Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2011 Nov 16;6(11):e27206. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027206

The Influence of Genetic and Environmental Factors among MDMA Users in Cognitive Performance

Elisabet Cuyàs 1,2, Antonio Verdejo-García 1,3, Ana Beatriz Fagundo 1,2, Olha Khymenets 1,4, Joan Rodríguez 1, Aida Cuenca 5, Susana de Sola Llopis 1, Klaus Langohr 1,6, Jordi Peña-Casanova 2,7, Marta Torrens 2,8, Rocío Martín-Santos 1,9, Magí Farré 1,2, Rafael de la Torre 1,4,10,*
Editor: Alessandro Serretti11
PMCID: PMC3217947  PMID: 22110616

Abstract

This study is aimed to clarify the association between MDMA cumulative use and cognitive dysfunction, and the potential role of candidate genetic polymorphisms in explaining individual differences in the cognitive effects of MDMA. Gene polymorphisms related to reduced serotonin function, poor competency of executive control and memory consolidation systems, and high enzymatic activity linked to bioactivation of MDMA to neurotoxic metabolites may contribute to explain variations in the cognitive impact of MDMA across regular users of this drug. Sixty ecstasy polydrug users, 110 cannabis users and 93 non-drug users were assessed using cognitive measures of Verbal Memory (California Verbal Learning Test, CVLT), Visual Memory (Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, ROCFT), Semantic Fluency, and Perceptual Attention (Symbol Digit Modalities Test, SDMT). Participants were also genotyped for polymorphisms within the 5HTT, 5HTR2A, COMT, CYP2D6, BDNF, and GRIN2B genes using polymerase chain reaction and TaqMan polymerase assays. Lifetime cumulative MDMA use was significantly associated with poorer performance on visuospatial memory and perceptual attention. Heavy MDMA users (>100 tablets lifetime use) interacted with candidate gene polymorphisms in explaining individual differences in cognitive performance between MDMA users and controls. MDMA users carrying COMT val/val and SERT s/s had poorer performance than paired controls on visuospatial attention and memory, and MDMA users with CYP2D6 ultra-rapid metabolizers performed worse than controls on semantic fluency. Both MDMA lifetime use and gene-related individual differences influence cognitive dysfunction in ecstasy users.

Introduction

3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, ecstasy) is one of the most popular illegal psychostimulants abused among youth. There is compelling evidence that MDMA produces selective long-lasting serotonergic neuroadaptations, including regulatory changes in the expression of the serotonin transporter [1][3]. In humans, ligand-binding imaging studies have reported decreased serotonin transporter (SERT) binding throughout the cerebral cortices and the hippocampus in MDMA users compared to healthy controls [4], [5]. Furthermore, these studies have shown that decreased SERT binding is associated with lower memory performance in MDMA users. Although some studies have observed MDMA abstinence-related recovery of SERT availability in the midbrain and thalamus [6], [7] there is no data about SERT recovery in the cortex, and post-mortem evidence indicates that cortical SERT protein reductions can be more robust and durable than indicated by neuroimaging studies [8]. Overall these findings are suggestive of MDMA-induced neurotoxicity, which primarily affects the serotonin system and is linked to memory dysfunction.

Despite these findings about serotonin neuroadaptations, there is still debate on the question if MDMA use is reliably associated with neuropsychological impairment, regardless of the effects of concomitant use of other substances (e.g., cannabis, alcohol or other stimulants). Literature on this topic is characterized by considerable heterogeneity of results, which is attributable to the large amount of confounders inherent to research on the deleterious effects of MDMA [9]. Two meta-analyses of neuropsychological studies in MDMA users have concluded that MDMA use is robustly associated with learning and memory impairments [10], [11]. This conclusion is substantiated by evidence from studies showing dose-related detrimental effects of MDMA use on learning and memory [12][14]. Nonetheless, the size of these dose-related effects is modest (6–11% of explained variance [13], suggesting that other relevant individual differences, may play an important role in MDMA-induced neuropsychological deficits.

A number of genes affecting serotonin function, including SERT and 5HT2A receptor gene polymorphisms, have demonstrated significant associations with cognition and may therefore importantly impact MDMA use related neuropsychological effects [15]. In addition, gene variants involved in MDMA pharmacodynamics and putative neurotoxic mechanisms, such as COMT [16], [17] and CYP2D6 polymorphisms [18], and those impacting neural signaling pathways involved in learning and memory (e.g., BDNF and glutamate genes) [19][21] may also contribute to explain MDMA-induced neuropsychological deficits in humans [22]. Some of the dopamine and serotonin gene polymorphisms are equally relevant for MDMA-related cognitive effects based on their well-recognized role in modulating prefrontal cortex functioning and executive control [23], [24]. There is growing evidence that memory decrements in MDMA users are more neatly observable when neuropsychological probes involve a greater degree of complexity in terms of organization demands [25], [26]. These findings suggest that executive control processes linked to prefrontal systems may be impacted by the use of MDMA, and that genetic differences related to these systems may likely mediate these effects. In summary, different gene polymorphisms related to reduced serotonin function (SERT s/s and 5HT2a Tyr genotypes), high enzymatic activity linked to the bioactivation of MDMA to neurotoxic metabolites (COMT val/val and CYP2D6 ultra-rapid metabolizers), and poor competency of executive control and memory consolidation systems (COMT val/val, BDNF met/met, and GRIN2B C/C) can contribute to explain variations in the cognitive impact of MDMA across regular users of this drug.

This study seeks to clarify the association between MDMA cumulative use and cognitive dysfunction, and the potential role of a number of relevant genetic polymorphisms in explaining individual differences in the cognitive effects of MDMA. To reach this aim, we examined cognitive performance in a sample of MDMA users recruited from a homogeneous socio-demographic context and thoroughly assessed to rule out psychiatric comorbidities. The sample also includes considerable variability in MDMA use patterns, which allowed us to characterize dose-related effects of cumulative MDMA use on cognitive performance. Neuropsychological testing was focused on those cognitive domains that have been consistently linked to MDMA use across studies: verbal and visual memory, attention/processing speed and executive functions. We hypothesize: (i) that heavier MDMA use would correlate with poorer neuropsychological performance in a dose-dependent fashion; (ii) that heavy MDMA users would perform poorer than cannabis and healthy comparison individuals on neuropsychological tests of processing speed, memory and fluency (indicating robust effects of MDMA on cognition regardless of co-abuse of cannabis); and (iii) that MDMA use would exacerbate cognitive performance decrements in individuals carrying genotypes associated with lower functionality of the serotonin, glutamate and dopamine systems.

Methods and Materials

Participants

Two hundred sixty-three Caucasian participants were recruited, of whom 60 were MDMA polydrug users, 110 were cannabis users and 93 were non-users. MDMA users were further classified into two subgroups according to their lifetime MDMA use applying a cut-off of 100 tablets (more than 100 tablets defined as heavy users) [27]. Volunteers were recruited through several sources: ‘word of mouth’, advertisement in the local university, and advertisement in the website of a local NGO (Energy Control) specialized in providing harm reduction guidelines among drug users. Potential participants were medically screened in the clinical research unit to rule out the presence of physical or neurological illness –as determined by standard physical examinations and biochemical determinations (supervised by the medical director –MFA). They were also carefully assessed to diagnose possible comorbid psychopathological disorders using a well-validated psychiatric interview –the Psychiatric Research Interview for Substance and Mental Disorders (PRISM; [28]) –which outcomes were supervised by two expert psychiatrists (MTM and RMS). Potential participants having medical illnesses or comorbid psychiatric disorders were excluded from the study. In addition, the PRISM interview also provides diagnoses for the whole spectrum of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) substance use disorders, such that we were able to rule out potential participants meeting criteria for abuse or dependence on drugs different than the ones targeted as the main aim of this study (MDMA and cannabis). Specifically, the following exclusion criteria were applied taking into account the data collected during the interview: for cannabis users, current history of regular use of other illegal drugs during last year, and past use of other illegal drugs for more than 5 occasions during lifetime; for non-users, current history of use of any illegal drugs during the past year, and past use of any illegal drugs in more than 5 occasions. Alcohol and nicotine use (but not abuse or dependence) was permitted. As for the MDMA group, because it was impossible to recruit exclusive MDMA users, we included MDMA users with exposure to other drugs if they did not meet abuse or dependence criteria for these other drugs.

Test procedure

This study was approved by and conducted in accordance with the local ethics committee (CEIC-IMAS). Upon arrival to the research centre (IMIM, Hospital del Mar Research Institute), participants were informed of the ensuing protocol and provided informed consent before participating in the study.

All participants were subjected to an initial exploration that included a detailed medical history, biochemical analyses, physical examination, urine and hair toxicology screens, and a brief neurological examination. They were also assessed with a structured psychiatric interview specifically designed to diagnose lifetime use of different drugs and psychiatric comorbidity among substance users (PRISM [28]) administered by a psychiatrist or a clinical psychologist. Toxicology history in the past six months was confirmed by hair testing [29], [30]. All participants were requested to observe a 72 h abstinence period, and urine and hair drug screens were carried out by immunoassay (CEDIA, Thermo-Fisher) to confirm abstinence. Drug classes screened for included: cannabis, MDMA, cocaine and amphetamine/methamphetamine. A positive urine drug test excluded the participant for further assessments. This procedure allowed us to reliably classify participants into the different subgroups according to their pattern of drug use. All participants meeting inclusion criteria underwent a neuropsychological assessment session of 180 minutes, although here we only report analyses from a subset of these measures. All subjects were economically compensated for their participation.

Cognitive measures

We administered the Vocabulary test from the WAIS-III (estimated IQ) [31], the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT II) (verbal memory) [32], the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCFT) (visual spatial organization –Copy, and memory –Recall) [33], the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (attention/processing speed) [34], and the Category Word Fluency test [35]. A more detailed description of these tests can be found in [36].

Genotyping

Genomic DNA was extracted from the peripheral blood leukocytes of all the participants using Flexi Gene DNA kit (Qiagen Iberia, S.L., Spain) according to the manufacturer's instructions.

5HTTLPR genotyping was performed using polymerase chain reaction (PCR) as described in [37]. The 5HTTLPR (A/G) polymorphism (rs25531) was detected by MspI restriction enzyme digestion [38] with minor modifications. Briefly, 1 µl of the 5HTTLPR PCR was digested in a 10 µl reaction assay containing 1× NEBuffer 2 and 3 U MspI at 37°C for 3 h and a final inactivation step of 20 minutes at 65°C. The resulting fragments were detected on an automatic ABI 3730XL capillary sequencer and analyzed by GeneMapper Software v3.5 (Applied Biosystems). Product sizes for the digest were: long A (LA) = 337 bp, short A (SA) = 292 bp, long G (LG) = 162 bp, and short G (Sg) = 162 bp.

In some cases, were MspI digestion gave unclear results the samples were sequenced to assign the correct genotype. Sequencing was performed in both, the sense and antisense orientations. The excess primers and deoxynucleotides in the polymerase chain reaction (PCR) products were then degraded by adding a 2 µl of a solution of 0.8 units of shrimp alkaline phosphatase (New England Biolabs, Ipswich, MA), 4 units of Escherichia coli Exonuclease I (New England Biolabs) and 0.64× shrimp alkaline phosphatase buffer. The mixture was incubated at 37°C for 15 min, followed by deactivation for 15 min at 80°C. Sequencing reactions were performed with BigDye v3.1 (Applied Biosystems) in 10 µl total volume containing 1 µl template (approximately 25 ng), 3.2 pmol primer, 1 µl Applied Biosystems 5× DNA sequencing buffer, 2 µl BigDye v3.1, and water. The reactions were cycled at 94°C for 3 min, followed by 30 cycles at 96°C for 10 sec, 50°C for 5 sec, and 60°C for 4 min. Reactions were then purified with PureLink Quick Gel extraction kit (Invitrogen) according to manufacturer's instructions. Samples were analyzed on a Prism 3730xl DNA Analyzer (Applied Biosystems).

The COMT val108/158met (rs4680) and BDNF val66met (rs6265) single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) allelic variants were determined using the 5′ exonuclease TaqMan assay with ABI 7900HT Sequence Detection System (Real Time PCR) supplied by Applied Biosystems. Primers and fluorescent probes were obtained from Applied Biosystems (TaqMan SNP Genotyping assays: assay ID C_2255335_10 and C_11592758_10 for rs4680 and rs6265, respectively). Reaction conditions were those described in the ABI PRISM 7900HT user's guide. Endpoint fluorescent signals were detected on the ABI 7900, and the data were analyzed using SDS software, version 2.3 (Applied Biosystems).

The CYP2D6, GRIN2B C2664T (rs1806201), the 5HT2A His452Tyr (rs6314) and T102C (rs6313) genotypes were performed using the PHARMAchip™ DNA array (Progenika Biopharma, Derio, Spain) [39]. This DNA microarray allows the screening of genetic variants for phase I and phase II drug metabolism enzymes (DME), drug transporters, and drug protein effectors based on allele-specific oligonucleotide hybridization (ASO).

Statistical Analyses

Sample characteristics, including drug consumption, are described by means of either mean and standard deviation (numerical variables) or absolute and relative frequencies (categorical variables). To quantify differences among the three groups, a generalization of Cohen's d, the standard mean difference for more than two groups, was computed. Following the suggestions of Cohen, values of 0.25 and 0.4 indicate medium and large effect sizes, respectively [40]. The chi-square test was applied to study the association between drug consumption (ecstasy consumption, cannabis consumption or control group) and each of the genotypes studied. In addition, it was used to check whether the Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium holds among each of the three populations under study. At a univariate level, the correlation between the cognitive performance and both lifetime ecstasy and lifetime cannabis consumption was quantified by Pearson's correlation coefficient among those individuals consuming ecstasy and those consuming cannabis, respectively. Lifetime consumption was measured in number of tablets (ecstasy) and number of joints (cannabis), respectively. These correlation coefficients were also computed for the subgroups defined by the COMT val158met genotype, on one hand, and the 5HTTLPR genotype, on the other. Since several significant correlations with the neuropsychological variables were found (as described in the following section), in the sequel, a distinction was made between regular and heavy ecstasy users taking the consumption of 100 ecstasy pills during lifetime as cutoff. Since the principal interest was to study the association between cognitive performance and both drug consumption and each of the genotypes of interest, ANCOVA models were fitted for all neuropsychological variables and each genotype separately. These models included drug consumption group and the respective genotype as predictive variables of interest as well as gender and the WAIS-III Vocabulary index score; the Vocabulary index was used as a proxy of cultural level and verbal IQ. These two variables, sex and WAIS-III Vocabulary index, were included in all ANCOVA models in order to rule out the possible confusion due to differences observed among the drug consumption groups with respect to sex and education/IQ. Initially, all models did also include the two-way interaction between genotype and drug consumption. Whenever the interaction could be discarded, both factors were studied separately using the ANCOVA model excluding interaction. If a significant effect was observed of either factor, post-hoc multiple comparisons were carried out in the framework of the corresponding model using the Tukey test. If, by contrast, the interaction was significant, the effect of drug consumption was studied separately for each genotype expression and, vice versa. Again, the Tukey test for multiple comparisons was applied for these analyses in the framework of the ANCOVA models including interaction.

Statistical significance was set at 0.05 for the two-way genotype-drug consumption interaction within all ANCOVA models in order to reduce the probability of a possible Type II error. For those variables showing a significant interaction effect, results from follow-up pairwise comparisons were thresholded at p< = 0.01 to protect against Type I error was applied. The statistical software package R (The R Foundation for Statistical Computing), version 2.11.1, was used for all analyses. In particular, R Package multcomp [41] was used for the multiple pairwise comparisons.

Results

Sample characteristics

Socio-demographic variables, drug use characteristics and genotype distributions are presented in Table 1. Largest standardized mean differences (SMD) among the three samples were observed with respect to smoking habits (SMD = 0.69) and age of first alcohol use (SMD = 0.48). In addition, the groups differed notably in terms of the WAIS-III score (SMD = 0.3) and age (SMD = 0.25) with highest values among controls (average WAIS-III score of 12.6) and MDMA users (23.2 years), respectively. Despite the differences observed, neither age nor smoking or age of first alcohol consumption were included in the regression models. These characteristics were not considered possible confounders because of the relatively small interquartile range of age (20–24 years) and the lack of correlation of the other two variables with the neuropsychological variables under study (results not shown), respectively.

Table 1. Demographic variables, drug consumption characteristics.

MDMA (n = 60) Cannabis (n = 110) Control (n = 93)
n (%) n (%) n (%) SMDc
Age a 23.2 (3.2) 21.6 (2.7) 22.8 (4.1) 0.25
Vocabulary WAIS-III a 11.4 (2.4) 11.5 (2.1) 12.6 (2.0) 0.30
Gender
Male 33 (55.0) 69 (68.2) 49 (52.7) 0.11
Female 27 (45.0) 41 (37.3) 44 (47.3)
University Degree b
Yes 41 (68.3) 75 (68.2) 83 (90.2) 0.30
No 19 (31.7) 35 (31.8) 9 (9.8)
Employment Status
Employed 17 (28.3) 29 (26.6) 26 (28.3) 0.04
Unemployed 13 (21.7) 24 (22.0) 13 (14.1)
Student 30 (50.0) 56 (51.4) 53 (57.6)
Smoker
Current Smoker 46 (76.7) 70 (63.6) 17 (18.7) 0.69
Non smoker/Ex-smoker 14 (23.3) 40 (36.4) 74 (81.3)
Age at first tobacco use a 16.4 (3.4) 18.5 (3.1) 18.1 (3.3) 0.34
Cigarettes per day a 11.1 (5.9) 8.9 (6.3) 6.8 (5.3) 0.36
Age at first alcohol use a 14.5 (1.8) 14.8 (1.4) 15.9 (1.4) 0.48
Years of alcohol consumption a 8.7 (3.0) 6.7 (2.8) 6.8 (4.4) 0.32
Age at first cannabis use a 15.6 (2.0) 15.5 (1.6) 0.02
Years of cannabis consumption a 7.7 (2.9) 6.1 (2.8) 0.56
Age at first MDMA use a 18.0 (2.9)
Years of MDMA consumption a 5.2 (3.2)
a

Mean (SD).

b

Including students.

c

Standardized mean difference.

Genotype distributions

Genotype distributions by group are presented in Table 2. The tests of Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium among the three different groups showed equilibrium for all genotypes except for the COMT val158met polymorphism in the cannabis group (p<0.01).

Table 2. Genotype distributions of the participants.

MDMA heavy MDMA light MDMA Cannabis Control p-valuea
5HTTLPR
L/L 7 (25.0) 9 (28.1) 16 (26.7) 31 (28.2) 29 (31.2) 0.121*0.280
L/S 12 (42.9) 12 (37.5) 24 (40.0) 58 (52.7) 49 (52.7)
S/S 9 (32.1) 11 (34.4) 20 (33.3) 21 (19.1) 15 (16.1)
5HTTLPR+rs25531 (n = 259)
High (La/La) 5 (17.9) 9 (29.0) 14 (23.7) 25 (23.4) 21 (22.6) 0.478*0.605
Medium (La/Lg+La/S) 13 (46.4) 12 (38.7) 25 (42.4) 55 (51.4) 52 (55.9)
Low (Lg/Lg+Lg/S+S/S) 10 (35.7) 10 (32.3) 20 (33.9) 27 (25.2) 20 (21.5)
rs25531 (n = 259)
A/A - - 53 (89.8) 93 (86.1) 77 (83.7) 0.568
G - - 6 (10.2) 15 (13.9) 15 (16.3)
5HT2A receptor his452tyr (n = 259)
His/His 21 (77.8) 21 (65.6) 42 (71.2) 59 (54.1) 59 (63.4) 0.598*0.529
His/Tyr 6 (22.2) 11 (34.4) 17 (28.8) 50 (45.9) 34 (36.6)
5HT2A receptor T102C
T/T - - 9 (15.3) 25 (22.9) 17 (18.7) 0.448
T/C - - 28 (47.5) 45 (41.3) 48 (52.7)
C/C - - 22 (37.3) 39 (35.8) 26 (28.6)
BDNF val66met (n = 262)
val/val 22 (78.6) 20 (62.5) 42 (70.0) 59 (54.1) 59 (63.4) 0.109*0.109
met 6 (21.4) 12 (37.5) 18 (30.0) 50 (45.9) 34 (36.6)
GRIN2B C2664T (n = 259)
C/C 19 (70.4) 19 (59.4) 38 (64.4) 57 (52.3) 54 (59.3) 0.288*0.360
T 8 (29.6) 13 (40.6) 21 (35.6) 52 (47.7) 37 (40.7)
COMT val158met
val/val 12 (42.9) 9 (28.1) 21 (35.0) 26 (23.6) 27 (29.0) 0.069*0.037
val/met 13 (46.4) 13 (40.6) 26 (43.3) 70 (63.6) 45 (48.4)
met/met 3 (10.7) 10 (31.2) 13 (21.7) 14 (12.7) 21 (22.6)
CYP2D6
Poor/Intermediate - - 9 (16.4) 14 (14.4) 13 (16.2) 0.928
Extensive/ultra-rapid - - 46 (83.6) 83 (85.6) 67 (83.8)
Genotype Combinations
5HTTLPR + COMT val158met
L+met - - 24 (40.0) 68 (61.8) 57 (61.3) 0.075
L+val/val - - 16 (26.7) 21 (19.1) 21 (22.6)
S/S+met - - 15 (25.0) 16 (14.5) 9 (9.7)
S/S+val/val - - 5 (8.3) 5 (4.5) 6 (6.5)
5HTTLPR + BDNF val66met (n = 262)
L+met - - 13 (21.7) 41 (37.6) 27 (29.0) 0.042
L+val/val - - 27 (45.0) 47 (43.1) 51 (22.6)
S/S+met - - 5 (8.3) 9 (8.3) 7 (7.5)
S/S+val/val - - 15 (25.0) 12 (11.0) 8 (8.6)
5HTTLPR + 5HT2A his452Tyr (n = 259)
L+His/His - - 27 (45.8) 70 (64.2) 56 (61.5) 0.070
L+Tyr - - 21 (20.3) 18 (16.5) 20 (22.0)
S/S - - 20 (33.9) 21 (19.3) 15 (16.5)

Results as the number of subjects (n) and percentage per genotype (%).

a

The first of both values corresponds to the comparison of ecstasy users, cannabis users, and controls. The second (with an *) corresponds to the comparison of heavy ecstasy users, light ecstasy users, cannabis users, and controls.

No significant differences were observed in the genotype distributions among the different groups, except for the 5HTTLPR and the COMT val158met polymorphisms. Significant differences were observed regarding the genotype distributions for the COMT val158met polymorphism (p<0.05) when the distinction between heavy and light MDMA users was taken into account. There were a higher number of individuals with the val/val genotype in the heavy MDMA users group (42.9%) compared to the control group (29.0%). The val/met genotype was also overrepresented in the cannabis group (63.6%) compared to the control group (48.4%) (p<0.05).

Correlations between neuropsychological variables and MDMA/cannabis lifetime consumption

MDMA lifetime use showed a significant negative association with ROCFT Copy Accuracy (r = −0.604, IC95%: [−0.744, −0.413]), Immediate Recall (r = −0.391, IC95%: [−0.587, −0.152]), and Delayed Recall (r = −0.464, IC95%: [−0.642, −0.238]). MDMA lifetime use was also significantly correlated with SDMT (r = −0.269, IC95%: [−0.489, −0.016]). No other correlations reached statistical significance.

Neuropsychological performance by group and genotype analyses

Results are presented in tables 3 and 4.

Table 3. Neuropsychological performance as a function of drug use status. Results are presented as mean (standard deviation).

Group Ecstasy heavy (n = 28) Ecstasy light (n = 32) Cannabis (n = 110) Control (n = 93) p-value
CVLT
Immediate Recall 11.3 (2.5) 11.6 (2.6) 11.6 (2.5) 12.5 (2.1) 0.301
Delayed Recall 11.5 (2.3) 11.7 (2.4) 11.6 (2.6) 12.6 (2.2) 0.221
Total Recognition 14.5 (0.9) 14.2 (0.9) 14.1 (1.2) 14.4 (1.0) 0.279
Total A1–A5 50.9 (8.3) 53.5 (8.6) 52.8 (8.3) 55.8 (6.7) 0.211
ROCFT
Copy Accuracy 31.6 (3.3) 34.1 (1.9) 34.5 (2.5) 34.5 (2.2) 0.000
Immediate Recall 20.5 (5.6) 24.7 (4.5) 24.9 (5.2) 25.2 (4.9) 0.001
Delayed Recall 20.6 (5.5) 24.4 (4.4) 24.9 (5.0) 24.7 (4.9) 0.002
SDMT 55.8 (10.1) 59.2 (11.9) 59.7 (9.2) 61.6 (10.4) 0.185
Semantic Fluency 23.6 (3.5) 23.8 (5.4) 24.7 (5.6) 26.3 (6.3) 0.218

Table 4. Neuropsychological performance as a function of gene x drug use interaction.

Groups
Ecstasy heavy (n = 28) Ecstasy light (n = 32) Cannabis (n = 110) Control (n = 93) p-value
ROCFT – copy accuracy
COMT val158met
val/val 32.2 (3.4) 33.2 (1.8) 33.5 (3.8) 35.1 (1.5) 0.020
val/met 31.1 (3.5) 34.5 (1.9) 34.7 (1.9) 34.7 (1.6)
met/met 31.7 (2.1) 34.3 (1.9) 35.1 (1.8) 33.2 (3.2)
COMT val158met and 5HTTLPR genotypes combinations
L+met 32.8 (2.8)* 34.8 (1.9) 34.2 (2.4) 0.014
L+val/val 33.2 (2.3)* 33.0 (4.1) 34.9 (1.7)
S/S+met 33.6 (3.2)* 34.9(1.7) 34.6 (1.4)
S/S+val/val 30.8 (3.7)* 35.6 (0.9) 35.8 (0.4)
ROCFT – immediate recall
COMT val158met and 5HTTLPR genotypes combinations
L+met 23.0 (5.4)* 24.2 (5.3) 24.6 (5.2) 0.015
L+val/val 22.6 (5.4)* 26.5 (5.0) 26.9 (4.2)
S/S+met 24.2 (5.2)* 26.4 (4.3) 22.2 (3.7)
S/S+val/val 17.5 (4.1)* 23.6 (5.0) 29.4 (1.6)
ROCFT – delayed recall
COMT val158met and 5HTTLPR genotypes combinations
L+met 22.6 (5.3)* 24.2 (5.3) 24.3 (5.1) 0.011
L+val/val 22.6 (5.4)* 25.8 (4.8) 26.4 (4.1)
S/S+met 24.5 (4. 9)* 26.6 (4.1) 21.1 (3.5)
S/S+val/val 18.0 (3.8)* 24.9 (4.2) 28.6 (2.5)
SDMT
5HTTLPR
L/L 56.7 (9.2) 57.2 (7.5) 57.1 (9.5) 62.1 (11.1) 0.016
L/S 59.2 (10.6) 62.8 (14.1) 60.0 (8.4) 59.7 (9.1)
S/S 50.4 (8.6) 56.7 (12.1) 62.6 (10.2) 66.9 (11.9)
COMT val158met and 5HTTLPR genotypes combinations
L+met 58.5 (10.6)* 58.4 (9.0) 60.3 (10.7) 0.001
L+val/val 60.9 (11.5)* 61.0 (8.2) 61.5 (7.3)
S/S+met 57.5 (9.6)* 63.9 (10.1) 62.7 (6.0)
S/S+val/val 43.2 (7.1)* 58.4 (10.8) 73.2 (16.0)
Semantic Fluency
CYP2D6 Phenotype
Intermediate/Poor 21.7 (4.7)* 25.7 (6.0) 22.5 (6.6) 0.047
Ultra-rapid/Extensive 24.0 (4.7)* 25.0 (5.6) 27.5 (5.9)

*Refers to the MDMA group irrespective of the lifetime consumption (n = 60).

Results are presented as mean (standard deviation).

Verbal Memory (CVLT)

We found no effects of group, genotype or the group x genotype interaction on performance indices from this test.

Visual Memory (ROFCT)

Copy Accuracy

We found a main group effect; paired contrasts indicated that heavy MDMA users had lower scores than light MDMA users (t = 4.1, df = 58, p<0.001), cannabis users (t = 5.6, df = 136, p<0.0001), and non-users (t = 5.7, df = 119, p<0.0001).

We observed a significant group x genotype interaction for the COMT val158met (p<0.05). When the val/met genotype was examined, heavy MDMA users with this genotype had lower scores than light MDMA users (t = 4.7, df = 58, p<0.0001), cannabis users (t = 5.9, df = 136, p<0.0001) or non-users (t = 6.1, df = 119, p<0.0001) with the same genotype.

When considering the combination of the COMT val158met and 5HTTLPR genotypes, we also found an effect of the group x genotype interaction (F = 2.7, df1 = 6, df2 = 249, p = 0.014). MDMA users carrying the S/S+val/val genotype scored poorer than non-users carrying the same genotype (t = 3.5, df = 9, p = 0.01). In addition, concerning the L+met genotype, MDMA users scored poorer than Cannabis users (t = 3.3, df = 90, p = 0.001).

Immediate Recall

We found a main effect of group, with heavy MDMA users having significantly lower scores than light MDMA users (t = 3.2, df = 60, p<0.01), cannabis users (t = 4, df = 136, p<0.001), and non-users (t = 3.7, df = 119, p<0.01).

When examining the combination of the COMT val158met and 5HTTLPR genotypes, we found a group x genotype effect (F = 2.7, df1 = 6, DF2 = 249, p = 0.015); MDMA individuals with the S/S+val/val genotype had significantly lower scores than non-users carrying the same genotype (t = 4.5, df = 9, p<0.01).

Delayed recall

We found a main group effect (p<0.01), with heavy MDMA users having significantly lower scores than cannabis users (t = 3.9, df = 136, p<0.001), and non-users (t = 3.2, df = 119, p<0.01). We also found a main effect of 5HT2A genotype, indicating that individuals carrying the His/Tyr variant had significantly poorer performance than those with the His/His genotype (t = 2.4, df = 252, p = 0.01).

The study of the interaction between the COMT val158met and 5HTTLPR genotypes showed an effect of the group x genotype interaction (p = 0.01). Pairwise comparisons showed that MDMA users carrying the S/S+val/val combination had significantly lower scores than non-users (t = 4.2, df = 9, p<0.01).

Attention/Speed (SDMT)

We found no significant main effect of group on performance in this test.

We found a significant group x 5HTTLPR genotype interaction (F = 2.7, df1 = 6, df2 = 249, p = 0.016), with heavy MDMA users carrying the S/S genotype performing poorer than S/S non-users (t = 3.1, df = 33, p = 0.01).

In addition, we observed a significant group x genotype interaction for the combination of 5HTTLPR and the COMT val158met genotypes (F = 4.1, df1 = 6, df2 = 249, p = 0.001). Pairwise comparisons showed that MDMA users carrying the S/S+val/val combination performed significantly more poorly than MDMA L+val/val carriers (t = 3.2, df = 19, p<0.01).

Semantic Fluency

The semantic word fluency was unaffected by group or genotypes alone. However, there was a significant effect of the group x CYP2D6 phenotype interaction (F = 3.1, df1 = 2, df2 = 224, p<0.05). In non-users, individuals who were Intermediate/Poor for the CYP2D6 performed worse than those who were Ultra-rapid/Extensive (t = 2.8, df = 78, p<0.01). In contrast, MDMA users with the Ultra-rapid/Extensive phenotype had significantly lower scores than those with the same phenotype in the non-user group (t = 2.8, df = 111, p<0.01).

Discussion

Our findings show detrimental effects of both MDMA lifetime use and variations in candidate genes on a number of neuropsychological measures, with particular relevance of visuospatial attention and memory. With respect to dose-related effects, we found that greater lifetime use of MDMA is negatively correlated with performance on visuospatial memory (ROCFT) and attention and perceptual speed (SDMT) tests. These results were further supported by group comparisons, which showed that heavy MDMA users (lifetime use>100 tablets) have significantly poorer visuospatial memory than light MDMA users, cannabis users and non-users. Importantly, we found a number of gene x MDMA interaction effects. Results for COMT and SERT genes showed that MDMA users carrying the COMT val/val and SERT S/S genotypes have poorer performance on tests of visuospatial memory and perceptual attention (ROCFT and SDMT). Delayed recall was also modulated by a gene polymorphism in the 5HT2a receptor, such that Tyr carriers had poorer performance irrespective of drug use. Finally, we found an interaction between MDMA use and CYP2D6 extra-high metabolic activity phenotype and lower performance on verbal fluency.

The main MDMA dose-related findings and MDMA x gene interactions were found in the ROCFT. This is a complex task involving visuospatial attention and planning/organization skills during the copy, and planning and episodic memory skills during immediate and delayed recall [42]. Copy performance is associated with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and parietal cortex functioning [43], whereas immediate and delayed recall are associated with the functioning of the DLPFC [44] and the hippocampus [45]. Therefore, our findings point to a substantial impact of MDMA lifetime amount of use on fronto-parietal and fronto-temporal systems supporting attentional and memory functions, in agreement with previous neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings [46][48]. The fact that the larger dose-related correlations are found for the Copy index indicates that MDMA cumulative use may have greater detrimental effects on visuospatial attention and organization skills than on recall per se; this is consistent with the finding that MDMA use is also negatively associated with SDMT-indexed perceptual attention, and with decreased gray matter volumes and SERT availability in posterior brain cortices [48].

The association between MDMA use and perceptual attention and visual memory/organization skills seems to be modulated by COMT and SERT genotypes. Our results consistently showed that MDMA users carrying the COMT val/val and SERT S/S genotypes perform significantly more poorly on ROCFT Copy, Immediate and Delayed Recall, and on SDMT, such that there was consistency between gene x drug effects on different probes involving perceptual attention and visuospatial planning/memory skills. These results fit with evidence showing that the COMT gene is significantly associated with visuospatial planning ability –gene-carriers with high enzymatic activity display poorer performance [49] and reduced DLPFC and parietal activation during planning tasks [50]. They are also in agreement with neuropsychological outcomes in individuals with microdeletion of chromosome 22q11.2 –in which the COMT is one of the genes in the deleted region– displaying robust deficits in the ROCFT [43]. There is also evidence of the influence of the SERT genotype on visuospatial attention/planning performance indexed by a Mental Rotation task after tryptophan depletion –which mimics MDMA-induced serotonin reductions [51]. Furthermore, MDMA chronic use is robustly associated with loss of SERT availability in occipital, hippocampal and parietal regions [48], all networks involved in perceptual/attentional processing and memory. Overall, our results are suggestive of the notion that heavy MDMA use and COMT val/val and SERT S/S genotypes interact to produce greater detrimental effects on perceptual attention and planning/organization, ultimately affecting visuospatial memory skills. However, these results would need follow-up in larger samples, and they need to be interpreted with caution considering that the role of the COMT gene on cognition and brain functioning is still controversial [52], [53]. In the case of delayed recall, performance was further modulated by a 5HT2a gene polymorphism, such that Tyr carriers have poorer performance irrespective of drug use; this is in agreement with previous observations on the association between this genotype and the consolidation process of episodic memory [54].

In agreement with our initial assumptions, MDMA users with CYP2D6 high metabolic activity phenotypes performed more poorly on the semantic fluency test. These results are in agreement with recent findings about the link between higher CYP2D6 activity and impaired executive performance, including semantic fluency, in methamphetamine users [55]. In this case, we extend their findings by showing specific effects of the Ultra-rapid/Extensive phenotype on executive performance in MDMA users, a drug-using group in which greater cognitive dysfunction was expected based on specific pharmacodynamic mechanisms [56]. Furthermore, fluency is one of the executive skills more consistently impaired in MDMA users [57], conferring clinical significance to this gene x drug interaction effect. This finding supports the proposal that CYP2D6 polymorphisms may modulate MDMA-induced neurotoxic effects [17] and subsequent decrements in executive performance. More research including additional executive phenotypes and larger sample sizes are warranted to further substantiate these promising findings. Three other polymorphisms –the BDNF val/met, the 5HT2a T102C, and the GRIN2B C2664T– were explored but failed to show significant results in relation to cognitive performance. Nonetheless, the GRIN2B genotype showed a trend to significance in relation to verbal memory (T allele carriers recalling more words), raising the possibility that effects of these genes on cognition may emerge in larger samples sizes.

In conclusion, this study reliably demonstrates dose-related effects of MDMA use on visual attention, organization and memory. In addition, we show an interaction between MDMA use and different gene polymorphisms in determining poorer performance of MDMA users in tests of visual attention and memory (COMT and SERT genes) and verbal fluency (CYP2D6). Strengths from the study include the successful recruitment of a large number of non-treatment seeking MDMA users who self-report an adequate academic/ professional and social functioning, thus avoiding socio-demographic confounders and approximating research to the reality of MDMA use beyond clinical settings; the carefully conducted medical and psychiatric explorations to discard potential confounders related to physical illness or psychopathology; and the use of well-validated neuropsychological measures taxing key cognitive domains related to MDMA use. Limitations include the relatively small sample size for a genetic study –especially when combining some of the rare genotypes, and the elevated number of statistical comparisons, which may have inflated the risk of Type I error. Nonetheless, we should note that our drugs x gene interaction findings are in agreement with initial assumptions, biologically plausible and overly consistent with previous literature, and consistent across independent but conceptually related neuropsychological probes and indices. Therefore, although these results would need to be further explored and replicated, we arguably reckon that the probability that they stem from false positive effects is low.

Footnotes

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Funding: This study was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NIH) grant no. 1 R01 DA017987, Grant 2005SGR00032, Fondo de Investigaciones Sanitarias (FIS-00/00777), Plan Nacional Sobre Drogas (INT/2012/2002) Spain, Plan Nacional Sobre Drogas: PNSD 2006/101(2007–2009) Spain. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Capela JP, Carmo H, Remiao F, Bastos ML, Meisel A, et al. Molecular and cellular mechanisms of ecstasy-induced neurotoxicity: an overview. Mol Neurobiol. 2009;39:210–271. doi: 10.1007/s12035-009-8064-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Green AR, Mechan AO, Elliott JM, O'Shea E, Colado MI. The pharmacology and clinical pharmacology of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, “ecstasy”). Pharmacol Rev. 2003;55:463–508. doi: 10.1124/pr.55.3.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Biezonski DK, Meyer JS. Effects of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) on serotonin transporter and vesicular monoamine transporter 2 protein and gene expression in rats: implications for MDMA neurotoxicity. J Neurochem. 2010;112:951–962. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2009.06515.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kish SJ, Lerch J, Furukawa Y, Tong J, McCluskey T, et al. Decreased cerebral cortical serotonin transporter binding in ecstasy users: a positron emission tomography/[(11)C]DASB and structural brain imaging study. Brain. 2010;133:1779–1797. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.McCann UD, Szabo Z, Vranesic M, Palermo M, Mathews WB, et al. Positron emission tomographic studies of brain dopamine and serotonin transporters in abstinent (+/−)-3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (“ecstasy”) users: relationship to cognitive performance. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;200:439–450. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1218-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Buchert R, Thomasius R, Wilke F, Petersen K, Nebeling B, et al. A voxel-based PET investigation of the long-term effects of “Ecstasy” consumption on brain serotonin transporters. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1181–1189. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.7.1181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Thomasius R, Zapletalova P, Petersen K, Buchert R, Andresen B, et al. Mood, cognition and serotonin transporter availability in current and former ecstasy (MDMA) users: the longitudinal perspective. J Psychopharmacol. 2006;20:211–225. doi: 10.1177/0269881106059486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kish SJ, Fitzmaurice PS, Chang LJ, Furukawa Y, Tong J. Low striatal serotonin transporter protein in a human polydrug MDMA (ecstasy) user: a case study. J Psychopharmacol. 2010;24:281–284. doi: 10.1177/0269881108097724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Krebs TS, Johansen PO, Jerome L, Halpern JH. Importance of psychiatric confounding in non-randomized studies of heavy ecstasy users. Psychol Med. 2009;39:876–878. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708005096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kalechstein AD, de la Garza R, Mahoney JJ, Fantegrossi WE, Newton TF. MDMA use and neurocognition: a meta-analytic review. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;189:531–537. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0601-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Zakzanis KK, Campbell Z, Jovanovski D. The neuropsychology of ecstasy (MDMA) use: a quantitative review. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2007;22:427–435. doi: 10.1002/hup.873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Medina KL, Shear PK, Corcoran K. Ecstasy (MDMA) exposure and neuropsychological functioning: a polydrug perspective. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11:753–765. doi: 10.1017/S1355617705050915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Schilt T, de Win MM, Jager G, Koeter MW, Ramsey NF, et al. Specific effects of ecstasy and other illicit drugs on cognition in poly-substance users. Psychol Med. 2008;38:1309–1317. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707002140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Bedi G, Redman J. Ecstasy use and higher-level cognitive functions: weak effects of ecstasy after control for potential confounds. Psychol Med. 2008;38:1319–1330. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708002730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Verdejo-Garcia A, Lawrence AJ, Clark L. Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32:777–810. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.de la Torre R, Farre M, Roset PN, Pizarro N, Abanades S, et al. Human pharmacology of MDMA: pharmacokinetics, metabolism, and disposition. Ther Drug Monit. 2004;26:137–144. doi: 10.1097/00007691-200404000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Perfetti X, O'Mathuna B, Pizarro N, Cuyàs E, Khymenets O, et al. Neurotoxic thioether adducts of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine identified in human urine after ecstasy ingestion. Drug Metab Dispos. 2009;37:1448–1455. doi: 10.1124/dmd.108.026393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Farre M, de la Torre R, O'Mathuna B, Roset PN, Peiro AM, et al. Repeated doses administration of MDMA in humans: pharmacological effects and pharmacokinetics. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;173:364–375. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1789-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Seripa D, Matera MG, Franceschi M, Bizzarro A, Paris F, et al. Association analysis of GRIN2B, encoding N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor 2B subunit, and Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;25:287–292. doi: 10.1159/000118634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ludwig KU, Roeske D, Herms S, Schumacher J, Warnke A, et al. Variation in GRIN2B contributes to weak performance in verbal short-term memory in children with dyslexia. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2010;153B:503–511. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Stein J, Luppa M, Brahler E, Konig HH, Riedel-Heller SG. The assessment of changes in cognitive functioning: reliable change indices for neuropsychological instruments in the elderly - a systematic review. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010;29:275–286. doi: 10.1159/000289779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Schilt T, Koeter MW, de Win MM, Zinkstok JR, van Amelsvoort TA, et al. The effect of Ecstasy on memory is moderated by a functional polymorphism in the cathechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2009;19:116–124. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2008.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Tunbridge EM, Harrison PJ, Weinberger DR. Catechol-o-methyltransferase, cognition, and psychosis: Val158Met and beyond. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:141–151. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.10.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ullsperger M. Genetic association studies of performance monitoring and learning from feedback: the role of dopamine and serotonin. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34:649–659. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Quednow BB, Jessen F, Kuhn KU, Maier W, Daum I, et al. Memory deficits in abstinent MDMA (ecstasy) users: neuropsychological evidence of frontal dysfunction. J Psychopharmacol. 2006;20:373–384. doi: 10.1177/0269881106061200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Brown J, McKone E, Ward J. Deficits of long-term memory in ecstasy users are related to cognitive complexity of the task. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010;209:51–67. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1766-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Parrott AC, Buchanan T, Scholey AB, Heffernan T, Ling J, et al. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2002;17:309–12. doi: 10.1002/hup.415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Torrens M, Serrano D, Astals M, Perez-Dominguez G, Martin-Santos R. Diagnosing comorbid psychiatric disorders in substance abusers: validity of the Spanish versions of the Psychiatric Research Interview for Substance and Mental Disorders and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1231–1237. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.7.1231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.de la Torre R, Civit E, Svaizer F, Lotti A, Gottardi M, et al. High throughput analysis of drugs of abuse in hair by combining purposely designed sample extraction compatible with immunometric methods used for drug testing in urine. Forensic Sci Int. 2010;196:18–21. doi: 10.1016/j.forsciint.2009.12.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Pichini S, Poudevida S, Pujadas M, Menoyo E, Pacifici R, et al. Assessment of chronic exposure to MDMA in a group of consumers by segmental hair analysis. Ther Drug Monit. 2006;28:106–109. doi: 10.1097/01.ftd.0000189900.01060.92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-III): Administration and Scoring Manual 3. Psychological Corporation: San Antonio; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Delis D, Kramer J, Kaplan E, Ober B. California Verbal Learning Test: Manual, 2nd ed. The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Rey A. L'examen psychologique dans les cas d'encephalopathie traumatique. Arch Psychol. 1941;28:286–340. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Smith R. Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) Manual (Revised) Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Benton A, Hamsher K. Multilingual Aphasia Examination. AJA Associates: Iowa City; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.de Sola Llopis S, Miguelez-Pan M, Peña-Casanova J, Poudevida S, Farre, et al. Cognitive performance in recreational ecstasy polydrug users: a two-year follow-up study. J Psychopharmacol. 2008;22:498–510. doi: 10.1177/0269881107081545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Fagundo AB, Cuyàs E, Verdejo-García A, Khymenets O, Langohr K, et al. The influence of 5-HTT and COMT genotypes on verbal fluency in ecstasy users. J Psychopharmacol. 2010;24:1381–1393. doi: 10.1177/0269881109354926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.De Luca V, Tharmalingam S, King N, Strauss J, Bulgin N, et al. Association study of a novel functional polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene in bipolar disorder and suicidal behaviour. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;182:128–131. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0046-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Cuyàs E, Olano-Martin E, Khymenets O, Hernandez L, Jofre-Monseny L, et al. Errors and reproducibility of DNA array-based detection of allelic variants in ADME genes: PHARMAchip. Pharmacogenomics. 2010;11:257–266. doi: 10.2217/pgs.09.165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Martocchio J. Research in Personnel and Human Resources Management. 2009. Emerald Group Publishing Limited, Cornell University, Volume 28.
  • 41.Hothorn T, Bretz F, Westfall P. Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biom J. 2008;50:346–363. doi: 10.1002/bimj.200810425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Shin MS, Park SY, Park SR, Seol SH, Kwon JS. Clinical and empirical applications of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test. Nat Protoc. 2006;1:892–899. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Antshel KM, Peebles J, AbdulSabur N, Higgins AM, Roizen N, et al. Associations between performance on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure and regional brain volumes in children with and without velocardiofacial syndrome. Dev Neuropsychol. 2008;33:601–622. doi: 10.1080/87565640802254422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Haroon E, Watari K, Thomas A, Ajilore O, Mintz J, et al. Prefrontal myo-inositol concentration and visuospatial functioning among diabetic depressed patients. Psychiatry Res. 2009;171:10–19. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Carlesimo GA, Cherubini A, Caltagirone C, Spalletta G. Hippocampal mean diffusivity and memory in healthy elderly individuals: a cross-sectional study. Neurology. 2010;74:194–200. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181cb3e39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Daumann J, Fimm B, Willmes K, Thron A, Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E. Cerebral activation in abstinent ecstasy (MDMA) users during a working memory task: a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2003;16:479–487. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(03)00075-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.de Win MM, Reneman L, Jager G, Vlieger EJ, Olabarriaga SD, et al. A prospective cohort study on sustained effects of low-dose ecstasy use on the brain in new ecstasy users. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:458–470. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Kish SJ, Lerch J, Furukawa Y, Tong J, McCluskey T, et al. Decreased cerebral cortical serotonin transporter binding in ecstasy users: a positron emission tomography/[(11)C]DASB and structural brain imaging study. Brain. 2010;133:1779–1797. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Roussos P, Giakoumaki SG, Pavlakis S, Bitsios P. Planning, decision-making and the COMT rs4818 polymorphism in healthy males. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:757–763. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Williams-Gray CH, Hampshire A, Robbins TW, Owen AM, Barker RA. Catechol O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype influences frontoparietal activity during planning in patients with Parkinson's disease. J Neurosci. 2007;27:4832–4838. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0774-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Roiser JP, Muller U, Clark L, Sahakian BJ. The effects of acute tryptophan depletion and serotonin transporter polymorphism on emotional processing in memory and attention. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2007;10:449–461. doi: 10.1017/S146114570600705X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Barnett JH, Scoriels L, Munafo MR. Meta-analysis of the cognitive effects of the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene Val158/108Met polymorphism. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64:137–144. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Dennis NA, Need AC, LaBar KS, Waters-Metenier S, Cirulli ET, et al. COMT val108/158 met genotype affects neural but not cognitive processing in healthy individuals. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20:672–683. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Wagner M, Schuhmacher A, Schwab S, Zobel A, Maier W. The His452Tyr variant of the gene encoding the 5-HT2A receptor is specifically associated with consolidation of episodic memory in humans. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;11:1163–1167. doi: 10.1017/S146114570800905X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Cherner M, Bousman C, Everall I, Barron D, Letendre S, et al. Cytochrome P450-2D6 extensive metabolizers are more vulnerable to methamphetamine-associated neurocognitive impairment: preliminary findings. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16:890–901. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.de la Torre R, Farre M, O'Mathuna B, Roset PN, Pizarro N, et al. MDMA (ecstasy) pharmacokinetics in a CYP2D6 poor metaboliser and in nine CYP2D6 extensive metabolisers. Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 2005;61:551–554. doi: 10.1007/s00228-005-0965-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Fernandez-Serrano MJ, Perez-Garcia M, Verdejo-Garcia A. What are the specific vs. generalized effects of drugs of abuse on neuropsychological performance? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:377–406. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES