Abstract
Background
Research on health-education programs requires longitudinal data. Loss to follow-up can lead to imprecision and bias, and complete loss to follow-up is particularly damaging. If that loss is predictable, then efforts to prevent it can be focused on those program participants who are at the highest risk. We identified predictors of complete loss to follow-up in a longitudinal cohort study.
Methods
Data were collected over 1 year in a study of adults with chronic illnesses who were in a program to learn self-management skills. Following baseline measurements, the program had one group-discussion session each week for six weeks. Follow-up questionnaires were sent 3, 6, and 12 months after the baseline measurement. A person was classified as completely lost to follow-up if none of those three follow-up questionnaires had been returned by two months after the last one was sent.
We tested two hypotheses: that complete loss to follow-up was directly associated with the number of absences from the program sessions, and that it was less common among people who had had face-to-face contact with one of the researchers. We also tested predictors of data loss identified previously and examined associations with specific diagnoses.
Using the unpaired t-test, the U test, Fisher's exact test, and logistic regression, we identified good predictors of complete loss to follow-up.
Results
The prevalence of complete loss to follow-up was 12.2% (50/409). Complete loss to follow-up was directly related to the number of absences (odds ratio; 95% confidence interval: 1.78; 1.49-2.12), and it was inversely related to age (0.97; 0.95-0.99). Complete loss to follow-up was less common among people who had met one of the researchers (0.51; 0.28-0.95) and among those with connective tissue disease (0.29; 0.09-0.98). For the multivariate logistic model the area under the ROC curve was 0.77.
Conclusions
Complete loss to follow-up after this health-education program can be predicted to some extent from data that are easy to collect (age, number of absences, and diagnosis). Also, face-to-face contact with a researcher deserves further study as a way of increasing participation in follow-up, and health-education programs should include it.
Background
Studies of health-education programs require that enough data be collected at the right times [1]. However, in most longitudinal studies some loss to follow-up is considered to be inevitable and it can cause imprecision and bias [2-6].
To increase precision, one option for both observational and experimental designs is to inflate the target sample size to compensate in advance for the expected loss [7-10]. Better still, some data loss can be prevented. Here we are concerned with follow-up data collected via postal questionnaires. Among the options that have been used to promote retention in this context are recorded delivery, monetary incentives, and use of handwritten addresses [11]. If loss to follow-up can be predicted, that is, if individuals who are unlikely to return questionnaires can be identified before those questionnaires are sent, then researchers will know on whom to focus their efforts to promote retention.
Problems caused by missing data can sometimes be mitigated with statistical techniques [12]. However, those techniques are less useful when, in the worst cases, some program participants do not respond to any requests for follow-up information, so it is particularly important to predict complete loss to follow-up. Our goal was to identify predictors of complete loss to follow-up in a longitudinal cohort study of people who participated in a health-education program, as a step toward increasing retention and reducing bias and imprecision in future studies. We tested hypotheses regarding two potential predictors: the number of absences, and face-to-face contact with a researcher.
Methods
Data were collected as part of a longitudinal study in Japan of adults with chronic illnesses who joined a program to learn self-management skills [13]. The program comprised group-discussion sessions with two lay leaders, and there was one session each week for six consecutive weeks. The program was open to women and men equally. Participation in the program and in this research were voluntary. This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Graduate School of Medicine at the University of Tokyo.
Before the first group-discussion session, informed consent was obtained in writing and data were collected with a questionnaire. This baseline questionnaire asked about age, schooling, marital status, and diagnoses. It also had questions asking about health status, health-related behaviors, and psychological factors, including self-efficacy for self-management of chronic disease.
The program was organized and administered by the Japan Chronic Disease Self-Management Association. That association invited researchers from the University of Tokyo (faculty and postgraduate students) to the first session for each discussion group. Thus, the people in some of the groups were introduced to one of the researchers, who explained the study and then distributed the informed-consent form and the baseline questionnaire. After distributing them, the researcher waited, and then collected them. The first baseline questionnaires were completed in August 2006. However, because the sessions were held outside Tokyo, this required at least one postgraduate student to be away for at least two days every time a new discussion group was organized, so the research team decided (in February 2008) to try sending the informed-consent forms and baseline questionnaires by postal mail instead. Those documents were then sent (together with a self-addressed post-paid envelope) and returned by postal mail about two weeks before the first group-discussion session. Researchers were not among the leaders of the discussion groups, and thus the people who received the informed-consent forms and baseline questionnaires by postal mail had no face-to-face contact with a researcher.
Follow-up questionnaires were sent by postal mail 3, 6, and 12 months later. A self-addressed post-paid envelope was included. If a follow-up questionnaire was not returned within two weeks, a reminder postcard was sent. The postcards were preprinted and then signed by hand (by MJP). A person was classified as completely lost to follow-up if none of the three follow-up questionnaires had been returned by two months after the last one was sent. The last follow-up questionnaires were sent in December 2010.
We tested the hypothesis that complete non-participation in follow-up was directly related to another form of non-participation: absence, defined as the number of group-discussion sessions not attended. Also, because some of the baseline questionnaires were distributed in person by a researcher and others were sent by postal mail, we were able to test the hypothesis that complete loss to follow-up was more common among people who had not had face-to-face contact with one of the researchers.
In addition, we quantified associations with predictors of attrition or missing data that have been studied previously, though in other countries and in different clinical contexts: self-efficacy [14,15], multimorbidity [16-18], diagnosis of depression [17], sex [16,19-21], age [16,19-23], schooling [16,23], and marital status [16,19,20], and we examined associations with other diagnoses (allergic disease, asthma, cancer, cardiovascular disease, connective tissue disease, diabetes, fibromyalgia syndrome, pulmonary disease, rheumatic disease, and vascular disease).
To analyze the data we used IBM SPSS version 19. As preliminary bivariate screening tests, for each categorical variable we used Fisher's exact test and for each continuous variable we used the unpaired t-test or the Mann-Whitney U test. Then, using the predictors with P < 0.05 from those tests we also did logistic regression analyses, including multivariate analysis (P values are listed in the supplementary table in Additional file 1).
Predictors can also be evaluated in terms of their sensitivity and specificity, and the area under the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve. Those indices are commonly used to evaluate predictive models [24,25], and they have been used previously to evaluate predictors of attrition in longitudinal studies of health-related interventions and educational programs [26-32]. They also provide a basis for estimating how many of the people who would otherwise be lost to follow-up could instead be identified beforehand if a predictor is used. The maximum values of sensitivity, specificity, and the area under the ROC curve are all 1, and, in general, better predictors have higher values. Introductions to this topic can be found on the Internet [33] and in reference 34 [34], and more details about the use of sensitivity, specificity, and ROC curves in the analysis of predictors can be found in references 24 [24], 25 [25], and 35 [35].
Results
Among 409 people in 78 discussion groups who filled out the baseline questionnaire, 206 (in 31 groups) received and returned it in person and 203 (in 47 groups) received and returned it by postal mail. Results for the study participants as a whole are shown in Table 1. The prevalence of complete loss to follow-up was 12.2% (50/409).
Table 1.
Number (%) | |||
Sex | Male | 84 | (20.5) |
Female | 325 | (79.5) | |
Age | Mean (range) | 47.4 | (18 - 83) |
Schooling | High school or less | 201 | (49.1) |
College or more | 208 | (50.9) | |
Marital status | Living together | 215 | (52.6) |
Others | 194 | (47.4) | |
Diagnoses* | Allergic disease | 105 | (25.7) |
Connective tissue disease | 67 | (16.4) | |
Diabetes | 65 | (15.9) | |
Vascular disease | 65 | (15.9) | |
Rheumatic disease | 47 | (11.5) | |
Fibromyalgia syndrome | 32 | (7.8) | |
Cardiovascular disease | 24 | (5.9) | |
Cancer | 23 | (5.6) | |
Asthma | 21 | (5.1) | |
Depression | 17 | (4.2) | |
Pulmonary disease | 13 | (3.2) | |
Number of diagnoses | 1 | 240 | (58.7) |
2 | 98 | (24.0) | |
3 | 46 | (11.2) | |
≥ 4 | 25 | (6.1) | |
Number of absences | 0 | 198 | (48.4) |
1 | 95 | (23.2) | |
2 | 52 | (12.7) | |
3 | 22 | (5.4) | |
4 | 19 | (4.6) | |
5 | 19 | (4.6) | |
6 | 4 | (1.0) |
* Includes multiple diagnoses.
Compared with the people who returned at least one follow-up questionnaire, those who were completely lost to follow-up had more absences and were younger (Tables 2 and 3). They were also less likely to have met one of the researchers. The percentages of people who were lost to follow-up from the "face-to-face contact: yes" and "face-to-face contact: no" groups were, respectively, 9% and 16% (18/206 and 32/203). Those having a diagnosis of connective tissue disease were less likely to be lost to follow-up than those not having connective tissue disease, and this predictor was very sensitive, though nonspecific.
Table 2.
Predictor variable | Lost to follow-up n = 50 | Not lost to follow-up n = 359 | Odds ratio (95% confidence interval) | Area under ROC curve, or sensitivity and specificity | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Hypothesized predictors | |||||
Number of absencesa | Median (25%, 75%) | 2.0 (0, 5) | 0 (0, 1) | 1.78 (1.49-2.12) | 0.723 |
95% CI | 1 to 4 | 0 to 1 | |||
Contactb | Yes | 18 | 188 | 0.51 (0.28-0.95) | 0.64, 0.52 |
No | 32 | 171 | |||
Other analysesc | |||||
Aged | Mean ± SD | 42.5 ± 14.1 | 48.1 ± 14.0 | 0.97 (0.95-0.99) | 0.623 |
95% CI | 38.6 to 46.4 | 46.6 to 49.5 | |||
Sex | Female | 40 | 285 | 1.04 (0.50-2.17) | 0.20, 0.79 |
Male | 10 | 74 | |||
Schoolinge | High | 23 | 185 | 0.80 (0.44-1.44) | 0.54, 0.52 |
Low | 27 | 173 | |||
Marital statusf | Together | 21 | 194 | 0.61 (0.34-1.11) | 0.58, 0.54 |
Not together | 29 | 164 | |||
Self-efficacyg | Mean ± SD | 32.2 ± 12.5 | 32.2 ± 12.2 | 1.00 (0.98-1.02) | 0.502 |
95% CI | 28.7 to 35.7 | 31.0 to 33.5 | |||
> 3 diagnoses | With | 1 | 24 | 0.29 (0.04-2.15) | 0.98, 0.07 |
Without | 49 | 335 | |||
Allergic disease | With | 15 | 90 | 1.28 (0.67-2.45) | 0.30, 0.75 |
Without | 35 | 269 | |||
Connective tissue disease | With | 3 | 64 | 0.29 (0.09-0.98) | 0.94, 0.18 |
Without | 47 | 295 | |||
Diabetes | With | 6 | 59 | 0.69 (0.28-1.70) | 0.88, 0.17 |
Without | 44 | 300 | |||
Vascular disease | With | 10 | 55 | 1.38 (0.65-2.93) | 0.20, 0.85 |
Without | 40 | 304 | |||
Rheumatic disease | With | 4 | 43 | 0.64 (0.22-1.86) | 0.92, 0.12 |
Without | 46 | 316 | |||
Fibromyalgia syndrome | With | 5 | 27 | 1.37 (0.50-3.73) | 0.10, 0.92 |
Without | 45 | 332 | |||
Cardiovascular disease | With | 0 | 24 | 0h | 1.00, 0.07 |
Without | 50 | 335 | |||
Cancer | With | 2 | 21 | 0.67 (0.15-2.95) | 0.96, 0.06 |
Without | 48 | 338 | |||
Asthma | With | 3 | 18 | 1.21 (0.34-4.26) | 0.06, 0.95 |
Without | 47 | 341 | |||
Depression | With | 4 | 13 | 2.31 (0.72-7.40) | 0.08, 0.96 |
Without | 46 | 346 | |||
Pulmonary disease | With | 3 | 10 | 2.23 (0.59-8.39) | 0.06, 0.97 |
Without | 47 | 349 |
For the number of absences, the table shows medians, 25th & 75th percentiles, and 95% confidence intervals.
For the other two continuous variables, the table shows means ± SDs, 95% confidence intervals, and areas under ROC curves.
For each categorical variable, the table shows numbers of people in each category, odds ratio and its 95% confidence interval, sensitivity, and specificity.
a Number of absences from program sessions; minimum = 0, maximum = 6. Odds ratio from simple logistic regression.
b No: did not have in-person contact with one of the researchers; yes: did have contact, once, at the time baseline data were collected.
c Analyses of predictors studied previously or suggested during peer review.
d Age in years. Odds ratio from simple logistic regression.
e Low: high school or less; high: college or more.
f Together: living together; not: all others
g Score on a 0-to-60 scale measuring self-efficacy to manage symptoms; odds ratio from simple logistic regression.
h Because no people with cardiovascular disease were completely lost to follow-up, there was complete separation in the "complete loss to follow-up" category. For a conservatively-biased estimate, changing the 0 to 1 and the 50 to 49 would yield an odds ratio of 0.29 (0.04-2.15).
Table 3.
Independent variablesa | Coefficient (β) | Standard error | Wald χ2 | P value | Odds ratiob | ROC curve area |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Four models, each with one independent variable | ||||||
Intercept | -2.91 | 0.25 | - | - | - | |
Number of absences | 0.58 | 0.09 | 41.54 | < 0.001 | 1.78 (1.49-2.12) | 0.723 |
Intercept | -1.68 | 0.19 | - | - | - | |
Contact | -0.67 | 0.31 | 4.58 | 0.032 | 0.51 (0.28-0.95) | 0.582 |
Intercept | -0.65 | 0.52 | - | - | - | |
Age | -0.03 | 0.01 | 6.59 | 0.010 | 0.97 (0.95-0.99) | 0.623 |
Intercept | -3.06 | 0.59 | - | - | - | |
Connective tissue disease | -1.22 | 0.61 | 4.01 | 0.045 | 0.29 (0.09-0.98) | 0.559 |
One model with three independent variables | 0.752c | |||||
Intercept | -1.60 | 0.61 | - | - | - | |
Number of absences | 0.55 | 0.09 | 35.58 | < 0.001 | 1.73 (1.44-2.07) | |
Contact | -0.59 | 0.34 | 3.02 | 0.083 | 0.56 (0.29-1.08) | |
Age | -0.02 | 0.01 | 3.49 | 0.062 | 0.98 (0.96-1.00) | |
One model with four independent variables | 0.771c | |||||
Intercept | -1.31 | 0.63 | - | - | - | |
Number of absences | 0.54 | 0.09 | 33.42 | < 0.001 | 1.72 (1.43-2.06) | |
Contact | -0.73 | 0.34 | 4.53 | 0.033 | 0.48 (0.25-0.94) | |
Age | -0.02 | 0.01 | 3.81 | 0.051 | 0.98 (0.95-1.00) | |
Connective tissue disease | -1.40 | 0.64 | 4.73 | 0.030 | 0.25 (0.07-0.87) |
a All variables are defined as in Table 2.
b Values in parentheses show 95% confidence intervals. For the models with more than one independent variable, adjusted odds ratios are shown.
c This ROC area applies to the full multivariate model.
Loss to follow-up was not associated with sex, schooling, marital status, self-efficacy, having more than three diagnoses, having a diagnosis of depression, or having any of the other nine diagnoses.
When the cutoff for predicting complete loss to follow-up was set at two or more absences, the sensitivity and specificity were, respectively, 0.62 and 0.76. Thus, 62% of the 50 people who ultimately were completely lost to follow-up could have been identified no later than the time when the last group-discussion session ended, which was six weeks before the first follow-up questionnaire was sent. Predictions based on not having met one of the researchers were less specific though they were slightly more sensitive (sensitivity and specificity: 0.64 and 0.52).
One multivariate model (Table 3) had three independent variables: the number of absences, face-to-face contact with a researcher, and age. The other model also included connective tissue disease (Table 3 and Figure 1). As predictors, they were better than the model with the number of absences alone (Table 3). For the model with four independent variables, with the cutoff set at a probability of 0.83, the sensitivity was 0.82 and the specificity was 0.62.
Discussion
The number of absences was a good predictor of complete loss to follow-up. This supports the hypothesis that one form of non-participation could be used to predict another. Aubrey et al. reported a similar finding regarding participation in psychological therapy: early non-attendance could be used to predict attrition [36]. The other good predictors were age, face-to-face contact with a researcher, and the diagnosis of connective tissue disease. For those latter three, the associations were negative, that is, loss to follow-up was more common among people who were younger, those who had no face-to-face contact with a researcher, and those who did not have connective tissue disease.
The results regarding the number of absences lead to specific recommendations for future studies. Administrators of this health-education program and of others similar to it, and researchers studying those programs, should keep records of group-discussion attendance, and they should use those records to identify people who are at high risk of loss to follow-up. In the present case, if "two or more absences" had been used as a predictor, then 31 people who ultimately were completely lost to follow-up could have been identified beforehand (0.62 × 50 = 31). Age and diagnoses should also be recorded, so a multivariate model can be used to increase that number further. Whether the predictor is a multivariate model or the number of absences alone, all of the data needed are available six weeks before the first follow-up questionnaire is sent. Once people who are at high risk of loss to follow-up are identified, at least some of the methods known to be effective [11] should be used to try to retain them in the study.
The results regarding face-to-face contact with a researcher are also noteworthy, because that is the only predictor in Table 2 that is modifiable. Meta-analysis has shown that pre-notification by telephone is more effective than pre-notification by postal mail for increasing the initial response rates to postal questionnaires (analysis 62.1 in reference 11 [11]), but not the final response rates (analysis 62.2 in reference 11 [11], although the results are very heterogeneous: I2 = 85%). We found no previous studies of the effects of face-to-face contact with a researcher on subsequent rates of postal questionnaire return. In the present study, people who had such contact immediately before they filled out the baseline questionnaire were less likely to be lost to follow-up over the following year. The implication for research is that such contact should be used as an independent variable in randomized controlled trials with participation in follow-up as the outcome. The implication for practice is that researchers should try to meet and talk with the people to whom they will later send follow-up postal questionnaires.
Such face-to-face contact will probably not be free of charge. The cost and the availability of funds to cover it will of course depend on local circumstances, though a reasonable generalization might be that face-to-face contact will be less costly in studies with fewer participants. In those situations its benefit would also be greater, because small studies can least afford the loss of precision caused by even small absolute numbers of missing data and the resulting bias if those data are not missing at random (for example, preventing 10 losses to follow-up in a study of 50 people is more beneficial than preventing 10 losses to follow-up in a study of 500 people).
Another point to consider is the fact that many more women than men were in this study. Although the present results might not apply to a program with a much smaller percentage of women, such programs seem to be rare, while programs with many more women than men are typical. In 17 studies of programs such as this one (i.e., focusing on self-management of chronic illness [10,37-52]), the percentage of women participants ranged from 61.1% [42] to 88.9% [44] and the mean was 75.7%. In the present study it was 79.5%.
Some limitations of this study should be kept in mind. We cannot be sure why people were absent or why questionnaires were not returned. Death is one possible explanation, but it is not likely, given the facts that the follow-up period was only 1 year, that those lost to follow-up were relatively young, and that they were no more likely than the others to have high multimorbidity. Change of address is also not a likely explanation. In Japan the post office forwards mail for 1 year, after which it is returned to the sender undelivered, but no questionnaires or reminder postcards were returned undelivered. Using the number of absences to predict loss to follow-up will be most beneficial if efforts are also made to find out each person's reasons for absence and for not returning follow-up questionnaires, so the type of missing data can be identified for each outcome of interest [2,12]. We also note that questions remain about the generalizability of the results across countries, programs, and types of surveys.
Conclusions
Even with the limitations mentioned above, the potential for bias and for loss of precision make it important to predict and prevent non-participation in follow-up. The present findings lead us to specific recommendations: First, face-to-face contact with a researcher deserves further study as a way of increasing participation in follow-up, and studies of these programs should include it. Second, particularly for research on these kinds of programs, one or more of the other predictors in a multivariate model (all of which are known before the first follow-up questionnaire is sent) should be used to identify people who are not likely to return follow-up questionnaires. Once identified, those people should receive special attention [11] to promote questionnaire return.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors' contributions
MJP contributed to conceiving and designing the study, collecting, entering, and analyzing the data, interpreting the results, and writing and revising the manuscript. YYa obtained funding for the study, and contributed to designing the study and revising the manuscript. YYo contributed to designing the study, collecting the data, and revising the manuscript. KY contributed to designing the study, collecting the data, and revising the manuscript. HI contributed to analyzing the data, interpreting the results, and revising the manuscript. TK contributed to analyzing the data, interpreting the results, and revising the manuscript. JG contributed to conceiving and designing the study, interpreting the results, and writing and revising the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Pre-publication history
The pre-publication history for this paper can be accessed here:
Supplementary Material
Contributor Information
MJ Park, Email: mjpark-tky@umin.ac.jp.
Yoshihiko Yamazaki, Email: yyamazak-tky@umin.ac.jp.
Yuki Yonekura, Email: yonekura0306@gmail.com.
Keiko Yukawa, Email: tkykeiko-tky@nifty.com.
Hirono Ishikawa, Email: hirono-tky@umin.ac.jp.
Takahiro Kiuchi, Email: tak-kiuchi@umin.ac.jp.
Joseph Green, Email: jgreen@m.u-tokyo.ac.jp.
Acknowledgements and Funding
The authors are very grateful to Ms. Noriko Okamoto for her assistance with data entry and administrative support, and to all the study's participants. The authors are also very grateful to the three reviewers for their careful reading of the manuscript and for their helpful suggestions. Funding for this study was provided by the Japanese Ministry of Health, Labor and Welfare aids for Scientific Research (health disease prevention, treatment and research projects for Allergy and Immunology).
References
- Green L. Evaluation and Measurement: Some Dilemmas for Health Education. American Journal of Public Health. 1977;67(2):155–161. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.67.2.155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kristman V, Manno M, Cote P. Loss to follow-up in cohort studies: how much is too much? European Journal of Epidemiology. 2004;19:751–760. doi: 10.1023/b:ejep.0000036568.02655.f8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wasiak R, Pransky GS, Webster BS. Methodological challenges in studying recurrence of low back pain. Journal of Occupational Rehabilitation. 2003;13(1):21–31. doi: 10.1023/A:1021893706683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corrigan JD, Harrison-Felix C, Bogner J, Dijkers M, Terrill MS, Whiteneck G. Systematic bias in traumatic brain injury outcome studies because of loss to follow-up. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2003;84(2):153–160. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2003.50093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedden SL, Woolson RF, Carter RE, Palesch Y, Upadhyaya HP, Malcolm RJ. The impact of loss to follow-up on hypothesis tests of the treatment effect for several statistical methods in substance abuse clinical trials. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2009;37(1):54–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2008.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zunzunegui MV, Béland F, Gutiérrez-Cuadra P. Loss to follow-up in a longitudinal study on aging in Spain. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2001;54(5):501–510. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(00)00325-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelsey JL, Whittemore AS, Evans AS, Thompson WD. Methods in Observational Epidemiology. Oxford University Press. 1996. p. 334.
- Hackshaw A. A Concise Guide to Clinical Trials. BMJ Books. 2009. p. 118.
- Little P, Somerville J, Williamson I, Warner G, Moore M, Wiles R, George S, Smith A, Peveler R. Randomised controlled trial of self management leaflets and booklets for minor illness provided by post. British Medical Journal. 2001;19:1214–1217. doi: 10.1136/bmj.322.7296.1214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fu D, Fu H, McGowan P, Yi-e S, Lizhen Z, Huiqin Y, Jianguo M, Shitai Z, Yongming D, Zhihua W. Implementation and quantitative evaluation of chronic disease self-management programme in Shanghai, China: randomized controlled trial. Bulletin of the World Health organization. 2003;81:174–182. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edwards PJ, Roberts I, Clarke MJ, DiGuiseppi C, Wentz R, Kwan I, Cooper R, Felix LM, Pratap S. Methods to increase response to postal and electronic questionnaires (Review) Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2009;3:1–12. doi: 10.1002/14651858.MR000008.pub4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Little RA, Rubin DB. Statistical Analysis with Missing Data. Second. Wiley-Interscience; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Lorig K, Holman H, Sobel D, Laurent D, Gonzalez V, Minor M. Living a healthy life with chronic conditions. Palo Alto, CA: Bull Publishing Company (textbook); 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Dakin CL. Predictive value of emergency department patients' self-efficacy beliefs on follow-up care of lacerations. Australasian Emergency Nursing Journal. 2006;8:157–163. doi: 10.1016/j.aenj.2005.11.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kakudate N, Morita M, Yamazaki S, Fukuhara S, Sugai M, Nagayama M, Kawanami M, Chiba I. Association between self-efficacy and loss to follow-up in long-term periodontal treatment. Journal of Clinical Periodontology. 2010;37:276–282. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-051X.2009.01529.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia M, Fernandez E, Schiaffino A, Borrell C, Marti M, Borras JM. Attrition in a population-based cohort eight years after baseline interview: The Cornella health interview survey follow-up (CHIS.FU) study. Annals of Epidemiology. 2005;15:98–104. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2004.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inelmen EM, Toffanello ED, Enzi G, Gasparini G, Miotto F, Busetto L. Predictors of drop-out in overweight and obese outpatients. International Journal of Obesity. 2005;29:122–128. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jerant A, Chapman BP, Duberstein P, Franks P. Is Personality a Key Predictor of Missing Study Data? An Analysis From a Randomized Controlled Trial. Annals of Family Medicine. 2009;7:148–156. doi: 10.1370/afm.920. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honas JJ, Early JL, Frederickson DD, O'Brien MS. Predictors of attrition in a large clinic-based weight-loss program. Obesity Research. 2003;11:888–894. doi: 10.1038/oby.2003.122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snow WM, Connett JE, Sharma S, Murray RP. Predictors of attendance and dropout at the Lung Health Study 11-year follow-up. Contemporary Clinical Trials. 2007;28:25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.cct.2006.08.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Beijsterveldt CEM, van Boxtel MPJ, Bosma H, Houx PJ, Buntinx F, Jolles J. Predictors of attrition in a longitudinal cognitive aging study: the Maastricht Aging Study (MAAS) Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2002;55:216–223. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(01)00473-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edlund MJ, Wang PS, Berglund PA, Katz SJ, Lin E, Kessler RC. Dropping Out of Mental Health Treatment: Patterns and Predictors Among Epidemiological survey Respondents in the United States and Ontario. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2002;159:845–851. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.5.845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warden D, Trivedi MH, Wisniewski SR, Davis L, Nierenberg A, Gaynes BN, Zisook S, Hollon SD, Balasubramani GK. Predictors of Attrition During Initial (Citalopram) Treatment for Depression: A STAR*D Report. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164:1189–1197. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06071225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraemer Helena C. Evaluating Medical Tests - Objective and Quantitative Guidelines. SAGE Publications; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Zhou X-H, Obuchowski NA, McClish DK. Statistical Methods in Diagnostic Medicine. Second. Wiley-Interscience; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Beyko MJ, Wong S. Predictors of Treatment Attrition as Indicators for Program Improvement not Offender Shortcomings: A Study of Sex Offender Treatment Attrition. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment. 2005;17:375–389. doi: 10.1177/107906320501700403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Villafranca SW, McKellar JD, Humphreys K. Predictors of retention in methadone programs: A signal detection analysis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2006;83:218–224. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.11.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warden D, Rush AJ, Carmody TJ, Kashner M, Biggs M, Crismon ML, Trivedi MH. Predictors of Attrition During One Year of Depression Treatment: A Roadmap to Personalized Intervention. Journal of Psychiatric Practice. 2009;15:113–124. doi: 10.1097/01.pra.0000348364.88676.83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kampman KM, Alterman AI, Volpicelli JR, Maany I, Muller ES, Luce D, Mulholland EM. Cocaine Withdrawal Symptoms and Initial Urine Toxicology Results Predict Treatment Attrition in Outpatient Cocaine Dependence Treatment. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2001;15:52–59. doi: 10.1037/0893-164x.15.1.52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulvaney KM, Alterman AI, Boardman CR, Kampman K. Cocaine Abstinence Symptomatology and Treatment Attrition. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 1999;16:129–135. doi: 10.1016/S0740-5472(98)00017-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deary IJ, Watson R, Hogston R. A longitudinal cohort study of burnout and attrition in nursing students. Journal of Advanced Nursing. 2003;43:71–81. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2648.2003.02674.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'connor SM, Davies JB, Heffernan DD, Eijk RV. An Alternative Method for Predicting Attrition from an Alcohol Treatment Programme. Alcohol & Alcoholism. 2003;38:568–573. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agg112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Receiver Operating Characteristic Curves: an Introduction. http://www.anaesthetist.com/mnm/stats/roc/Findex.htm
- Fawcett Tom. An introduction to ROC analysis. Pattern Recognition Letters. 2006;27:861–874. doi: 10.1016/j.patrec.2005.10.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pepe Margaret S. The Statistical Evaluation of Medical Tests for classification and Prediction. Oxford University Press. 2003.
- Aubrey R, Self R, Halstead JE. Early non-attendance as a predictor of continued non-attendance and subsequent attrition from psychological help. Clinical Psychology. 2003;32:6–10. [Google Scholar]
- Elzen H, Slaets JP, Snijders T, Steverink N. Evaluation of the chronic disease self-management program (CDSMP) among chronically ill older people in the Netherlands. Social Science & Medicine. 2007;64:1832–1841. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jordan JE, Haynes K, Livingston JA, Osborne RH. Comparison of the pre-post and transition question assessments in a health education setting. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2009;62:642–649. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2008.07.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barlow JH, Turner AP, Wright CC. Long-term outcomes of an arthritis self-management programme. British Journal of Rheumatology. 1998;37:1315–1319. doi: 10.1093/rheumatology/37.12.1315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hass M, Groupp E, Muench J, Kraemer D, Mrummel-Smith K, Shama R, Ganger B, Attwood M, Fairweather A. Chronic Disease Self-Management Program for Low Back Pain in the Elderly. Journal of Manipulative and Physiological Therapeutics. 2005;28:228–237. doi: 10.1016/j.jmpt.2005.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorig KR, Ritter P, Laurent DD, Plant K. Internet-Based Chronic Disease Self-Management: A Randomized Trial. Medical Care. 2006;44:964–971. doi: 10.1097/01.mlr.0000233678.80203.c1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith L, Bosnic-Anticevich SZ, Mitchell B, Saini B, Krass I, Armour C. Treating asthma with a self-management model of illness behavior in an Australian community pharmacy setting. Social Science & Medicine. 2007;64:1501–1511. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorig KR, Sobel DS, Stewart AL, Brown BW, Bandura A, Ritter P, Gonzalez VM, Laurent DD, Holman HR. Evidence Suggesting That a Chronic Disease Self-Management Program Can Improve Health Status While Reducing Hospitalization: A Randomized Trial. Medical Care. 1999;37:5–14. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199901000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yip YB, Sit JW, Wong D, Chong S, Chung LH. A 1-year follow-up of an experimental study of a self-management arthritis programme with an added exercise component of clients with osteoarthritis of the knee. Psychology, Health & Medicine. 2008;13:402–414. doi: 10.1080/13548500701584030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damush TM, Weinberger M, Perkins SM, Rao JK, Tierney WM, Qi R, Clark DO. The Long-term Effects of a Self-management Program for Inner-city Primary Care Patients With Acute Low Back Pain. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2003;163:2632–2638. doi: 10.1001/archinte.163.21.2632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorig KR, Sobel DS, Ritter PL, Hobbs M. Effect of a Self-Management Program on Patients with Chronic Disease. Effective Clinical Practice. 2001;4:256–262. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swerissen H, Belfrage J, Weeks A, Jordan L, Walker C, Furler J, McAvoy B, Carter M, Peterson C. A randomized control trial of a self-management program for people with a chronic illness from Vietnamese, Chinese, Italian and Greek backgrounds. Patient Education and Counseling. 2006;64:360–368. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2006.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan SC, Siu AMH, Poon PKK, Chan CCH. Chronic disease self-management program for Chinese patients: a preliminary multi-baseline study. International Journal of Rehabilitation Research. 2005;28:351–354. doi: 10.1097/00004356-200512000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorig KR, Ritter PL, Jacquez A. Outcomes of Border Health Spanish/English Chronic Disease Self-Management Programs. The Diabetes Educator. 2005;31:401–409. doi: 10.1177/0145721705276574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gitlin LN, Chernett NL, Harris LF, Palmer D, Hopkins P, Dennis MP. Harvest Health: Translation of the Chronic Disease Self-Management Program for Older African Americans in a Senior Setting. The Gerontologist. 2008;48:698–705. doi: 10.1093/geront/48.5.698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goeppinger J, Armstrong B, Schwartz T, Ensley D, Brady TJ. Self-Management Education for Persons With Arthritis: Managing Comorbidity and Eliminating Health Disparities. Arthritis & Rheumatism (Arthritis Care & Research) 2007;57:1081–1088. doi: 10.1002/art.22896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jerant A, Kravitz R, Moore-Hill M, Franks P. Depressive Symptoms Moderated the Effect of Chronic Illness Self-Management Training on Self-Efficacy. Medical Care. 2008;46:523–531. doi: 10.1097/MLR.0b013e31815f53a4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.