Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Jul 11.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Health. 2011 Jul 11;26(6):651–666. doi: 10.1080/08870441003721251

Dyadic concordance among prostate cancer patients and their partners and health-related quality of life: Does it matter?

Erin Merz 3, Vanessa Malcarne 1,2,3,*, Celine M Ko 3,4, Melody Sadler 1, Lisa Kwack 1, James W Varni 5,6, Georgia Robins Sadler 2
PMCID: PMC3216478  NIHMSID: NIHMS182959  PMID: 20680885

Abstract

Serious and chronic illnesses occur within a family context, affecting not only the patient but the spouse/partner, children, and extended family network. Spouses/partners are likely to experience the greatest personal impact, and may influence patient adjustment. Also, the intimate relationship may be affected by the illness experience. This study examined whether dyadic concordance on characteristics of prostate cancer (PC) was related to health-related quality of life (HRQOL), psychological distress, and marital adjustment in PC patients and their female partners. Couples (N = 164) completed questionnaires on appraisals of PC, and individual and dyadic adjustment. Patient and partner PC appraisal ratings were positively correlated. There was a general pattern of patients and partners in concordant dyads, versus those in dyads in which spouses maximized or minimized PC characteristics, reporting significantly better individual HRQOL outcomes, although there were several exceptions. Patient-partner appraisal (dis)agreement generally did not significantly predict dyadic adjustment. Overall, results suggest that dyadic disagreement is associated with worse HRQOL in couples facing PC.

Keywords: chronic illness, prostate cancer, appraisals, coping, spouses, survivorship

Introduction

In 2008 there were an estimated 186,320 new cases of prostate cancer (PC), making it the most common type of cancer and second leading cause of cancer death among men (American Cancer Society, 2008). Advances in early detection and treatment have increased 5-year survival rates up to 99%, and there are approximately 2 million survivors in America today (American Cancer Society, 2008; Jemal, Murray, & Ward, 2005). Thus, increased attention is being given to the physical and psychological consequences of PC and its treatment. The physical consequences can be damaging and enduring, with urinary, bowel, and sexual dysfunction commonly reported as problematic (Bremner, Chong, Tomlinson, Alibhai, & Krahn, 2007; Eton & Lepore, 2002). It is well-documented that these difficulties negatively impact the patient’s physical and mental health-related quality of life (HRQOL) and intimate relationships (Eton & Lepore, 2002; Gray et al., 1997; Hassouna & Heaton, 1999; Kornblith, Herr, Ofman, Scher, & Holland, 1994; Lavery & Clarke, 1999; Penson, Feng et al., 2003; Penson, Litwin, & Aaronson, 2003; Resendes & McCorkle, 2006).

A committed romantic relationship, usually a marriage, may be a male patient’s most important social relationship, and the influence of PC on both members of a couple is central to understanding patient adjustment. Considering the documented role of social support, especially spousal/partner support, in promoting adjustment to chronic disease, disruption to this intimate relationship could result in poorer patient outcomes (Ben-Zur, Gilbar, & Lev, 2001; Galbraith, Arechiga, Ramierz & Pedro, 2005; Krongrad, Lai, Burke, Goodkin, & Lai 1996; Northouse, Mood, Templin, Mellon, & George 2000; Revenson, 1994; Revenson, 2003; Revenson, & Majerovitz, 1991; Revenson, Wollman, & Felton, 1983). While the majority of studies focus on patients and their spouses, committed romantic partners are often included, thus the term ‘partner’ will be used here to describe that role in the intimate dyad.

Prostate cancer in particular has been described as a “relationship disease” (Gray, Fitch, Phillips, Labrecque, & Klotz, 1999) in that both patient and partner experience significant stress and their responses reciprocally influence each other and the relationship. This may be true across other types of cancer; a recent meta-analysis of 46 studies of cancer patients and partners (three of which were prostate cancer studies) found a medium-sized association of patient-partner distress, supporting the notion that patients and partners react as an “emotional system” (Hagedoorn, Sanderman, Bolks, Tuinstra, & Coyne, 2008). In prostate cancer, high quality marriages have been associated with increased longevity (Krongrad et al., 1996), better HRQOL (Galbraith et al., 2005), and less distress (Banthia et al., 2002). Determining the processes that underlie, promote, and maintain healthy marriages in the context of chronic illness can assist efforts to identify and intervene in at-risk relationships.

Patient and Partner Appraisals

Lazarus and Folkman’s (1984) stress-appraisal model delineates the essential role of cognitive appraisals in mediating the relationship between a stressor (such as a serious illness) and coping behaviours that ultimately result in positive or negative outcomes. Thus, dyadic appraisals may be an important starting point in studying adjustment to disease (Bodenmann, 1995). Although there are very few studies of appraisals in dyads facing PC, one recently published study found that patient and spouse appraisals of self-efficacy, concerns and communication strongly influence their own adjustment to PC, while hopelessness and number of baseline symptoms appraisals influenced the other’s adjustment (Kershaw et al., 2008). This underscores the notion that patients and partners experience, appraise, and cope with PC both individually and as an interpersonal unit (Kershaw et al., 2008),

(Dis)agreement of Appraisals

There is evidence that, across diseases, dyads agree on many disease-related appraisals. Patient-partner dyads report agreeing on social roles and pain domains (Beaupre et al., 1997; Wilson, Dowling, Abdolell, & Tannock, 2000), symptoms and HRQOL (Sneeuw et al., 2001), and social support (Kuijer et al., 2000; Ptacek, Pierce, Ptacek, & Nogel 1999). Other studies have found evidence of disagreement. Dyads disagree on reports of depressive symptoms (Klinedinst, Clark, Blanton, & Wolf, 2007), treatment outcomes (Volk et al., 2004), and pain (Clipp & George, 1992; Martire et al., 2006; Redinbaugh, Baum, DeMoss, Fello, & Arnold, 2002).

Does (dis)agreement influence adjustment?

A related question concerns the implications of (dis)agreement for the adjustment of the patient, the partner, and their relationship. Revenson (1994) has proposed a congruence model that emphasizes the importance of similarities and differences between patients and partners, but to date this model has been primarily applied in examinations of how couples cope with illness rather than how they appraise the illness experience (Berg & Upchurch, 2007). Relatively little is known regarding the extent of divergence between patient and partner disease appraisals, and whether differences influence important outcomes such as individual HRQOL and marital quality. A growing literature has examined whether agreement of stressor appraisals among patients and close relations (usually partners) predicts adjustment to chronic disease. In general, greater patient-partner agreement on disease-related appraisals has been associated with better adjustment. Concordance of pain appraisals has been associated with less caregiver stress and better emotional support among osteoarthritis patients and their partners (Cremeans-Smith et al., 2003; Martire et al., 2006). However, agreement of optimistic appraisals may be conceptually different from agreement of pessimistic appraisals. In myocardial infarction (Figueiras & Weinman, 2003) and rheumatoid arthritis (Sterba et al., 2008), patient-partner dyads with concordant optimistic appraisals report better physical function and psychological adjustment over time, whereas dyads with concordant pessimistic appraisals do not.

Dissimilar appraisals have been associated with worse caregiver and patient coping and adjustment in individuals with chronic pain (Cano, Johansen, & Geisser, 2004) and myocardial infarction (Figueiras & Weinman, 2003). Patients with Chronic Fatigue Syndrome and Addisons Disease, and their partners, who have greater levels of dissimilarity also report more maladaptive outcomes (Heijmans, de Ridder, & Bensing, 1999). The direction of dissimilarity may also be important. Appraisal patterns of ‘maximization’ (in which the partner appraises disease-related problems as more severe than does the patient) versus ‘minimization’ (in which the partner appraises disease-related problems as less severe than does the patient) may be related to adjustment. There is some evidence that minimization is maladaptive; minimization has also been associated with decreased disease-management confidence, health status and positive affect in patients with osteoarthritis (Cremeans-Smith et al., 2003) and cancer (Miaskowski, Zimmer, Barrett, Diblle, & Wallhagen, 1997). In contrast, the literature regarding maximization is mixed; some studies find maximization to be maladaptive, while others find it to be adaptive. In a study of patients with rheumatoid arthritis and their partners, Riemsma, Taal, and Rasker (2000) found that both maximization and minimization were associated with poorer mental health for both individuals. Maximization has also been associated with increased caregiver distress and decreased patient HRQOL in end-stage cancer patients (Redinbaugh et al., 2002), less satisfying emotional support (Martire et al., 2006) and increased depressive symptoms (Cremeans-Smith et al., 2003) in osteoarthritis patients, and decreased well-being for patients with heart disease (Franks, Hong, Pierce, & Ketterer, 2002). Conversely, some studies have shown maximization is associated with increased support for patients with heart disease (Benyamini, Medalion, & Garfinkel, 2007) and better adjustment for women with rheumatoid arthritis (Sterba et al., 2008).

The Present Study

The present study addressed three questions: (1) How do patients and partners appraise PC-related urinary, bowel and sexual problems? (2) What is the level of (dis)agreement between patient and partner appraisals? (3) Is the level and direction of (dis)agreement (i.e., minimization, congruence, maximization) of patient and partner appraisals associated with a) patients’ and partners’ individual physical and mental HRQOL, and b) relationship quality?

Method

Participants

This study used baseline data collected as part of a randomized controlled trial designed to test a psychosocial intervention aimed at improving problem-solving skills in partners of men with PC. Participants were 164 men diagnosed with PC and their female partners. To be included in the study, participants had to live in San Diego County, speak English, and be married or cohabitating with their partner. Most couples (90.2%) were married. Sample characteristics are described in Table 1.

Table 1.

Sample characteristics

Patient (n = 164) Spouse (n = 164)
Agea 65.78 (09.93) 61.54 (10.73)
Latency since diagnosis (months)a 05.26 (04.53)
Stage of Cancerb, c 1 80 (48.8)
2 45 (27.4)
3 17 (10.4)
4 07 (04.3)
Ethnicityb Caucasian 142 (86.6) 134 (81.7)
African American 11 (07.6) 09 (05.5)
Hispanic 03 (01.8) 09 (05.5)
Asian American 05 (03.0) 08 (04.9)
Other 03 (01.8) 04 (02.4)
Educationb High school or less 25 (15.2) 42 (26.2)
Some college 48 (29.3) 55 (33.5)
College graduate 34 (20.7) 29 (17.7)
Graduate school 57 (34.8) 37 (22.6)
Family income per yearb Under $20,000 11 (06.9) 10 (06.6)
$20,000–$30,000 15 (09.1) 19 (11.6)
$30,000–$50,000 37 (22.6) 41 (25.0)
$50,000–$75,000 46 (28.0) 36 (22.0)
Over $75,000 50 (30.5) 46 (28.0)
Treatment Typeb Radical Prostatectomy 50 (30.5)
Radiation 29 (17.7)
Orchiectomy 4 (2.4)
Lupron/Zoladex shots 51 (31.1)
Flutamide pills 15 (9.1)

Note.

a

M (SD).

b

n (%).

c

Stage 1: confined to prostate, Stage 2: confined to prostate, biopsy borders not clean, Stage 3: spread outside prostate, within abdomen region, Stage 4: spread beyond abdomen region

Measures

UCLA Prostate Cancer Index (PCI)

The 20-item UCLA PCI measures urinary, bowel and sexual function and bother for PC patients (Litwin et al., 1998). Patients reported on their own function (e.g., Over the past 4 weeks, how often have you leaked urine?) and bother (e.g., How big of a problem has dripping urine or wetting your pants been for you?). Partners were asked to report their perceptions of patients’ function (e.g., Over the past 4 weeks, how often has your spouse leaked urine?) and bother (e.g., How big of a problem has dripping urine or wetting his pants been for your spouse?). The PCI includes three subscales measuring function: urinary function (UF; 5 items), bowel function (BF; 4 items), and sexual function (SF; 8 items), and three single-item bother scales: urinary bother (UB), bowel bother (BB), and sexual bother (SB). Raw scores are linearly transformed to a 0 to 100 scale, with higher scores indicating better function and less bother. Overall internal consistency for the present sample was: patient α = .74, partner α = .82.

Medical Outcomes Study Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36)

The SF-36 is a widely-used 36-item HRQOL survey that yields 8 subscales: Physical Functioning, Role-Physical, Bodily Pain, General Health, Vitality, Social Functioning, Role-Emotional, and Mental Health (Ware & Sherbourne, 1992). These subscales are combined into two composite scores of physical and mental health. Total scores on the composites range from 0–100; higher scores indicate better HRQOL. The overall alpha for the present sample was: patient α = .92, partner α = .93.

Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS)

The 32-item DAS measures perceived dyadic quality in married or cohabitating unmarried couples (Spanier, 1976). Respondents rate the extent of their agreement between themselves and their partner on a 6-point scale (0 = Always disagree, 5 = Always agree). A sample item is: “Demonstrations of affection.” Total scores can range from 0–150 with higher scores indicating better adjustment. Alphas were: patient α = .93, partner α = .93.

Procedure

This research protocol was approved by Institutional Review Boards at the University of California, San Diego (UCSD) and San Diego State University. Over a three-year period, patients and partners were recruited to participate in a randomized controlled trial investigating the efficacy of a problem-solving therapy program for partners of men diagnosed with prostate cancer within the past 18 months. Broad recruitment was conducted via print and electronic media, flyer distribution, and poster displays in the waiting rooms of collaborating physicians, medical centres, churches, beauty salons and barber shops. Collaborating physicians from the UCSD Medical Center, the Rebecca and John Moores UCSD Cancer Center, the San Diego Veterans Healthcare System, the Naval Medical Center, other local medical care centres and oncology practices throughout San Diego County mailed personal recruitment letters and fliers, and made phone calls to eligible patients. In-person appeals were made by physicians, support group leaders, and community business and organization leaders. Both patients and partners were required to consent to participate in order to be enrolled. When eligible couples were identified, research assistants administered baseline questionnaires at the couples’ homes, before random assignment to study condition was made. Of the 169 couples who consented, 5 refused randomization and were excluded from analyses. Patients and partners completed questionnaire packets independently. Couples were compensated $30 for completing the baseline portion of the study. Only baseline data were used for the present analysis.

Analytic plan

We conducted the following analyses to address the aims of this study: To assess how patients and partners appraise PC-related problems, we calculated the range, mean, and standard deviation for each PC appraisal. Pearson correlations were calculated to reflect patient-partner agreement on the PCI scores. The level of (dis)agreement between patient and partner appraisals for each of the six PCI subscales was calculated by subtracting the patient’s PCI score from their partner’s score to yield a (dis)agreement score, a continuous variable that represented the direction of divergence from the patient’s appraisal for that subscale. To assess whether (dis)agreement was associated with patient or partner HRQOL or relationship quality, we conducted hierarchical multiple regression analysis. Separate regressions were conducted for each PCI subscale, testing (dis)agreement on that subscale as a predictor of patient or partner outcomes. In the first block, latency (time since diagnosis) and cancer stage were entered as covariates. (Dis)agreement scores were centred to reduce multicollinearity among predictors (Aiken & West, 1991; Cohen & Cohen, 1983), and were entered in the second block. Because we predicted that dyadic appraisal patterns (i.e., minimizing, concordant, and maximizing) would differentially relate to adjustment, a curvilinear model was tested1 (Miles & Shevlin, 2001). Specifically, we predicted that increases in the predictor variables (dyadic appraisal patterns) would be not associated with a constant increase in the HRQOL and relationship quality outcomes. Rather, we hypothesized that minimizing and maximizing couples would report lower HRQOL and relationship quality, and concordant couples would report higher HRQOL and relationship quality. The (dis)agreement scores were raised to the second power to create the quadratic term, which was entered in the third block. To explore the nature of any significant quadratic terms, follow-up tests of the simple slopes were conducted. Because (dis)agreement scores are derived from two subjects, some variability is expected, thus dyads scoring within .5 standard deviation (SD) of the centred mean in either direction were conceptualized as concordant. Dyads scoring more than .5 SD below the mean were conceptualized as maximizing (i.e., the partners rated function or bother worse [lower] than did the patients), and dyads scoring more than .5 above the mean were conceptualized as minimizing (i.e., partners rated function or bother better [higher] than did the patients). Accordingly, when there were significant quadratic effects, tangent lines were tested at the mean (concordant), −1 SD (maximizing), and +1 SD (minimizing) to determine whether (dis)agreement patterns predicted HRQOL in patients and partners.

Results

Patient and partner appraisals of PC-related urinary, bowel and sexual problems

Descriptive data for all study measures are presented in Table 2. Patient and partner PC appraisals were similar to those previously reported for PC patients (Litwin et al., 1998). The three PC-related appraisals of function and bother were highly intercorrelated for both patients (Urinary: r =.641, p < .001; Bowel: r = .711, p < .001; Sexual: r = .476, p < .001) and partners (Urinary: r = .617, p < .001; Bowel: r = .802, p < .001; Sexual: r = .234, p = .005). The sample reported physical and mental HRQOL comparable to data from other PC samples (e.g., Litwin et al., 1998; Love et al., 2008), and good marital adjustment (Spanier, 1976).

Table 2.

Means and standard deviations of study measures and correlations for patients and partners

Measure Patients Partners

Predictors Range M (SD) Range M (SD) r t df
PCI - UFa 00.00–100.00 81.55 (23.70) 06.60–100.00 82.04 (23.01) .75** .284 159
PCI - BFa 16.75–100.00 81.46 (18.06) 08.25–100.00 80.32 (20.23) .51** −.706 159
PCI - SFa 00.00–87.50 24.17 (24.93) 00.00–90.63 26.96 (30.15) .83** 1.066 142
PCI - UBa 00.00–100.00 75.16 (29.91) 00.00–100.00 76.39 (30.56) .56** .417 159
PCI - BBa 00.00–100.00 81.40 (27.64) 00.00–100.00 77.78 (27.72) .52** −1.587 161
PCI - SBa 00.00–100.00 48.91 (41.40) 00.00–100.00 62.58 (39.00) .41** 3.963*** 153

Outcomes
SF-36 (Physical) 14.64–66.12 46.75 (11.32) 13.08–69.67 50.49 (12.22) −3.58*** 161
SF-36 (Mental) 13.40–67.45 48.91 (11.99) 07.24–68.58 44.06 (14.34) 3.80*** 161
DAS 67.00–148.00 118.57 (16.38) 56.00–150.00 118.62 (16.82) −.10 160

Note.

a

Abbreviations for the PCI are as follows: UF = Urinary Function, BF = Bowel Function, SF = Sexual Function, UB = Urinary Bother, BB = Bowel Bother, SB = Sexual Bother.

*

p < .05;

**

p < .01;

***

p < .001

(Dis)agreement of patient-partner appraisals

Patient and partner scores on each measure were significantly and positively correlated (see Table 2). (Dis)agreement scores are shown in Table 3. A positive (dis)agreement score indicates partner minimization (i.e., partner appraises disease-related problems as less serious); a negative (dis)agreement score indicates partner maximization (i.e., partner appraises disease-related problems as more serious).

Table 3.

Means and standard deviations of (dis)agreement scores on PC appraisals

PCIa Range Mean SD
UF (n = 160) −41.60 – 55.00 .3750 16.70223
BF (n = 160) −70.75 - 45.00 −1.0688 19.14507
SF (n = 143) −59.38 – 71.88 1.6766 18.80164
UB (n = 160) −75.00 – 100.00 .9375 28.44053
BB (n = 162) −75.00 - 75.00 −3.3951 27.23064
SB (n = 154) −100.00 - 100.00 13.9610 43.72252

Note.

a

Abbreviations for the PCI are as follows: UF = Urinary Function, BF = Bowel Function, SF = Sexual Function, UB = Urinary Bother, BB = Bowel Bother, SB = Sexual Bother.

Associations of (dis)agreement patterns to patient and partner HRQOL, and relationship quality

Patients

The regression models relating (dis)agreement to patient HRQOL are summarized in Table 4. After controlling for latency and stage, the quadratic terms for UB and BB were significant predictors of physical HRQOL. There were no other significant quadratic predictors for physical HRQOL. For UB, greater minimization was significantly related to poorer patient physical HRQOL (b = −.15, p < .05). Maximization (b = .07) and concordance (b = −.04) were not significantly related to physical HRQOL, ps > .05. For BB, both greater maximization (b = .19, p < .05), and greater minimization (b = −.14, p < .05) were significantly related to poorer physical HRQOL. Concordance was not significantly related to physical HRQOL, b = .03, p > .05. None of the quadratic terms significantly predicted mental HRQOL or marital strength.

Table 4.

Hierarchical regression analysis for (dis)agreement scores on PCI predicting patient HRQOL outcomes (SF-36 physical and mental health composites, DAS marital strength).

Outcome Predictora F p df R2 ΔR2 β p
Physical Health
Urinary Function Centered UF −.096 .248
Quadratic UF 2.938 .023 4, 138 .078 .000 −.008 .922
Bowel Function Centered BF .127 .146
Quadratic BF 3.880 .005 4, 138 .101 .007 −.088 .312
Sexual Function Centered SF −.103 .235
Quadratic SF 3.280 .014 4, 123 .096 .009 −.097 .272
Urinary Bother Centered UB −.101 .214
Quadratic UB 4.509 .002 4, 137 .116 .049 −.225 .006
Bowel Bother Centered BB .065 .402
Quadratic BB 8.136 .000 4, 140 .189 .114 −.344 .000
Sexual Bother Centered SB −.065 .455
Quadratic SB 2.723 .032 4, 131 .077 .002 −.043 .621

Mental Health
Urinary Function Centered UF −.087 .309
Quadratic UF 0.816 .517 4, 138 .023 .002 .050 .556
Bowel Function Centered BF −.029 .752
Quadratic BF 0.670 .614 4, 138 .019 .007 −.093 .308
Sexual Function Centered SF .061 .812
Quadratic SF 0.422 .793 4, 123 .014 .000 .015 .495
Urinary Bother Centered UB −.027 .754
Quadratic UB 0.631 .641 4, 137 .018 .008 −.089 .303
Bowel Bother Centered BB .097 .253
Quadratic BB 1.101 .359 4, 140 .030 .007 −.087 .304
Sexual Bother Centered SB .027 .765
Quadratic SB 0.547 .701 4, 131 .016 .004 −.061 .492

Marital Strength
Urinary Function Centered UF −.034 .693
Quadratic UF 0.163 .957 4, 139 .005 .003 .052 .540
Bowel Function Centered BF .062 .499
Quadratic BF 0.121 .975 4, 139 .003 .001 .032 .728
Sexual Function Centered SF .086 .338
Quadratic SF 0.482 .749 4, 124 .015 .006 .082 .370
Urinary Bother Centered UB .055 .515
Quadratic UB 1.124 .348 4, 138 .032 .025 −.161 .061
Bowel Bother Centered BB −.056 .511
Quadratic BB 0.205 .935 4, 141 .006 .002 .042 .626
Sexual Bother Centered SB −.126 .161
Quadratic SB 0.539 .707 4, 132 .016 .003 .060 .504

Note.

a

Abbreviations for the PCI are as follows: UF = Urinary Function, BF = Bowel Function, SF = Sexual Function, UB = Urinary Bother, BB = Bowel Bother, SB = Sexual Bother.

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001

Partners

The regression models relating (dis)agreement to partner HRQOL are summarized in Table 5. After controlling for latency and stage, the quadratic terms for UF and BF were significant predictors of physical HRQOL. For UF, greater maximization (b = .35, p < .05) was significantly related to poorer physical HRQOL, while concordance (b = .15, p < .05) was significantly related to better physical HRQOL. Minimization (b = −.06, p > .05) was not significantly related to physical HRQOL. For BF, greater maximization was significantly related to poorer physical HRQOL, b = .12, p = .05. Concordance (b = .01) and minimization (b = −.11) were not significantly related to physical HRQOL, ps > .05.

Table 5.

Hierarchical regression analysis for (dis)agreement scores on PCI predicting partner HRQOL outcomes (physical and mental health, marital strength).

Outcome Predictora F p df R2 ΔR2 β p
Physical Health
Urinary Function Centered UF .192 .020
Quadratic UF 3.745 .006 4, 138 .098 .039 −.197 .016
Bowel Function Centered BF .015 .866
Quadratic BF 2.333 .059 4, 138 .063 .033 −.197 .030
Sexual Function Centered SF .046 .605
Quadratic SF 0.815 .518 4, 123 .026 .003 −.060 .508
Urinary Bother Centered UB .213 .012
Quadratic UB 2.489 .046 4, 137 .068 .000 −.001 .987
Bowel Bother Centered BB .024 .773
Quadratic BB 1.778 .137 4, 140 .048 .022 −.152 .074
Sexual Bother Centered SB −.096 .282
Quadratic SB 1.535 .196 4, 131 .045 .018 −.137 .122

Mental Health
Urinary Function Centered UF .047 .578
Quadratic UF 1.646 .166 4, 138 .046 .001 .031 .714
Bowel Function Centered BF .125 .166
Quadratic BF 2.027 .094 4, 138 .055 .000 −.006 .951
Sexual Function Centered SF .004 .962
Quadratic SF 1.419 .232 4, 123 .044 .012 .111 .219
Urinary Bother Centered UB .079 .336
Quadratic UB 3.931 .005 4, 137 .103 .050 −.228 .006
Bowel Bother Centered BB .098 .245
Quadratic BB 1.952 .105 4, 140 .053 .003 .053 .528
Sexual Bother Centered SB .166 .060
Quadratic SB 2.388 .054 4, 131 .068 .011 −.109 .212

Marital Strength
Urinary Function Centered UF .108 .211
Quadratic UF 0.605 .660 4, 136 .017 .005 .071 .404
Bowel Function Centered BF .054 .560
Quadratic BF 0.235 .918 4, 136 .007 .005 .076 .417
Sexual Function Centered SF .166 .061
Quadratic SF 1.944 .107 4, 122 .060 .028 .172 .058
Urinary Bother Centered UB .123 .152
Quadratic UB 1.597 .179 4, 135 .045 .024 −.158 .067
Bowel Bother Centered BB .074 .392
Quadratic BB 1.496 .207 4, 138 .042 .039 .205 .019
Sexual Bother Centered SB −.046 .615
Quadratic SB 0.299 .878 4, 130 .009 .006 .083 .359

Note.

a

Abbreviations for the PCI are as follows: UF = Urinary Function, BF = Bowel Function, SF = Sexual Function, UB = Urinary Bother, BB = Bowel Bother, SB = Sexual Bother.

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001

The quadratic term for UB was a significant predictor of mental HRQOL. For UB, greater maximization was significantly related to poorer mental HRQOL, b = .15, p < .05. Concordance (b = .04) and minimization (b = −.07) were not significant predictors of mental HRQOL, ps > .05.

The quadratic term for BB was a significant predictor of marital strength. For BB, greater minimization was significantly related to marital strength, b = .21, p <.05. However, maximization (b = −.12), and concordance (b = .05) were not significant, ps > .05.

Discussion

In this sample of prostate cancer patients within 18 months of diagnosis, and their partners, both patients and partners generally reported that patients had significant PC-related urinary, bowel, and sexual problems, at levels consistent with prior reports (Bremner, et al., 2007; Eton & Lepore, 2002). Patient-partner appraisals were strongly and positively correlated, similar to previous findings (Hagedoorn et al., 2008). While there is a great range of levels of (dis)agreement among couples facing PC, most couples, in most appraisal domains, were concordant. The exception to this was sexual bother. (Dis)agreement scores for SB had the largest range and standard deviation of any appraisal domain. The mean patient-partner (dis)agreement was 13 points, with partners perceiving that patients experienced lower levels of sexual bother than the patients experienced in actuality. Such discrepancies are common; patients regularly report greater worry regarding their sexual dysfunction than partners report regarding patient sexual dysfunction (Couper et al., 1999).

The nature of (dis)agreement was assessed for patterns of minimization, concordance and maximization. Overall, patient-partner (dis)agreement was not significantly associated with many outcomes. Several significant curvilinear relationships emerged, suggesting that physical and mental HRQOL outcomes are related to (dis)agreement patterns in the domains of urinary and bowel function. Minimization and maximization were generally associated with negative outcomes; however, the effects were modest and dependent on the appraisal domain.

(Dis)agreements on urinary and bowel, versus sexual, problems were most strongly related to negative outcomes, as has been previously reported (Baider et al., 2003; Kornblith et al. 1994). While both function and bother (dis)agreement scores were predictive of outcomes, (dis)agreement about bother appraisals appeared to be most predictive, at least for patients. Thus, outcomes were most negative when patients and partners differently perceived the degree of bother patients experienced, rather than when they differently perceived the actual levels of function.

When partners minimize urinary bother or disagree in either direction on bowel bother (i.e., minimization or maximization) patients experienced poorer physical HRQOL, findings similar to those of Riemsa, Taal, and Rasker (2000). This suggests that disagreement, regardless of the direction, may be maladaptive for the patient. Minimization may distress patients if they feel their partner does not accurately perceive their PC-related bother and thus is underappreciative of the negative aspects of their experience, while maximization may unnecessarily increase patient dependence on the partner due to the partners’ inflated perception of patient bother. Thus, either disagreement pattern may negatively influence HRQOL (Weinman, Heijmans & Figueiras, 2003).

Partners who maximized patient problems in urinary function/bother and bowel function reported poorer physical or mental HRQOL. Maximization, or partner exaggeration of the disease appraisal, was the most maladaptive (dis)agreement pattern for partners. When a partner overstates the patient’s level of dysfunction and/or bother, the partner may become excessively worried, overprotective, and stressed, which could compromise their own HRQOL. Because it has been well established that partners experience psychological consequences from PC (Couper et al., 2006), it is surprising that the majority of studies examining (dis)agreement have focused exclusively on patient outcomes. Because PC is a “relationship disease” (Gray et al., 1999), it is equally important to assess factors that may negatively influence partner HRQOL.

Interestingly, the relatively large level of disagreement between patients and partners on sexual function and bother, which represent the most interactive of the three domains examined here, did not predict maladaptive adjustment in either member of the dyad. While this seems surprising, given that sexual problems are greatly distressing to patients, partner minimization in this case may reflect an acceptance that sexual problems are an unavoidable result of life-saving PC treatments (Korfage, deKoning, Habbema, Schröder & Essink-Bot, 2007). In general, partners tend to placate patients who experience sexual dysfunction (Couper et al., 2006). Thus the pattern of (dis)agreement on sexual problems found here, in which the partner views the problems as less serious than the patient, may not be maladaptive.

Overall, these findings suggest that (dis)agreement on urinary and bowel disease appraisals may predict maladaptive adjustment outcomes for both patients and partners and thus may be a clinically important point of intervention. Interestingly, in most cases (dis)agreement was associated with negative individual patient or partner outcomes, and not with dyadic adjustment. In this sample, most couples reported high-functioning relationships, perhaps reflecting a self-selection bias in which more well-adjusted couples were receptive to enrolling in the problem-solving clinical trial. Thus, limited variance may have restricted our ability to detect relationships between (dis)agreement and marital strength. The only significant finding was that minimization of bowel bother was associated with better marital strength. Bowel problems are of particular concern to patients with prostate cancer, and thus it is interesting that it is for this side effect that partner minimization of patient experience was associated with happier marriages. That patients may have experienced their partners’ minimization as reassuring is one possibility; however, this was an isolated finding, and overall there was little evidence of (dis)agreement being associated with marital strength in these relatively happy couples.

The present findings suggest that one way to identify patients and partners who are at risk for maladaptive HRQOL outcomes may be to determine the dyad’s level of agreement on PC-related function and bother. Psychoeducation and the use of well-established strategies for improving couples’ communication patterns may be helpful in resolving points of disagreement, or helping to promote better understanding of different perspectives on aspects of prostate cancer.

Limitations and Future Directions

Although this study contributes to the growing literature on couples’ adjustment to prostate cancer, it has several limitations. Recruitment of a larger and more diverse sample is needed. Despite our best efforts at recruitment, the sample was predominantly Caucasian and all couples were heterosexual, limiting generalizability. Also, although HRQOL levels reported by patients and partners were consistent with those reported in the existing literature on adjustment in PC, patients with poor HRQOL are likely underrepresented in the sample, thus limiting our ability to identify risk factors for maladaptive outcomes. As already noted, the preponderance of couples with strong marriages limits variance as well. Although a contribution of the study was to examine (dis)agreement on two types of appraisals (function and bother) across several important prostate cancer problem areas, this necessarily inflated family-wise error rate, and thus patterns of findings should be given greater emphasis than any individual result. Finally, all data were cross-sectional, precluding any conclusions about cause-effect relationships. Couples’ adjustment to prostate cancer must be followed over time in order to understand the relationship between concordance of appraisals and individual/couples outcomes.

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by an intramural grant from the UCSD Cancer Center Foundation, the National Cancer Institute Grant # R25CA65745 and the California Cancer Research Program Grant #1II0049. We would like to recognize the following San Diego health care professionals for their help in recruiting patients to this study: Christopher Amling, M.D., Israel Barken, M.D., the late Howard Fuerst, M.D., Jeffrey Gaines, M.D., Richard Hall, M.D., Robert Hathorne, M.D., Peter Johnstone, M.D., Linas Kazlo, M.D., Huathin Khaw, M.D., Robert Langer, M.D., Kenneth Nitahara, M.D., Fred Pardo, M.D., Jonathon Polikoff, M.D., Marianne Rochester, M.D., Carol Salem, M.D., Marilyn Sanderson, R.N., Kenneth Schimizu, M.D., Stephen Seagren, M.D., John Smiley, M.D., Thomas Szolar, M.D., and Linda Wasserman, M.D. We also wish to thank the following organizations for promoting the public’s awareness of this study: American Cancer Society, Prostate Cancer Research & Education Forum, Informed Men’s Support Group, and Wellness Community.

Footnotes

1

The primary research question of interest presumes categorization of couples (those who minimize, agree, maximize), however, dividing dyads into categories for mean comparison creates arbitrary boundaries and diminishes statistical power. We would like to thank two anonymous reviewers for suggesting alternate data analysis strategies.

References

  1. American Cancer Society. Cancer facts and figures. American Cancer Society; Atlanta, GA: 2008. [Google Scholar]
  2. Aiken L, West S. Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  3. Banthia R, Malcarne VL, Varni JW, Ko CM, Sadler GR, Greenbergs HL. The effects of dyadic strength and coping styles on psychological distress in couples faced with prostate cancer. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2003;26:31–52. doi: 10.1023/a:1021743005541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beaupre P, Keefe FJ, Lester N, Affleck G, Frederickson B, Caldwell DS. A computer-assisted observational method for assessing spouses’ ratings of osteoarthritis patients’ pain. Psychology, Health, & Medicine. 1997;2:99–108. [Google Scholar]
  5. Ben-Zur H, Gilbar O, Lev S. Coping with breast cancer: Patient, spouse and dyad models. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2001;63:32–39. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200101000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Benyamini Y, Medalion B, Garfinkel D. Patient and spouse perceptions of the patient’s heart disease and their associations with received and provided social support and undermining. Psychology & Health. 2007;22:765–785. [Google Scholar]
  7. Berg C, Upchurch R. A developmental-contextual model of couples coping with chronic illness across the adult life span. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133:920–954. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.6.920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bodenmann G. A systemic-transactional conceptualization of stress and coping in couples. Swiss Journal of Psychology. 1995;54:34–49. [Google Scholar]
  9. Boehmer U, Clark JA. Communication about prostate cancer between men and their wives. Journal of Family Practice. 2001;50:226–231. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Burman B, Margolin G. Analysis of the association between marital relationships and health problems: An interactive perspective. Psychological Bulletin. 1992;112:39–63. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bremner K, Chong C, Tomlinson G, Alibhai S, Krahn M. A Review and meta-analysis of Prostate Cancer utilities. Medical Decision Making. 2007;27:288–298. doi: 10.1177/0272989X07300604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cano A, Johansen A, Geisser M. Spousal congruence on disability, pain, and spouse responses to pain. Pain. 2004;109:258–265. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.01.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cliff AM, MacDonagh RP. Psychosocial morbidity in prostate cancer: II. A comparison of patients and partners. British Journal of Urology International. 2000;86:834–839. doi: 10.1046/j.1464-410x.2000.00914.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Clipp E, George LK. Patients with cancer and their spouse caregivers. Cancer. 1992;69:1074–1079. doi: 10.1002/1097-0142(19920215)69:4<1074::aid-cncr2820690440>3.0.co;2-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cohen J, Cohen P. Applied multiple regression/correlational analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  16. Couper J, Bloch S, Love A, Macvean M, Duchesne G, Kissane D. Psychosocial adjustment of female partners of men with Prostate Cancer: A review of the literature. Psycho-Oncology. 2006;15:937–953. doi: 10.1002/pon.1031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cremeans-Smith JK, Stephens MAP, Franks MM, Martire LM, Druley JA, Wojno WC. Spouses’ and physicians’ perceptions of pain severity in older women with osteoarthritis: Dyadic agreements and patients’ well-being. Pain. 2003;106:27–34. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(03)00268-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. DeLamater J, Sill M. Sexual Desire in Later Life. Journal of Sex Research. 2005;42:138–149. doi: 10.1080/00224490509552267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Eton D, Lepore S. Prostate cancer and health-related quality of life: A review of the literature. Psycho-Oncology. 2002;11:307–326. doi: 10.1002/pon.572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Figueiras M, Weinman J. Do similar patient and spouse perceptions of myocardial infarction predict recovery? Psychology & Health. 2003;18:201–216. [Google Scholar]
  21. Franks MM, Hong TB, Pierce LS, Ketterer MW. The association of patients’ psychosocial well-being with self and spouse ratings of patient health. Family Relations. 2002;51:22–27. [Google Scholar]
  22. Galbraith ME, Arechiga A, Ramirez J, Pedro LW. Prostate cancer survivors’ and partners’ self-reports of health-related quality of life, treatment symptoms and marital satisfaction 2.5–5.5 years after treatment. Oncology Nursing Forum. 2005;32:E30–E41. doi: 10.1188/05.ONF.E30-E41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gray RE, Fitch MI, Phillips C, Labrecque M, Klotz L. Presurgery experiences of prostate cancer patients and their spouses. Cancer Practice. 1999;7:130–135. doi: 10.1046/j.1523-5394.1999.07308.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gray RE, Klotz LH, Iscoe NA, Fitch MI, Franssen E, Johnson BJ. Results of a survey of Canadian men with prostate cancer. Canadian Journal of Urology. 1997;4:359–365. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Hagedoorn M, Sanderman R, Bolks HN, Tuinstra J, Coyne JC. Distress in couples coping with cancer: A meta-analysis and critical review of role and gender effects. Psychological Bulletin. 2008;134:1–30. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hassouna MM, Heaton JP. Prostate cancer: 8. Urinary incontinence and erectile dysfunction. Canadian Medical Association Journal. 1999;160:78–86. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Heijmans M, de Ridder D, Bensing J. Dissimilarity in patients’ and spouses’ representations of chronic illness: Exploration of relations to patient adaptation. Psychology & Health. 1999;14:451–466. [Google Scholar]
  28. Jemal A, Murray T, Ward E. Cancer statistics, 2005. A Cancer Journal for Clinicians. 2005;55:10–30. doi: 10.3322/canjclin.55.1.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kershaw T, Mood D, Newth G, Ronis D, Sanda M, Vaishampayan U, Northouse LL. Longitudinal analysis of a model to predict quality of life in prostate cancer patients and their spouses. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2008;36:117–128. doi: 10.1007/s12160-008-9058-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Klinedinst NJ, Clark PC, Blanton S, Wolf SL. Congruence of depressive symptom appraisal between persons with stroke and their caregivers. Rehabilitation Psychology. 2007;52:215–225. [Google Scholar]
  31. Korfage IJ, de Koning HJ, Habbema JDF, Schröder FH, Essink-Bot ML. Side-effects of treatment for localized prostate cancer: are they valued differently by patients and healthy controls? British Journal of Urology International. 2007;99:801–806. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-410X.2006.06707.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kornblith AB, Herr HW, Ofman US, Scher HI, Holland JC. Quality of life of patients with prostate cancer and their spouses: The value of a data base in clinical care. Cancer. 1994;73:2791–2802. doi: 10.1002/1097-0142(19940601)73:11<2791::aid-cncr2820731123>3.0.co;2-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Krongrad A, Lai H, Burke MA, Goodkin K, Lai S. Marriage and mortality in prostate cancer. Journal of Urology. 1996;156:1696–1670. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kuijer R, Ybema J, Buunk B, DeJong G, Thijs-Boer F, Sanderman R. Active engagement, protective buffering and overprotection: Three ways of giving support by intimate partners of patients with cancer. Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology. 2000;19:256–275. [Google Scholar]
  35. Lavery JF, Clarke VA. Prostate cancer: Patient’s and spouse’s coping and marital adjustment. Psychology, Health & Medicine. 1999;4:289–302. [Google Scholar]
  36. Lazarus RS, Folkman S. Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer Publishing Company, Inc; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  37. Litwin M, Hays R, Fink A, Ganz P, Leake B, Brook R. The UCLA Prostate Cancer Index: Development, reliability, and validity of a health-related quality of life measure. Medical Care. 1998;36:1002–1012. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199807000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Love AW, Scealy M, Bloch S, Duchesne G, Couper J, Macvean M, Costello A, Kissane DW. Psychosocial adjustment in newly diagnosed prostate cancer. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;42:423–429. doi: 10.1080/00048670801961081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Martire L, Keefe F, Schulz R, Ready R, Beach S, Rudy T, Starz TW. Older spouses’ perceptions of partners’ chronic arthritis pain: Implications for spousal responses, support provision, and caregiving experiences. Psychology and Aging. 2006;21:222–230. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Miaskowski C, Zimmer EF, Barrett KM, Dibble SL, Wallhagen M. Differences in patients’ and family caregivers’ perceptions of the pain experience influence patient and caregiver outcomes. Pain. 1997;72:217–226. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(97)00037-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Miles J, Shevlin M. Non-linear and logistic regression. In: Miles J, Shevlin M, editors. Applying regression and correlation: A guide for students and researchers. London, England, CA: Sage Press; 2001. pp. 136–164. [Google Scholar]
  42. Neese L, Schover L, Klein E, Zippe C, Kupelian P. Finding help for sexual problems after prostate cancer treatment: A phone survey of men’s and women’s perspectives. Psycho-Oncology. 2003;12:463–473. doi: 10.1002/pon.657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Northouse LL, Mood D, Templin T, Mellon S, George T. Couples’ patterns of adjustment to colon cancer. Social Science and Medicine. 2000;50:271–284. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(99)00281-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Penson DF, Feng Z, Kuniyuki A, McClerran D, Albertsen PC, Deapen D, Gilliland F, Hoffman RM, Stephenson R, Potosky AL, Stanford J. General quality of life 2 years following treatment for prostate cancer: What influences outcomes? Results from the prostate cancer outcomes study. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2003;21:1147–1154. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2003.07.139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Penson DF, Litwin MS, Aaronson NK. Health related quality of life in men with prostate cancer. Journal of Urology. 2003;169:1653–1661. doi: 10.1097/01.ju.0000061964.49961.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Ptacek JT, Pierce GR, Ptacek JJ, Nogel C. Stress and coping process in men with prostate cancer: The divergent views of husbands and wives. Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology. 1999;18:299–324. [Google Scholar]
  47. Redinbaugh E, Baum A, DeMoss C, Fello M, Arnold R. Factors associated with the accuracy of family caregiver estimates of patient pain. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management. 2002;23:31–38. doi: 10.1016/s0885-3924(01)00372-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Resendes LA, McCorkle R. Spousal responses to prostate cancer: An integrative review. Cancer Investigation. 2006;24:192–198. doi: 10.1080/07357900500524652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Revenson TA. Social support and marital coping with chronic illness. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 1994;16:122–130. [Google Scholar]
  50. Revenson TA. Scenes from a marriage: Examining support, coping, and gender within the context of chronic illness. In: Suls J, Wallston KA, editors. Social psychological foundations of health and illness. Oxford, England: Blackwell Publishers; 2003. pp. 530–559. [Google Scholar]
  51. Revenson TA, Majerovitz SD. The effects of chronic illness on the spouse. Arthritis Care and Research. 1991;4:63–72. doi: 10.1002/art.1790040203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Revenson TA, Wollman CA, Felton BJ. Social supports as stress buffers for adult cancer patients. Psychometric Medicine. 1983;45:321–331. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198308000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Riemsma R, Taal E, Rasker J. Perceptions about perceived functional disabilities and pain of people with rheumatoid arthritis: Differences between patients and their spouses and correlates with well-being. Arthritis Care & Research. 2000;13:255–261. doi: 10.1002/1529-0131(200010)13:5<255::aid-anr3>3.0.co;2-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Schag CA, Ganz PA, Wing DS, Sim MS, Lee JJ. Quality of life in adult survivors of lung, colon and prostate cancer. Quality of Life Research. 1994;3:127–141. doi: 10.1007/BF00435256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Sneeuw K, Aaronson N, Osoba D, Muller M, Hsu M, Yung W, Brada M, Newlands ES. The use of significant others as proxy raters of the quality of life of patients with brain cancer. Medical Care. 2001;35:490–506. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199705000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Spanier G. Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage & the Family. 1976;38:15–28. [Google Scholar]
  57. Sterba KR, DeVellis RF, Lewis MA, DeVellis BM, Jordan JM, Baucom DH. Effect of couple illness perception congruence on psychological adjustment in women with rheumatoid arthritis. Health Psychology. 2008;27:221–229. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.2.221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Volk RJ, Cantor SB, Cass AR, Spann SJ, Weller SC, Krahn MD. Preferences of husbands and wives for outcomes of prostate cancer screening and treatment. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2004;19:339–348. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2004.30046.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Ware JE, Sherbourne CD. The MOS 36-item short form health survey. Medical Care. 1992;30:473–483. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Weinman J, Heijmans M, Figueiras MJ. Carer perceptions of chronic illness. In: Cameron LD, Leventhal H, editors. The self-regulation of health and illness behaviour. New York, New York: Routledge; 2003. pp. 207–219. [Google Scholar]
  61. Wilson K, Dowling A, Abdolell M, Tannock I. Perception of quality of life by patients, partners and treating physicians. Quality of Life Research: An International Journal of Quality of Life Aspects of Treatment, Care & Rehabilitation. 2000;9:1041–1052. doi: 10.1023/a:1016647407161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES