socially withdrawn samples have been inconsistent (e.g. Cohen et al., 1985; Pellegrini, 1986). Often researchers have failed to distinguish between active isolation by peers and withdrawal from the peer group due to social anxiety and negative self-regard, obscuring important conceptual and empirical differences between excluded and withdrawn youth (see Rubin et al, 2009). Therefore, it is unclear in previous studies whether it was anxious-withdrawn behavior or peer isolation that was associated with friendship understanding. We addressed this issue by conceptualizing and assessing anxious withdrawal independently of peer rejection.

In summary, our goal was to examine differences in friendship understanding between anxiously withdrawn preadolescents and their more typical classmates. We addressed notable gaps in the literature by focusing on specific friendship issues, assessing social withdrawal independently from peer rejection, and including only preadolescents who had mutual best friends.

Method

Participants

Our 827 participants (406 boys) were drawn from a larger sample of 5th graders from eight mid-Atlantic U.S. elementary schools, with a mean age of 10.33 years (SD = 0.52). Parental consent rate was 84%. Ethnic and racial compositions within the schools (40% Caucasian, 22% Hispanic/Latino, 22% African American, and 15% Asian) paralleled countywide distributions.

School Measures

Friendship nominations (Bukowski, Hoza, & Boivin, 1994)

Participants nominated their two same-sex “best” friends and all had at least one same-sex mutually reciprocated friendship (Asher, Parker, & Walker, 1996).

Child Behaviors

Participants nominated same-sex classmates for various roles in an extended version of the Revised Class Play (ECP; adapted from Masten, Morison, & Pellegrini, 1985). Item scores were standardized within gender and classroom. The Aggression (7 items; e.g. “Someone who fights”; Cronbach’s α=.91) and Anxious Withdrawal (4 items; e.g. “Someone who gets nervous about participating in group discussions;” Cronbach’s α=.87) factors of the ECP were used herein (see Burgess, Wojslawowicz, Rubin, Rose-Krasnor, & Booth-LaForce, 2006 for details). This behaviorally based measure has been used successfully in other studies of peer relationships (Bowker & Spencer, 2010; Rubin et al., 2006).

Identification of the Risk and Control Groups

ECP scores were used to demarcate the sample further. Anxiously Withdrawn preadolescents (n = 52, 18 boys) had Anxious Withdrawal ECP scores in the top 33% and Aggression scores in the bottom 50%. Comparison preadolescents (n = 64, 30 boys) had Anxious Withdrawal and Aggression scores in the bottom 50% (see Ladd & Burgess, 1999 and Schneider, 2009 for similar procedures). As expected, preadolescents in the Anxiously Withdrawn group had higher ECP withdrawal scores than did preadolescents in the Comparison group, F(1, 114) = 157.67, p=.001. The Anxiously Withdrawn group members also had lower ECP aggression scores, F(1,114) = 6.66, p=.01.

Laboratory Measure

Friendship Conceptions Interview (adapted from Schultz, Yeates, & Selman, 1989; Selman, 1980)

A hypothetical friendship dilemma was followed by probes to elicit preadolescents’ understanding of friendship formation (e.g. Why does a person need a good friend?), closeness (e.g. What makes a good close friendship last?), trust (e.g. Do you think trust is important for a good friendship?), conflict resolution (e.g. How should arguments be settled between friends?), and friendship termination issues (e.g. What makes friendships break up?). Each response was coded into one of five stage levels; the highest stage score within an issue was used for analyses. The measure showed good inter-rater reliability (percentage agreement = 83.4%; Cohen’s Kappa = 0.72, based on independent ratings of 27 transcripts).

Procedures

Following IRB approval, school-based measures were administered in large group format to all preadolescents who received parental consent. Participants were subsequently invited to the laboratory with their mutual best friends to complete additional questionnaires and the Friendship Conceptions Interview.

Results

Differences in Friendship Understanding

A Group (Anxiously Withdrawn, Comparison) by Gender MANCOVA was conducted for the five friendship understanding issues, with aggression as a covariate. Pillai’s Trace criterion revealed significant multivariate effects for Group, F (5, 107) = 4.46, p=.001, n2 =.17 and Gender, F (5, 107) = 2.30, p=.05, n2 =.10. Post-hoc tests for Group revealed significant main effects for closeness, F (1, 111) = 11.74, p=.001, n2 =.10 and friendship termination, F (1, 111) = 5.60, p=.02, n2 =.05 (see Table 1). Anxiously withdrawn preadolescents responded to closeness and friendship termination issues with less social cognitive sophistication than did Comparison preadolescents.

Table 1.

Standardized Variable Means and Standard Deviations for Study Sample

Socially Withdrawn group (n = 52) Comparison group (n = 64) F p-value
M SD M SD
Child Behaviors
Social withdrawal 1.04 1.00 - 0.54 0.10 157.67 0.000
Aggression - 0.57 0.10 - 0.52 0.10 6.66 0.011

Friendship Issues
Formation 2.29 0.50 2.22 0.49 1.00 0.321
Closeness 2.71 0.46 2.91 0.30 11.74 0.001
Trust 2.11 0.38 2.25 0.47 3.06 0.083
Conflict resolution 2.42 0.54 2.55 0.50 1.32 0.252
Termination 2.29 0.54 2.53 0.56 5.60 0.020

Follow-up univariate tests for Gender revealed a significant main effect for the issue of closeness within friendship, F (1, 111) = 7.79, p=.006, n2 =.07; girls responded at higher levels of sophistication than did boys, consistent with prior literature (e.g., Zarbatany, McDougall, & Hymel, 2000). All interaction effects were non-significant.

Discussion

Our results revealed that anxiously withdrawn and nonwithdrawn preadolescents demonstrated similar understanding of friendship formation, offering support for previously established similarity in friendship prevalence in withdrawn and non-withdrawn children (Rubin, Wojslawowicz et al., 2006; Schneider, 1999). Withdrawn youth often have the skills and knowledge to form friendships; however, the ability to sustain high quality friendships may require different cognitions and social skills (Bowker, Rubin, Rose-Krasnor, & Booth-LaForce, 2007). Indeed, our study revealed that anxiously withdrawn preadolescents had lower levels of understanding for closeness and termination issues. The lack of understanding of closeness and intimacy found in the current study may explain these relatively poor friendship quality interactions. Indeed, withdrawn young adolescents have been found to interact less frequently with their friends than non-withdrawn adolescents while showing more neutral affect (Schneider, 2009).

Our results also showed that anxiously withdrawn adolescents had lower levels of reasoning about friendship termination, suggesting they tend to view friendship termination as the product of unilateral rather than mutual causes. In reality, it may be the best friend, rather than the anxiously withdrawn preadolescent, who decides to end the relationship. Presently, researchers have not examined how preadolescents think about or place responsibility for the break-up of their friendships.

Unexpectedly, the friendship cognitions of anxiously withdrawn preadolescents did not differ from comparison peers for conflict resolution and trust issues. Withdrawn children have been shown to provide more immature resolution strategies to conflict than do less withdrawn children, as well as less assertive techniques and more other-oriented goals (Adalbjamardottir, 1995; Rubin, Daniels-Beirness, & Bream, 1984). One explanation for our dissimilar results may be that the Selman perspective-taking coding, unlike those used in previous research, is based on the rationale for the response rather than its specific content. A second explanation centers on recent evidence suggesting that anxiously withdrawn children may be as adept as their more sociable peers at negotiation and perspective taking but only within the context of a mutual best friendship (Burgess et al., 2006). The social cognitive deficits previously reported may be a result of samples that included socially withdrawn children who were friendless.

Several limitations should be noted. First, the construct of social withdrawal is markedly heterogeneous. We focused on what researchers have typically referred to as shyness in familiar settings or anxious withdrawal. Researchers would do well to further disentangle anxious withdrawal, shyness, unsociability, and social isolation in future studies of friendship cognitions (Rubin et al, 2009). Second, we studied a sub-sample of anxiously withdrawn preadolescents - those who have a mutual best friend. Although this sampling decision allowed us to “unconfound” anxious withdrawal and friendlessness, it also limits the generalizability of our findings.

In summary, anxiously withdrawn preadolescents who had a mutual best friendship demonstrated specific social-cognitive deficiencies in their understanding of friendship closeness and termination. These deficiencies may help explain why they experience success in initiating a friendship, but have difficulty maintaining high quality relationships (Schneider, 1999). In the future, researchers should examine associations among friendship quality, concepts, and behaviors in greater detail, using coding systems that capture both the rationale and content of responses of withdrawn and nonwithdrawn preadolescents.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Institute of Mental health grant #MH58116 to Kenneth H. Rubin. The authors would like to thank the children, parents, and teachers who participated in the study, as well as all the dedicated research staff who assisted with data collection and input.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Adalbjarnardottir S. How schoolchildren propose to negotiate: The role of social withdrawal, social anxiety, and locus of control. Child Development. 1995;66:1739–1751. doi: 10.2307/1131907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Asher SR, Parker JG, Walker DL. Distinguishing friendship from acceptance: Implications for intervention and assessment. In: Bukowski WM, Newcomb AF, Hartup WW, editors. The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1996. pp. 366–405. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bowker JC, Rubin KH, Rose-Krasnor L, Booth-LaForce C. Good friendships, bad friends: Friendship factors as moderators of the relation between aggressive and social information processing. European Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2007;4:1–20. doi: 10.1080/17405620701632069. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bowker JC, Spencer SV. Friendship and adjustment: A focus on mixed-grade friendships. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2010;39:1318–1329. doi: 10.1007/s10964-009-9474-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bukowski WM, Hoza B, Boivin M. Measuring friendship quality during pre- and early adolescence: The development and psychometric properties of the Friendship Qualities Scale. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 1994;11:471–484. doi: 10.1177/0265407594113011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Burgess KB, Wojslawowicz JC, Rubin KH, Rose-Krasnor L, Booth-LaForce C. Social information processing and coping strategies of shy/withdrawn and aggressive children: Does friendship matter? Child Development. 2006;77:371–383. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00876.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cohen NJ, Kershner J, Wherspann W. Characteristics of social cognition in children with different symptom patterns. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 1985;6:277–290. doi: 10.1016/0193-3973(85)90002-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Fordham K, Stevenson-Hinde J. Shyness, friendship quality, and adjustment during middle childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1999;40:757–768. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Gurucharri C, Phelps E, Selman R. Development of interpersonal understanding: A longitudinal and comparative study of normal and disturbed youths. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1984;52:26–36. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.52.1.26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Hart D, Keller M, Edelstein W, Hoffmann V. Childhood personality influences on social-cognitive development: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:1278–1289. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Hodges EVE, Malone MJ, Perry DG. Individual risk and social risk as interacting determinants of victimization in the peer group. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33:1032–1039. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.1032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Ladd GW. Peer rejection, aggressive, or withdrawn behavior, and psychological maladjustment from ages 5 to 12: An examination of four predictive models. Child Development. 2006;77:822–846. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00905.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Ladd GW, Burgess KB. Charting the relationship trajectories of aggressive, withdrawn, and aggressive/withdrawn children during early grade school. Child Development. 1999;70:910–929. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Masten AS, Morsion P, Pellegrini DS. A revised class play method of peer assessment. Developmental Psychology. 1985;21:523–533. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.21.3.523. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Pellegrini D. Variability in children’s level of reasoning about friendship. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 1986;7:341–354. doi: 10.1016/0193-3973(86)90004-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Rubin KH, Bukowski W, Parker JG. Peer interactions, relationships, and groups. In: Damon W, Lerner R, Eisenberg N, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Vol 3 Social, emotional, and personality development. 6. New York: Wiley; 2006. pp. 571–645. [Google Scholar]
  17. Rubin KH, Coplan RJ, Bowker JC. Social withdrawal and shyness in childhood and adolescence. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:141–171. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Rubin KH, Daniels-Beirness T, Bream L. Social isolation and social problem solving: A longitudinal study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1984;52:17–25. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.52.1.17. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Rubin KH, Krasnor LR. Social-cognitive and social behavioral perspectives on problem solving. In: Perlmutter M, editor. Cognitive perspectives on children’s social and behavioral development: The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology. Vol. 18. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1986. pp. 1–68. [Google Scholar]
  20. Rubin KH, Wojslawowicz JC, Rose-Krasnor L, Booth-LaForce C, Burgess KB. The best friendships of shy/withdrawn children: Prevalence, stability, and relationship quality. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2006;34:143–157. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-9017-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Schneider BH. A multimethod exploration of the friendships of children considered socially withdrawn by their school peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1999;27:115–123. doi: 10.1023/A:1021959430698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Schneider BH. An observational study of the interactions of socially withdrawn/anxious early adolescents and their friends. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009;50:799–806. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02056.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Schneider BH, Tessier NG. Close friendship as understood by socially withdrawn, anxious early adolescents. Child Psychiatry & Human Development. 2007;38:339–351. doi: 10.1007/s10578-007-0071-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Schultz LH, Yeates KO, Selman RL. The interpersonal negotiation strategies interview manual. Harvard University; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  25. Selman R. The growth of interpersonal understanding: Developmental and clinical analyses. NY: Academic Press; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  26. Zarbatany L, McDougall P, Hymel S. Gender-differentiated experience in the peer culture: Links to intimacy in preadolescence. Social Development. 2000;9:62–79. doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00111. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES