Abstract
In the present study we used longitudinal methods to examine body image development during the early part of college. Students (N = 390; 54% female) who identified as African American (32%), Latino/a American (27%), and European American (41%) completed surveys during their first, second, and third semesters at college. There were overall gender and racial/ethnic differences in all three aspects of body image, and both stability and change in body image development. Female students’ appearance evaluation became more positive, whereas male students’ appearance evaluation showed no significant change. Individuals’ body areas satisfaction increased over time, but remained stable when controlling for BMI. Appearance orientation did not change, and there were no racial/ethnic differences in body image development. Experiences in the college environment may play a role in these trends.
Keywords: body image development, longitudinal design, college students, gender, race/ethnicity
The early part of college may be important for body image development. Students often make crucial decisions about their health and social life independently of direct adult supervision. Living in close quarters with peers and joining campus groups that emphasize appearance or unhealthy eating habits (e.g., sororities, certain sports teams) may increase appearance-related concerns (Basow, Foran, & Bookwala, 2007; Milligan & Pritchard, 2006). Moreover, the campus environment may allow students freedom from some adult responsibilities (Arnett, 2000), heightening the potential for exploration. Students who are exploring may develop unhealthy attitudes or behaviors with respect to the body, such as eating unhealthy foods, binge drinking, or developing friendships with individuals who are concerned about their looks. In turn, their attitudes toward their body and appearance may transition during this time.
In the current study, we examine changes in three aspects of body image during the early part of college. The first two aspects are evaluative, and include appearance evaluation, which represents evaluation of overall physical appearance, and body areas satisfaction, which captures satisfaction with different areas of the body (Cash, 2000). The third aspect, appearance orientation, is the extent to which individuals are cognitively and behaviorally invested in their looks, and is non-evaluative (Cash, 2000). In particular, we focus on whether these three aspects change differentially by gender and racial/ethnic group.
Appearance Evaluation/Satisfaction
We examine the evaluation/satisfaction component because body satisfaction is linked to self-esteem, depression, and eating disorders (Frost & McKelvie, 2004; Polivy & Herman, 2002; Siegel, 2002). Previous studies that address change over time in this component of body image tend to focus on body dissatisfaction in adolescents. For example, some work shows that adolescent girls experience an increase in body dissatisfaction (Bearman, Presnell, Martinez, & Stice, 2006; Eisenberg, Neumark-Sztainer, & Paxton, 2006), stability (Jones, 2004; Schooler, 2008), or a decline in dissatisfaction for high school girls transitioning into emerging adulthood (Eisenberg et al., 2006). Among adolescent boys, results are mixed as well. Some studies illustrate a decline in body dissatisfaction during adolescence (Bearman et al., 2006; Jones, 2004), but another study showed an overall increase in body dissatisfaction (although patterns differed by initial BMI and change in BMI, as well as racial/ethnic group; Eisenberg et al., 2006).
Longitudinal research on adolescents has also focused on racial/ethnic differences in body dissatisfaction. One study found that African American girls had a less steep increase in body dissatisfaction from ages 11 to 16 than did European Americans girls (Striegel-Moore, Schreiber, Lo, Crawford, Obarzanek, & Rodin, 2000). In another study, African Americans and European Americans did not differ in body dissatisfaction in a 5-year span beginning in early adolescence (approximately ages 13 - 18) but African Americans experienced less of an increase in body dissatisfaction than did European Americans in a 5-year span beginning in middle adolescence (approximately ages 16 - 21; Paxton, Eisenberg, & Neumark-Sztainer, 2006).
Here, we build on past work by examining whether appearance evaluation and body areas satisfaction change or remain stable in early emerging adulthood. We use a short-term longitudinal design to examine changes in body image in a large sample of men and women from different racial/ethnic groups. This design allows us to capture development during a time of possibly rapid change. Previous studies have reported cross-sectional gender and racial/ethnic differences in appearance evaluation and body satisfaction (Altabe, 1998; Clark et al., 2005; Gillen & Lefkowitz, 2006; Grabe & Hyde, 2006; Ricciardelli, McCabe, Williams, & Thompson, 2007). We know of no studies that have examined longitudinal changes in this construct in college students, including how development might differ by gender and racial/ethnic group.
Past research has, however, examined longitudinal changes in related constructs, such as eating behavior and weight/body mass index (BMI). One study found that for female students, maladaptive eating patterns remain stable during the first 20 months of college (Cooley, Toray, Valdez, & Tee, 2007). However, other work suggests that students tend to gain weight and experience an increase in BMI during the early part of college (Gillen & Lefkowitz, 2011; Hoffman, Policastro, Quick, & Lee, 2006; Morrow, Heesch, Dinger, Hull, Kneehans, & Fields, 2006). Students report that campuses offer unhealthy food options (Cluskey & Grobe, 2009), and they tend to store unhealthy food items in their dormitory rooms (Nelson & Story, 2009), which may in part explain this trend. Given that BMI is associated with body dissatisfaction in male and female college students (Yates, Edman, & Aruguete, 2004), we expect that students will evaluate their appearance less positively and become less satisfied with their bodies over time.
Appearance Orientation
There is less research on constructs related to appearance orientation. Appearance orientation is associated with neuroticism and disordered eating in women (Davis, Dionne, & Shuster, 2001; Petrie, Greenleaf, Reel, & Carter, 2009). A related construct, self-objectification, is linked to lower math performance in women (Fredrickson, Roberts, Noll, Quinn, & Twenge, 1998), and more recently, in men and women from different racial/ethnic groups (Hebl, King, & Lin, 2004). Given these associations, it is important to examine trends in appearance orientation. Previous research on appearance orientation shows cross-sectional gender and racial/ethnic differences (Clark et al., 2005; Gillen & Lefkowitz, 2006), but we know of no longitudinal studies on this variable in college students.
It is possible that appearance orientation increases during the early part of college. According to objectification theory (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997) women exist in a culture that views women’s bodies as sexual objects and teaches women to assume this same perspective on their own bodies. As a result of self-objectification, women repeatedly self-monitor their physical appearance, as if assuming an outsider’s view of how they look. Although not predicted by objectification theory (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997), recent research indicates that men are also vulnerable to self-objectification in certain situations (Hebl et al., 2004). We expect that self-objectification will be particularly relevant for men and women who are beginning college, as they are exposed to new peers who may gaze at and evaluate them in sexually objectifying ways (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). Thus, individuals’ appearance orientation may increase over their first several semesters at college.
Research Questions
How do appearance evaluation, body areas satisfaction, and appearance orientation change across the first three semesters of college?
Do changes in appearance evaluation, body areas satisfaction, and appearance orientation vary by gender and race/ethnicity?
Method
Participants and Procedures
Data are from a larger study described to participants as “research intended to examine how college aged men and women interact, and what their attitudes are like.” Participants were told that questionnaires included a range of topics, including physical appearance, but were not aware of the specifics of the topic of this particular study. The study was conducted at a large, predominantly European American university (total enrollment during participants’ first semester was 4% African American, 3% Latino/a American, and 81% European American) in the Northeast USA. We obtained contact information from the registrar’s office for all African American, Latino/a American, and European American first year students. We then mailed letters to all African American and Latino/a American students, as well as a randomly selected 9% of European American students (to obtain approximately equal numbers in each racial/ethnic group) in September of their first year (Time 1 = T1, Fall 2002). We only recruited traditional age students who ranged from 17 to 19 years old. These students are simultaneously entering college and emerging adulthood, a period marked by exploration in multiple domains (Arnett, 2000). The campus environment may allow emerging adult students to be free of some adult responsibilities (Arnett, 2000), heightening the potential for exploration. Exploration may include developing unhealthy attitudes or behaviors with respect to the body. Non-traditional age students may have more family and work responsibilities, and thus, fewer opportunities for exploration. Of the 844 contacted students, 52% agreed to participate, including 58% of African Americans, 54% of Latino/a Americans, and 46% of European Americans, χ2(2, 844) = 9.44, p < .01. Participants completed a survey in a classroom-based setting and were invited to complete the survey again during the next two semesters (T2- Spring 2003, T3- Fall 2003). Compensation was $25, $30, and $35 for each time point, respectively.
Of the original 434 students who completed surveys at T1, 414 (95%) completed surveys at T2, and 390 (90%) completed surveys at T3. To examine attrition biases, we compared individuals who completed surveys at T3 (N = 390) to those who did not (N = 44) on all analytic variables. Two of the two χ2 and four t-tests were significant. Specifically, retained participants were less likely to be male, χ2(1, 434) = 8.05, p < .01, Latino/a American, and African American, χ2(2, 434) = 7.28, p < .05.
Analyses are on the 390 students (54% female) who participated in all three waves. They ranged in age from 18.49 to 20.81 years (M = 19.50, SD = 0.40; females, M = 19.44, SD = 0.39; males, M = 19.57, SD = 0.41). Thirty two percent identified as African American (n = 124, 55% female), 27% as Latino/a American (n = 107, 55% female), and 41% as European American (n = 159, 54% female). Students had an average BMI in the upper range of normal at each time point (T1: M = 23.66, SD = 4.50; T2: M = 23.99, SD = 4.36; T3: M = 24.07, SD = 4.56; overall range 13.39 – 46.17), as defined by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2011). At T1, 99% of students in the sample lived in a campus dormitory.
Measures
Body image
We assessed three aspects of body image, all from the Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire (Cash, 2000), a measure with considerable psychometric support among college students (e.g., Cash, Morrow, Hrabosky, & Perry, 2004; Clark et al., 2005). The appearance evaluation subscale measures the extent to which individuals feel physically attractive or unattractive (e.g., seven items, “Most people would consider me good-looking”). Response options range from 1 = definitely disagree to 5 = definitely agree. Body areas satisfaction assesses the extent to which individuals are satisfied with various parts of their body (e.g., nine items, “hair”, “mid torso”). Participants report their agreement on a scale ranging from 1 = very dissatisfied to 5 = very satisfied. The appearance orientation subscale assesses the degree of cognitive and behavioral investment in appearance (e.g., 12 items, “I am self-conscious if my grooming isn’t right”). It captures a relatively benign aspect of appearance investment, more similar to the Motivational Salience subscale of the Appearance Schemas Inventory-Revised than the Self-Evaluative Salience subscale, which assesses a more dysfunctional type of investment (Cash, Melnyk, & Hrabosky, 2004). Responses on the appearance orientation subscale range from 1 = definitely disagree to 5 = definitely agree. Internal consistency reliability for appearance evaluation (T1, α = .88; T2, α = .90; T3, α = .90), body areas satisfaction (T1, α = .78; T2, α = .80; T3, α = .81), and appearance orientation (T1, α = .87; T2, α = .88; T3, α = .88) was satisfactory and comparable to Cash (2000).
Results
We performed mixed model ANOVAs to address our research questions of whether body image changes over time and whether these changes vary by gender and race/ethnicity. We entered time as a within-person factor and gender and race/ethnicity as between-person factors. The three facets of body image were the dependent variables.
For appearance evaluation, there were main effects of gender and race/ethnicity (see Table 1). Male students (M = 3.63, SE = 0.06) were more satisfied with their appearance than female students (M = 3.47, SE = 0.05). Tukey post hoc tests revealed that African Americans (M = 3.69, SE = 0.07) were more satisfied with their appearance than European Americans (M = 3.46, SE = 0.06, p < .05). Latino/a Americans (M = 3.51, SE = 0.07) did not differ from either group (ps > .05). There was also an interaction between time and gender. To follow up on the time × gender effect, we performed the ANOVA separately by gender (removing gender as a factor). The time effect was significant only for female students (p < .01), who became increasingly satisfied with their appearance over time. Next, we performed simple contrast tests with T1 as the reference point. Female students’ T1 and T2 appearance evaluation did not differ (p > .05), but T1 appearance evaluation was significantly lower than T3 (p < .01).
Table 1.
T1 M (SE) |
T2 M (SE) |
T3 M (SE) |
F (gender) |
F (race/ethnicity) |
F (time) |
F (time × gender) |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Appearance evaluation | |||||||
Total sample | 3.55 (0.04) | 3.53 (0.04) | 3.58 (0.04) | 4.21* | 3.42* | 1.99 | 5.22** |
Male students | 3.68 (0.06) | 3.60 (0.06) | 3.63 (0.06) | ||||
Female students | 3.43 (0.06) | 3.46 (0.06) | 3.54 (0.06) | ||||
Body areas satisfaction | |||||||
Total sample | 3.53 (0.03) | 3.53 (0.03) | 3.58 (0.03) | 12.79*** | 3.38* | 4.42* | 0.07 |
Appearance orientation | |||||||
Total sample | 3.41 (0.04) | 3.41 (0.03) | 3.40 (0.04) | 32.39*** | 3.65* | 0.13 | 1.00 |
Note -- T: Time. Due to missing data, sample size ranged from N = 378 to 380. Effect sizes (η2) for the Fs are: Appearance Evaluation—gender (.01), race/ethnicity (.02), time × gender (.01); Body Areas Satisfaction—gender (.03), race/ethnicity (.02), and time (.01); and Appearance Orientation—gender (.08), race/ethnicity (.02). Two-way interactions between time and race/ethnicity and three-way interactions were nonsignificant and therefore are not reported.
p < .05,
p < .01,
p < .001
In the body areas satisfaction model, there were main effects for gender, race/ethnicity, and time (see Table 1). Male students (M = 3.66, SE = 0.05) were more satisfied with areas of their body than female students (M = 3.44, SE = 0.04). Tukey post hoc tests revealed that African Americans (M = 3.66, SE = 0.06) were more satisfied with areas of their body than European Americans (M = 3.49, SE = 0.05) and Latino/a Americans (M = 3.49, SE = 0.06; ps < .05). Simple contrast tests using T1 as the reference point revealed that body areas satisfaction at T1 did not differ from T2 (p > .05), but body areas satisfaction at T1 was significantly lower than at T3 (p < .05).
In the ANOVA on appearance orientation, results showed significant main effects for gender and race/ethnicity (see Table 1). Female students (M = 3.59, SE = 0.04) were more oriented toward their appearance than male students (M = 3.22, SE = 0.05). Tukey post hoc tests revealed that African Americans (M = 3.50, SE = 0.06) were more oriented toward their appearance than European Americans (M = 3.30, SE = 0.05, p < .05). Latino/a Americans (M = 3.42, SE = 0.06) did not differ from either group (ps > .05). There were no significant main effects of or interactions with time.
Because BMI is associated with body dissatisfaction in college students (Yates et al., 2004), we performed the analyses described above as mixed model ANCOVAs, controlling for BMI at T1. The reported effects were the same for all variables with the exception of the time effect for body areas satisfaction which was no longer significant, F(2, 734) = 1.10, p > .05.
Discussion
During the early part of college, students experience a number of social and environmental changes, including living away from their parents, living in close quarters with same- and other-sex peers, and increased independence in daily living and decision making about all domains, including health. Although these changes have the potential to impact body image, few studies employ longitudinal designs to examine body image development during this time. In the present study, we used a short-term longitudinal design to examine changes in body image during the first three semesters of college. Overall, results indicated both stability and change in body image during this important period. The evaluation/satisfaction aspects of body image showed improvement, particularly for female students. However, appearance orientation remained stable.
Students’ body areas satisfaction, and for female students only, appearance evaluation, increased. Body image may improve after taking courses that encourage criticism of unrealistic images in the media and/or raise awareness of body image and eating problems, or after engaging in student activity programs that encourage positive body image. Also, many students likely transitioned from smaller high schools to this large university, with increased exposure to people who differ in physical appearance and attitudes. However, after controlling for BMI during the first semester, the trend toward improved body areas satisfaction disappeared. This finding suggests that students’ BMI during their first semester of college may be an important factor in determining changes in body areas satisfaction over the next two semesters. In contrast, BMI during the first semester did not appear to impact changes in appearance evaluation as findings for this variable were the same with and without BMI as a control.
Despite increases over time, female students’ appearance evaluation remained less positive, on average, than male students’ appearance evaluation. Thus, although female students’ appearance evaluation became more similar to men’s, it nonetheless remained lower. The overall gender difference mirrors past cross-sectional work (Clark et al., 2005), including cross-sectional work with this sample (Gillen & Lefkowitz, 2006), and supports objectification theory (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). As women’s bodies are culturally objectified, women begin to objectify themselves. Self-objectification may lead women to be overly critical of their bodies, and thus to have poorer appearance evaluation than men. Results also show that African Americans evaluated their appearance more positively than European Americans, and were more satisfied with areas of their body than European Americans and Latino/a Americans. African American college students desire larger body sizes than European American students (Aruguete, Nickleberry, & Yates, 2004), and African American adolescent girls have more flexible ideas of attractiveness than European American girls (Parker, Nichter, Nichter, Vuckovic, Sims, & Ritenbaugh, 1995). Because of these preferences, African Americans may have more positive attitudes about their appearance.
In contrast to the evaluation/satisfaction aspects of body image, appearance orientation showed relative stability. Based on previous research on objectification theory (Hebl et al., 2004), we expected that appearance orientation would increase during the early part of college. Perhaps students learn appearance-related cognitions and behaviors at an early age from parents and, over time, these develop into stable habits. For example, a female student who applies makeup each morning and frequently thinks about ways to improve her appearance is unlikely to stop because of the social rewards of doing so. Going to college may play a minimal role in altering these sorts of cognitions and behaviors.
Although appearance orientation did not change over time, there were overall group differences by gender and race/ethnicity, similar to prior cross-sectional work on this sample (Gillen & Lefkowitz, 2006). In particular, women had higher appearance orientation than men, as suggested by objectification theory (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). Women objectify their bodies, as the larger culture does, and thus, are attentive to their physical appearance. Results also showed that African Americans had higher appearance orientation than European Americans. Research on African American girls suggests that their physical appearance serves a dual function: to express individuality and to communicate an image of their family and community members (Parker et al., 1995). Thus, African Americans may be more attentive to appearance because it reflects the image of more than one individual. Also, African American girls and women, as compared to European American girls and women, seem to be more flexible when defining beauty (Parker et al., 1995). For African American women, being well-groomed, having a unique personal style, and exuding confidence is attractive, not conforming to someone else’s or society’s standard of attractiveness (Parker et al., 1995). Therefore, African American women may be more concerned with appearance, but are not necessarily more dissatisfied with their looks.
Body image development did not differ by racial/ethnic group. That is, despite initial differences, Latino/a American, African American, and European American students showed similar changes or stability in body image. Students experience common ecological changes during the early part of college (Maggs, 1997), such as living in dormitories, and exposure to similar food choices, student clubs, and parties. These changes may shape body image in similar ways for students from different racial/ethnic backgrounds. Future research should consider the role of acculturation in body image development, as students who are less acculturated to U.S. values, attitudes, and behaviors may show different patterns of change. Perhaps these students have had less exposure to the thin ideal in the U.S. media, and with exposure to it during college, may show declining satisfaction with appearance/body shape.
Limitations and Conclusions
This study has several limitations. Self-report measures were the only means of assessment; self-reported height and weight in particular may not be accurate. Also, the results of this study only pertain to the first two years of college. Future studies should extend these findings by including multiple assessments of body image development from before the first year of college to beyond college graduation. Findings should not be applied to early emerging adults who are not in college and individuals at more racially/ethnically diverse universities. The effects of the college environment on body image may be very different for minority students at diverse universities than for students at a predominantly European American university.
Despite these limitations, this study adds to the literature in a number of ways. We know of no prior studies that have examined body image development across the early part of college in a racially/ethnically diverse sample. This study extends prior longitudinal work on adolescents by illustrating trends in body image as individuals enter early emerging adulthood. Results illustrate improvement in the evaluation/satisfaction components of body image, particularly for female students. Appearance orientation, however, remained stable. Taken together, results suggest that the early part of college is a dynamic period for body image development, with potential mental and physical health implications.
Research Highlights.
We examine body image development longitudinally during the early part of college.
Female students become more satisfied with their appearance.
Male and female students become more satisfied with areas of their body.
Orientation toward appearance remains stable during the first three semesters.
Body image differs by racial/ethnic group, but body image changes do not differ.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by grant R01 HD 41720 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development to Eva S. Lefkowitz, and the Joachim Wolhwill Endowment in Human Development and Family Studies to Meghan M. Gillen. We gratefully acknowledge Jill Boelter, Tanya Boone, Amber Curtin, Graciela Espinosa-Hernandez, Stephanie Hensen, Shelley Hosterman, Sophia Khan, Eric Loken, Christen Mannino, Shevaun Neupert, and Cindy Shearer for their help with study design, data scoring and entering, data cleaning, and statistical analyses. We also thank Megan Patrick, the editors, and two anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Contributor Information
Meghan M. Gillen, Email: mmg204@psu.edu.
Eva S. Lefkowitz, Email: EXL20@psu.edu.
References
- Altabe M. Ethnicity and body image: Quantitative and qualitative analysis. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1998;23:153–159. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199803)23:2<153::aid-eat5>3.0.co;2-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist. 2000;55:469–480. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aruguete MS, Nickleberry LD, Yates A. Acculturation, body image, and eating attitudes among Black and White college students. North American Journal of Psychology. 2004;6:393–404. [Google Scholar]
- Basow SA, Foran KA, Bookwala J. Body objectification, social pressure, and disordered eating behavior in college women: The role of sorority membership. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2007;31:394–400. [Google Scholar]
- Bearman SK, Presnell K, Martinez E, Stice E. The skinny on body dissatisfaction: A longitudinal study of adolescent girls and boys. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2006;35:229–241. doi: 10.1007/s10964-005-9010-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cash TF. The Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire user’s manual. 2000 Available from the author at www.body-images.com.
- Cash TF, Melnyk SE, Hrabosky JI. The assessment of body image investment: An extensive revision of the Appearance Schemas Inventory. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2004;35:305–316. doi: 10.1002/eat.10264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cash TF, Morrow JA, Hrabosky JI, Perry AA. How has body image changed? A cross-sectional investigation of college women and men from 1983 to 2001. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:1081–1089. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.6.1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. About BMI for adults. 2011 Retrieved April 18, 2011, from http://www.cdc.gov/healthyweight/assessing/bmi/adult_bmi/index.html.
- Clark MM, Croghan IT, Reading S, Schroeder DR, Stoner SM, Patten CA, …Vickers KS. The relationship of body image dissatisfaction to cigarette smoking in college students. Body Image. 2005;2:263–270. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cluskey M, Grobe D. College weight gain and behavior transitions: Male and female differences. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 2009;109:325–329. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2008.10.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooley E, Toray T, Valdez N, Tee M. Risk factors for maladaptive eating patterns in college women. Eating and Weight Disorders. 2007;12:132–139. doi: 10.1007/BF03327640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis C, Dionne M, Shuster B. Physical and psychological correlates of appearance orientation. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;30:21–30. [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg ME, Neumark-Sztainer D, Paxton SJ. Five-year change in body satisfaction among adolescents. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2006;61:521–527. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson BL, Roberts TA. Objectification theory: Toward understanding women’s lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 1997;21:173–206. [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson BL, Roberts TA, Noll SM, Quinn DM, Twenge JM. That swimsuit becomes you: Sex differences in self-objectification, restrained eating, and math performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;75:269–284. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.75.1.269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frost J, McKelvie S. Self-esteem and body satisfaction in male and female elementary school, high school, and university students. Sex Roles. 2004;51:45–54. [Google Scholar]
- Gillen MM, Lefkowitz ES. Gender role development and body image among male and female first year college students. Sex Roles. 2006;55:25–37. [Google Scholar]
- Gillen MM, Lefkowitz ES. The “freshman 15”: Trends and predictors in a sample of multiethnic men and women. Eating Behaviors. 2011;12:261–266. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2011.07.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grabe S, Hyde JS. Ethnicity and body dissatisfaction among women in the United States: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132:622–640. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.4.622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hebl MR, King EB, Lin J. The swimsuit becomes us all: Ethnicity, gender, and vulnerability to self-objectification. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2004;30:1322–1331. doi: 10.1177/0146167204264052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman DJ, Policastro P, Quick V, Lee SK. Changes in body weight and fat mass of men and women in the first year of college: A study of the freshman 15’. Journal of American College Health. 2006;55:41–45. doi: 10.3200/JACH.55.1.41-46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones DC. Body image among adolescent girls and boys: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology. 2004;40:823–835. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maggs JL. Alcohol use and binge drinking as goal-directed action during the transition to postsecondary education. In: Schulenberg J, Maggs J, Hurrelmann K, editors. Health risks and developmental transitions during adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1997. pp. 345–371. [Google Scholar]
- Milligan B, Pritchard M. The relationship between gender, type of sport, body dissatisfaction, self-esteem, and disordered eating behaviors in division I athletes. Athletic Insight: The Online Journal of Sports Psychology. 2006;8:32–46. [Google Scholar]
- Morrow ML, Heesch KC, Dinger MK, Hull HR, Kneehans AW, Fields DA. Freshman 15: Fact or fiction? Obesity. 2006;14:1438–1443. doi: 10.1038/oby.2006.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson MC, Story M. Food environments in university dorms: 20,000 calories per dorm room and counting. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2009;36:523–526. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.01.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parker S, Nichter M, Nichter M, Vuckovic N, Sims C, Ritenbaugh C. Body image and weight concerns among African American and white adolescent females: Differences that make a difference. Human Organization. 1995;54:103–114. [Google Scholar]
- Paxton SJ, Eisenberg ME, Neumark-Sztainer D. Prospective predictors of body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls and boys: A five-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42:888–899. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrie TA, Greenleaf C, Reel J, Carter J. Personality and psychological factors as predictors of disordered eating among female collegiate athletes. Eating Disorders: The Journal of Treatment and Prevention. 2009;17:302–321. doi: 10.1080/10640260902991160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polivy J, Herman CP. Causes of eating disorders. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53:187–213. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ricciardelli LA, McCabe MP, Williams RJ, Thompson JK. The role of ethnicity and culture in body image and disordered eating among males. Clinical Psychology Review. 2007;27:582–606. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schooler D. Real women have curves: A longitudinal investigation of TV and the body image development of Latina adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research. 2008;23:132–153. [Google Scholar]
- Siegel JM. Body image change and adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal of Adolescent Research. 2002;17:27–41. [Google Scholar]
- Striegel-Moore RH, Schreiber GB, Lo A, Crawford P, Obarzanek E, Rodin J. Eating disorder symptoms in a cohort of 11 to 16-year-old Black and White Girls: The NHLBI Growth and Health Study. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2000;27:49–66. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(200001)27:1<49::aid-eat6>3.0.co;2-e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yates A, Edman J, Aruguete M. Ethnic differences in BMI and body/self-dissatisfaction among Whites, Asian subgroups, Pacific Islanders, and African-Americans. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2004;34:300–307. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2003.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]