Abstract
In the community at large, many older adults with minimal cognitive and functional impairment remain stable or improve over time, unlike patients in clinical research settings, who typically progress to dementia. Within a prospective population-based study, we identified neuropsychological tests predicting improvement or worsening over one year in cognitively-driven everyday functioning as measured by Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR). Participants were 1682 adults aged 65+ and dementia-free at baseline. CDR change was modeled as a function of baseline test scores, adjusting for demographics. Among those with baseline CDR=0.5, 29.8% improved to CDR=0; they had significantly better baseline scores on most tests. In a stepwise multiple logistic regression model, tests which remained independently associated with subsequent CDR improvement were Category Fluency, a modified Token Test, and the sum of learning trials on Object Memory Evaluation. In contrast, only 7.1% with baseline CDR=0 worsened to CDR=0.5. They had significantly lower baseline scores on most tests. In multiple regression analyses, only the Mini-Mental State Exam, delayed memory for visual reproduction, and recall susceptible to proactive interference, were independently associated with CDR worsening. At the population level, changes in both directions are observable in functional status, with different neuropsychological measures predicting the direction of change.
Keywords: Epidemiology, community, aging, Clinical Dementia Rating, cognition, prediction
INTRODUCTION
There is growing interest in older individuals whose cognitive impairments, and cognitively-driven functional impairments, appear to place them on the border between normal aging and dementia. In specialized memory disorders clinics, the majority of individuals with mild impairments are highly likely to progress over time to frank dementia and to have underlying neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer's disease (Morris, 2006; Petersen, 2004; Petersen et al., 2009; Winblad et al., 2004). In contrast, all studies conducted in population-based community cohorts show that while mildly impaired individuals are indeed at elevated risk of progressing to dementia, substantial proportions remain mildly impaired or revert to normal (Busse et al., 2006; Ganguli et al., 2004; Larrieu et al., 2002; Manly et al., 2008; Palmer et al., 2002; Ritchie et al., 2001). This phenomenon is typically attributed to selection factors that result in different groups of individuals participating in different types of studies (Bruscoli & Lovestone, 2004). Those who seek services or volunteer for research in memory clinics are a relatively homogeneous group. Typically, they or their families are subjectively conscious of their cognitive changes, and they are relatively free of other health problems, compared to others within their own base population with the same objective level of impairment. In contrast, individuals whose cognitive and functional impairments are detected as part of community surveys are a larger, more heterogeneous group. In these persons, mild impairments may be attributable to a wider range of underlying factors and conditions, not limited to progressive brain disorders leading to dementia; some of these other conditions may be reversible or self-limiting. Thus, while extremely valuable information is obtained from samples of patients and study participants in clinical research settings, their inherent referral bias limits the generalizability of their data to the community at large.
A range of objective neuropsychological tests are used to measure global and domain-specific cognition in clinical and clinical research settings, and increasingly also in epidemiological, population-based studies. Cognitively-driven functional impairments are frequently measured on the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (CDR) (Hughes et al., 1982; Morris, 1993). The CDR is used primarily to rate patients presenting to clinical or clinical research programs. In these settings, the individuals being rated have typically referred themselves, or been referred by a family member, because of concerns about their cognitive functioning. In some studies, a CDR rating of 0.5, defined variously as questionable dementia or very mild dementia, is considered equivalent to the entity known as mild cognitive impairment [MCI] (Marquis et al., 2002; Morris, 2006; Petersen et al., 1999). The CDR has also been employed in population-based epidemiologic studies, where study participants are not patients seeking help, may not express their concerns spontaneously. Rather, their subjective reports are solicited by researchers using standardized assessment protocols and performing CDR ratings based on these data. Validated in these settings, the CDR has been used to identify and estimate incidence and prevalence of not only dementia but also lesser degrees of cognitive impairment that affect everyday functioning (Ganguli et al., 2000; Ott et al., 1995; Saxton et al., 2009).
A further potential application of the CDR in both clinical and non-clinical settings is to determine change over time. There is considerable public health value in determining changes, and identifying predictors of these changes, at the population level. We have previously reported (Ganguli et al., 2010b) the prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in a cohort representative of the local population, using several criterion/ classification systems including the CDR. From the same cohort, we now report on changes over one year in CDR, and neuropsychological tests that predicted these changes. Since the CDR is rated independently of the neuropsychological tests, and the CDR scoring algorithm is weighted towards memory difficulties, we hypothesized that lower initial performance on objective measures of memory would predict changes in observed functional level as measured by the CDR. Neuropsychological measures that predict decline from mild impairment to dementia have been widely investigated (DeCarli et al., 2004; Fleisher et al., 2007; Loewenstein et al., 2007, 2009; Tabert et al., 2006) and a few studies have identified predictors of new onset mild impairment (Marquis et al., 2002) or dementia (Grober et al., 2008; Howieson et al., 2008; Iwasa et al., 2008; Kluger et al., 1999) in previously normal individuals. Only a few previous studies have examined cognitive variables that predict future improvement, measured by reversion to normal from MCI as classified using neuropsychological, subjective, and functional criteria (Loewenstein et al., 2007, 2009; Manly et al., 2008).
METHOD
Study area, sampling, and recruitment
The study cohort named the Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team (MYHAT) was an age-stratified random sample of the population aged 65+ years, drawn from the publicly available voter registration list in a small-town region of southwestern Pennsylvania, in the USA. Community outreach, recruitment, and assessment procedures were approved by the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board for protection of human subjects. Additional details have been reported previously (Ganguli et al., 2009, 2010a, 2010b). Recruitment criteria were (a) age 65 years or older, (b) living within the selected area, (c) not already in long-term care institutions. Individuals were considered ineligible if they (d) were too ill to participate, (e) had severe vision impairment, (f) had severe hearing impairment, or (g) were decisionally incapacitated. Over the approximately two-year recruitment period, a total of 2036 individuals were recruited.
Assessment (overview)
A single-stage assessment (Prince, 2000) was employed to avoid delays and potentially non-random attrition between screening and definitive assessment stages (Ganguli et al., 2009). The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975) was administered and scored on the spot, applying a standard correction for age and education (Mungas et al., 1996). Fifty-four individuals (2.7%) scoring <21 /30 (age-education corrected) were classified as having moderate to severe cognitive impairment and therefore unsuited to a study of mild impairment; those individuals were not assessed further. The remaining 1982 participants, who scored ≥21 on the age-education corrected MMSE, were classified as having normal or mildly impaired cognition. They proceeded to the full assessment, which included the neuropsychological assessment briefly described below. Following the full in-home assessment, individuals were contacted by telephone every 3 months to minimize loss to followup, and invited to undergo a repeat assessment at home one year later.
Neuropsychological assessment
Cognitive functioning was assessed by the following test battery, categorized here according to the principal cognitive domain tapped by the tests.
Attention/Processing Speed
Trail Making Test A (Reitan, 1955), Digit Span Forward (Wechsler, 1997)
Executive Function
Trail Making Test B (Reitan, 1955), clock drawing (Freedman et al., 1994), verbal fluency for initial letters P&S (Benton & Hamsher, 1989)
Language
Boston Naming Test (Kaplan et al., 1978), verbal fluency for categories (animals) (Benton & Hamsher, 1989), Indiana University (IU) Token Test (Snitz et al., 2009; Unverzagt et al., 1999)
Memory
WMS-R Logical Memory (immediate and delayed recall) (Wechsler, 1987), WMS-R Visual Reproduction (immediate and delayed recall) (Wechsler, 1987), 3-trial Fuld Object Memory Evaluation (OME) with Semantic Interference (Fuld, 1981; Loewenstein et al., 2003, 2004; Snitz et al.,2010)
Visuospatial Function
WAIS-III-Block Design (Wechsler, 1997)
Population-based norms on these tests from the MYHAT cohort free of dementia have been previously published (Ganguli et al. 2010a) and are also available on our study website (http://www.wpic.pitt.edu/research/dementia_epidemiology/MYHAT/MYHATHomePage.htm).
In addition, the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT-3) (Wilkinson, 1993; Manly et al., 2002) was also administered to participants at baseline as a measure of premorbid reading level.
Clinical Dementia Rating
Trained interviewers rated participants on the CDR scale (Morris, 1993) after obtaining online CDR training and certification via the Washington University Alzheimer's Disease Research Center website (Washington University in St. Louis, 2006). The rating was based on an assessment protocol composed of standardized questions, as well as raters' observation, regarding the participant's daily functioning in the six areas of memory, orientation, judgment, home and hobbies, community affairs, and personal care. In this population-based sample, the majority of normal or only mildly impaired older adults provided their own self-report information, i.e. did not have surrogate informants. The CDR assessment is not based on neuropsychological test performance, although the same interviewer performed the neuropsychological testing and later the CDR rating on each participant. The CDR rating for each participant was finalized by consensus after discussion between two or more interviewers of the functional assessment, ignoring the neuropsychological data, but determining that the reported or observed functional impairments were attributable to cognitive difficulties, as opposed to, e.g., sensory or motor impairments. Each of the six areas is rated on a scale of 0 through 0.5, 1, 2, and 3, and a standard algorithm is used to generate a summary CDR rating of 0 (no dementia), 0.5 (variously designated as mild cognitive impairment, possible/questionable dementia, or very mild dementia), 1.0 (mild dementia), 2.0 (moderate dementia), and 3.0 (severe dementia) (Hughes et al., 1982; Morris, 1993). As the scoring algorithm for the CDR global summary score is weighted in favor of memory, the summary score does not capture the full range of dysfunction represented by the “box scores” that represent the six areas of functioning. The CDR Sum of Boxes (CDR-SoB) metric, which is the aggregate of all six box scores, is thus a better indicator of the spread of difficulties experienced by the participant than the summary CDR.
Statistical Analyses
Baseline cognitive test scores and demographic characteristics were examined in pairwise comparisons between (a) participants with baseline CDR=0.5 who remained at CDR=0.5 at one-year followup, vs. those who improved to CDR=0 at followup; and (b) participants with baseline CDR=0 who remained at CDR=0 during followup, vs. those who worsened to CDR=0.5 at followup. Given the skewness of the neuropsychological data in this population, the non-parametric Wilcoxon Rank Sum Test was used to identify statistically significant differences in test scores between groups; the chi-square test was used for categorical demographic characteristics; the effect of age was also tested by the Cochran-Armitage test for trend. In posthoc analyses, baseline CDR-SoB was also examined in relation to CDR change, using the Cochran-Armitage trend test.
Univariable logistic regression models were fit for each neuropsychological test, first unadjusted and then adjusting for age, sex, and education, to examine the odds of CDR improvement from CDR=0.5, and the odds of CDR worsening from CDR=0, with stable CDR as the reference group in each case. Finally, we explored the independent contributions that each individual neuropsychological measure makes to predicting CDR change, in the presence of the other measures. Stepwise multiple logistic regression models were fit for CDR improvement and CDR worsening, potentially including as independent variables all neuropsychological tests that were significant in the univariable analyses. Age, sex, and education were included as covariates. The entering and removing probabilities in the stepwise regression were 0.15 and 0.2, respectively. After fitting the stepwise model, we examined the selected neuropsychological measures for multicollinearity, using the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) for each test. All statistical analyses were performed using SAS v. 9.2 (SAS Institute, 2002).
RESULTS
Sample size and demographics
At study entry (baseline), we recruited 2036 individuals, of whom 54(2.7%) had moderate to severe cognitive impairment (age-education corrected MMSE <21), leaving 1982 with normal or mildly impaired cognition to undergo the full assessment. These 1982 participants had mean (SD) baseline age of 77.6 (7.4) years with a range of 65 to 99 years. As regards education, 13.8%, 45.1%, and 41.1% had completed less than high school, high school, and more than high school. Women comprised 61.1% while 94.8% of the cohort was Caucasian.
Attrition between baseline recruitment and one-year followup was 13.0%. Those not assessed at this followup included 59 who died, 26 who relocated outside the study area 33 who were too ill to continue participating, 107 who chose to drop out of the study, 31 who were untraceable (e.g., telephone disconnected, no forwarding information, no information obtainable from contact person), and 2 in other situations. A further 27 who skipped the first annual followup have remained in the study for future assessments. Thus, of the original 1982 individuals, 1697 (85.6%) underwent the repeat assessment a year later; of these, 15 individuals who had CDR >=1 at baseline are excluded from these analyses. Among the remaining 1682 participants, the mean baseline age of 77.4 (7.3) years, proportion of women (62.3%), Caucasians (95.0%), and those at the three educational levels (13.1%, 45.0%, and 41.9%) were not different to those of the original cohort of 1982 as detailed above. Since this report is focused on neuropsychological test performance at baseline, these analyses are restricted to the 1432 participants with complete data on all tests and CDR 0 or 0.5 at baseline. These individuals had mean (SD) age 76.9 (7.1) years, 62.3% were women, 96.4% were Caucasians, and 11.4%, 45.7%, and 42.9% of them had less than high school, high school, and more than high school education.
Baseline CDR and CDR Sum-of-Boxes
At baseline, among the 1432 participants included in these analyses, 1096 obtained CDR summary scores of 0 (no dementia), with 336 being rated as CDR=0.5 (very mild impairment). Of those with CDR=0, 1046 (95.4%) had CDR_SoB of 0; 48 (4.4%) had CDR_SoB of 0.5; and 2 (0.2%) had CDR_SoB=1. Among those with CDR=0.5 at baseline, 203 (60.4%), 94 (28.0%), 25 (7.4%) and 14 (4.2%) had CDR_SoB of 0.5, 1, 1.5, and ≥2.
CDR Worsening
Of the 1096 with baseline summary CDR=0, 1018 (92.9%) remained at CDR=0 at one-year followup, while 78 (7.1%) worsened to CDR=0.5. (Table 1). In the subgroup with baseline CDR_SoB of 0, 64 (6.1%) had CDR worsening; of the 48 with CDR_SoB of 0.5; 14 (29.2%) had CDR worsening, while neither of the 2 participants with CDR_SoB of 1 showed CDR worsening. As only 3 individuals with summary CDR=0.5 worsened to CDR >=1, they are excluded from these analyses.
Table 1.
Demographic characteristics and mean (SD) baseline neuropsychological test scores among participants with Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) =0.5 at baseline
All participants with baseline CDR=0.5 | Stable over one year at CDR=0.5 | Improved over one year from CDR=0.5 to CDR=0 | Significance level for difference between stable and improved | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | (n=336) | (n=236) | (n=100) | ||||
n | % | n | % | n | % | ||
Age (in years) | * | ||||||
65–74 | 104 | 31.0 | 63 | 26.7 | 41 | 41.0 | |
75–84 | 164 | 48.8 | 117 | 49.6 | 47 | 47.0 | |
≥85 | 68 | 20.2 | 56 | 23.7 | 12 | 12.0 | |
Female | 189 | 56.3 | 129 | 54.7 | 60 | 60.0 | |
Education | * | ||||||
<High school education | 49 | 14.6 | 41 | 17.4 | 8 | 8.0 | |
≥High school education | 287 | 85.4 | 195 | 82.6 | 92 | 92.0 | |
|
|||||||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
|
|||||||
Mini-Mental State Exam | 26.37 | 2.33 | 26.00 | 2.43 | 27.25 | 1.81 | ** |
WRAT-3 Reading | 46.74 | 4.53 | 46.17 | 4.70 | 48.09 | 3.78 | ** |
Digit Span Forward (maximum span) | 6.44 | 1.01 | 6.38 | 1.02 | 6.60 | 0.97 | |
Category Fluency (animals) | 14.33 | 4.61 | 13.50 | 4.46 | 16.28 | 4.38 | ** |
Letter Fluency (mean ofP&S) | 11.23 | 4.15 | 10.85 | 4.18 | 12.13 | 3.97 | ** |
Boston Naming Test | 52.22 | 5.68 | 51.41 | 5.90 | 54.12 | 4.61 | ** |
Indiana University Token Test | 22.41 | 1.72 | 22.10 | 1.82 | 23.14 | 1.17 | ** |
WAIS-III Block Design | 26.54 | 8.72 | 25.11 | 8.50 | 29.93 | 8.33 | ** |
WMS-R Visual Reproduction (Immediate) | 26.34 | 7.38 | 25.46 | 7.47 | 28.41 | 6.78 | ** |
WMS-R Visual Reproduction (Delayed) | 15.10 | 10.53 | 13.42 | 10.49 | 19.05 | 9.56 | ** |
WMS-R Logical memory (Immediate) | 17.64 | 6.92 | 16.60 | 7.13 | 20.10 | 5.71 | ** |
WMS-R Logical Memory (Delayed) | 12.21 | 7.03 | 11.26 | 7.18 | 14.45 | 6.13 | ** |
OME sum of 3 trials† | 20.46 | 4.48 | 19.60 | 4.72 | 22.50 | 3.02 | ** |
Semantic Interference Test “Bag B” Recall†† | 5.60 | 1.85 | 5.28 | 1.90 | 6.33 | 1.52 | ** |
Clock Drawing (15-point scale) | 13.42 | 1.72 | 13.26 | 1.85 | 13.81 | 1.28 | ** |
Trail Making Test A | 49.32 | 21.47 | 51.11 | 22.54 | 45.10 | 18.13 | ** |
Trail Making Test B | 132.99 | 52.42 | 140.39 | 53.01 | 115.52 | 46.79 | ** |
p<0.05 based on the chi-square test for education, gender, and age; age was also tested by the Cochran-Armitage test for trend (p<0.05).
p<0.05 based on the Wilcoxon Rank Sum test.
Fuld Object Memory Evaluation (OME), sum of 3 learning trials;
OME with Semantic Interference recall of second list after pro-active interference.
CDR improvement
Of the 336 individuals with baseline summary CDR=0.5 at baseline, 236 (70.2%) remained stable at CDR=0.5, while 100 (29.8%) improved to CDR=0. (Table 2). In the subgroup with baseline CDR_SoB of 0.5, 1, 1.5, and >= 2, the n(%) with improving CDR were 75 (36.9%), 19 (20.2%), 5 (20.0%), and 1 (7.1%), i.e., lower baseline CDR_SoB was associated with greater probability of improvement, a significant trend (Cochrane-Armitage p <.05).
Table 2.
Demographic characteristics and mean (SD) baseline neuropsychological test scores among participants with CDR=0 at baseline
All participants with baseline CDR=0 | Stable at CDR=0 over one year | Worsened over one year from CDR=0 to CDR=0.5 | Significance level for difference between stable and worsened | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | (n=1096) | (n=1018) | (n=78) | ||||
n | % | n | % | n | % | ||
Age (in years) | * | ||||||
65–74 | 429 | 39.1 | 414 | 40.7 | 15 | 19.2 | |
75–84 | 512 | 46.7 | 471 | 46.3 | 41 | 52.6 | |
≥85 | 155 | 14.1 | 133 | 13.1 | 22 | 28.2 | |
Female | 703 | 64.1 | 647 | 63.6 | 56 | 71.8 | |
Education | |||||||
<High school education | 114 | 10.4 | 102 | 10.0 | 12 | 15.4 | |
≥High school education | 982 | 89.6 | 916 | 90.0 | 66 | 84.6 | |
|
|||||||
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | Mean | SD | ||
|
|||||||
Mini-Mental State Exam | 27.61 | 1.88 | 27.70 | 1.82 | 26.44 | 2.15 | ** |
WRAT -3 Reading | 47.31 | 4.66 | 47.37 | 4.57 | 46.49 | 5.69 | |
Digit Span Forward | 6.63 | 1.01 | 6.65 | 1.01 | 6.40 | 1.00 | ** |
Category Fluency | 16.68 | 4.61 | 16.87 | 4.54 | 14.10 | 4.76 | ** |
Letter Fluency | 12.69 | 4.30 | 12.80 | 4.29 | 11.30 | 4.22 | ** |
Boston Naming Test | 54.19 | 4.88 | 54.44 | 4.77 | 50.88 | 5.06 | ** |
I.U.Token Test | 23.07 | 1.30 | 23.12 | 1.25 | 22.42 | 1.74 | ** |
WAIS-III Block Design | 30.02 | 9.16 | 30.31 | 9.14 | 26.35 | 8.58 | ** |
WMS-R Visual Reproduction (Immediate) | 29.18 | 6.36 | 29.46 | 6.29 | 25.44 | 6.05 | ** |
WMS-R Visual Reproduction (Delayed) | 20.69 | 9.87 | 21.35 | 9.57 | 12.03 | 9.71 | ** |
WMS-R Logical Memory (Immediate) | 21.19 | 6.75 | 21.48 | 6.70 | 17.47 | 6.36 | ** |
WMS-R Logical Memory (Delayed) | 15.91 | 7.06 | 16.23 | 6.97 | 11.82 | 7.06 | ** |
OME sum of 3 trials† | 22.96 | 3.41 | 23.11 | 3.27 | 21.01 | 4.43 | ** |
SIT “Bag B” Recall†† | 6.50 | 1.67 | 6.59 | 1.62 | 5.40 | 1.90 | ** |
Clock Drawing | 13.89 | 1.33 | 13.91 | 1.29 | 13.53 | 1.73 | |
Trail Making Test A | 42.78 | 16.75 | 42.13 | 16.08 | 51.18 | 22.32 | ** |
Trail Making Test B | 109.08 | 45.96 | 106.84 | 44.71 | 138.38 | 51.94 | ** |
p<0.05 based on the chi-square test for education, gender, and age; age was also tested by the Cochran-Armitage test for trend (p<0.05).
p<0.05 based on the Wilcoxon Rank Sum test.
Fuld Object Memory Evaluation (OME), sum of 3 learning trials;
OME with Semantic Interference recall of second list after pro-active interference.
Neuropsychological tests associated with improvement vs. stability at CDR=0.5
Pairwise comparisons of mean (SD) baseline cognitive test scores (Table 1) showed significant differences between those who remained at CDR=0.5 and those who improved to from CDR=0.5 to CDR=0. Among those who improved, higher baseline scores were seen on all tests except Digit Span. Demographic characteristics of the stable and improved groups (Table 1) indicate that younger age and higher education were associated with global CDR improvement. In unadjusted logistic regression models examining each test individually, all tests except Digit Span were significantly associated with CDR improvement (data not shown); adjusting for age, sex, and education, all tests except Digit Span, Letter Fluency, and Trailmaking Test A were significantly associated with CDR improvement (Table 3). In the stepwise multiple logistic regression combining all tests and adjusting for demographics, only Category Fluency (Animals), the IU Token Test, and the sum of 3 learning trials on Object Memory Evaluation test, remained significantly associated with subsequent CDR improvement from 0.5 to 0. Model fit was good, with area under the ROC curve (AUC) of 0.76.
Table 3.
Baseline measures associated with improvement from CDR=0.5 at baseline to CDR=0 at followup. Logistic regression models based on participants with CDR=0.5 at baseline:
Univariable model (adjusted for age, gender, and education) | Stepwise multivariable model* | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | OR | 95% CI for OR | P value | OR | 95% CI for OR | P value | ||
Age 75–84† | -- | -- | -- | -- | 1.30 | 0.71 | 2.39 | 0.4017 |
Age ≥85† | -- | -- | -- | -- | 0.91 | 0.39 | 2.15 | 0.8346 |
Female gender | -- | -- | -- | -- | 1.30 | 0.76 | 2.22 | 0.3448 |
Education ≥high school†† | -- | -- | -- | -- | 1.31 | 0.55 | 3.16 | 0.5424 |
MMSE | 1.26 | 1.11 | 1.42 | 0.0003 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
WRAT-3 Reading | 1.09 | 1.03 | 1.16 | 0.0049 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
Digit Span Forward | 1.17 | 0.92 | 1.49 | 0.2081 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
Category Fluency | 1.13 | 1.06 | 1.20 | <.0001 | 1.07 | 1.00 | 1.14 | 0.0434 |
Letter Fluency | 1.06 | 1.00 | 1.12 | 0.0653 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
Boston Naming Test | 1.10 | 1.04 | 1.16 | 0.0009 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
I.U. Token Test | 1.50 | 1.23 | 1.83 | <.0001 | 1.34 | 1.08 | 1.65 | 0.0072 |
WAIS-III Block Design | 1.07 | 1.03 | 1.10 | <.0001 | 1.03 | 0.99 | 1.06 | 0.1443 |
WMS-R Visual Reproduction (Immediate) | 1.05 | 1.01 | 1.08 | 0.0166 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
WMS-R Visual Reproduction (Delayed) | 1.05 | 1.02 | 1.07 | 0.0006 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
WMS-R Logical Memory (Immediate) | 1.07 | 1.03 | 1.11 | 0.0009 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
WMS-R Logical Memory (Delayed) | 1.05 | 1.02 | 1.09 | 0.0046 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
OME sum of 3 trials | 1.19 | 1.11 | 1.28 | <.0001 | 1.13 | 1.05 | 1.23 | 0.0018 |
SIT “Bag B” Recall | 1.37 | 1.17 | 1.60 | <.0001 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
Clock Drawing | 1.19 | 1.01 | 1.40 | 0.0399 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
Trail Making Test A | 0.99 | 0.98 | 1.00 | 0.1450 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
Trail Making Test B | 0.99 | 0.99 | 1.00 | 0.0032 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
During the stepwise procedure, age, gender, and education were always included. Area under the ROC curve = 0.76 in final stepwise multivariable model.
The two older groups each compared to the 65–74 group (the reference group).
≥ High school education compared to < high school education (the reference group).
Neuropsychological tests associated with worsening vs. stability at CDR=0
Pairwise comparisons of mean (SD) baseline neuropsychological test scores (Table 2) also showed significant differences between those who remained at CDR=0 and those who progressed to CDR=0.5; among those who worsened, lower baseline scores were seen on all tests except the WRAT-3 and Clock Drawing. Demographic characteristics of the stable and worsened groups (Table 2) showed that older age was associated with CDR worsening. In unadjusted logistic regression models examining each test individually, all tests except the WRAT-3 were significantly associated with CDR worsening (data not shown). Logistic regression models (Table 4) examining each test individually, and adjusting for age, sex, and education, showed CDR worsening to be significantly associated with lower baseline scores on all tests except WRAT-3, Digit Span, and Clock Drawing. In the stepwise multiple logistic regression combining all tests and adjusting for demographics, only baseline MMSE, Visual Reproduction (Delayed), and recall susceptible to proactive interference (“Bag B” recall on the Semantic Interference Test) (Loewenstein et al., 2003; Snitz et al.,2010) at baseline remained significantly and independently associated with subsequent CDR worsening from 0 to 0.5. The model had good fit (AUC = 0.78).
Table 4.
Baseline measures associated with worsening from CDR=0 at baseline to CDR=0.5 at followup. Logistic Regression Models based on participants with CDR=0 at baseline:
Univariable model (adjusted for age, gender, and education) | Stepwise multivariable model* | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | OR | 95% CI for OR | P value | OR | 95% CI for OR | P value | ||
Age 75–84† | -- | -- | -- | -- | 1.30 | 0.68 | 2.48 | 0.4213 |
Age ≥85† | -- | -- | -- | -- | 1.28 | 0.58 | 2.81 | 0.5456 |
Female | -- | -- | -- | -- | 1.62 | 0.95 | 2.77 | 0.0788 |
Education ≥high school†† | -- | -- | -- | -- | 1.39 | 0.67 | 2.92 | 0.3801 |
MMSE | 0.76 | 0.67 | 0.86 | <.0001 | 0.86 | 0.75 | 0.97 | 0.0177 |
WRAT-3 Reading | 0.97 | 0.93 | 1.02 | 0.2869 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
Digit Span Forward | 0.87 | 0.69 | 1.09 | 0.2203 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
Category Fluency | 0.89 | 0.84 | 0.95 | 0.0002 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
Letter Fluency | 0.93 | 0.88 | 0.99 | 0.0244 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
Boston Naming Test | 0.91 | 0.87 | 0.95 | <.0001 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
I.U. Token Test | 0.77 | 0.66 | 0.89 | 0.0005 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
Block Design | 0.97 | 0.94 | 0.99 | 0.0185 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
WMS-R Visual Reproduction (Immediate) | 0.93 | 0.90 | 0.97 | <.0001 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
WMS-R Visual Reproduction (Delayed) | 0.91 | 0.88 | 0.94 | <.0001 | 0.93 | 0.90 | 0.96 | <.0001 |
WMS-R Logical Memory (Immediate) | 0.93 | 0.89 | 0.97 | 0.0002 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
WMS-R Logical Memory (Delayed) | 0.92 | 0.89 | 0.96 | <.0001 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
OME sum of 3 trials | 0.87 | 0.82 | 0.93 | <.0001 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
SIT “Bag B” Recall | 0.70 | 0.61 | 0.81 | <.0001 | 0.84 | 0.72 | 0.98 | 0.0266 |
Clock Drawing | 0.91 | 0.77 | 1.06 | 0.2246 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
Trail Making Test A | 1.02 | 1.01 | 1.03 | 0.0020 | -- | -- | -- | -- |
Trail Making Test B | 1.01 | 1.01 | 1.02 | <.0001 | 1.01 | 1.00 | 1.01 | 0.0908 |
During the stepwise procedure, age, gender, and education were always included. Area under the ROC curve = 0.78 in final stepwise multivariable model.
The two older groups each compared to the 65–74 group (the reference group).
≥ High school education cCompared to < high school education (the reference group).
Collinearity
For both CDR improvement (Table 3) and CDR worsening (Table 4), the multivariable model identified the neuropsychological measures that independently predicted the outcomes after adjustment for the other measures. None of the variables in the final stepwise models had VIF values above 2.5, i.e., there was no multicollinearity within these models.
DISCUSSION
The clinical and prognostic significance of minimal cognitive and functional difficulties can be difficult to ascertain in older adults. In specialty health care and research settings, the state represented by a Clinical Dementia Rating of 0.5, when assessed by trained clinicians exercising clinical judgment, has been considered equivalent to an entity referred to as mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and even to early Alzheimer's disease (Morris, 2006). It is also a widely employed measure of functional status in longitudinal studies of cognitive decline among elderly subjects in clinical and epidemiological studies, and is rated without reference to objective cognitive deficits. For these reasons, we chose CDR as the outcome measure for our current study examining neuropsychological predictors of functional change, although we recognize it was never intended as a fine-grained measure of everyday functioning. Note that, as used here, the CDR does not represent a disease severity rating or any specific etiological entity. MCI is characterized by both subjective complaints and objectively measured deficits in the presence of minimal functional impairment, and typically described as a state intermediate or transitional between normal aging and dementia (Petersen, 2004; Winblad et al., 2004). However, the terms “intermediate” and “transitional” are not synonymous or interchangeable, i.e., not everyone with mild impairment is on the road to dementia. In specialty clinical research settings, individuals with MCI (especially of the amnestic variety) or CDR=0.5 do have an overwhelming probability of progressing to dementia of the Alzheimer's type (Morris, 2006; Petersen et al., 2009). Yet, in every population study that has been reported worldwide, substantial proportions of older adults in the community meet the same criteria but remain stable at the mildly impaired level, or even improve and revert to normal. A recent meta-analysis of 41 cohort studies concluded that only 5–10% of individuals with MCI will progress to dementia even after 10 years of followup (Mitchell & Shiri-Feshki, 2009). The reasons for the discrepancy between results of clinical and population studies may be myriad but must surely include referral/selection factors (Bruscoli & Lovestone, 2004). A study comparing individuals with MCI recruited from clinic and community settings in the same area found that they progressed to dementia at annual rates of 13% and 2% respectively, with recruitment source itself carrying an over threefold risk elevation of incident dementia. Baseline functional level was the only variable that accounted for this difference (Farias et al., 2009). Older adults who seek services for cognitive difficulties, and particularly those who volunteer for clinical research programs, are presumably those who feel more distressed or functionally impaired by these difficulties than those who do not present themselves at these facilities. Further, participation in many clinical studies of cognitive impairment requires the absence of substantial medical comorbidity and the availability of a reliable surrogate (informant). In contrast, individuals are included in population studies regardless of whether they have spontaneous complaints, comorbidity, or informants. By definition, they are more representative of the community from which they are drawn than are participants in typical clinical studies. Memory clinics have the advantage of applying in-depth clinical and laboratory assessments and expert individualized clinical judgment to every case. Population studies have the advantages of standardized assessments by trained but not expert examiners, large numbers, and enhanced generalizability. Thus, the two broad genres of studies provide essential complements to each other.
In a large, population-based cohort of older adults, drawn randomly from the local communities, we performed detailed neuropsychological assessments as well as clinical dementia ratings at study entry and a year later. The CDR assesses cognitively-driven everyday functioning. The majority of study participants remained stable at either CDR=0 or CDR=0.5 over one year. Of those with no functional impairment (CDR=0) at baseline, 7% had developed mild impairment a year later. In epidemiological terms, this might be considered analogous to “incidence” of mild impairment, although we emphasize that we do not consider CDR=0.5 in this setting to be equivalent to the clinical concept of MCI. Nearly all the neuropsychological test measures at the initial assessment were lower in those participants whose everyday functioning subsequently worsened, compared to the stable group. However, only tests of global cognitive function, of delayed visual memory, and of recall susceptible to proactive semantic interference, were significantly and independently associated with worsening, after adjusting for demographics and all other tests. This finding was as hypothesized given that the CDR is weighted towards memory. Some tests were univariably associated with change but lost statistical significance in the final model because they were collinear with other tests; however, they still robustly predicted CDR worsening on their own.
On the other hand, nearly 30% of individuals with mild impairment at baseline appeared normal a year later, a figure consistent with earlier population studies (Ganguli et al., 2004; Larrieu et al., 2002; Manly et al., 2008; Palmer et al., 2002; Ritchie et al., 2001). This phenomenon of reversion to normal is broadly attributed to the heterogeneity of mild impairment outside treatment settings but its mechanism remains a matter for speculation. It might represent true improvement in the condition underlying the original impairment, if the condition was transient and self-limiting, or reversed by treatment. Alternatively it might represent random fluctuation or instability of measurement, which should rectify itself with longer followup. To our knowledge, there have been few previous reports of cognitive or other factors which might distinguish between those who will remain mildly impaired and those who will demonstrate the apparent improvement. In one earlier population study, individuals with impairment in only a single cognitive domain at baseline were the most likely to revert to normal at followup (Manly et al., 2008). In a study of volunteers in a memory research program, individuals who were classified as MCI on the basis of only one test in a given domain, and had higher test scores at baseline, were those most likely to be normal at followup (Loewenstein et al., 2009). In our cohort, better initial performance on a list learning measure and two language tests independently predicted subsequent improvement.
Our findings help to reinforce and consolidate several points. We have previously shown in this population that, at baseline, cognitive test scores were significantly lower in individuals with concomitant CDR=0.5 than in those with CDR=0 (Ganguli et al., 2010). We now show that lower test performance at baseline also independently predicts subsequent change in CDR, in the expected direction. Since the CDR rates impairments in everyday functioning without reference to neuropsychological test data, the two measures are independent and their associations provide some cross-validation of each other, while also suggesting that objective tests have demonstrable predictive value with regard to functional change.
Further, by demonstrating these findings in a population-based cohort, using trained interviewers who are not expert clinicians but employ a standardized assessment protocol, we have shown that these associations are potentially generalizable from the clinical research setting to the community at large. Our findings also show these standard measurement tools and ratings to be robust in well-trained non-expert hands. This is not a trivial issue given the prohibitive costs of providing participants in population studies with the type of assessments conducted by highly qualified and experienced clinicians in tertiary care and research settings.
An unbiased population-based sample can at best be representative of the population from which it was drawn. Consequently, its results are at best generalizable to other similar populations. Our sample is representative of the region's older population, but over 10% had completed less than high school education, and only 5% were from ethnic U.S. minorities. Thus, our results are likely not completely generalizable to populations with substantially lesser education or substantially greater racial/ethnic diversity, even within the U.S. For example, literacy or reading level (as measured by the WRAT-3) might be more informative in such a cohort than it was in ours (Manly et al, 2002). Hence, replication is needed in a variety of population samples.
Some limitations are inherent in the population study design. The large cohort, its accrual by random sampling from the voter registration list, and the conduct of most of the assessments in participants' homes, are all assets that enhance the generalizability of its results. The same characteristics limit the depth of the assessments that can be conducted and render it impractical to have expert clinicians perform them. Most cognitively intact or very mildly impaired older adults in the community, who are not seeking help for cognitive concerns, do not have their everyday functioning closely monitored by others who can serve as reliable informants. Thus, in our study, assessments were based on participants' self-reports, objective measurements, and the observations of trained raters. Our interviewers were trained to complete the CDR ratings based on these data, and after review and discussion of these with other interviewers, ignoring the neuropsychological data. Because the entire assessment of each participant was done by the same interviewer, the possibility remains that, at some level, the CDR could not be entirely independent of the neuropsychological data. We would point out, however, that the current analyses examined the relationship between individual tests at baseline and change in CDR over one year. It is unlikely that an interviewer rating the CDR at followup could remember and be influenced by the participant's neuropsychological test performance or CDR rating in the previous year, in a cohort this large. Finally, this report is focused on neuropsychological predictors, and does not examine other variables such as depression (Teng et al., 2007), anxiety (Palmer et al., 2007), vascular risk factors (Solfrizzi et al., 2004), or brain atrophy (Jack et al., 2005), all of which have previously been reported as predictors of progression from mild impairment to dementia. Over a single year of followup, most normal or very mildly impaired individuals do not show substantial changes in functioning, and some improvements and decrements in the short term might represent random fluctuations. Continued followup with repeated assessments will be necessary to tease out these patterns and to determine their clinical significance.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors gratefully acknowledge the valuable input of Frederick Unverzagt, PhD, the valiant efforts of the MYHAT research team, and the cooperation of the entire MYHAT cohort of senior citizens.
The work reported here was supported in part by grants # K24AG022035, P30AG005133, R01AG023651, R01AG020094, from the National Institute on Aging, US DHHS
REFERENCES
- Benton AL, Hamsher KD. Multilingual Aphasia Examination. 3rd edition AJA Associates; Iowa City: 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Bruscoli M, Lovestone S. Is MCI really just early dementia? A systematic review of conversion studies. International Psychogeriatrics. 2004;16(2):129–140. doi: 10.1017/s1041610204000092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Busse A, Hensel A, Guhne U, Angermeyer MC, Riedel-Heller SG. Mild cognitive impairment: long-term course of four clinical subtypes. Neurology. 2006;67(12):2176–2185. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000249117.23318.e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeCarli C, Mungas D, Harvey D, Reed B, Weiner M, Chui H, Jagust W. Memory impairment, but not cerebrovascular disease, predicts progression of MCI to dementia. Neurology. 2004;63(2):220–227. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000130531.90205.ef. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farias ST, Mungas D, Reed BR, Harvey D, DeCarli C. Progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia in clinic- vs. community-based cohorts. Archives of Neurology. 2009;66(9):1151–1157. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleisher AS, Sowell BB, Taylor C, Gamst AC, Petersen RC, Thal LJ, Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative, S. Clinical predictors of progression to Alzheimer disease in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neurology. 2007;68(19):1588–1595. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000258542.58725.4c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-Mental State: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freedman M, Leach L, Kaplan E, Winocur G, Shulman KI, Delis D. Clock drawing: A neuropsychological analysis. Oxford University Press, Inc.; New York: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Fuld PA. Fuld Object-Memory Evaluation. Stoelting Company; Woodale, IL: 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Ganguli M, Dodge HH, Chen P, Belle S, DeKosky ST. Ten-year incidence of dementia in a rural elderly US community population: the MoVIES Project. Neurology. 2000;54(5):1109–1116. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.5.1109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ganguli M, Dodge HH, Shen C, DeKosky ST. Mild cognitive impairment, amnestic type: an epidemiologic study. Neurology. 2004;63(1):115–121. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000132523.27540.81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ganguli M, Snitz B, Vander Bilt J, Chang CC-H. How much do depressive symptoms affect cognition at the population level? The Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team (MYHAT) study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2009;24(11):1277–1284. doi: 10.1002/gps.2257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ganguli M, Snitz BE, Lee C-W, Vander Bilt J, Saxton JA, Chang C-CH. Age and education effects and norms on a cognitive test battery from a population-based cohort: The Monongahela -Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team (MYHAT) Aging and Mental Health. 2010a;14(1):109–116. doi: 10.1080/13607860903071014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ganguli M, Chang C-CH, Snitz BE, Saxton JA, Vander Bilt J, Lee C-W. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment by multiple classifications: The MYHAT project. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2010b doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181cdee4f. ePub ahead of print: DOI 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181cdee4f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grober E, Hall CB, Lipton RB, Zonderman AB, Resnick SM, Kawas C. Memory impairment, executive dysfunction, and intellectual decline in preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2008;14(2):266–278. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708080302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howieson DB, Carlson NE, Moore MM, Wasserman D, Abendroth CD, Payne-Murphy J, Kaye JA. Trajectory of mild cognitive impairment onset. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2008;14(2):192–198. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708080375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL, Coben LA, Martin RL. A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1982;140:566–572. doi: 10.1192/bjp.140.6.566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iwasa H, Gondo Y, Yoshida Y, Kwon J, Inagaki H, Kawaai C, Masui Y, Kim H, Yoshida H, Suzuki T. Cognitive performance as a predictor of functional decline among the non-disabled elderly dwelling in a Japanese community: a 4-year population-based prospective cohort study. Archives of Gerontology & Geriatrics. 2008;47(1):139–149. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2007.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jack CR, Jr., Shiung MM, Weigand SD, O'Brien PC, Gunter JL, Boeve BF, Knopman DS, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Petersen RC. Brain atrophy rates predict subsequent clinical conversion in normal elderly and amnestic MCI. Neurology. 2005;65(8):1227–1231. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000180958.22678.91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan EF, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. The Boston Naming Test. E Kaplan & H Goodglass; Boston: 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Kluger A, Ferris SH, Golomb J, Mittelman MS, Reisberg B. Neuropsychological prediction of decline to dementia in nondemented elderly. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry & Neurology. 1999;12(4):168–179. doi: 10.1177/089198879901200402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larrieu S, Letenneur L, Orgogozo JM, Fabrigoule C, Amieva H, Le Carret N, Barberger-Gateau P, Dartigues JF. Incidence and outcome of mild cognitive impairment in a population-based prospective cohort. Neurology. 2002;59(10):1594–1599. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000034176.07159.f8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loewenstein DA, Acevedo A, Schram L, Ownby R, White G, Mogosky B, Barker WW, Duara R. Semantic interference in mild Alzheimer disease: preliminary findings. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2003;11(2):252–255. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loewenstein DA, Acevedo A, Luis C, Crum T, Barker WW, Duara R. Semantic interference deficits and the detection of mild Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment without dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2004;10(1):91–100. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704101112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loewenstein DA, Acevedo A, Agron J, Duara R. Stability of neurocognitive impairment in different subtypes of mild cognitive impairment. Dementia & Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 2007;23(2):82–86. doi: 10.1159/000097304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loewenstein DA, Acevedo A, Small BJ, Agron J, Crocco E, Duara R. Stability of different subtypes of mild cognitive impairment among the elderly over a 2- to 3-year follow-up period. Dementia & Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 2009;27(5):418–423. doi: 10.1159/000211803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manly JJ, Jacobs DM, Touradji P, Small SA, Stern Y. Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2002;8:341–348. doi: 10.1017/s1355617702813157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manly JJ, Tang MX, Schupf N, Stern Y, Vonsattel JP, Mayeux R. Frequency and course of mild cognitive impairment in a multiethnic community. Annals of Neurology. 2008;63(4):494–506. doi: 10.1002/ana.21326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marquis S, Moore MM, Howieson DB, Sexton G, Payami H, Kaye JA, Camicioli R. Independent predictors of cognitive decline in healthy elderly persons. Archives of Neurology. 2002;59(4):601–606. doi: 10.1001/archneur.59.4.601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell AJ, Shiri-Feshki M. Rate of progression of mild cognitive impairment to dementia--meta-analysis of 41 robust inception cohort studies. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2009;119(4):252–265. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01326.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993;43(11):2412–2414. doi: 10.1212/wnl.43.11.2412-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris JC. Mild cognitive impairment is early-stage Alzheimer disease: time to revise diagnostic criteria. Archives of Neurology. 2006;63(1):15–16. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.1.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mungas D, Marshall SC, Weldon M, Haan M, Reed BR. Age and education correction of Mini-Mental State Examination for English and Spanish-speaking elderly. Neurology. 1996;46(3):700–706. doi: 10.1212/wnl.46.3.700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ott A, Breteler MM, van Harskamp F, Claus JJ, van der Cammen TJ, Grobbee DE, Hofman A. Prevalence of Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia: association with education. The Rotterdam study. BMJ. 1995;310(6985):970–973. doi: 10.1136/bmj.310.6985.970. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer K, Berger AK, Monastero R, Winblad B, Backman L, Fratiglioni L. Predictors of progression from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2007;68(19):1596–1602. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000260968.92345.3f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer K, Wang HX, Backman L, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Differential evolution of cognitive impairment in nondemented older persons: results from the Kungsholmen Project. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2002;159(3):436–442. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.3.436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Archives of Neurology. 1999;56(3):303–308. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. Journal of Internal Medicine. 2004;256(3):183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen RC, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, Boeve BF, Geda YE, Ivnik RJ, Smith GE, Jack CR. Mild cognitive impairment: Ten years later. Archives of Neurology. 2009;66(12):1447–1455. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prince M. Methodological issues for population-based research into dementia in developing countries. A position paper from the 10/66 Dementia Research Group. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2000;15(1):21–30. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-1166(200001)15:1<21::aid-gps71>3.0.co;2-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reitan RM. The relation of the Trailmaking Test to organic brain damage. J Consult Psychol. 1955;19:393–394. doi: 10.1037/h0044509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ritchie K, Artero S, Touchon J. Classification criteria for mild cognitive impairment: a population-based validation study. Neurology. 2001;56(1):37–42. doi: 10.1212/wnl.56.1.37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- SAS Institute . SAS v 9.2. Cary. North Carolina: 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Saxton J, Snitz BE, Lopez OL, Ives DG, Dunn LO, Fitzpatrick A, Carlson MC, Dekosky ST, Investigators GEMS. Functional and cognitive criteria produce different rates of mild cognitive impairment and conversion to dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry. 2009;80(7):737–743. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.160705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snitz BE, Unverzagt FW, Chang C-CH, Vander Bilt J, Gao S, Saxton J, Hall KS, Ganguli M. Effects of age, gender, education, and race on two language tests of language ability in community-based older adults. International Psychogeriatrics. 2009;21(6):1051–1062. doi: 10.1017/S1041610209990214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snitz BE, Loewenstein DA, Chang C-CH, Lee C-W, Vander Bilt J, Saxton JA, Ganguli M. A novel approach to assessing memory at the population level: vulnerability to semantic interference. International Psychogeriatrics. 2010 doi: 10.1017/S1041610209991657. ePub doi: 10.1017/S1041610209991657. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solfrizzi V, Panza F, Colacicco AM, D'Introno A, Capurso C, Torres F, Grigoletto F, Maggi S, Del Parigi A, Reiman EM, Caselli RJ, Scafato E, Farchi G, Capurso A, Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging Working, G. Vascular risk factors, incidence of MCI, and rates of progression to dementia. Neurology. 2004;63(10):1882–1891. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000144281.38555.e3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tabert MH, Manly JJ, Liu X, Pelton GH, Rosenblum S, Jacobs M, Zamora D, Goodkind M, Bell K, Stern Y, Devanand DP. Neuropsychological prediction of conversion to Alzheimer disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63(8):916–924. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.8.916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teng E, Lu PH, Cummings JL. Neuropsychiatric symptoms are associated with progression from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer's disease. Dementia & Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 2007;24(4):253–259. doi: 10.1159/000107100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Unverzagt FW, Farlow MR, Hendrie HC. Clinical utility of new visual learning memory and language subtests for use in the CERAD neuropsychologigcal battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 1999;5:129. doi: 10.1017/s1355617799003082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Washington University in St. Louis Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Clinical Dementia Rating, On-line Training. 2006 http://alzheimer.wustl.edu/cdr/default.htm.
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale Revised: The Psychological Corporation. 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Third Edition. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Wilkinson GS. Wide Range Achievement Test–Revision 3. Jastak Association; Wilmington, DE: 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L, Wahlund L-O, Nordberg A, Backman L, Albert M, Almkvist O, Arai H, Basun H, Blennow K, de Leon M, DeCarli C, Erkinjuntti T, Giacobini E, Graff C, Hardy J, Jack C, Jorm A, Ritchie K, van Duijn C, Visser P, Petersen RC. Mild Cognitive Impairment - beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. Journal of Internal Medicine. 2004;256:240–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]