Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2011 May 25;32(6):1268–1278. doi: 10.1007/s10764-011-9524-9

Reaching a Consensus: Terminology and Concepts Used in Coordination and Decision-Making Research

Lennart W Pyritz 1,, Andrew J King 2, Cédric Sueur 3,4, Claudia Fichtel 1
PMCID: PMC3228941  PMID: 22207769

Abstract

Research on coordination and decision-making in humans and nonhuman primates has increased considerably throughout the last decade. However, terminology has been used inconsistently, hampering the broader integration of results from different studies. In this short article, we provide a glossary containing the central terms of coordination and decision-making research. The glossary is based on previous definitions that have been critically revised and annotated by the participants of the symposium “Where next? Coordination and decision-making in primate groups” at the XXIIIth Congress of the International Primatological Society (IPS) in Kyoto, Japan. We discuss a number of conceptual and methodological issues and highlight consequences for their implementation. In summary, we recommend that future studies on coordination and decision-making in animal groups do not use the terms “combined decision” and “democratic/despotic decision-making.” This will avoid ambiguity as well as anthropocentric connotations. Further, we demonstrate the importance of 1) taxon-specific definitions of coordination parameters (initiation, leadership, followership, termination), 2) differentiation between coordination research on individual-level process and group-level outcome, 3) analyses of collective action processes including initiation and termination, and 4) operationalization of successful group movements in the field to collect meaningful and comparable data across different species.

Keywords: Animal groups, Coordination, Decision-making, Terminology

Introduction

The number of studies on group coordination, leadership, and decision-making in humans and nonhuman primates has increased considerably during the last decade (Conradt and Roper 2005; Conradt and List 2009; Fichtel et al. 2011; King et al. 2009; Petit and Bon 2010). However, integration of results from different studies has been impeded by ambiguous terminology and conceptual/methodological shortcomings (see Jacobs 2010; Petit and Bon 2010; Pyritz et al. 2010 for recent discussions). We discussed these inconsistencies with participants during the symposium “Where next? Coordination and decision-making in primate groups” at the XXIIIth Congress of the International Primatological Society (IPS) at Kyoto, Japan, in September 2010. As a result of the strong positive feedback, we decided to initiate a public discussion about these terms via an interactive online platform joined by participants of the symposium and associated researchers. The glossary contained 20 central terms regarding coordination and decision-making research, most of which were previously compiled in a comprehensive review by Conradt and Roper (2005). Each participant had the opportunity to contribute additional terms, definitions, references, and commentaries. Table I provides the final glossary based on the summary of all entries. In addition, we identified a number of conceptual and methodological issues that have been discussed rarely or controversially in the literature so far. We here discuss the most debated terms of the glossary and a number of crucial conceptual issues in more detail.

Table I.

Central terms of coordination and decision-making research and their respective definitions, selected references, and commentaries compiled by participants of the XXIIIth Congress of the International Primatological Society (IPS) in Kyoto, Japan

Term Definition References (selection) Comments
Combined decision Members of a group choose individually (but not necessarily independently) between ≥2 actions. They do not aim for consensus, but the combined result of their decisions usually affects the group as a whole. Conradt and Roper 2005 The distinction between a quorum threshold, e.g., in ants or Tonkean macaques, and a combined decision seems inconclusive. See text for detailed discussion.
Consensus decision Group members choose between ≥2 mutually exclusive actions to reach a consensus on the group level. Conradt and Roper 2005; King et al. 2008; Sueur and Petit 2008a,b
Consistent leadership The same individual always leads group actions. Conradt and Roper 2005 Opposite of distributed/variable leadership.
Democratic and despotic A consensus may be reached by the averaging of preferences (democracy), or by following the choices of specific leaders (despotism). Democratic=shared decision-making; despotic=unshared decision-making. King et al. 2008; King and Cowlishaw 2009 In primatology, we usually use “despotic” and “egalitarian” (or “tolerant”) to characterize the social system of a species and “shared” or “unshared” to describe consensus decisions. See text for detailed discussion.
Follower An individual that follows/joins the initiator/leader for a certain activity. Jacobs et al. 2008; Pyritz et al. 2010; Ramseyer et al. 2009a, b, c; Sueur and Petit 2008a,b The definition of a follower (of a group movement) should be operational and taxon-specific. See text for discussion.
Hidden leadership The same individual initiates and terminates a group activity (movement), although it is not guiding the movement in front of the group. Kummer 1968; Pyritz et al. 2010 The new definition of a leader (see below) includes hidden leadership. Therefore, we no longer need this term.
Initiator The group member that initiates a group activity. Bourjade and Sueur 2010; Jacobs et al. 2008; Pyritz et al. 2010; Ramseyer et al. 2009a, b, c; Sueur and Petit 2008a,b; The definition of an initiator (of a group movement) should be operational and taxon-specific. See text for discussion.
Leader Individual eliciting follower behavior/exerting social influence on others, by its rank into the progression, its behavior, or its social status. Harcourt et al. 2009; King 2010; Petit and Bon 2010; Pillot et al. 2010; Sueur and Petit 2008a,b, 2010 The definition of a leader should not be restricted by the spatial position during a group movement (Kummer 1968) but comprise the whole process of a collective action. It is also a defining characteristic of a leader that he exerts social influence on conspecifics. See text for detailed discussion.
Mimetism The probability that an individual performs a behavior depends on the number of individuals already performing this behavior (anonymous mimetism, allelomimetism). It can also depend on the social relationships the individual has with group members already displaying the behavior (selective mimetism). Camazine et al. 2001; Deneubourg and Goss 1989; Meunier et al. 2006; Petit et al. 2009; Sueur et al. 2009; Sumpter 2006
Overtaking Followers overtake the individual at the forefront of the group without diverging >45° from the initial trajectory. Boinski 1991; Erhart and Overdorff 1999; Pyritz et al. 2010; Trillmich et al. 2004
Predeparture behavior Behavior performed before the departure of the initiator, making the timing of the departure predictable and potentially indicating the direction in which individuals want to move. Bourjade and Sueur 2010; Sueur and Petit 2008a,b Other names: preliminary behavior, notifying behavior, voting behavior, intention movement, priming behavior.
Predeparture period Period preceding the departure of the initiator and delineated by the presence of predeparture behaviors. Bourjade and Sueur 2010; Sueur and Petit 2008a,b Other names: preliminary period.
Quorum Minimum number, i.e., threshold, of group members that need to take or favor a particular action for the whole group to adopt this action. Bousquet et al. 2010; Franks et al. 2002; Seeley et al. 2006; Sueur et al. 2010; Ward et al. 2008; In principle, the quorum could be a majority, submajority (less than a majority), or supermajority (more than a majority) of members. In practice, animals are likely to determine whether a quorum has been reached by estimating the relative numerousness of members contributing to the quorum, often by relying on indirect cues.
Recruitment behavior Behavior that increases the probability that other group members will join a certain activity. It results in a larger number of joiners or in quicker joining of the collective action than when not performed. Bourjade and Sueur 2010
Self-organizing system Individual group members follow local behavioral rules, resulting in organized behavior by the whole group without the need for global control. Camazine et al. 2001; Conradt and Roper 2005; Couzin et al. 2002; Sueur et al. 2009 Emergent properties due to the interactions between individuals in self-organized systems are more complex than the emergent properties that should be observed by the sum of individual behaviors.
Shared consensus decision All members contribute equally (and independently of individual identity) to the decision outcome. The consensus is usually determined by a quorum or by averaging over all votes. Conradt and Roper 2005; Sueur and Petit 2008a,b Opposite of unshared consensus decision.
Terminator Individual that stops and seems to suggest the termination of a group movement. Pyritz et al. 2010 Group movements can feasibly comprise 2 linked decisions: 1) initiation: when and in which direction to move and 2) termination: when and where to stop See text for detailed discussion.
Unshared consensus decision One particular group member, e.g., the dominant, makes the decision on behalf of all group members. All other members abide by this decision. Conradt and Roper 2005; Sueur and Petit 2008a,b Opposite of shared consensus decision.
Variable/distributed leadership Different group members lead group actions on different occasions. Conradt and Roper 2005; Jacobs et al. 2008; Petit and Bon 2010 Opposite of consistent leadership.
Voting An individual communicates its individual preference with regard to the decision outcome. Bousquet et al. 2010; Prins 1996; Sellers et al. 2007; Sueur et al. 2010

Terminology

Combined Decisions

Conradt and Roper (2005) defined a “combined decision” as the sum of individual group members` decisions that —unintentionally— affects the group as a whole (Table I). However, this definition is ambiguous and cannot be separated clearly from the often used and well established term “quorum decisions,” in which group members also choose individually between different options until a certain threshold is reached that affects the entire group (Seeley et al. 2006; Ward et al. 2008; Table I). In fact, the mechanisms of reaching a group decision employed in a large colony of ants (Leptothorax albipennis) choosing between several new nest sites (Franks et al. 2002) are principally the same as those employed by Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana) in a predeparture quorum (Sueur et al. 2010) or meerkats (Suricata suricatta) increasing travel speed via a vocal voting mechanism (Bousquet et al. 2010). In these species, each group member decides individually where and when to move, even if this decision is influenced by the behaviors of conspecifics. Further, there is no easy way to test whether individuals aim to reach a group consensus, which was the criterion Conradt and Roper (2005) used to distinguish consensus decisions from combined decisions. We therefore recommend not using the term “combined decision” in future research.

Democratic and Despotic Decisions

Researchers have used the terms “democratic” and “despotic” interchangeably with “shared” and “unshared” in a number of recent studies on decision-making (Conradt and Roper 2003; King et al. 2008; King and Cowlishaw 2009). “Democracy” describes a consensus reached by the majority principle, whereas “despotism” characterizes a consensus reached by following the choices of specific leaders in these studies. However, in primatology the term “despotic” is also used to characterize the social structure of a species, opposed to “egalitarian” or “tolerant” societies (Matsumura 1999), and the social structure of a species undoubtedly influences the process and outcome of consensus decision-making. For instance, Sueur and colleagues studied the decisions of 2 macaque species, with contrasting social structures, beginning a group movement after a resting period (Sueur and Petit 2008a,b). They found that egalitarian Tonkean macaques displayed an equally shared consensus whereas more despotic rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) displayed a partially shared consensus. These differences were attributable to the fact that in the despotic rhesus macaques, dominant individuals had a disproportionate influence on the movement decisions of group-mates: they were followed more often than the lower-ranked individuals. In contrast, all individuals in the more egalitarian Tonkean macaques had a similar influence on one another’s decisions to move and the decision was equally shared. However, the social structure of a primate group can more broadly be viewed as both the cause and consequence, reinforced via a feedback loop, of social interactions such as intensity of aggression, grooming, or reconciliatory patterns. These social interactions may be independent of group level decision-making processes. Thus, so as to not confound characteristics of the social system and collective decision-making in a group or causal relationships between these 2 realms, we suggest that future decision-making research should use only the terms “shared” and “unshared,” and not “democratic” and “despotic.”

Leadership

The definition of a leader should not be restricted to its spatial position during a group movement because individuals may also lead from behind, i.e., initiate and terminate a movement without being at the forefront of the group (Kummer 1968; called “hidden leadership” in Pyritz et al. 2010). Instead, a leader should be defined as an individual that elicits follower behavior (from a majority of or all group members) and exerts social influence on group members either by its rank, experience, social status/connectedness, or specific behavior (King 2010; Petit and Bon 2010; Sueur and Petit 2008b). Further, an individual that leads may not do so intentionally, i.e., leadership can be a passive process (Fischer and Zinner 2011; King et al. 2009; King and Sueur 2011). For instance, in sheep (Ovis aries), individuals triggered follower behavior by merely moving away from their group following a sound they had been trained to, thus eliciting group movements as incidental leaders (Pillot et al. 2010).

Concepts and Methods

Taxon-Specific Definitions

It remains a major practical challenge for human observers to identify reliably and define operationally a group movement in the field. The following is the most detailed definition employed in recent studies on the coordination of group movements in different species: An individual moves a certain distance toward the edge of the group either in a defined time period, e.g., 10 m within 40 s (Leca et al. 2003; Stueckle and Zinner 2008; Sueur and Petit 2008a,b), or without stopping and feeding (Bourjade et al. 2009; Ramseyer et al. 2009a, b, c), and is followed by a certain number of group members. However, even in this fairly detailed definition, most parameters were not assessed empirically. In fact, a definition of group movements must account for a number of taxon-specific characteristics such as different traveling types (directed movements vs. “feed-as-you-go,” ameboid-like movements that do not necessarily require an initiator or coordination among group members, e.g., bonobos [Pan paniscus]: Wrangham 2000); mean travel distances; ecological conditions (resource abundance, predation risk); as well as the size, composition, and cohesion of the group. Therefore, we encourage the use of operational group movement definitions for different taxa that are built upon empirical data collected before the study period used for the analyses of group movements. In the long term, these empirical details will generate a comprehensive database for multiple species and different contexts. Researchers will then be able to compare and contrast the results to see if general patterns are shared among different species. For practical details of how to operationalize definitions see Trillmich et al. (2004) and Pyritz et al. (2010).

Individual-Level Process and Group-Level Outcome

Studies of group coordination and decision-making examine 2 different levels that should be clearly distinguished. First, studies can focus on the group level, i.e., the question of whether there is consistent or variable leadership among several group movements or other collective actions (Erhart and Overdorff 1999; Jacobs et al. 2008; King et al. 2008; Pyritz et al. 2011). Second, studies can focus on the level of single movements and study the process, i.e., the question of whether decisions are shared or unshared and if they are mediated by mimetism, affiliative/genetic network relations, quorum thresholds, or self-organized processes (King and Sueur 2011; Petit and Bon 2010; Sueur and Petit 2008a,b, 2010; Sueur et al. 2009). It is important to separate these levels and clearly highlight the level at which the research is focused. King and Sueur (2011) and Sueur and Deneubourg (2011) explain how a consistent leadership or a consistent order of individuals could be observed even though the decision process is shared. For example, imagine n % of group members have to follow the first-moving individual, i.e., initiator, within n minutes for a group movement to occur; otherwise the initiator stops moving and the whole group remains stationary. A study observing movement patterns during departure would conclude that the process is shared because multiple individuals contribute to the decision of the group to move. Researchers who observed only the order of departure when the group chose to move would observe group movements that were always led by the same individuals, and the decision appears unshared. Because the outcome at the group level, i.e., departure order, is easier to observe, it is no surprise that most early studies in the area of coordination focused on who leads (King 2010). However, identifying who leads may not tell the whole story. For instance, an individual can always, or in most cases, be the leader of a group because his energetic reserves are always the first to be depleted, and he has to move to feed, but might have the same, or at least no stronger, influence on the joining process as other group members, e.g., in the case of an anonymous mimetism. In fact, a number of recent findings, e.g., macaques (Sueur and Petit 2008a,b, 2010; Sueur et al. 2009), and horses (Bourjade et al. 2009) suggest shared consensus between group members at the movement level, even though a single leader or a few leaders, i.e., an unshared consensus with consistent leadership, has previously been reported, e.g., macaques (Reinhardt et al. 1987) and horses (Feist and Mc Cullough 1976). For a further discussion of this topic see Bourjade and Sueur (2010).

Initiation and Termination of Collective Actions

During the course of a group movement, animals have to make 2 basic decisions, 1) initiation: when and in which direction to move and 2) termination: when and where to stop. Theoretically, the decision where to stop may be a second consensus decision independent from the decision during initiation, and the 2 decisions can be initiated by the same or different leaders (Boinski 1991; Erhart and Overdorff 1999; Pyritz et al. 2010). Although this issue has been addressed rarely, it yields important implications for the decision type, i.e., there could be unshared decision-making at departure that is mitigated by individuals overtaking the initial leader and terminating the movement, resulting in a shared decision-making with regard to the whole movement process. Accordingly, we suggest making a distinction between these 2 events and exploring the consistency of leadership during travel. For instance, one might classify leadership as “stable,” i.e., the initiator leads the movement until termination, or “unstable,” i.e., the terminator is different from the initiator (Pyritz et al. 2011). In general, it is important to study the entire process of group movements from initiation to termination to gain a realistic image of coordination, leadership, and decision-making in a given species (Fichtel et al. 2011; Pyritz et al. 2010; Trillmich et al. 2004). Indeed, such detail will be crucial if we are to determine the temporal scale over which decisions are made, and outcomes occur. Take an example where the same individual, X, both initiates and terminates the movement. This could imply that 1) X made the decision to move and the decision to stop, which was followed by group-mates, or 2) X made the decision to move in a particular direction of travel (Noser and Byrne 2007; Sueur et al. 2010) in a “goal-directed fashion” toward a particular destination, and was first to arrive, but did not make the decision to terminate the movement, because all group-mates that followed were aware of the target destination at the initiation (Pyritz et al. in prep.). In other words, the decision of where to terminate had been made at the initiation.

Successful Group Movements

Early studies set a threshold of 50% of group members having to follow an initiator, within a certain time frame, to consider a movement as successful (Erhart and Overdorff 1999). More recent studies considered an initiation attempt as failed only if no individual followed (Jacobs et al. 2008; Sueur and Petit 2008a; Trillmich et al. 2004). However, the mean number of followers that determines a successful group movement is presumably taxon-specific and may change with ecological season or resource abundance (King et al. 2008; Pyritz et al. 2011). Further, fission into subgroups —and an accordingly lower number of followers for single initiators— may represent the most beneficial group decision under certain conditions (Jacobs 2010; Kerth 2010). Hence, it is difficult to provide a general definition for successful or unsuccessful movements. We suggest giving a taxon- (and season-) specific threshold of success regarding followers below which the initiator does not stop and show recruitment behavior (Table I), or “give-up” on the movement entirely, e.g., 5 followers in chacma baboons (Papio hamadryas: Stueckle and Zinner 2008); 3 followers in Verreaux’s sifakas (Propithecus verreauxis: Trillmich et al. 2004), white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus: Petit et al. 2009), meerkats (Suricata suricatta: Bousquet et al. 2010), and Tonkean macaques (Sueur and Petit 2010). Combining this with operational group movement definitions for different taxa, it may be possible to uncover a common threshold —a universal— to primate group movements. In fact, examination of the aforementioned studies indicates that a threshold of approximately 3 followers seems to be sufficient to elicit a group movement, whatever the species. This number may provide sufficient protection against predators or enough collective knowledge to orientate within the home range and detect resources, for example.

Conclusion

After a public discussion about terms used in coordination and decision-making research via an interactive online platform, we have come to a number of conclusions that will aid future research on this topic. First, we highlighted a number of ambiguous terms (“combined decision”; “despotic/democratic decision”) and suggested that these not be used in future coordination research. We also suggest maintaining the terms “leader” and “leadership” because although they can sometimes be ambiguous, as long as an operational definition is provided these terms are useful to broader scientific communication and integration (King 2010; Pyritz et al. 2010). Further, we discussed a number of conceptual issues: group vs. movement level; operationalized successful group movements; collective action process including initiation and termination. Ultimately, the implementation of our suggestions should provide comparable data on coordination and decision-making in different species and facilitate the identification of general patterns that are shared among different taxa.

Acknowledgments

We thank the participants of the symposium “Where next? Coordination and decision making in primate groups” at the XXIIIth Congress of the International Primatological Society (IPS) for valuable discussions and comments on the glossary and conceptual issues. Henning Lahmann kindly implemented the online glossary. We also thank Joanna Setchell and the International Journal of Primatology for providing financial support and making this special issue possible. L. W. Pyritz is funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG: KA 1082/16-1), A. J. King by a NERC Postdoctoral Fellowship (NE/H016600/2), and C. Sueur by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science. Joanna Setchell and 2 anonymous reviewers provided valuable comments on the manuscript.

Open Access

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.

References

  1. Boinski S. The coordination of spatial position: A field study of the vocal behaviour of adult female squirrel monkeys. Animal Behaviour. 1991;41:89–102. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bourjade M, Sueur C. Shared or unshared consensus to move collectively? Towards methodological concerns. Behavioural Processes. 2010;84:648–652. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.02.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bourjade M, Thierry B, Maumy M, Petit O. Decision-making in Przewalski horses (Equus ferus przewalskii) is driven by the ecological contexts of collective movements. Ethology. 2009;115:321–330. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bousquet CAH, Sumpter DJT, Manser MB. Moving calls: A vocal mechanism underlying quorum decisions in cohesive groups. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 2010 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.1739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Camazine S, Deneubourg JL, Franks NR, Sneyd J, Theraulaz G, Bonabeau E. Self-organization in biological systems. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  6. Conradt L, Roper TJ. Group decision-making in animals. Nature. 2003;421:155–158. doi: 10.1038/nature01294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Conradt L, Roper TJ. Consensus decision making in animals. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2005;20:449–456. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Conradt L, List C. Introduction. Group decisions in humans and animals: A survey. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 2009;364:719–742. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Couzin ID, Krause J, James R, Ruxton GD, Franks NR. Collective memory and spatial sorting in animal groups. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 2002;218:1–11. doi: 10.1006/jtbi.2002.3065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Deneubourg JL, Goss S. Collective patterns and decision-making. Ethology, Ecology and Evolution. 1989;1:295–311. [Google Scholar]
  11. Erhart EM, Overdorff DJ. Female coordination of group travel in wild Propithecus and Eulemur. International Journal of Primatology. 1999;20:927–941. [Google Scholar]
  12. Feist JD, Mc Cullough DR. Behaviour patterns and communication in feral horses. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie. 1976;41:337–371. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1976.tb00947.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fichtel C, Pyritz L, Kappeler PM. Coordination of group movements in non-human primates. In: Boos M, Kolbe M, Ellwart S, Kappeler PM, editors. Coordination in human and non-human primate groups. Heidelberg, Berlin: Springer; 2011. pp. 37–56. [Google Scholar]
  14. Fischer, J., & Zinner, D. (2011). Communication and cognition in primate group movement. International Journal of Primatology, 32 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  15. Franks NR, Pratt SC, Mallon EB, Britton NF, Sumpter DJT. Information flow, opinion polling and collective intelligence in house-hunting social insects. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2002;357:1567–1583. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Harcourt JL, Ang TZ, Sweetman G, Johnstone RA, Manica A. Social feedback and the emergence of leaders and followers. Current Biology. 2009;19:248–252. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.12.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Jacobs A. Group cohesiveness during collective movements: Travelling apart together. Behavioural Processes. 2010;84:678–680. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Jacobs A, Maumy M, Petit O. The influence of social organisation on leadership in brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus fulvus) in a controlled environment. Behavioural Processes. 2008;79:111–113. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kerth G. Group decision-making in fission-fusion societies. Behavioural Processes. 2010;84:662–663. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.02.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. King AJ. Follow me! I’m a leader if you do; I’m a failed initiator if you don’t. Behavioural Processes. 2010;84:671–674. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. King AJ, Cowlishaw G. Leaders, followers and group decision-making. Communicative and Integrative Biology. 2009;2:147–150. doi: 10.4161/cib.7562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. King, A. J., & Sueur, C. (2011). Where next? Group coordination and collective decision-making by primates. International Journal of Primatology, 32
  23. King AJ, Douglas CMS, Huchard E, Isaac NJB, Cowlishaw G. Dominance and affiliation mediate despotism in a social primate. Current Biology. 2008;18:1833–1838. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.10.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. King AJ, Johnson DDP, Van Vugt M. The origins and evolution of leadership. Current Biology. 2009;19:911–916. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kummer, H. (1968). The coordination of travel. In Social organization of hamadryas baboons: A field study (pp. 122–156). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  26. Leca JB, Gunst N, Thierry B, Petit O. Distributed leadership in semi-free ranging white-faced capuchin monkeys. Animal Behaviour. 2003;66:1045–1052. [Google Scholar]
  27. Matsumura S. The evolution of “egalitarian” and “despotic” social systems among macaques. Primates. 1999;40:23–31. doi: 10.1007/BF02557699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Meunier H, Leca JB, Deneubourg JL, Petit O. Group movement decisions in capuchin monkeys: The utility of an experimental study and a mathematical model to explore the relationship between individual and collective behaviours. Behaviour. 2006;143:1511–1527. [Google Scholar]
  29. Noser R, Byrne RW. Travel routes and planning of visits to out-of-sight resources in wild chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) Animal Behaviour. 2007;73:257–266. [Google Scholar]
  30. Petit O, Bon R. Decision-making processes: The case of collective movements. Behavioural Processes. 2010;84:635–647. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Petit O, Gautrais J, Leca JB, Theraulaz G, Deneubourg JL. Collective decision-making in white-faced capuchins. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 2009;276:3495–3503. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Pillot MH, Gautrais J, Gouello J, Michelena P, Sibbald A, Bon R. Moving together: Incidental leaders and naïve followers. Behavioural Processes. 2010;83:235–241. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Prins HHT. Ecology and behaviour of the African buffalo. London: Chapman and Hall; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  34. Pyritz L, Fichtel C, Kappeler P. Conceptual and methodological issues in the comparative study of collective group movements. Behavioural Processes. 2010;84:681–684. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.02.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Pyritz, L. et al. (2011). Coordination of group movements in wild red-fronted lemurs: Processes and influence of ecological and reproductive seasonality. International Journal of Primatology, 32 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  36. Ramseyer A, Petit O, Thierry B. Patterns of group movements in juvenile domestic geese. Journal of Ethology. 2009a;27:369–375. [Google Scholar]
  37. Ramseyer A, Boissy A, Dumont B, Thierry B. Decision making in group departures of sheep is a continuous process. Animal Behaviour. 2009b;78:71–78. [Google Scholar]
  38. Ramseyer A, Thierry B, Boissy A, Dumont B. Decision-making processes in group departures of cattle. Ethology. 2009c;115:948–957. [Google Scholar]
  39. Reinhardt V, Reinhardt A, Houser D. Prompted progression order in a troop of captive rhesus monkeys. Folia Primatologica. 1987;48:121–124. doi: 10.1159/000156289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Seeley TD, Visscher PK, Passino KM. Group decision making in honey bee swarms. American Scientist. 2006;94:220–229. [Google Scholar]
  41. Sellers WI, Hill RA, Logan BS. An agent-based model of group decision making in baboons. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 2007;362:1699–1710. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Stueckle S, Zinner D. To follow or not to follow: Decision making and leadership during the morning departure in chacma baboons. Animal Behaviour. 2008;75:1995–2004. [Google Scholar]
  43. Sueur C, Petit O. Shared or unshared consensus decision in macaques? Behavioural Processes. 2008;78:84–92. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Sueur C, Petit O. Organization of group members at departure is driven by social structure in Macaca. International Journal of Primatology. 2008;29:1085–1098. [Google Scholar]
  45. Sueur C, Petit O. Signals use by leaders in Macaca tonkeana and Macaca mulatta: Group-mate recruitment and behaviour monitoring. Animal Cognition. 2010;13:239–248. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0261-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Sueur, C., & Deneubourg, J-L. (2011). Self-organization in primates: Understanding the rules underlying collective movements. International Journal of Primatology, 32
  47. Sueur C, Petit O, Deneubourg JL. Selective mimetism at departure in collective movements of Macaca tonkeana: An experimental and theoretical combination of mimetic rules in macaques. Animal Behaviour. 2009;78:1087–1095. [Google Scholar]
  48. Sueur C, Deneubourg J-L, Petit O. From the first intention movement to the last joiner: Macaques combine mimetic rules to optimize their collective decisions. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 2010 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Sumpter DJ. The principles of collective animal behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 2006;361:5–22. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Trillmich J, Fichtel C, Kappeler PM. Coordination of group movements in wild Verreux`s sifakas (Propithecus verreauxi) Behaviour. 2004;141:1103–1120. [Google Scholar]
  51. Ward AJW, Sumpter DJT, Couzin ID, Hart PJB, Krause J. Quorum decision-making facilitates information transfer in fish shoals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008;105:6948–6953. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710344105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Wrangham RW. Why are male chimpanzees more gregarious than mothers? A scramble competition hypothesis. In: Kappeler PM, editor. Primate males. Causes and consequences of variation in group composition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2000. pp. 248–258. [Google Scholar]

Articles from International Journal of Primatology are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES