Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Epilepsy Res. 2011 Sep 14;97(1-2):214–219. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2011.07.015

Cognitive and EEG Fluctuation in Benign Childhood Epilepsy with Central-Temporal Spikes: A Case Series

Joshua Ewen 1
PMCID: PMC3215903  NIHMSID: NIHMS321926  PMID: 21920705

Abstract

Aware of parental reports of academic variability, we investigated month-to-month fluctuations in cognitive abilities and EEG status by repeated measures testing in six children with Benign Epilepsy with Central-Temporal Spikes (BECTS). All showed greater than normal test-retest variability. Daytime EEG abnormalities were also variable. Short-term fluctuations in cognitive function appear common in children with BECTS, potentially impacting academic performance.

1. INTRODUCTION

Benign epilepsy with central-temporal spikes (BECTS), or benign rolandic epilepsy, is the most common idiopathic partial epilepsy of childhood (Heijbel, Blom, & Bergfors, 1975). BECTS is characterized by central-temporal sharp waves and sleep-onset partial somatosensory/motor seizures, beginning 3–10 years of age and resolving by age 16 years. Although most children with BECTS have normal intelligence, previous studies have reported impairments in various cognitive domains (Baglietto et al., 2001; Croona, Kihlgren, Lundberg, Eeg-Olofsson, & Eeg-Olofsson, 1999; D’Alessandro et al., 1990; Northcott et al., 2006). However, no consistent cognitive phenotype has emerged.

Parents report day-to-day inconsistencies in children’s cognitive and academic performance, but short-term fluctuations in cognitive function have not been studied. It is possible that within-patient variability in cognitive function contributes to the heterogeneity of impairments found in previous research. Moreover, failure to recognize cognitive variability in children with BECTS could lead to underestimating their risk for academic difficulties.

Prior attempts to relate EEG findings in BECTS to the presence of neuropsychological impairment have yielded inconsistent results (Nicolai et al., 2007). However, the stability of EEG findings in BECTS is not known. The purpose of this case series was to investigate short-term (month-to-month) variability of cognitive function and EEG findings in six children with BECTS.

2. METHODS

2.1 Participants and Screening

The six BECTS participants were ages 7–11 years (Table 1). All were in the active stage of BECTS (Commission on Classification and Terminology of the International League Against Epilepsy, 1989). Mean age of diagnosis was 8.4 years (range 6–9 years). No medications were adjusted during the study. Based on parental report, all participants had one or more night-time seizures within eight months prior to enrollment but were seizure-free for at least one month before testing. Average Full Scale IQ was 112 (SD±14), as measured by the Wechsler Abbreviated Intelligence Scale for Children. No participant met criteria for ADHD as determined by the Behavior Assessment System for Children (BASC) (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1992) questionnaire and the parental interview (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Subjects also participated in a concurrent study of auditory function (Boatman et al., 2008). Written parental consent was obtained for all participants in compliance with the Johns Hopkins Medicine Institutional Review Board.

Table 1.

Subject Demographics and Testing

DEMOGRAPHICS
TESTING
Subject Sex Age at Testing (yr.mo) Age at Diagnosis (yr.mo) FSIQ AED Session 1 Session 2 Session 3
Cognitive EEG Cognitive EEG Cognitive EEG
1 M 10.0 7.10 97 CBZ Y N Y Y Y Y
2 F 11.9 9.8 109 CBZ Y N Y Y Y Y
3 M 11.1 9.2 105 VPA Y Y Y Y N N
4 F 7.1 6.7 116 NONE Y Y Y Y Y Y
5 F 11.9 8.0 107 CBZ Y Y Y Y Y Y
6 M 9.9 9.3 137 CBZ Y Y Y Y Y Y
Mean ( ± SD) 10.3 (±1.8) 8.4 (±1.3) 111.8 (±13.8)

Abbreviations: AED (anti-epilepsy drug); CBZ (carbamazepine); VPA (valproate); FSIQ (full-scale intelligence quotient); M (male); F (female).

2.3 Cognitive Testing

All participants underwent repeat neuropsychological testing at 4–5 week intervals. Five participants completed three testing sessions; one participant completed two sessions. A battery of 15 neuropsychological tests was administered (Table 2) to assess multiple cognitive functions, including selective and sustained attention (visual, auditory), executive function, auditory-verbal learning, visual-spatial skills, auditory and verbal memory, receptive and expressive language, and motor function. At the first visit, parents completed the BASC and were interviewed by a neuropsychologist (CAS) regarding behavioral and academic performance.

Table 2.

Neuropsychological Battery

Test Name Abbreviation Full Name Domain Citation

TOVA Test of Variables of Attention (ADHD score) Attention Leark, Greenberg, Kindschi, Dupuy, & Hughes, 2007
TEA-Ch SCORE! Test of Everyday Attention for Children, SCORE! Subtest Auditory Attention Manly, Robertson, Anderson, & Niparko, 1999
TEA-Ch CC-TS Test of Everyday Attention for Children, Creature Count Subtest, Total Score Executive Functioning Set Shifting Manly, Robertson, Anderson, & Niparko, 1999
JLO Judgment of Line Orientation Visual-Spatial Perception Benton, Varney, & Hamsher, 1978
RCFC Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure copy Visual-Spatial Organization & Planning Osterrieth, 1944; Rey, 1942
RCFR Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Recall Visual-Spatial Memory Osterrieth, 1944; Rey, 1942
RAVLT-A7 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (List 7 Score) Verbal Memory (recall) Schmidt, 2004
RAVLT-RECOG Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test, Recognition score Verbal Memory (recognition) Schmidt, 2004
WJ-III UD Woodcock-Johnson-III Understanding Directions Language: Comprehension Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001
MAE-SR Multilingual Aphasia Examination—Sentence Repetition Language: Repetition; Working Memory Benton, Hamsher, & Sivan, 1994
Pegboard Grooved Pegboard Motor Speed and Dexterity Lafayette Instruments
FT Finger Tapping Motor Speed Lafayette Instruments

The same neuropsychological tests were administered in fixed order at each session by a clinical neuropsychologist (CAS). Different test versions, when available (WJ-III, Rey Complex Figure, RAVLT, MAE, TEA-Ch), were administered at consecutive sessions to reduce familiarity and practice effects.

2.4 EEG Recordings

All participants underwent awake, 20–30 minute EEGs at each visit, prior to neuropsychological testing. EEGs were recorded using standard clinical technique. At each session, parents confirmed no seizures occurred the previous evening.

2.5 Data Analysis

Neuropsychological raw test scores were converted to age-normed z-scores based on published norms. The likelihood of test-retest changes were computed using published test-retest coefficients. Specifically, a = (var(y1) + var(y2)) · (1 − corr(y1y2)), where a = the z-score related to the difference between the first (y1) and second (y2) measurement. Variances var(y1) and var(y2) each equal 1 (each measurement is expressed as an age-normed z-score). corr(y1y2) is the published test-retest coefficient. Test-retest differences of >2SD (i.e., <5% likelihood of occurring) were considered to be significant.

EEGs were reviewed independently by three pediatric epileptologists (EPV, JBE, WHT) using standard clinical viewing parameters in a bipolar longitudinal montage. Records were examined for central-temporal abnormalities: spike rate, asynchronous spike foci and presence of a slow-wave focus (reviewed in Nicolai et al., 2007). Spike rate was averaged across the entire recording and coded using a five-category index (no spikes; <1 spike/min; 1–5 spikes/min; 6–10 spikes/min; >10 spikes/min). Inter-rater agreement in coding EEG spike rate yielded a kappa statistic of 0.856, indicating very good agreement.

3. RESULTS

3.1 Neuropsychological Findings

All participants demonstrated scores more than 1 standard deviation (SD) below the mean on at least one test. Within each session, up to 33% of patients (range 17–33%) showed moderate-to-severe impairments on at least one test (≥2 SD below the normative mean), and up to 67% of patients (17–67%) were mildly impaired on at least one test (≥1 SD below the mean).

Score changes of a magnitude consistent with ≤5% likelihood occurred for every subject, for each session, and, independently, for every test (Table 3). The TOVA (ADHD index score) test of attention was the most variable, with unexpectedly large within-subjects changes occurring in all session-to-session comparisons, however the direction of change was not always the same as for other cognitive tests. Creature Count, RAVLT recognition memory and Rey Complex Figure copy had the least variability, with only one unexpectedly large session-to-session change occurring out of the 11 pairs of testing sessions.

Table 3.

Session-to-Session Changes in Neuropsychological Z-Scores for Each Subject, for Each Test

Subj Sessions TOVA SCORE! CC-TS JLO RCFC RCFR RAVLT-ACQ RAVLT-A7 RAVLT-RECOG WJ-III UD MAE- SR Pegboard- Left Pegboard- Right FT-Left FT-Right

1 1 → 2 0.84 −3.33 0.67 0.38 2.67 5.09 −1.01 −1.04 0 0.53 −0.5 0.27 0.8 0.59 0.65
2 → 3 0.96 2 0.33 0.57 −1.31 −1.75 0.7 0.69 0.29 −1.07 −0.65 0.47 0.53 1.07 1.79
2 1 → 2 1.92 −1 −0.67 0.19 0.8 1.36 1.6 0.5 1.25 0.4 1.05 0.2 0.53 −0.36 −0.66
2 → 3 −3.66 1 2 −0.54 1.66 −0.23 −0.54 −0.5 −0.84 −0.53 −0.26 0.33 0.4 0.92 −0.21
3 1 → 2 −1.67 −3.67 −0.67 1.16 0.13 −0.16 −0.93 −0.5 −1.25 0.47 0.5 0.2 −0.4 1.32 1.35
4 1 → 2 −1.59 0 0.67 −0.02 0.95 0.6 −0.94 −1.5 0 0.27 0 −0.2 0.4 0.55 0.46
2 → 3 2.52 0 0 0.66 0.25 0.62 −0.76 −1.53 0.29 0.13 −2.86 1.6 1.07 1.7 1.55
5 1 → 2 −4.84 −3 −0.33 1.58 0.75 1.35 −0.2 −0.71 1.25 −0.13 −2.42 0 1.87 −0.49 −1.06
2 → 3 4.57 0.33 2 −0.4 −0.34 −1.28 1.8 −1.42 0.37 1.26 1.01 −0.4 −0.2 0.26 −0.04
6 1 → 2 * −1.33 1.33 −0.98 0 1.36 0 −0.87 0 −0.07 −0.06 1.13 1.53 2.26 3.65
2 → 3 * 2.33 1 0.98 0.29 0.68 0.69 0 −1.67 0.07 −1 0.2 0.8 −0.61 −2.54
Test-Retest Coeff. 0.80 0.76 0.57 0.90 0.47 0.79 0.70 0.62 0.60 0.76 0.91 0.86 0.86 0.77 0.78
Sig. Change ±0.80 ±0.96 ±1.72 ±0.40 ±2.12 ±0.84 ±1.20 ±1.52 ±1.60 ±0.96 ±0.36 ±0.56 ±0.56 ±0.92 ±0.88

Notes: Test-Retest Coeff. = published test-retest coefficient; Sig. Change = Z-score change which has ≤ 5% probability of occurring, based on published test-retest coefficient. NT = Not tested for one of the sessions. NA = Not applicable. L = Left; R = Right; B = Bilateral.

Neuropsychological session-to-session change values marked in bold are those in which variability was considered significant.

*

TOVA could not be scored for Subject 6 in any of the three sessions due to high Intra-session trial-by-trial response variability.

3.2 EEG Findings

A total of 15 serial EEGs were obtained. All participants had at least two EEGs (Table 1). No clinical or electrographic seizures were observed. Twelve EEGs (80%) were classified as abnormal, in all cases due to features typical of BECTS. All participants had at least one abnormal EEG during the study, and two (33%) maintained abnormal EEGs across all sessions.

Of the nine repeat EEGs, four (44%) showed a change in status from the previous recording (normal-to-abnormal, abnormal-to-normal) (Table 4). Six repeat EEGs (67%) showed changes in spike rate category. Side of EEG spiking changed in two subjects (Subjects 4, 6), focal slowing was inconsistent on three subjects’ repeat EEGs (Subjects 1, 2, 4), and presence of an asynchronous focus was inconsistent in one repeat EEG (Subject 5). In summary, 100% of repeat EEGs had at least one change from the immediately preceding EEG; moreover, all participants had EEGs that changed by one or more central-temporal features associated previously with cognitive impairment in BECTS (Nicolai et al., 2007).

Table 4.

Session-to-Session Changes in EEG Features for Each Subject, for Each Test

Subject Sessions Spike Rate Spike Side Asynchronous Focus Slowing

1 2 → 3 11.50 L → NA N → N YN
2 2 → 3 0 → 0 NA N → N NY
3 1 → 2 04.2 NA → R N → N N → N
4 1 → 2 2.30 LNA N → N YN
2 → 3 04.8 NAR N → N NY
5 1 → 2 0.83.8 B → B Y → Y N → N
2 → 3 3.80.8 B → B YN N → N
6 1 → 2 0.7 → 0.2 BR N → N Y → Y
2 → 3 0.2 → 0.8 RL N → N Y → Y

Note: EEG session-to-session change values marked in bold are those in which the change was significant: i.e., change from one category to another.

3.3 Neuropsychological correlation with EEG

We cannot comment on whether fluctuations in EEG correlate with the variability in neuropsychological testing, due to limitations in statistical power. However, visual inspection of the data showed no apparent trends.

4. DISCUSSION

Our BECTS participants showed considerable short-term (month-to-month) variability in their neuropsychological and EEG findings. Specifically, all month-to-month comparisons showed greater-than-expected changes in at least one neuropsychological test, based on published test-retest coefficients. All subjects had at least one month-to-month comparison with at least five test scores in the comparison demonstrating greater-than-expected changes. Additionally, all subjects showed impairments (scores >1SD below the normative mean) in at least one neuropsychological domain. The test of attention showed the most variability, which is perhaps not surprising, given that attention is generally recognized to fluctuate with stress, sleep disturbances, affective state and alertness. Similarly, all repeat EEGs showed significant change from the previous session when the EEGs were examined for features previously implicated as being significant for characterizing inter-individual differences in BECTS.

One implication of the EEG and neuropsychological findings is that future studies on BECTS should take into account the degree of intra-individual variability, and future researchers may need to be cautious about basing the characterization of a subject on a single EEG or neuropsychological measurement. Additionally, the findings of neuropsychological variability may in part explain the elusiveness of a cognitive phenotype in BECTS. Finally, the cognitive findings may have practical clinical implications: first, children with BECTS who perform well on a single neuropsychological evaluation may have deficits if re-evaluated even a short time later; second, short-term cognitive variability may be a feature of BECTS. This interpretation would be consistent with anecdotal reports from parents, who note day-to-day fluctuations in children with BECTS. Clinicians, educators and family member should be aware of the possibility that children with BECTS may demonstrate cognitive variability. Neuropsychological assessment should take into consideration any parental or teacher report of cognitive and academic variability, and repeated testing may be helpful to document the difference between “good” and “bad” days.

There are a number of limitations to this study. The first is the small sample size. The second is the lack of a control group. The published test-retest coefficients may not have been derived using an experimental design consistent with the approach taken in this study, and it was impractical in this dataset to correct for multiple comparisons. Nevertheless, the fact that 65 of 163 (40%) neuropsychological test × session comparisons showed greater-than-expected changes suggests that the assertion of excessive variability may be valid. The use of a control group would address concerns related to the use of published normative values. Finally, a major limitation of this study is the lack of overnight EEGs preceding the testing sessions. There is considerable evidence linking sleep quality to cognitive abilities. Moreover there is evidence relating t nocturnal inter-ictal and ictal findings to cognitive performance the following day in BECTS (Nicolai et al., 2007). Future larger studies that address the issue of the relationship between neuropsychological and EEG variability and are sufficiently powered to make comparisons between the two should record overnight EEGs prior to cognitive testing sessions. In summary, this case series suggests that cognitive performance and EEG findings may vary considerably over the short-term in children in the active phase of BECTS and thereby adversely impact educational opportunities and quality of life for these children.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported, in part, by NIH-NIDCD grants R01 DC005645 and K24 DC10028 (DBR) and NIH-NINDS grant K12 NS001696 (JBE; PI: Harvey Singer). The authors thank Jenna Los, Sarah Morrison-Colwell, and Heather Wied for technical assistance. Special thanks to Dr. Ernst Niedermeyer for helpful discussion of the EEG findings, to Dr. Brian Caffo for statistical guidance and to Dr. Jason Brandt for his insightful comments. We are grateful to the families who gave generously of their time to participate in this study.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV. 4. American Psychiatric Association; Washington, DC: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  2. Baglietto MG, Battaglia FM, Nobili L, Tortorelli S, De Negri E, Calevo MG, et al. Neuropsychological disorders related to interictal epileptic discharges during sleep in benign epilepsy of childhood with centrotemporal or Rolandic spikes. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2001;43:407–412. doi: 10.1017/s0012162201000755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Benton A, Hamsher K, Sivan A. Multilingual Aphasia Examination. AJA Associates, Inc; Washington, DC: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  4. Benton A, Varney N, Hamsher K. Visuospatial judgment. A clinical test. Arch Neurol. 1978;35:364–367. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1978.00500300038006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Boatman DF, Trescher WH, Smith C, Ewen J, Los J, Wied HM, et al. Cortical auditory dysfunction in benign rolandic epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2008;49:1018–1026. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2007.01519.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Commission on Classification and Terminology of the International League Against Epilepsy. Proposal for revised classification of epilepsies and epileptic syndromes. Epilepsia. 1989;30:389–399. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1989.tb05316.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Croona C, Kihlgren M, Lundberg S, Eeg-Olofsson O, Eeg-Olofsson KE. Neuropsychological findings in children with benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1999;41:813–818. doi: 10.1017/s0012162299001620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. D’Alessandro P, Piccirilli M, Tiacci C, Ibba A, Maiotti M, Sciarma T, et al. Neuropsychological features of benign partial epilepsy in children. Ital J Neurol Sci. 1990;11:265–269. doi: 10.1007/BF02333856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Heijbel J, Blom S, Bergfors PG. Benign epilepsy of children with centrotemporal EEG foci. A study of incidence rate in outpatient care. Epilepsia. 1975;16:657–664. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1975.tb04748.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Leark R, Greenberg L, Kindschi C, Dupuy T, Hughes S. Test of Variables of Attention: Clinical Manual. The TOVA Company; Los Alamitos: 2007. [Google Scholar]
  11. Lezak M. Neuropsychological Assessment. 2. Oxford University Press; New York: 1983. [Google Scholar]
  12. Llorente AM, Amado AJ, Voigt RG, Berretta MC, Fraley JK, Jensen CL, et al. Internal consistency, temporal stability, and reproducibility of individual index scores of the Test of Variables of Attention in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2001;16:535–546. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Manly T, Robertson I, Anderson V, Niparko J. The Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch) Thames Valley Test Company; London: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  14. Nicolai J, van der Linden I, Arends JB, van Mil SG, Weber JW, Vles JS, et al. EEG characteristics related to educational impairments in children with benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes. Epilepsia. 2007;48:2093–2100. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2007.01203.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Northcott E, Connolly AM, McIntyre J, Christie J, Berroya A, Taylor A, et al. Longitudinal assessment of neuropsychologic and language function in children with benign rolandic epilepsy. J Child Neurol. 2006;21:518–522. doi: 10.1177/08830738060210062601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Osterrieth P. Le test due copie d’une figure complexe. Archives of Psychology. 1944;30:206–356. [Google Scholar]
  17. Rey A. L’examen psychologique dans les cas d’encephalopathie traumatique. Archives of Psychology. 1942;28:286–340. [Google Scholar]
  18. Reynolds C, Kamphaus R. Behavior Assessment System for Children: Manual. American Guidance, Circle Pines; MN: 1992. [Google Scholar]
  19. Schmidt M. Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test: A Handbook. Western Psychological Services; Los Angeles, CA: 2004. [Google Scholar]
  20. Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. Pearson Education, Inc; San Antonio: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  21. Woodcock R, McGrew K, Mather N. Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities. Riverside; Itasca, IL: 2001. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES