Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: Schizophr Res. 2011 Sep 9;133(1-3):156–158. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.08.014

Administration of Kynurenine during Adolescence, but not during Adulthood, Impairs Social Behavior in Rats

Katelyn V Trecartin 1, David J Bucci 1
PMCID: PMC3225639  NIHMSID: NIHMS321136  PMID: 21907545

Abstract

Kynurenic acid (KYNA) is a tryptophan metabolite that is present at high concentrations in the brains of persons with schizophrenia. This study tested the hypothesis that treatment with l-kynurenine (L-KYN), which increases KYNA concentration, would produce deficits in social behavior similar to those associated with schizophrenia. Rats treated with L-KYN throughout adolescence exhibited decreased social interaction when tested drug-free as adults. In contrast, neither acute nor chronic treatment during adulthood affected social behavior. These findings demonstrate that increases in KYNA concentration produce deficits in social behavior and that the adolescent brain is particularly susceptible to the effects of high KYNA concentration.

Keywords: l-kynurenine, schizophrenia, glutamate, acetylcholine, glia

1. Introduction

Although neuroleptic drugs are often successful in treating positive symptoms of schizophrenia (e.g., delusions, hallucinations), negative symptoms such as social withdrawal are less responsive to current drug therapies (Blin, 1999). Moreover, little is known about the neurobiological substrates of impaired social behavior. The present study tested the hypothesis that increased concentration of kynurenic acid (KYNA) contributes to deficits in social behavior. KYNA is a tryptophan metabolite that is increased in the brains of persons with schizophrenia (Schwarcz et al., 2001). Interestingly, KYNA is synthesized and released by astrocytes and acts as an endogenous antagonist of both NMDA glutamate receptors and α7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (Stone, 1993; Hilmas et al., 2001). Both receptors are critically involved in cognitive function (Bast et al., 2003; Gould and Higgins, 2003) and various studies have shown that pharmacological blockade of NMDA receptors induces social withdrawal (Sams-Dodd, 1999).

Rats were treated with 100mg/kg of l-kynurenine (L-KYN; the precursor of KYNA), which results in a 3–4 fold increase in KYNA concentration (Erhardt et al., 2004), consistent with the increase observed in persons with schizophrenia (Erhardt et al., 2001; Schwarcz et al., 2001). Importantly, rats were treated chronically throughout adolescence since genotypic alterations may underlie changes in KYNA concentration associated with schizophrenia (Miller et al. 2004; 2006; 2008; 2009) and thus expose the brain to high levels of KYNA during this critical stage of development. Indeed, there is substantial evidence that schizophrenia often develops during adolescence (Harrop and Trower, 2001). It is well established that NMDA receptors and α7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors are involved in synaptic plasticity and neural development (Brodie and Leslie, 1999; Komuro and Rakic, 1993), and we have recently shown that increased KYNA concentration throughout adolescence has lasting impacts on cognitive function later in adulthood (Akagbosu et al., 2010). Thus, we expected that social behavior would be impaired in rats treated with L-KYN throughout adolescence.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Subjects

Male Long Evans rats were obtained from Harlan Laboratories (Indianapolis, IN) and maintained in groups of 4 on a 12:12 light-dark cycle with food (Purina standard rat chow; Nestle Purina, St. Louis, MO) and water available ad libitum. All rats were allowed 6 days to acclimate to the vivarium before any treatment began and maintained according to AAALAC guidelines.

2.2. Drug preparation

L-kynurenine (L-KYN; Sigma, St Louis, MO) was prepared fresh daily as described previously (Chess et al., 2009).

2.3. Experimental design and treatment regimen

Experiment 1 (adolescent chronic group)

Sixteen rats were treated chronically with L-KYN or vehicle solution (0.1 M HEPES) beginning on postnatal day 27 and continuing through postnatal day 53, using a 3-day-on/3-day-off treatment regimen described previously (Akagbosu et al., 2010). This procedure reduces distress and also minimizes the potential for metabolic adaptations observed following daily L-KYN treatment (Vecsei et al., 1992). Using this procedure, KYNA concentration is increased 4-fold on days when rats are treated with L-KYN (Akagbosu et al., 2010). The social interaction task was carried out 8 days after the last injection (i.e., postnatal day 61), at which time KYNA concentration was no longer elevated (Akagbosu et al., 2010).

Experiment 2 (adult chronic group)

Sixteen adult rats were treated as described in Experiment 1 (8/group), beginning at 8 weeks of age. Thus, behavioral testing occurred at ~84 days of age.

Experiment 3 (acute treatment group)

Another set of 8 week old rats received a single injection of L-KYN (or vehicle) 2 hours before the social interaction task. This group of rats was tested at 61 days of age, like those in Experiment 1.

2.4. Behavioral Apparatus

The social interaction task was conducted in a white plastic tub measuring 119.4 cm x 59.7 cm x 59.7 cm and located in a small, dimly lit room. A clear Plexiglas cylinder (27.9 cm long x 7.6 cm diameter) was located in the center of the tub and used to separate the target rat from the experimental rat during the test session. There were five holes (1.9cm) on each side of the cylinder, two holes on top, and one hole on either end.

2.5. Behavioral Procedures

Social interaction was assessed using a procedure adapted from File and colleagues (File, 1980; File and Seth, 2003) as described previously (Hopkins et al., 2009). Briefly, an unfamiliar ‘target’ rat was placed in the cylindrical apparatus inside the center of the tub and then an ‘experimental rat’ that had previously been treated with vehicle or L-KYN was placed in the right-hand corner of the tub. The experimenter left the room and the rat was allowed 10 min to explore the tub, after which the rats were removed and all surfaces cleaned with disinfectant.

2.6. Behavioral observations

2.6.1. Social behavior

The primary measure of social interaction was the number of times the experimental rat approached and sniffed inside a hole in the cylinder that contained the target rat. Walking around and continuously sniffing the cylinder was also counted as interaction. Behavioral observations were conducted by an observer who was blind to the identity of the experimental rats using videotapes of the session.

2.6.2. Locomotor behavior

The videotape of the social interaction session was scored a second time to assess locomotor activity by drawing two lines perpendicular to the long sides of tub on the video screen, dividing it into 3 equal areas. An observer blind to condition counted the number of times each rat crossed one of the lines. Assessing locomotor activity was important because any group differences that emerged in social behavior could merely be due to treatment-related changes in locomotor activity. Moreover, measuring locomotor behavior during the social interaction test provided a direct task-relevant measure of activity.

2.7. Data analysis

The number of interactions or line crossings was analyzed using independent measures t- tests. All statistical analyses were conducted using an alpha level of 0.05.

3. Results

The amount of time rats engaged in social interaction is illustrated in Figure 1. Rats treated with L-KYN during adolescence exhibited significantly less social interaction compared to vehicle-treated rats when they were tested as adults [t(14)=2.5, p < 0.03]. In contrast, neither chronic nor acute treatment with L-KYN during adulthood had an effect on social interaction (ps > 0.9). The amount of locomotor activity exhibited during the social interaction procedure is shown in Table 1. Importantly, locomotor activity did not differ between the L-KYN and vehicle groups in any of the experiments (ps > 0.1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Chronic treatment with L-KYN during adolescence decreased the amount of time rats interacted with an unfamiliar rats when they were later tested drug-free as adults. In contrast, neither chronic nor acute exposure during adulthood impaired social behavior. Data are means ± SEM.

Table 1.

Number of line crossings during the social interaction task

Vehicle L-KYN
Adolescent chronic 34 ± 4.4 41.8 ± 1.4
Adult Chronic 41.9 ± 1.5 40.9 ± 2.7
Adult Acute 25.3 ± 3.2 31.1 ± 4.8

4. Discussion

Here we demonstrate that treatment with L-KYN, which has been shown to increase KYNA levels, decreases social behavior in rats in consistent with the observed social dysfunction in persons with schizophrenia (Sams-Dodd, 1999). Moreover, the adolescent brain may be particularly sensitive to the effects of increased KYNA concentration. Indeed, rats that were treated chronically as adults did not exhibit any impairment in social behavior. Together these findings indicate that prolonged antagonism of NMDA and/or α7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors during development can lead to social impairments later in adulthood.

It is unlikely that the observed deficits in social interaction were merely due to drug-induced changes in locomotor behavior, since activity levels were comparable in rats treated with L-KYN and control rats in each experiment (consistent with prior data; Akagbosu et al., 2010). Likewise, the decrease in social interaction in rats treated during adolescence was not due to high levels of KYNA at the time of testing since rats were tested 8 days after the final injection and we have previously shown that KYNA levels are no longer elevated at that time (Akagbosu et al., 2010). Moreover, at the time of behavioral testing, the rats treated during adolescence were the same age as rats in the adult-acute treatment group, which did not exhibit any changes in social behavior. These findings further support the notion that the adolescent brain may be hypersensitive to L-KYN treatment.

It is possible that the effects of L-KYN administration could be due to changes in the concentration of kynurenine metabolites other than KYNA. However, previous data indicate that a higher dose of L-KYN (150mg/kg compared to 100mg/kg in our study) failed to significantly increase levels of quinolinic acid, for example (Shepard et al., 2003). Nevertheless, this is a question that should be addressed further in future studies. Similarly, it is of interest that the three groups of rats used in this study had different baseline levels of social interaction. Although this may simply be due to age-related differences in exploratory activity (Stansfield and Kirstein, 2006) or the effects of acute versus repeated injection stress, it would be of interest to further investigate interactions between variable such as age and gender with regard to the effects of L-KYN administration on social behavior.

In summary, the present findings add to a growing literature indicating that increased concentration of KYNA leads to cognitive deficits commonly associated with schizophrenia. Deficits in spatial working memory, sensory gating, contextual memory, and attention have been observed after acute treatment with L-KYN (Chess and Bucci, 2006; Chess et al., 2007, 2009; Erhardt et al. 2004; Shepard et al., 2003). Other cognitive domains, such as object memory (Akagbosu et al., 2010) and social behavior (present report), are insensitive to acute elevations in KYNA, but are impaired in drug-free adults that had been previously exposed to KYNA during development. This differential pattern of effects following exposure to KYNA at different ages and for different durations may provide new insight into the neural substrates underlying cognitive and social dysfunction in schizophrenia.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Lan Guo and Gretchen Evans for assistance in collecting some of the data.

Role of Funding Source. Funding for this study was provided by NIMH Grant R01DA027688 (DJB) and a David C. Hodgson Undergraduate Research Award (KVT). The NIMH had no further role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Footnotes

Contributors. Both authors participated in the design of the study, writing the protocol, conducting literatures searches and data analyses, and writing drafts of the manuscript. Both authors contributed to and have approved the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest. Both authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Akagbosu CO, Evans GC, Gulick D, Suckow RF, Bucci DJ. Exposure to kynurenic acid during adolescence produces memory deficits in adulthood. Schizophrenia Bulletin. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq151. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bast T, Zhang WN, Feldon J. Dorsal hippocampus and classical fear conditioning to tone and context in rats: Effects of local NMDA-receptor blockade and stimulation. Hippocampus. 2003;13:657–675. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Blin O. A comparative review of new antipsychotics. Can J Psychiatry. 1999;44(3):235–244. doi: 10.1177/070674379904400303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Broide RS, Leslie FM. The alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor in neuronal plasticity. Mol Neurobiol. 1999;20(1):1–16. doi: 10.1007/BF02741361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Chess AC, Landers AM, Bucci DJ. L-kynurenine treatment alters contextual fear conditioning and context discrimination but not cue-specific fear conditioning. Behav Brain Res. 2009;201:325–331. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chess AC, Simoni MK, Alling TE, Bucci DJ. Elevations of endogenous kynurenic acid produce spatial working memory deficits. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33:797–804. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chess AC, Bucci DJ. Increased concentration of cerebral kynurenic acid alters stimulus processing and conditioned responding. Behav Brain Res. 2006;170:326–332. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Erhardt S, Blennow K, Nordin C, Skogh E, Lindstrom LH, Engberg G. Kynurenic acid levels are elevated in the cerebrospinal fluid of patients with schizophrenia. Neurosci Lett. 2001;313:96–98. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(01)02242-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Erhardt S, Schwieler L, Emanuelsson C, Geyer M. Endogenous kynurenic acid disrupts prepulse inhibition. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56:255–260. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. File SE. The use of social interaction as a method for detecting anxiolytic activity of chlordiazepoxide-like drugs. J Neurosci Methods. 1980;2:219–238. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(80)90012-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. File SE, Seth P. A review of 25 years of the social interaction test. Eur J Pharmacol. 2003;463:35–53. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(03)01273-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gould TJ, Higgins JS. Nicotine enhances contextual fear conditioning in C57BL/6J mice at 1 and 7 days post-training. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2003;80:147–157. doi: 10.1016/s1074-7427(03)00057-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Harrop C, Trower P. Why does schizophrenia develop at late adolescence? Clin Psychol Rev. 2001;21:241–65. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(99)00047-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hilmas C, Pereira EFR, Alkondon M, Rassoulpour A, Schwarcz R, Albuquerque EX. The brain metabolite kynurenic acid inhibits alpha7 nicotinic receptor activity and increases non-alpha7 nicotinic receptor expression: Physiopathological implications. J Neurosci. 2001;21:7463–7473. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-19-07463.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hopkins ME, Sharma M, Evans GC, Bucci DJ. Voluntary physical exercise alters attentional orienting and social behavior in rat model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav Neurosci. 2009;123:599–606. doi: 10.1037/a0015632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Komuro H, Rakic P. Modulation of neuronal migration by NMDA receptors. Science. 1993;260(5104):95–7. doi: 10.1126/science.8096653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Miller CL, Llenos IC, Dulay JR, Barillo MM, Yolken RH, Weis S. Expression of the kynurenine pathway enzyme tryptophan 2,3-dioxygenase is increased in the frontal cortex of individuals with schizophrenia. Neurobio of Disease. 2004;15:618–629. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2003.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Miller CL, Llenos IC, Dulay JR, Weis S. Upregulation of the initiating step of the kynurenine pathway in postmortem anterior cingulate cortex from individuals with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Brain Res. 2006;1073–1074:25–37. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Miller CL, Llenos IC, Cwik M, Walkup J, Weis S. Alterations in kynurenine precursor and product levels in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Neurochem Int. 2008;52:1297–303. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2008.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Miller CL, Murakami P, Ruczinski I, Ross RG, Sinkus M, Sullivan B, Leonard S. Two complex genotypes relevant to the kynurenine pathway and melanotropin function show association with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophr Res. 2009;113:259–67. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.05.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Sams-Dodd F. Phencyclidine in the social interaction test: an animal model of schizophrenia with face and predictive validity. Rev Neurosci. 1999;10(1):59–90. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.1999.10.1.59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Schwarcz R, Rassoulpour A, Wu H-Q, Medoff D, Tamminga CA, Roberts RC. Increased cortical kynurenate content in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:521–530. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01078-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Shepard PD, Joy B, Clerkin L, Schwarcz R. Micromolar brain levels of kynurenic acid are associated with a disruption of auditory sensory gating in the rat. Neuropsychopharm. 2003;28:1454–1462. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Stansfield KH, Kirstein CL. Effects of novelty on behavior in the adolescent and adult rat. Dev Psychobiol. 2006;48(1):10–15. doi: 10.1002/dev.20127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Stone TW. Neuropharmacology of quinolinic and kynurenic acids. Pharmacol Rev. 1993;45:309–379. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Vecsei L, Miller J, MacGarvey U, Beal FM. Effects of kynurenine and probenecid on plasma and brain tissue concentrations of kynurenic acid. Neurodegeneration. 1992;1:17–26. [Google Scholar]
  27. Wu HQ, Guidetti P, Goodman JH, Varasi M, Ceresoli-Borroni G, Speciale C, Scharfman HE, Schwarcz R. Kynurenergic manipulations influence excitatory synaptic function and excitotoxic vulnerability in the rat hippocampus in vivo. Neuroscience. 2000;97(2):243–51. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00030-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES