Abstract
This study examined stopping and performance adjustments in four age groups (M ages: 8, 12, 21, and 76 years). All participants performed on three tasks, a standard two-choice task and the same task in which stop-signal trials were inserted requiring either the suppression of the response activated by the choice stimulus (global stop task) or the suppression of the response when one stop-signal was presented but not when the other stop-signal occurred (selective stop task). The results showed that global stopping was faster than selective stopping in all age groups. Global stopping matured more rapidly than selective stopping. The developmental gain in stopping was considerably more pronounced compared to the loss observed during senescence. All age groups slowed the response on trials without a stop-signal in the stop task compared to trials in the choice task, the elderly in particular. In addition, all age groups slowed on trials following stop-signal trials, except the elderly who did not slow following successful inhibits. By contrast, the slowing following failed inhibits was disproportionally larger in the elderly compared to young adults. Finally, sequential effects did not alter the pattern of performance adjustments. The results were interpreted in terms of developmental change in the balance between proactive and reactive control.
Keywords: stop-signal paradigm, development, cognitive aging, lifespan, cognitive control
Introduction
The ability to dynamically adjust to the changing environment is critical for survival. One important component of this ability is to refrain from responding when a readied response is no longer required. In the laboratory, the ability to withhold motor responses has been examined frequently by using the stop-signal paradigm (Logan and Cowan, 1984; Verbruggen and Logan, 2008). In this paradigm, participants are typically presented with a stimulus requiring either a speeded left- vs. right-hand response. On some trials, a stop-signal is presented, occasionally and unpredictably, just following the onset of the choice stimulus indicating that the response activated by the choice stimulus should be withheld. Assuming that the go process activated by the choice stimulus and the stop process activated by the stop-signal are involved in a race, won by whichever process finishes first, allows for an estimate of the duration of the stop process, or stop-signal reaction time (SSRT, Logan and Cowan, 1984). Most stop studies use a global stop-signal that requires participants to stop whatever response is activated by the choice stimulus. Global SSRT is typically around 200 ms (Logan, 1994; Verbruggen and Logan, 2008). Some stop studies used a selective stop-signal indicating that the response activated by the choice stimulus should be withheld whereas the other (invalid) stop-signal indicates that the readied response is to be executed. Selective SSRT is typically longer than global SSRT, about 30 ms (Bedard et al., 2002; van den Wildenberg and van der Molen, 2004a; van de Laar et al., 2010). The first aim of the current study is to examine developmental trends in global and selective stopping along the lifespan.
Developmental studies showed that global stopping is usually slower in children than adults (Williams et al., 1999; but see Oosterlaan and Sergeant, 1996; Band et al., 2000; Johnstone et al., 2007). With advancing age the speed of global stopping seems to decline during senescence (Kramer et al., 1994; May and Hasher, 1998; Ridderinkhof et al., 1999; Rush et al., 2006; Gamboz et al., 2009) but the age-related decline in global stopping speed has been observed to be considerably less than the developmental gain during childhood. Thus, a lifespan analysis of global stopping revealed that older children (11 years) stopped about 50 ms faster than young children (7.5 years). By contrast, the elderly (60–81 years) were only about 20 ms slower in global stopping compared to young adults (Williams et al., 1999). The age-related decline was even absent in a study reported by Kray et al. (2009). Larger declines have been observed by Kramer et al. (1994) and May and Hasher (1998) but in these studies the primary choice task was more difficult than in the Williams et al. (1999) study and, thus, the cognitive demands imposed by the primary choice might have reduced the resources available for stopping, in particular in the elderly (cf., Williams et al., 1999).
Although only a few studies investigated developmental change in selective stopping, the results that emerged from these studies are similar to the pattern observed for global stopping. The lifespan study of selective stopping reported by Bedard et al. (2002) showed that selective stopping was slower; 248 ms in young adults compared to 209 ms for global stopping as observed previously by Williams et al. (1999). Furthermore, it was observed that the speed of selective stopping improved until young adulthood indicating that selective stopping takes longer to mature than global stopping. Finally, the difference between young adults and the elderly was about 60 ms whereas it was only 21 ms for global stopping, as observed by Williams et al. (1999), indicating that selective stopping may impose a larger burden on the elderly compared to global stopping. We observed a similar developmental pattern in a direct comparison of global vs. selective stopping (van den Wildenberg and van der Molen, 2004a). That is, selective stopping was slower than global stopping, in young adults (i.e., 232 vs. 205 ms), and selective stopping matured slower than global stopping.
The current study will provide a direct comparison between age-related change in global vs. selective stopping along the lifespan. Such a comparison is still lacking in the stop-signal literature and may reveal potentially interesting differences in the developmental trends associated with global vs. selective stopping. A particular focus of the current study was on the performance on invalid stop-signal trials of the selective stop task. Invalid stop-signal trials are trials on which a stop-signal is presented that should be ignored by the participant. In our previous studies we observed that the speed of responding on invalid stop-signal trials was considerably delayed compared to trials without a stop-signal (van den Wildenberg and van der Molen, 2004a,b; van de Laar et al., 2010). Thus the stop-signal is not ignored by the participants (or not ignored on a substantial proportion of the invalid trials). Previously, we have argued that the delay might be due to the initial inhibition of all responses and the subsequent re-activation of the appropriate response following the complete analysis of the information provided by the stop-signal (van de Laar et al., 2010). In this regard, the performance on these trials might provide valuable information regarding the strategy employed by the participant. Based on previous findings (Band et al., 2000), we predicted that the re-initiation strategy would be more prominent in children compared to young adults. The current study would be first in providing data regarding invalid stop-signal trial performance in the elderly.
The second aim of the current study is to examine performance adjustments related to the insertion of stop-signal trials in the choice reaction task (CRT). Several studies focused on performance adjustments in the stop-signal paradigm (e.g., Rieger and Gauggel, 1999; Verbruggen and Logan, 2008, 2009; Verbruggen et al., 2008; Aron, 2011; Bissett and Logan, 2011). Rieger and Gauggel (1999) observed that the speed of responding to the choice stimulus was delayed on trials following a stop-signal trial. The delay was somewhat larger following a failed inhibit compared to following a successful inhibit and larger for trials repeating the choice stimulus compared to trials with alternating stimuli. The delay in responding following successful inhibits has been attributed to repetition priming (Verbruggen et al., 2008) and the added delay after failed inhibits has been interpreted in terms of trial adjustments or reactive control (Bissett and Logan, 2011).
Another type of control that might be exercised in the stop-signal task is referred to as proactive control (Verbruggen and Logan, 2009; see for a review Aron, 2011). In stop-signal experiments, participants are typically instructed not to wait for a stop-signal to occur. In spite of these instructions and despite experimental manipulations used to counter waiting strategies (e.g., the use of a tracking algorithm, see below) participants tend to slow their response when stop-signal trials are inserted in the CRT (Logan and Burkell, 1986; Ramautar et al., 2004; van de Laar et al., 2010). The slowing of choice reactions when stop-signal trials are inserted is interpreted in terms of a proactive change in response strategy aimed at increasing the success of stopping at the cost of the speed of choice reactions (e.g., Verbruggen and Logan, 2009).
Based on the performance data currently available we were led to predict that young adults would slow when stop-signal trials are inserted in the choice task (a manifestation of proactive control) and following stop-signal trials (indicating repetition priming when the choice stimulus is repeated and reactive control for alternating stimuli). Assuming that both children (e.g., Huizinga and van der Molen, 2010; Somerville and Casey, 2010) and the elderly (e.g., West and Schwarb, 2006; Mayda et al., 2011) are less efficient in implementing control procedures, we assumed that the response slowing associated with proactive and reactive control would be more pronounced in these age groups compared to young adults. Finally, it was predicted that the slowing associated with repetition priming would be more pronounced for children than adults based on recent findings indicating that the effects of repetition priming decrease when children are growing older (e.g., Crone et al., 2004; Smulders et al., 2005) and less pronounced or even absent for the elderly based on the repetition priming literature showing that repetition priming effects are basically age-invariant during senescence (e.g., Fleischman, 2007; Bergerbest et al., 2009, for reviews).
Materials and Methods
Participants
Four age groups participated in the study. There were two groups of children (a young child group of 8-years-old and an older child group of 12-years-old) and two groups of adults (young adults of 21-years-old and elderly adults of 76-years-old). Participant characteristics are presented in Table 1. Children were recruited from local elementary schools and selected with the help of their teachers. Informed consent was obtained from their primary caregivers. The children received a small present for participation. Young adults were university students who received course credits for participation. Healthy elderly participants were recruited from the metropolitan area. They received 40 euro for their participation. Informed consent was obtained from the adult participants. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. The study was approved by the local Ethics Committee.
Table 1.
Age group | Number | Age (M, SD) | Gender (f/m) | Raven percentile |
---|---|---|---|---|
Young children | 20 | 7.6 (0.59) | 10/10 | 90 (13.4) |
Older children | 20 | 11.9 (1.23) | 7/13 | 79 (17.5) |
Young adults | 17 | 20.8 (3.30) | 9/8 | 88 (3.6) |
Elderly adults | 19 | 75.6 (5.68) | 8/11 | 92 (4.9) |
Chi-square analyses indicated that gender distribution did not differ between groups, χ2(3) = 3.36, p = 0.34. To compare estimates of general levels of intelligence between age groups, the standard progressive matrices (SPM; Raven et al., 1985) was administered to children and the elderly, whereas young adults completed the Advanced Progressive Matrices (APM, Raven et al., 1993). Mean percentile scores differed significantly across age groups, F(3, 70) = 4.53, p < 0.006. As can be seen in Table 1, the older children had a lower percentile score compared to the other age groups (ps < 0.03). Additional analyses verified that significant outcomes survived when using Raven percentile scores as covariate.
Materials and stimuli
Participants were seated in a comfortable chair facing a computer screen at a distance of about 1.5 m. Each trial started with a white fixation cross (3 × 3 mm) appearing in the center of the screen for 500 ms followed by the go-signal. The go-signal consisted of a white left- or a right-pointing arrow (2.2 cm × 1.8 cm) presented centrally for 1000 ms against a dark-gray background subtending a visual angle of 1.9°. The inter-trial interval varied randomly between 1750 and 2250 ms, in steps of 50 ms. During this interval, the fixation cross was presented. Motor responses were collected until the offset of the arrow stimulus. On 35% of the trials the color of the white arrow changed to pink or blue, indicating a stop-signal. The stop-signal delay (SSD; i.e., the interval between the onset of the go-signal and the stop-signal) of the first stop trial in the practice block was set at 225 ms and was dynamically adjusted after valid stop trials throughout the experiment as a function of the stopping performance of the participant. Upon successful stopping, SSD on the subsequent stop trial increased with 25 ms, whereas a failure to inhibit decreased SSD on the next stop trial by 25 ms. This tracking algorithm (Levitt, 1971) was set to ensure 50% failed inhibits, which yields accurate estimates of stop-signal RT (Band et al., 2003).
Responses were collected by pressing a left or right zero-displacement force sensor (Honeywell, model FS03) with the left- or right thumb to the direction of the arrow stimulus. Thumbs rested on force sensors that were individually adjusted according to the arm length of the participant. A continue sample frequency was applied (>1000 Hz) and the proportional voltage signal was A/D converted online (Keithley, model KPCI-1802 using DrvrLINX4). The value of 15% of the maximum force served as individual RT threshold which was determined prior to the experiment for each thumb separately (van Boxtel et al., 2001).
Experimental tasks
The choice RT task CRT consisted of go-signals only, stop-signals were not included. Participants were instructed to press the left or right thumb to a white arrow pointing left or right, respectively. The CRT task consisted of one experimental block of 50 trials. All blocks contained equal numbers of right- and left-pointing arrows that were varied randomly within a block.
When performing the global stop-signal task (GST), participants were required to respond to go-signals according to the CRT instruction described above. Trials without a stop-signal are referred to as go trials. On 35% of the trials, the stop-signal occurred instructing the participant to refrain from responding. The stop-signal consisted of either a blue or pink color change of the white arrow (counterbalanced across participants). Participants were informed about the tracking algorithm and it was explained that a “waiting” strategy would not increase the success of stopping. The GST consisted of four experimental blocks, each containing 100 trials.
In the selective stop-signal task (SST), participants were instructed to respond to go trials as described above, but in this task both stop-signals occurred. Participants were required to discriminate between stop-signals and to inhibit their response to one stop-signal (e.g., white-to-pink color change) but to ignore the other stop-signal (e.g., white-to-blue color change). Stop-signal colors were counterbalanced across participants. Stop trials requiring participants to stop were coined “valid” stop-signal trials and stop trials on which the stop-signal should be ignored were coined “invalid” stop-signal trials (e.g., van de Laar et al., 2010). Stop-signals occurred on 35% of the trials – half of the trials were valid and the other half of the trials were invalid. Participants performed eight experimental blocks of 100 trials.
Procedure
The participants performed on all three experimental tasks – CRT, GST, and SST. The tasks were administered during two separate sessions on different days that were scheduled within 5 days. Each session started with the CRT task followed by one of the stop tasks. The order of the two stop tasks was counterbalanced across participants. The duration of an experimental trial block was approximately 5 min and each block was followed by performance feedback. Each task started with one practice block followed by the experimental blocks. A block started with five warm-up trials that were discarded from analyses. Short breaks were given between blocks and a longer rest was given between tasks.
Data analyses
Data of six young children (CRT data from five children and GST data from one child), an older child (CRT data), and one elderly participant (SST data) were discarded from analyses, because of unreliable recordings of force pressure. Repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVA) were conducted on latency measures (median RT) and error proportions (errors and omissions). Multiple comparisons were used to confirm effects. Degrees of freedom and p-values were adjusted using Greenhouse–Geisser corrections. Since error proportions are not normally distributed, tests were performed on square rooted error values.
The integration method based on the horse-race model was used to obtain estimates of stopping latencies (the stop-signal RT or SSRT, Logan and Cowan, 1984). Using this model, the finish of the stop process can be estimated from the go RT distribution. The left side of the distribution of the RTs on go trials represents fast responses that escape inhibition whereas the right side represents slow responses that will be inhibited. If a given participant actually failed to inhibit on n% of the stop trials, the finishing time of the stop process will approximately be equal to the nth percentile of the go RT distribution. The mean SSD is then subtracted from the nth percentile of the go RT distribution, resulting in an estimate of SSRT (see Figure 1).
Performance adjustments following a stop trial were examined by comparing trial duplets (go–go vs. successful stop–go vs. failed stop–go vs. invalid stop–go). Within duplets, repetitions vs. alternations were distinguished. A repetition duplet consisted of a trial in which the go stimulus indicated the same response as the one on the immediately preceding trial, whereas an alternation duplet consisted of a trial on which the go stimulus indicated the opposite response.
Significant interaction effects including Age Group were analyzed further by transforming RTs to the natural logarithm to reduce the influence of differences between age groups in baseline performance (e.g., Meiran, 1996; Huizinga and van der Molen, 2010). Thus, interactions resulting from ANOVA on the transformed data indicate a disproportional difference in RT between age groups in one condition relative to another.
Results
The results will be presented in three major sections. In the first section, we will examine age-related trends in the speed of responding on go trials and will ask whether these trends differ between tasks (CRT, GST, and SST). A particular question that will be addressed refers to age-related changes in the speed of responding when stop-signals are inserted into the task. Based on previous studies, we anticipated that, in spite of the tracking algorithm, participants would delay their speed of responding on go trials when they might encounter stop-signals (Verbruggen and Logan, 2009; Aron, 2011).
In the second section, we focus on age-related changes in the speed of stopping and we will ask whether trends differ between global vs. selective stopping. A second question that will be addressed in this section is on the speed of responding when stopping fails (i.e., failed inhibits). Based on the horse-race model underlying the stop-signal paradigm (Logan and Cowan, 1984), it would be predicted that the failed inhibits are faster than responses on go trials. We anticipated obtaining this pattern for each of the age groups. Finally, we will focus on the speed of responding on invalid stop-signal trials. In previous studies, we observed that the speed of responding on invalid stop-signal trials was considerably slower than on go trials (van den Wildenberg and van der Molen, 2004a; van de Laar et al., 2010). Age-related differences in the slowing on invalid stop-signal trials may contribute to our understanding of how stopping is realized in each of the age groups participating in the current study. In the third section, we will examine performance adjustments following stop-signal trials. First, we asked whether age groups differed in the speed of responding following a successful inhibit relative to the speed on a go trial following another go trial. Second, we asked whether the age groups differed in the speed of responding following a failed inhibit. Third, we examined age-related changes in the speed of responding following an invalid stop-signal trial. For all analyses we examined whether performance adjustments differed between repetition vs. alternation trials. Obviously, it would be of considerable interest to assess whether age-related patterns in performance adjustment are similar across trial type suggesting a single underlying mechanism.
Performance on go trials
Median RTs and error percentages on go trials are presented in Table 2 for each task (CRT, GST, and SST) and age group. The typical lifespan pattern is observed for the RTs of each task – the speed of responding increases into adulthood and slows down in the elderly (see Figure 2). It can be seen that, in spite of the instructions and the dynamic tracking of SSD, all age groups slowed their speed of responding when the task included stop-signals. For all age groups, the slowing seems more pronounced on the GST compared to the SST. These visual impressions were verified by submitting median RTs to ANOVA with Task (3), as a within Ss factor, and Age Group as a between Ss factor. The ANOVA yielded main effects of Task, F(2, 128) = 82.66, p < 0.001, and Age Group, F(3, 64) = 49.35, p < 0.001, and their interaction was significant also, F(6, 128) = 8.47, p < 0.001. Subsequent analyses indicated that all age groups showed a delay in responding associated with the insertion of stop-signals (ps < 0.001). The delay was more pronounced for the GST compared to the SST in the child groups and young adults (ps < 0.002), but for the elderly adults the delay did not differ between tasks (p = 0.44). Log transformed analyses indicated that the delay was disproportionally larger in the elderly adults compared to the other age groups on both stop-signal tasks (ps < 0.007). The delay was disproportionally larger for the young children compared to young adults on the GST (p < 0.02), and marginally larger for the SST (p = 0.06).
Table 2.
Task | CRT |
GST |
SST |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age group | RT (ms) | Error (%) | Omission (%) | RT (ms) | Error (%) | Omission (%) | RT (ms) | Error (%) | Omission (%) |
Young children | 488 (70.8) | 9.6 (5.6) | 2.3 (3.1) | 613 (59.0) | 5.3 (3.0) | 4.1 (4.7) | 577 (70.7) | 6.2 (3.5) | 2.7 (2.3) |
Older children | 373 (45.3) | 8.0 (5.7) | 0.1 (0.5) | 462 (56.8) | 3.2 (2.5) | 1.2 (2.5) | 426 (45.6) | 4.2 (2.9) | 0.5 (0.9) |
Young adults | 296 (22.2) | 2.7 (3.0) | 0.1 (0.5) | 342 (25.9) | 1.3 (1.7) | 0.2 (0.4) | 326 (28.6) | 1.5 (1.9) | 0.0 (0.1) |
Elderly adults | 435 (61.7) | 3.2 (2.8) | 0.1 (0.5) | 631 (153.7) | 1.5 (1.7) | 0.6 (1.2) | 601 (137.8) | 0.9 (1.2) | 1.0 (2.3) |
A similar analysis was performed on the error percentages. Analyses yielded a main effect of Task, F(2, 128) = 20.03, p < 0.001, and Age Group, F(3, 64) = 17.51, p < 0.001, but their interaction was not significant, F(6, 128) = 1.54, p = 0.19. Subsequent analysis revealed higher error rates on the CRT compared to the stopping tasks (ps < 0.001). Young children committed more errors compared to older children (p = 0.04), and older children made more errors than the two adult groups (ps < 0.001). Error rates did not differ between the two adult groups (p = 0.91).
Analyses on omission errors (i.e., errors associated with a failure to respond to the go-signal) yielded a significant main effect of Task, F(2, 128) = 8.72, p < 0.001, and Age Group, F(3, 64) = 15.64, p < 0.001, but their interaction was not significant, F(6, 128) = 1.12, p = 0.35. Omission rate was higher on the stopping tasks compared to the CRT (ps < 0.002). Young children committed more omissions than the other age groups (ps < 0.001).
Performance on stop-signal trials
Stop-signal reaction time, SSD, and the percentage of successful inhibits are presented in Table 3 for each age group and both stop-signal tasks. It can be seen that the SSRTs are relatively short in both tasks. Typically, young adults’ global SSRT to auditory stop-signals is about 200 ms (Logan, 1994; Verbruggen and Logan, 2008), and SSRT to visual stop-signals is longer compared to auditory ones (van der Schoot et al., 2005). The current SSRT to visual stop-signals, however, is only 181 ms in young adults. In a previous study, using a similar GST, adult SSRT was 210 ms (van de Laar et al., 2010). Most likely, the relatively short SSRTs in the present study are due to the specifics of the response device (see also van Boxtel et al., 2001). Force transducers were used in the current study whereas a computer keyboard was used in the study reported by van de Laar et al. (2010). Typically, computer keyboards involve a considerable delay in transmission time (e.g., Li et al., 2010).
Table 3.
Task | GST |
SST |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age group | SSRT (ms) | SSD (ms) | Inhibits (%) | SSRT (ms) | SSD (ms) | Inhibits (%) |
Young children | 276 (36.4) | 329 (59.5) | 51.1 (1.6) | 356 (49.3) | 218 (87.8) | 49.7 (2.7) |
Older children | 203 (29.6) | 256 (61.9) | 50.0 (1.3) | 277 (47.2) | 149 (55.3) | 49.0 (2.9) |
Young adults | 181 (17.4) | 159 (27.2) | 50.6 (0.9) | 203 (26.8) | 124 (32.9) | 49.6 (1.7) |
Elderly adults | 206 (23.5) | 416 (154.4) | 52.0 (2.6) | 224 (23.7) | 368 (132.2) | 52.1 (2.7) |
The Task × Age Group ANOVA on SSRT yielded significant main effects of Task, F(1,70) = 138.28, p < 0.001, and Age Group, F(3, 70) = 61.6, p < 0.001, which were included in a significant Task × Age Group interaction, F(3,70) = 15.71, p < 0.001, which is plotted in Figure 3. Subsequent analyses for each task, separately, yielded a significant effect of Age Group for the GST, F(3, 71) = 40.98, p < 0.001. Analysis on the log transformed data showed that young children stopped slowest and young adults stopped fastest (ps < 0.009), with older children and elderly adults having intermediate stopping times (p = 0.67). The effect of Age Group was significant also for the SST, F(3, 71) = 57.18, p < 0.001. Again, analysis on log transformed SSRTs indicated that young children stopped slowest and young adults stopped fastest (ps < 0.03). But now, older children stopped disproportionally faster than elderly adults (p < 0.001). Finally, selective stopping was slower than global stopping in all age groups (ps < 0.001). The analysis on log transformed data revealed that stopping speed on the SST relative to GST was disproportionally slower in the older and young child groups (respectively 1.4 and 1.3 times, p = 0.78) compared to the two adult groups (1.1 times, p = 0.72; ps < 0.001).
Finally, as can be seen in Table 3, the percentage of successful inhibits was close to 50% for all groups on both stopping tasks. This indicates that the tracking algorithm, which was targeted at 50% successful inhibits, worked quite well. The analysis done on the square rooted proportions showed that the proportion of successful inhibits was somewhat higher on the GST compared to the SST, F(1, 70) = 5.27, p = 0.03. Stopping success was somewhat higher in the elderly compared to the other age groups (ps < 0.006).
Table 4 presents the RTs associated with failed inhibit responses for both stopping tasks and the RTs associated with invalid stop-signal trials of the SST together with the percentage of omissions on this task (i.e., an invalid stop-signal was presented but the participant failed to respond). The age-related changes in the speed of failed inhibits and responses on invalid stop-signal trials are plotted in Figure 4.
Table 4.
Task | GST | SST |
||
---|---|---|---|---|
Age group | FIRT (ms) | FIRT (ms) | IRT (ms) | Omissions (%) |
Young children | 519 (53.0) | 517 (56.7) | 648 (77.4) | 3.6 (3.5) |
Older children | 403 (40.1) | 400 (41.4) | 503 (78.0) | 1.1 (1.7) |
Young adults | 316 (21.3) | 320 (30.7) | 394 (37.0) | 0.6 (1.1) |
Elderly adults | 554 (134.4) | 532 (110.3) | 721 (155.0) | 2.6 (3.5) |
Median invalid reaction time (IRT) and proportion of omissions for the SST in each age group (SD between parentheses).
First, the Task × Age Group ANOVA revealed that failed inhibit RTs were shorter than go RTs on the GST for all age groups, Fs > 64.43, ps < 0.001, as predicted by the horse-race model underlying the analysis of stopping speed (e.g., Logan and Cowan, 1984). This observation is consistent with the notion that failed inhibits are fast responses escaping inhibition. For the SST, failed inhibit RTs were shorter than go RTs for the two child groups and elderly adults, Fs > 48.69, ps < 0.001, but not for young adults, F(1, 16) = 2.66, p = 0.12. Failed inhibit RTs did not differ between stop tasks, F < 1, but there was a substantial difference across age groups, F(3, 70) = 45.85, p < 0.001. Log transformed analysis indicated that failed inhibit RT was shortest in young adults compared to all three other age groups (ps < 0.001). Failed inhibit RT was shorter in older children relative to young children and the elderly (ps < 0.001). Failed inhibit RT did not differ between young children and the elderly (p = 0.58).
Secondly, in Table 4 it can be seen that failed inhibit RTs are basically similar across the two stopping tasks suggesting that the impact of stop-signal processing on the response process must have been minimal or even absent on these trials. In contrast, the responses on invalid stop trials of the SST are very slow; much slower than the speed of responding on (non-signal) go trials. The Task × Age Group analysis revealed that invalid RTs were longer compared to go RTs in all age groups, F(3, 71) = 4.76, p = 0.004. The analysis on log transformed data indicated that the slowing on invalid stop trials relative to go trials was disproportionally larger in elderly adults compared to the other age groups (ps < 0.009).
Finally, performance on invalid stop-signal trials was relatively accurate. The proportion of omissions remained below 5% for each age group. Statistical analysis yielded a significant Age Group effect on the proportion of omissions, F(3, 71) = 6.2, p < 0.001. Elderly adults failed to respond somewhat more frequently on invalid stop trials than young children and young children failed to respond somewhat more frequently than older children and young adults (ps < 0.01).
Performance adjustments
Go trials following go trials
To assess sequential effects on go RTs following go trials, we performed an ANOVA including Task (3), and Sequence (2) as within Ss factors and Age Group (4) as between Ss factor (see Table 5). The analysis yielded a significant interaction between Age Group and Sequence, F(3, 64) = 8.11, p < 0.001. Sequence did not interact with Task, F < 1. Follow-up analysis indicated that the speed of responding on repetitions was similar to alternations in young adults (315 vs. 319 ms, p = 0.09) whereas the three other age groups showed the opposite pattern (564 vs. 526 ms for younger children, p < 0.001; 423 vs. 409 ms for older children, p < 0.01; and 561 vs. 542 ms, p < 0.04, for elderly adults). Subsequent analysis on log transformed data indicated that the relative alternation–repetition difference was larger in younger children and smaller in young adults relative to the two other age groups (ps < 0.03). Older children did not differ from the elderly adults in this regard (p = 0.62).
Table 5.
Task | CRT |
GST |
SST |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age group | Repetition | Alternation | Repetition | Alternation | Repetition | Alternation |
Young children | 517 (76.8) | 466 (71.0) | 619 (53.3) | 583 (63.0) | 587 (79.8) | 559 (61.9) |
Older children | 383 (46.2) | 365 (52.7) | 458 (59.5) | 445 (49.4) | 428 (49.4) | 417 (43.2) |
Young adults | 297 (25.7) | 296 (22.0) | 330 (24.7) | 337 (25.7) | 319 (28.3) | 325 (29.6) |
Elderly adults | 445 (64.5) | 424 (61.0) | 638 (165.4) | 621 (152.2) | 606 (168.3) | 591 (127.8) |
Go trials following stop trials
The Task (2; GST and SST) × Duplet (3; go–go, successful inhibit–go, and failed inhibit–go) × Age Group (4) ANOVA yielded a significant main effect of Duplet, F(2, 142) = 17.73, p < 0.001, which was included in a significant interaction with Age Group, F(6, 140) = 3.49, p = 0.003. A significant main effect of Task was found, F(1, 70) = 11.48, p = 0.001; the overall delay was larger on the GST compared to the SST. Neither Age Group nor Duplet interacted with Task, Fs < 2.12. The data are presented in Table 6 and age-related trends are plotted in Figure 5.
Table 6.
Task | GST |
SST |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age group | GO–GO | SI–GO | FI–GO | GO–GO | SI–GO | FI–GO | I–GO |
Young children | 601 (20.4) | 631 (22.0) | 639 (25.6) | 573 (18.9) | 587 (18.0) | 602 (20.8) | 584 (21.3) |
Older children | 451 (19.9) | 490 (21.4) | 475 (25.0) | 423 (18.9) | 440 (18.0) | 435 (20.8) | 430 (21.3) |
Young adults | 333 (21.5) | 355 (23.3) | 359 (27.1) | 322 (20.5) | 338 (19.5) | 337 (22.6) | 331 (23.1) |
Elderly adults | 630 (20.4) | 628 (22.0) | 665 (25.6) | 598 (19.9) | 601 (18.9) | 630 (21.9) | 629 (22.4) |
Median RTs (in milliseconds) are presented associated with go trials following a go trial (GO–GO), go trials following a successful inhibit (SI–GO), go trials following a failed inhibit (FI–GO) and, for the SST, go trials following an invalid stop-signal trial (I–GO; SD between parentheses).
The delay in responding following successful inhibits, relative to the speed of responding to go trials following non-signal (go) trials, was significant for the two child groups and young adults (ps < 0.004), but not for elderly adults (p = 0.97). The analysis on the log transformed data indicated that the delay did not discriminate disproportionally between the child groups and young adults (ps > 0.43), but the delay was disproportionally longer for the child groups and young adults compared to elderly adults (ps < 0.02). The delay of responding following failed inhibits compared to go trials was significant for all age groups (ps < 0.01). The analysis on the log transformed data failed to reveal disproportional differences between age groups in this regard (p = 0.50). The delay following a failed inhibit was similar to the delay following a successful inhibit for young children and young adults (ps > 0.11). Failed inhibit delay was larger than successful inhibit delay for elderly (p = 0.02), whereas the opposite pattern was observed for older children (p = 0.02). The analysis on log transformed data revealed that elderly adults delayed the go response following a failed inhibit compared to following a successful inhibit disproportionally more than the other age groups (ps < 0.02) and young children somewhat more than older children (p = 0.07). Overall, it appears that the delay following stop trials is dependent on whether the response on the immediately preceding trial was successfully stopped or not.
Performance adjustments following stop-signal trials may depend on trial-by-trial stimulus sequence (e.g., Verbruggen et al., 2008). The following analysis was conducted to assess the effect of repetition or alternation stimulus sequence on the performance adjustments following successful and failed inhibited trials. ANOVA was performed on Task (2), Duplet (2; successful inhibit–go, and failed inhibit–go), Sequence (2) × Age Group (4). The data are presented in Table 7. This analysis revealed a significant Sequence effect, F(1, 70) = 91.93, p < 0.001. The speed of responding on repetitions (528 ms) was longer than on alternations (497 ms). Neither Sequence nor Task did interact with Duplet, F < 1. There was a significant interaction between Sequence and Age Group, F(3, 70) = 7.89, p < 0.001. Subsequent analysis on log transformed data indicated that the relative alternation–repetition difference was larger in younger children compared to older children and young adults (ps < 0.006), and tended to be larger compared to elderly adults (p = 0.06). The relative alternation–repetition difference was also larger in elderly adults compared to young adults (p = 0.02), but did not differ from the older children (p = 0.40). Older children did not differ from young adults either (p = 0.11).
Table 7.
Task | GST |
SST |
||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
SI–GO |
FI–GO |
SI–GO |
FI–GO |
I–GO |
||||||
Age group | Repetition | Alternation | Repetition | Alternation | Repetition | Alternation | Repetition | Alternation | Repetition | Alternation |
Young children | 662 (100.9) | 600 (76.6) | 669 (82.6) | 609 (108.6) | 606 (76.1) | 567 (68.9) | 629 (109.2) | 574 (81.4) | 603 (92.7) | 565 (87.6) |
Older children | 502 (83.9) | 478 (69.0) | 490 (66.0) | 461 (77.4) | 449 (59.2) | 430 (47.5) | 449 (65.5) | 421 (56.0) | 439 (50.1) | 421 (50.7) |
Young adults | 358 (33.0) | 352 (32.5) | 360 (37.5) | 358 (34.3) | 347 (30.0) | 328 (32.9) | 344 (31.5) | 331 (34.2) | 333 (30.1) | 329 (27.2) |
Elderly adults | 636 (146.9) | 619 (164.5) | 681 (180.2) | 650 (200.7) | 625 (146.2) | 578 (126.8) | 652 (151.7) | 608 (158.6) | 635 (163.3) | 623 (161.1) |
A separate analysis was done on the SST task to test performance adjustments following invalid stop trials. The ANOVA Duplet (4; go–go, successful inhibit–go, failed inhibit–go, and invalid stop–go) × Age Group yielded a significant main effect of Duplet, F(3, 213) = 12.34, p < 0.001, that interacted with Age Group, F(9, 213) = 2.57, p = 0.008 (see Table 6). More specifically, the speed of performance adjustment following an invalid stop trial compared to following a go trial was larger in older children and in the two adult groups (ps < 0.02), but failed to reach significance in young children (p = 0.19). The log transformed analysis showed that elderly adults delayed the response following an invalid stop-signal trial relative to following a go trial disproportionally more compared to the other age groups (ps < 0.02). The delay following an invalid stop was disproportionally larger compared to following successful inhibits in the elderly adults compared to the other age groups (ps < 0.01). Finally, the delay following an invalid stop-signal trial did not significantly differ from those observed following failed inhibit trials in all age groups (ps > 0.07).
A final analysis was conducted to assess whether the performance adjustments following invalid stop-signal trials differed between repetitions vs. alternations. The Sequence (2) × Age Group (4) ANOVA yielded a significant Sequence, F(1, 71) = 19.86, p < 0.001, that interacted with Age Group, F(3, 71) = 3.12, p = 0.03. As can be seen in Table 7, the delay following an invalid stop-signal trial was longer when the stimulus was repeated compared to when it was alternated for the two child groups (ps < 0.005), but not for the two adult groups (ps > 0.16). The log transformed analysis showed that the relative alternation–repetition difference was disproportionally larger in young children compared to the two adults groups (ps < 0.04).
Discussion
The current study aimed at examining lifespan changes in global and selective stopping and in post-stopping performance adjustments. With regard to the first aim, the results showed that global stopping was faster than selective stopping, which is typically interpreted in terms of the time consumed by additional processing (e.g., stimulus discrimination) in selective stopping (van den Wildenberg and van der Molen, 2004b; Verbruggen et al., 2005; van de Laar et al., 2010). It could be argued that a longer SSRT in the SST is due to the lower probability of valid stop-signals in this task relative to the global task (i.e., 17.5 vs. 35%, respectively). A lower probability of stopping is typically associated with faster responses on go trials, as has been observed in both go/no–go paradigms (e.g., Luce, 1986) and in stop-signal paradigms (e.g., van den Wildenberg and van der Molen, 2003; Ramautar et al., 2004; van de Laar et al., 2010). Indeed, in the current study responses on go trials were faster on the selective stopping task compared to the global stopping task (483 vs. 513 ms, respectively). The longer SSRT on the selective stop task is then explained by assuming that it is harder to stop because responses on go trials become more pre-potent when stop-signals are fewer. It should be noted, however, that in studies manipulating stop-signal probability, SSRT did not change (Ramautar et al., 2004; van de Laar et al., 2010) or was even shorter, not longer, when stop-signals were fewer (e.g., van den Wildenberg and van der Molen, 2003). Thus, at this point, the most likely interpretation of longer SSRTs on the SST is in terms of added signal discrimination demands.
Both global and selective SSRTs showed the typical U-shaped relation with advancing age albeit less pronounced than RTs on non-signal (go trials) or signal–respond trials (failed inhibits and invalid stop trials). The U-shaped relation between the processing speed and advancing age is a ubiquitous phenomenon (e.g., review in Cerella and Hale, 1994) that attracted a score of interpretations, including age-related changes in the setting of response thresholds (Starns and Ratcliff, 2010), information loss (Myerson et al., 1990), inhibitory control (Dempster, 1992), frontal lobe function (West, 1996), and neural noise (Kail, 1997).
A host of studies indicated that the ability to inhibit pre-potent responses improves rapidly when children are growing older (e.g., Ridderinkhof et al., 1999; Band et al., 2000; van den Wildenberg and van der Molen, 2004a) and starts to decline during senescence (e.g., Williams et al., 1999; Bedard et al., 2002). The current findings are basically consistent with this literature. More specifically, the current findings obtained for the GST showed that the speed of global stopping was slower for young children and faster for young adults compared to older children and elderly adults who did not differ in this regard. This pattern was similar for selective stopping with one exception; the speed of selective stopping did not discriminate between the young vs. elderly adults. It should be noted that the developmental gain in global stopping was larger during childhood (young vs. older children) than adolescence (older children vs. young adults). For selective stopping the developmental gain during childhood was basically similar to the gain during adolescence. This pattern is consistent with the findings reported previously by van den Wildenberg and van der Molen (2004a) and suggests that selective stopping matures slower than global stopping.
Based on a “last-in-first-out” hypothesis of lifespan changes in neurocognition (e.g., Davis et al., 2009), one would be led to predict that with advancing age selective stopping would be affected more than global stopping. The current findings, however, showed the opposite pattern. Global, but not selective, stopping discriminated significantly between young vs. elderly adults. Typically, global stopping is slower in the elderly compared to young adults (e.g., Williams et al., 1999; Rush et al., 2006; but see Kramer et al., 1994). The single study of selective stopping including elderly adults (Bedard et al., 2002) showed a pronounced difference between young vs. elderly adults in the speed of selective stopping; 248 vs. 329 ms, respectively. In the current study, the difference between young vs. elderly adults was only 203 vs. 224 ms, respectively. But the elderly adults differed from young adults in being disproportionally slow on invalid stop-signal trials (721 vs. 394 ms, respectively).
One interpretation of the conspicuous absence of a sizable difference in the speed of selective stopping between young vs. elderly adults would be that on a proportion, if not all, of the trials, the elderly adults stopped all responses first and then decided to recruit the appropriate response after determining that inhibition is not required (e.g., Aron and Verbruggen, 2008; van de Laar et al., 2010). The deployment of a “stop-all” strategy in the elderly adults might be another instance of their inclination of preventing errors even when it slows them down doing so. It has been observed that a considerable portion of response slowing in the elderly is due to extra time consumed by response generation at the cortical level, as indicated by brain potentials (e.g., Kolev et al., 2005), the corticospinal level, as indicated by motor-evoked potentials (e.g., Fujiyama et al., 2011), and response activation, as indicated by movement kinematics (e.g., Trewartha et al., 2011). Moreover, it has been observed that the elderly show excessive response activation on no–go trials (e.g., Vallesi and Stuss, 2010; see also Vallesi, 2011; Vallesi et al., 2011). Thus, re-activation following the suppression of response-related over-recruited neural circuitry might be particularly time-consuming in the elderly (e.g., Vallesi, 2011; see also Vallesi and Stuss, 2010).
The second aim of the current study was to examine lifespan changes in post-stopping adjustment. Previous studies indicated that post-stopping adjustment is critically dependent on stimulus–response repetitions and alternations across trials. That is, the speed of responding is delayed on post-stopping trials and the delay is largest when the stimulus is repeated (Verbruggen et al., 2008; Bissett and Logan, 2011). Verbruggen et al. (2008) argued that the goal to stop on successful inhibits replaces the go goal on the following go trial if the stimulus is repeated. In previous studies we examined age-related changes in trial-by-trial sequential effects (Melis et al., 2002; Smulders et al., 2005). This research showed that relatively long intervals between trials (i.e., >500 ms) are associated with a repetition benefit for young children (Smulders et al., 2005) and an alternation benefit for young and elderly adults (Melis et al., 2002). The former has been interpreted in terms of automatic facilitation or priming and the latter has been taken to be a manifestation of subjective expectancy (e.g., Gao et al., 2009). Our data deviate from previous findings in showing an alternation benefit for all age groups with the exception of young adults who failed to show significant sequential effects. One interpretation would be that the current discrepancy is due to the insertion of stop-signals into the trial series. This interpretation is unlikely since the alternation benefit associated with invalid stop trials was absent also in the two adult groups. Thus, an alternative interpretation assumes that subjective expectancies are relatively short-lived and dissipate rapidly so that their influence cannot be detected when inter-trial intervals are relatively long. Along those lines, it would be predicted that with increasing inter-trial intervals alternation benefits should disappear gradually in all age groups.
Most important, the current young adult results are consistent with previous reports showing a delay in the speed of responding on trials following successful or failed inhibits in GSTs (e.g., Rieger and Gauggel, 1999; Verbruggen et al., 2008). The current results add to this literature by showing that a similar pattern occurs on a SST. The present data revealed that responses following failed inhibits are not significantly slower than responses following successful inhibits. Similarly, responses following invalid stop-signal trials in the selective stop-signal trials were equally slow compared to responses following successful inhibits. In contrast to previous studies, the post-stop trial adjustments in the speed of responding were not influenced by sequential effects. Thus the current results cannot be interpreted easily in terms of repetition priming suggesting that the stop-signal on the previous successful inhibit trial automatically primes the stop process on the current trial (e.g., Verbruggen et al., 2008). Instead the current findings are compatible with several notions assuming that a stop-signal on the previous trial induces a switch to a more conservative response set resulting in a slowing of response execution. These notions assume that the conflict between responding and stopping induced by the stop-signal recruit control processes resulting in performance adjustments on the subsequent trial (e.g., Rieger and Gauggel, 1999; Botvinick et al., 2001) or that the infrequent stop-signal elicits an orienting response reducing the speed of responding on the subsequent trial (e.g., Notebaert et al., 2009). Along these lines it would be predicted that response slowing on trials following a stop-signal does not discriminate between successful vs. failed inhibit vs. invalid stop-signal trials.
To our surprise, the developmental findings did not differ from the results obtained in young adults. Response slowing following stop-signal trials on both the global and SSTs was somewhat more pronounced in young adults but not disproportionally so. Studies examining age-performance adjustments following stop-signal trials are scarce and focused on comparisons between clinical groups vs. typically developing children rather than different age groups. Some developmental studies examined post-error slowing but the outcomes of these studies are inconsistent (for a review see Smulders et al., under review). Typically, in studies examining developmental change in post-error slowing, the results are interpreted in terms of conflict monitoring (Botvinick et al., 2001) and, based on event-related brain potentials or bold responses associated with error processing, it is usually suggested that conflict monitoring is still immature in children and, in some studies, not fully developed even in adolescents (e.g., Velanova et al., 2008; Braet et al., 2009). Obviously, the current pattern of findings is difficult to reconcile with the notion of developmental improvements in conflict monitoring, at least across the age span under study. By contrast, the current pattern suggests that the mechanisms engaged on stop-signal trials and resulting in response slowing on subsequent trials are already in place and mature in children as young as 7 years of age. A major difference between studies examining developmental change in post-error slowing and the current study focusing on performance adjustments following stop-signal trials is that errors are typically less frequent than stop-signals. It would be of considerable interest to examine developmental change in post-error and stop-signal slowing by calibrating the frequencies of error and stop-signal occurrence.
The findings obtained for the elderly adults differed from the results of the other age groups in three respects. First, the elderly adults did not slow following successful inhibits on both stop-signal tasks. Second, on both stop-signal tasks, the slowing following failed inhibits was disproportionally larger than in young adults. Third, the slowing following invalid stop-signal trials was disproportionally larger than in young adults. The observation that the elderly adults slowed following failed inhibits but did not following successful inhibits presents a challenge for the orienting interpretation of conflict adjustment proposed by Notebaert et al. (2009). According to the orienting account performance adjustments following successful and failed inhibits should be the same because the probability of success and failure is identical (around 50%). In this regard, the current findings are inconsistent with the prediction derived from the orienting account. The current findings are problematic also with regard to the conflict monitoring hypothesis (Botvinick et al., 2001). Conflict arises on all three stop-signal trials and, thus, performance adjustments should be observed on all trials following a stop-signal trial. The current data of elderly adults do suggest that performance adjustment is triggered by conflict but only when this conflict is resolved by the execution of an overt response, either correct, as on trials following invalid stop-signal trials, or incorrect, as on trials following failed inhibits.
A unified account of the data could be provided by assuming age-related changes in the balance between proactive and reactive performance adjustments in the stop-signal tasks. Although participants were instructed not to delay their speed of responding, and in spite of the tracking algorithm that should discourage participants from slowing down, the speed of responding on the stop-signal tasks was considerably slower than on the standard choice task in all age groups. The delay in responding could be considered an instance of proactive control exercised by the participants in an attempt to increase their chances to withhold the response when a stop-signal instructed them to do so. Proactive control in the current study is manifested also by differential slowing in the two stop-signal tasks. Larger delays were observed on the GST compared to the SST, presumably because proactive control is guided by the probability of stopping rather than stop-signal occurrence, as stop-signals might turn out to be invalid in the SST. Proactive control in stop-signal performance has been demonstrated in previous studies, both at the behavioral (e.g., Verbruggen and Logan, 2009) and neurocognitive level (for a review see Aron, 2011). Importantly, the slowing associated with the insertion of stop-signals was most pronounced for the elderly adults. Most likely, the occurrence of stop-signals indicated a greater need for caution in the elderly participants relative to the other age groups and, thus, response slowing on go trials is disproportionally larger in the elderly adults. This interpretation is consistent with the typical observation that elderly adults favor accuracy over speed (e.g., Band and Kok, 2000; Starns and Ratcliff, 2010). If elderly adults exercised more proactive control compared to the younger age groups there might be less need for them to resort to reactive control when they are confronted with a stop-signal trial. The current absence of a delay in responding on go trials following successful inhibits might suggest that proactive control was sufficient in dealing with the conflict between stopping and going. Apparently, proactive control was not sufficient on failed inhibits, which could signal the need for more control on subsequent trials and this is reflected in slowing on go trials following failed inhibits. Finally, the slowing on go trials following invalid stop-signal trials may suggest that reactive control is elicited on those trials by the added requirement of re-initiating the response that was stopped first in reaction to the detection of the stop-signal. The current interpretation is similar to the goal-priority hypothesis proposed recently by Bissett and Logan (2011) to account for the dynamic interplay between pro- and reactive control in the stop-signal paradigm. The goal-priority hypothesis assumes that stop-signals indicate the need for more caution and thus participants reduce their speed of responding. This might happen following a stop-signal trial (reactive control) but also during the anticipation of a stop-signal trial (proactive control) when, for example, explicit cues signal the importance of stopping (e.g., Verbruggen and Logan, 2009). It should be of considerable interest to further examine lifespan changes in the dynamic balance between pro- and reactive control in the stop-signal trial by cueing the importance of stopping vs. response speed.
In conclusion, the current findings indicate that, with regard to global and selective stopping, development, and aging are not simply two sides of the same coin. Developmental change in global and selective stopping revealed different trajectories, with global stopping maturing more rapidly than selective stopping. The pattern of findings is inconsistent with a simple “last-in-first-out” hypothesis of lifespan changes in (neuro)cognition. Importantly, children slowed their speed of responding following stop-signal trials, but the current data revealed little difference between children and young adults in this regard. Consequently, the type of reactive control exercised in the stop-signal paradigm seems to mature very rapidly. This finding has important implications for developmental theories of cognitive control (e.g., Durston and Casey, 2006), in particular for developmental concepts of conflict monitoring (e.g., Braet et al., 2009). All age groups delayed the speed of responding when stop-signals were inserted in the trial series and the delay was interpreted in terms of proactive control aimed at enhancing stopping success and in terms of an adjustment of response thresholds (e.g., Bissett and Logan, 2011). The data pattern obtained for the elderly adults suggest that they shifted the balance between proactive and reactive cognitive control toward proactive control. Most likely this shift is related to their willingness to trade speed for accuracy. Further investigation of the factors influencing the balance between pro- and reactive control in development and cognitive aging is an important goal for future research.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by NWO grants to Maurits W. van der Molen and Wery P. M. van den Wildenberg (MaGW grant 400-03-261 and VENI grant 451-06-012, respectively). The support of the Netherlands Institute for Advanced Study in the Humanities and Social Sciences is also gratefully acknowledged. We thank Bert van Beek for programming the computer tasks and Marcus Spaan and Bert Molenkamp for technical support.
References
- Aron A. R. (2011). From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 55–68 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron A. R., Verbruggen F. (2008). Stop the presses: dissociating a selective from a global mechanism for stopping. Psychol. Sci. 19, 1146–1153 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02216.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Band G. P. H., Kok A. (2000). Age effects on response monitoring in a mental-rotation task. Biol. Psychol. 51, 201–221 10.1016/S0301-0511(99)00038-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Band G. P. H., van der Molen M. W., Logan G. D. (2003). Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 112, 105–142 10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00079-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Band G. P. H., van der Molen M. W., Overtoom C. C., Verbaten M. N. (2000). The ability to activate and inhibit speeded responses: separate developmental trends. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 75, 263–290 10.1006/jecp.1999.2538 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bedard A., Nichols S., Barbosa J. A., Schachar R., Logan G. D., Tannock R. (2002). The development of selective inhibitory control across the life span. Dev. Neuropsychol. 21, 93–111 10.1207/S15326942DN2101_5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bergerbest D., Gabrieli J. D. E., Whitfield-Gabrieli S., Kim H., Stebbins G. T., Bennett D. A., Fleischman D. A. (2009). Age-associated reduction of asymmetry in prefrontal function and preservation of conceptual repetition priming. Neuroimage 45, 237–245 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bissett P. G., Logan G. D. (2011). Balancing cognitive demands: control adjustments in the stop-signal paradigm. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 37, 392–404 10.1037/a0021800 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick M. M., Braver T. S., Barch D. M., Carter C. S., Cohen J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 108, 624–652 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braet W., Johnson K. A., Tobin C. T., Acheson R., Bellgrove M. A., Robertson I. H., Garavan H. (2009). Functional developmental changes underlying response inhibition and error-detection processes. Neuropsychologia 47, 3143–3151 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cerella J., Hale S. (1994). The rise and fall in information-processing rates over the life span. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 86, 109–197 10.1016/0001-6918(94)90002-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crone E. A., Jennings J. R., van der Molen M. W. (2004). Developmental change in feedback processing as reflected by phasic heart rate changes. Dev. Psychol. 40, 1228–1238 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1228 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis S. W., Dennis N. A., Buchler N. G., White L. E., Madden D. J., Cabeza R. (2009). Assessing the effects of age on long white matter tracts using diffusion tensor tractography. Neuroimage 46, 530–541 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.068 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dempster F. N. (1992). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging. Dev. Rev. 12, 45–75 10.1016/0273-2297(92)90003-K [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Durston S., Casey B. J. (2006). What have we learned about cognitive development from neuroimaging? Neuropsychologia 44, 2149–2157 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleischman D. A. (2007). Repetition priming in aging and Alzheimer’s disease: an integrative review and future directions. Cortex 43, 889–897 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70688-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujiyama H., Tandonnet C., Summers J. J. (2011). Age-related differences in corticospinal excitability during a go/nogo task. Psychophysiology 48, 1448–1455 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01201.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gamboz N., Borella E., Brandimonte M. A. (2009). The role of switching, inhibition, and working memory in older adults’ performance in the Wisconsin card sorting test. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 16, 260–284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao J., Wong-Lin K., Holmes P., Simen P., Cohen J. D. (2009). Sequential effects in two-choice reaction time tasks: decomposition and synthesis of mechanisms. Neural Comput. 21, 2407–2436 10.1162/neco.2009.11-06-391 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huizinga M., van der Molen M. W. (2010). Task switching and shifting between stopping and going: developmental change in between-trial control adjustments. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 108, 484–503 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnstone S. J., Dimoska A., Smith J. L., Barry R. J., Pleffer C. B., Chiswick D., Clarke A. R. (2007). The development of stop-signal and go/nogo response inhibition in children aged 7–12 years: performance and event-related potential indices. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 63, 25–38 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kail R. (1997). The neural noise hypothesis: evidence from processing speed in adults with multiple sclerosis. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 4, 157–165 10.1080/13825589708256644 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kolev V., Falkenstein M., Yordanova J. (2005). Motor-response generation as a source of aging-related behavioural slowing in choice-reaction tasks. Neurobiol. Aging 27, 1719–1730 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.09.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kramer A. F., Humphrey D. G., Larish J. F., Logan G. D., Strayer D. L. (1994). Aging and inhibition: beyond a unitary view of inhibitory processing in attention. Psychol. Aging 9, 491–512 10.1037/0882-7974.9.4.491 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kray J., Kipp K. H., Karbach J. (2009). The development of selective inhibitory control: the influence of verbal labeling. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 130, 48–57 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.10.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levitt H. (1971). Transformed up–down methods in psychoacoustics. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 49, 467–477 10.1121/1.1912388 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li X., Liang Z., Kleiner M., Lu Z. L. (2010). RTbox: a device for highly accurate response time measurements. Behav. Res. Methods 42, 212–225 10.3758/BRM.42.1.212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logan G. D. (1994). “On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm,” in Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language, eds Dagenbach D., Carr T. H. (San Diego, CA: Academic Press; ), 189–239 [Google Scholar]
- Logan G. D., Burkell J. (1986). Dependence and independence in responding to double stimulation: a comparison of stop, change, and dual-task paradigms. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 12, 549–563 10.1037/0096-1523.12.4.549 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Logan G. D., Cowan W. B. (1984). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a theory of an act of control. Psychol. Rev. 91, 295–327 10.1037/0033-295X.91.3.295 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luce R. D. (1986). Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization. New York, NY: Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
- May C. P., Hasher L. (1998). Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 24, 363–379 10.1037/0096-1523.24.2.363 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayda A. B. V., Westphal A., Carter C. S., DeCarli C. (2011). Late life cognitive control deficits are accentuated by white matter disease burden. Brain 134, 1673–1683 10.1093/brain/awr065 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meiran N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 22, 1423–1442 10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1423 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Melis A., Soetens E., van der Molen M. W. (2002). Process-specific slowing with advancing age: evidence derived from the analysis of sequential effects. Brain Cogn. 49, 420–435 10.1006/brcg.2001.1508 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myerson J., Hale S., Wagstaff D., Poon L., Smith G. (1990). The information-loss model: a mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychol. Rev. 97, 475–487 10.1037/0033-295X.97.4.475 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Notebaert W., Houtman F., Opstal F. V., Gevers W., Fias W., Verguts T. (2009). Post-error slowing: an orienting account. Cognition 111, 275–279 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oosterlaan J., Sergeant J. A. (1996). Inhibition in ADHD, aggressive, and anxious children: a biologically based model of child psychopathology. J. Abnorm. Child. Psychol. 24, 19–36 10.1007/BF01448371 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramautar J. R., Kok A., Ridderinkhof K. R. (2004). Effects of stop-signal probability in the stop-signal paradigm: the N2/P3 complex further validated. Brain Cogn. 56, 234–252 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raven J., Raven J. C., Court J. H. (1993). Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Mill Hill Vocabulary Scales. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press [Google Scholar]
- Raven J. C., Court J. H., Raven J. (1985). Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. Standard Progressive Matrices, Sect. 3. London: H. K. Lewis [Google Scholar]
- Ridderinkhof K. R., Band G. P. H., Logan G. D. (1999). A study of adaptive behavior: effects of age and irrelevant information on the ability to inhibit one’s actions. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 101, 315–337 10.1016/S0001-6918(99)00010-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rieger M., Gauggel S. (1999). Inhibitory after-effects in the stop signal paradigm. Br. J. Psychol. 90, 509–518 10.1348/000712699161585 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rush B. K., Barch D. M., Braver T. S. (2006). Accounting for cognitive aging: context processing, inhibition or processing speed? Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 13, 588–610 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smulders S. F., Notebaert W., Meijer M., Crone E. A., van der Molen M. W., Soetens E. (2005). Sequential effects on speeded information processing: a developmental study. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 90, 208–234 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Somerville L. H., Casey B. J. (2010). Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational systems. Neurobiology 20, 236–241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Starns J., Ratcliff R. (2010). The effects of aging on the speed-accuracy compromise: boundary optimality in the diffusion model. Psychol. Aging 25, 377–390 10.1037/a0018022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trewartha K. M., Penhune V. B., Li K. Z. (2011). Movement kinematics of prepotent response suppression in aging during conflict adaptation. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 66, 185–194 10.1093/geronb/gbq090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vallesi A. (2011). Targets and non-targets in the aging brain: a go/nogo event-related potential study. Neurosci. Lett. 487, 313–317 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.10.046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vallesi A., McIntosh A. R., Stuss D. T. (2011). Overrecruitment in the aging brain as a function of task demands: evidence for a compensatory view. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 801–815 10.1162/jocn.2010.21490 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vallesi A., Stuss D. T. (2010). Excessive sub-threshold motor preparation for non-target stimuli in normal aging. Neuroimage 50, 1251–1257 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Boxtel G. J. M., van der Molen M. W., Jennings J. R., Brunia C. H. M. (2001). A psychophysiological analysis of inhibitory motor control in the stop-signal paradigm. Biol. Psychol. 58, 229–262 10.1016/S0301-0511(01)00117-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van de Laar M. C., van den Wildenberg W. P. M., van Boxtel G. J. M., van der Molen M. W. (2010). Processing of global and selective stop signals: application of Donders’ subtraction method to stop-signal task performance. Exp. Psychol. 57, 149–159 10.1027/1618-3169/a000019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Wildenberg W. P. M., van der Molen M. W. (2003). The duration of response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm varies with response force. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 114, 115–129 10.1016/S0001-6918(03)00062-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Wildenberg W. P. M., van der Molen M. W. (2004a). Developmental trends in simple and selective inhibition of compatible and incompatible responses. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 87, 201–220 10.1016/j.jecp.2003.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Wildenberg W. P. M., van der Molen M. W. (2004b). Additive factors analysis of inhibitory processing in the stop-signal paradigm. Brain Cogn. 56, 253–266 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.06.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Schoot M., Licht R., Horsley T. M., Sergeant J. A. (2005). Effects of stop signal modality, stop signal intensity and tracking method of inhibitory performance as determined by use of the stop signal paradigm. Scand. J. Psychol. 46, 331–341 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2005.00463.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Velanova K., Wheeler M. E., Luna B. (2008). Maturational changes in anterior cingulate and frontoparietal recruitment support the development of error processing and inhibitory control. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2505–2522 10.1093/cercor/bhn012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verbruggen F., Liefooghe B., Vandierendonck A. (2005). On the difference between response inhibition and negative priming: evidence from simple and selective stopping. Psychol. Res. 69, 262–271 10.1007/s00426-004-0177-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verbruggen F., Logan G. D. (2008). Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 418–424 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verbruggen F., Logan G. D. (2009). Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 35, 835–854 10.1037/a0012726 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verbruggen F., Logan G. D., Liefooghe B., Vandierendonck A. (2008). Short-term aftereffects of response inhibition: repetition priming or between-trial control adjustments? J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 34, 413–426 10.1037/0096-1523.34.2.413 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West R., Schwarb H. (2006). The influence of aging and frontal function on the neural correlates of regulative and evaluative aspects of cognitive control. Neuropsychology 20, 468–481 10.1037/0894-4105.20.4.468 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychol. Bull. 120, 272–292 10.1037/0033-2909.120.2.272 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams B. R., Ponesse J. S., Schachar R. J., Logan G. D., Tannock R. (1999). Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Dev. Psychol. 35, 205–213 10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]