Abstract
Our current understanding of sympatric speciation is that it occurs primarily through disruptive selection on ecological genes driven by competition, followed by reproductive isolation through reinforcement-like selection against inferior intermediates/heterozygotes. Our evolutionary model of selection on resource recognition and preference traits suggests a new mechanism for sympatric speciation. We find speciation can occur in three phases. First a polymorphism of functionally different phenotypes is established through evolution of specialization. On the gene level, regulatory functions have evolved in which some alleles are conditionally switched off (i.e. are silent). These alleles accumulate harmful mutations that potentially may be expressed in offspring through recombination. Second mating associated with resource preference invades because harmful mutations in parents are not expressed in the offspring when mating assortatively, thereby dividing the population into two pre-zygotically isolated resource-specialist lineages. Third, silent alleles that evolved in phase one now accumulate deleterious mutations over the following generations in a Bateson-Dobzhansky-Muller fashion, establishing a post-zygotic barrier to hybridization.
Introduction
The relevance of sympatric speciation, as opposed to allopatric speciation, in nature has been controversial. It has been a theoretical issue to understand how evolutionary bifurcation can occur when disruptive selection is opposed by inter-breeding in the population. Focusing on the dichotomy between allopatric and sympatric speciation is questionable; rather there is a plea for research on the speciation processes and its mechanisms [1]. Genetic studies of sympatric populations exhibiting a monophyletic origin suggest recent ecological divergence, reproductive isolation, and speciation without geographical barriers [2]–[5]. Particularly striking is the colonization of post-glacially emerging habitats by marine snails and sticklebacks, in which bifurcation has occurred repeatedly across sites and in parallel giving rise to homologous phenotypes [6]–[9]. These studies raise the issue of mechanisms behind (i) evolutionary diversification and (ii) the maintenance of apparent lineages: mechanisms that need not be the same for both processes [10]. Sympatric speciation is currently understood in terms of two consecutive processes. First, loci for niche-specific adaptations give rise to multiple alleles in a multi-niche environment. Second, assortative mating evolves to reduce heterozygotes that are poorly adapted. These processes have been analyzed using 1–2 loci models with discrete alleles [11]–[15], and with continuous alleles [16]. In a model of Dieckmann & Doebeli [17], quantitative traits are under the control of multiple loci interacting additively. Furthermore, they assume that the niche-breadth of the individuals is fixed in a continuum of resources. Such models, for example, apply to Darwin's finches in the Galapagos Islands, where disruptive selection acts on mouthpart morphology in the context of scramble competition in an environment with a range of variably sized prey. Under these circumstances, the assumption of additive genetics implies that morphologically intermediate heterozygotes have reduced fitness induced by intense competition [17]. This competition promotes selection for assortative mating that will evolve if ecological genes (genes for adaptation to a niche) have a pleiotropic effect on mating [15], or a mating preference gene that is in linkage disequilibrium with the ecological gene for which it expresses preference [18], [19]. In the first case, the existence of such genes (called magic genes) may seem obscure [20], but it is not uncommon that mating takes place in locations where preferred resources abound. The required linkage in the second case, however, imposes a restriction on the likelihood of sympatric speciation [21]. However, evolutionary branching of a genetic lineage is one plausible outcome of disruptive selection against intermediate phenotypes while evolution of dominance and a protected polymorphism is another [22], [23]. When trait-specific dominance has evolved, there is no disadvantage for heterozygotes and the selection for assortative mating and speciation has been exhausted [24].
In this paper we show that evolved polymorphism does not necessarily prevent selection for assortative mating. Instead assortative mating evolves due to costs of deleterious mutations on epistatic alleles essential to the polymorphism. We will also show that once assortative mating has evolved, the epistatic alleles are inactive and are a target for the evolution of Bateson-Dobzhansky-Muller incompatibility [25], [26]. In our case the precise mechanism relates to the following: 1.) Resources sometimes require resource-specific discrimination and cannot perceptually be generalized with other resources; 2.) A population of exploiters can utilize two such resources in a protected polymorphism of both homo- and heterozygote specialists; 3.) The haplotypes of the polymorphism carries two types of alleles: discrimation alleles and modifiers. In homozygotes discrimation alleles express resource-specific discrimation whereas modifers are silent. Alleles on heterozygotic loci will interact: modifier alleles alter or turn off the discrimator allele, or two different discrimator alleles interact codominantly altering or silencing the gene expression; 4.) Following from the genetic structure, some of the alleles are inactive, either in the homozygote or the heterozygote form. These alleles are susceptible to and can accumulate harmful mutations that are not subjected to selection until they are re-organized and expressed in a subsequent generation of offspring. 5.) Accumulated harmful mutations impose a cost for dis-assortative mating and hence selection for assortative mating occurs.
In highly specialized herbivores, parasites, and parasitoids, disruptive selection can operate on the niche-recognition trait itself. Striking examples are “cryptic species”, a pair (or guild) of species that are morphologically indistinguishable but select different species-specific hosts [27]. Correlations among haplotype sequences and host preference have led to previously regarded host-races of generalist species being assigned the status of “cryptic” species [28], [29]. Habitat preference has been studied with additive multilocus models, also including host-adaptation genes with opposite alleles being adapted to different hosts, called “Bush-models” [30]–[32]. Speciation has then been driven by evolving linkage between host-adaptation alleles and host-preference alleles, the latter also determining mating [32]. Here we model such niche (or host) preferences of exploiters using genetically-coded artificial neural nets (ANN) and, as in Bush models, without any niche adaptation genes under diversifying selection. ANNs have been used as models of neural and perceptual systems [33], [34] that are capable of non-linear discrimination of signals [35]. Individual nodes within ANNs participate in linear discrimination: in our model we identify such nodes that are controlled by epistatic genes. The nodal weightings of our ANNs are identified with a pair of chromosomes, subject to mutations. Our model contains additional elements that are identified as second chromosomal pair that holds a mating preference gene with a modifier allele for assortative mating, i.e. mating takes place on preferred resources [17], [36], a condition known as heteropatry [15]. A population of exploiters compete for resources in two niches identifiable by their ANN. The ANN enables the exploiters to evolve preference of any niche-breadth or modality (e.g. bimodality) without any pre-defined costs or trade-offs. These ANN automatons reproduce sexually, including chromosome recombination and crossover. In the simulated evolutionary process we study the emergence of reproductively isolated phenotypes with regard to their niche and mating preferences. In order to understand the selection at the level of emergent alleles, we dissect the evolutionary process into phases of distinct genetic organisation and selection pressure.
Methods
The Model
The environment
We modelled a dynamic population of about 400 evolvable exploiters in an environment of two suitable and two unsuitable niches represented by the set of resource values [N 1, N 2, N 3, N 4]. We used the values [250, 0.01, 250, 0.01] in all simulations, where 250 and 0.01 individuals are the “carrying capacities” respectively of the suitable and unsuitable niches (the latter value is slightly above 0 to avoid division by 0 in computations). Unsuitable niches can be thought of as containing resources that are defended (e.g. chemically or physically) against consumers. The resources (k = 1,…,4) are assumed to be perceived by consumers through a two-channel signal set (sk 1, sk 2), where 0≤skl≤1 for all k and l. In the context of plant-herbivore interactions, for example, the two components might be odorants of an odor signal, where the ratio of components and their total intensities are the salient cues. The signals in our simulations were [(0.2, 0.8), (0.4, 0.6), (0.6, 0.4), (0.8, 0.2)], which lines up the resources along a diagonal in the signal space (Fig. 1). This particular arrangement of the resources provides the most difficult discrimination task for the ANN, given the number of resources and dimensions of the signal space [37].
Exploiter perception and the ANN
The signals are ‘perceived’ by exploiter perceptrons: a feed-forward ANN (Fig. 2) capable of non-linear discrimination if the number of layers are at least three. ANNs are models of biological neural circuits with nodes having the functionality of a neural cell [35]. We have chosen a three-layer perceptron architecture with two sensory input nodes (one for each signal channel), three hidden nodes (the minimum required to discriminate four resources), and one output node (Fig. 2). Sensory nodes propagate the signals to the hidden nodes through a weight (mimicking synapses of real neurons). The weighted signals excite or inhibit the node, which will switch its output (from 0 to 1 or the reverse) should the node excitation pass a threshold (the switch, being defined by a sigmoidal function as is explained below, effectively occurs over an interval rather than at a point). The slope of the threshold over the switching interval is controlled by a “bias-weight,” which is commonly used in ANNs. Depending on weight settings, the node switches around a certain ratio of the two input signals (Fig. 1). The output signals of the hidden nodes are similarly propagated via weights to the single output node. Depending on the value of the weights, the output node integrates the sigmoidal responses of the hidden nodes into an effectively off-on (0–1) response. The output from the ANN is interpreted as strength-of-preference for the niche signals (Fig. 1), which is 0 (avoid niche) and 1 (utilize niche).
The genetic model
The 13 weights (parameters) in our perceptron automatons metaphorically represent 13 genes on a chromosome-pair. The alleles associated with these genes are assumed to be codominant: i.e. the weight of the synapse they are associated with is calculated by taking the average of the values of the two alleles. There are three sets of four genes each (w 1j, w 2j, bh j, w out j) that determine the contribution of the j th hidden neuron to the overall perceptron response to each of the resource signals. We interpret these four genes that are functionally associated with each node as ‘super-gene’ (SG) and its associated alleles a ‘super-allele’ (SA). This designation turns out to be useful for interpreting the evolutionary process. The order of genes on a chromosomal haplotype is w 11, w 21, bh 1, w 12, w 22, bh 2, w 13, w 23, bh 3, w out 1, w out 2, w out 3, bo and linkage applies in our rules for combining haplotypes (cf. Fig. 3). An additional mating gene, unlinked to the genes on the perceptron chromosome, determines whether the exploiter mates on the resource of its preference (effectively a form of assortative mating) or randomly across all niches [36]. These associated mating alleles are designated a (assortative) and r (random) respectively, with allele a dominant over r. We have made runs with the opposite dominance, without any significant differences in the results. Our model elaborates on the details of the genes for niche preference, whereas the genetic coding for mating behaviour is highly idealized to facilitate assortative mating (and speciation) after niche preference has emerged (i.e. evolved).
The algorithm – exploiter fitness
Each exploiter senses each resource-niche through the application of the resource's signal to the exploiter's two sensory (input) nodes. The signals are propagated through each exploiter i's perceptron one resource at the time, creating an output array Ψ = [ψ1,i, ψ2,i, ψ3,i, ψ4,i] for the response of this individual exploiter to niches I–IV, where resources k = 1…4 have the identification 1 = I, 2 = II, 3 = III, 4 = IV. The output response array is now the basis of the exploiter's (i) reproductive output (ek,i) prior to density-dependent effects on a resource k, which is computed using the formula
(1) |
where ϕ ( = 3) is the maximum reproductive output before progeny utilize any resources. In effect, the reproductive output depends on both the relative response (of resource k to the sum of all resources) and the absolute response, hence the squared ψ. If the reproductive rates are proportional to ψ rather than its square, simulations indicate that the ANN responses drift downwards towards zero. Hence eqn 1 has the effect that intermediate responses result in a reduced reproductive output, which implies selection for a bimodal (ψ = 0 or 1) [38] rather than gradual response to a niche landscape mapped onto the sensory input space. This has also the consequence that the reproductive output is the same for all exploiters expressing any of the 16 binary output arrays, regardless of being a specialist on one resource or generalist on all of them. Other models having a continuum of resources that usually apply convex niche-functions of fixed width [17]. In our model the ANN is capable of evolving niche-functions with multiple peaks of any width.
Next our model computes the fitness of exploiter i that includes a niche-specific density-dependent effect using the competition function
(2) |
where parameter ε ( = 1.5) sets the half-saturation density of the total reproductive output (Σi ek,i) in proportion to the carrying capacity (Nk) of the competitive function and a ( = 2.5) determines the abruptness in the onset of density-dependence around the population density level ε [39]. This phenomenological function with a sigmoidal shape provides gradual selection potentials at extreme ends, and rapid evolution during transition phases. Thus our model incorporates two costs: the cost of using unfavorable resources and the cost of competition with other exploiters. No other explicit costs relating to the degree of resource specialization or assortative mating are included in the model. This does not mean that we regard such costs as uncommon in nature; the evolution of phenotype-genotype interactions is more transparent without them, and they can easily be included in elaborated studies.
The algorithm – mating
The exploiters are assigned to one of five mating pools: a random mating pool p 0 and p 1–p 4 associated with the four niches. Mating genotypes rr belonging to pool p 0 and select mates within the whole population, whereas genotypes ra and aa in the various output response phenotypes select mates in one of p 1–p 4 with probabilities in proportion to their their response array Ψ. Specifically, to categorize the exploiters' mating types, we used the rounded integer response values of Ψ. Thus niche 1 specialists [1000] were assigned to pool p 1 with probability 1, selective-generalists [1010] were assigned to pools p 1 and p 3 with probabilities 0.5 respectively, and so on. The first exploiter of a pair was chosen at random from the whole population. It was paired with a random partner within its pools if it belonged to any of pools p 1–p 4 or with a random partner in the whole population if it belonged to pool p 0. This has the consequence that individuals mating assortatively cannot choose to mate with individuals mating randomly. Paired exploiters were removed from the pool once mated, until there was either a single individual with no mate or no one was left. The number of offspring assigned to each reproducing pair equalled the nearest integer of the average fitness value of the individuals in the pair (eq. 2).
The algorithm – formation of recruits
The genotypes of the diploid offspring are created through sexual recombination of parental genes. At reproduction each exploiter produces a haploid gamete consisting of a preference chromosome and a mating chromosome. The preference chromosome is created by copying the alleles from one randomly chosen parental chromosome to the gamete, with a probability ( = 0.0001 per allele) of a cross-over at any locus. With the numbers in our simulation this amounted to an average of about one chromosome in the population exhibiting cross-over in each generation. We also allowed for point mutations to occur with probability 0.01 per allele. where the size of the change to the value of the weight mutation was drawn from a uniform probability distribution on (−10… 10), with the constraint to weight values to lie within the range (−20… 20). This avoided the weights drifting into very large or small (large negative) numbers. With this mutation rate, around 100 chromosomes on average were experienced to at least one point mutation each generation, but some changes were small and others constrained. Mating chromosomes were copied by randomly selecting one of the parental chromosomes, and when allowed were subject to a mutational event that transformed them from r to a, or the reverse, with probability 0.0005 (i.e. c. 0.4 mating chromosomes were altered in this way every generation). Model sensitivity to mutation rates and range of perturbations of the resource preference genes has been explored in a previous study [38]. Although higher mutation rates generally promote faster evolution and shorter simulation times, that study shows that increasing the rate of mutation in the preference genes from 0.01, as reported bere, to 0.15, made it much more unlikely for guilds that were matching the resources to evolve. Additionally, that study shows that the range of mutational perturbations of resource alleles has to be suffiently large (−3…3 was compared with −0.2…0.2) to enable simulations to escape local minima to find globally close-to-optimal solutions. Since the mutations on the mating genes have a strong and direct effect, we selected rates that provide a balance between the total mutational rates of the mating gene and the whole resource preference gene complex. The code is available on request.
The algorithm – iterations
The simulations were initialized with 500 individuals in the exploiter population. Each was assigned a diploid genome (two haploid neural chromosomes) with values at each of the 13 haploid loci drawn from a uniform probability distribution on (−1…1). This created a population of non-discriminating phenotypes with high preference for all habitats, and with some genetic variation (a standard initialization of ANNs) [35]. The evolutionary process was iterated over 100,000 generations where random mating was enforced during the first 20,000 generations by not allowing mutations to occur to the random mating allele initially assigned to all individuals. This constraint allowed time for a guild of exploiters of niches I–IV to evolve before continuing with the simulation to evaluate the process whereby mating alleles evolve and separate the population into assertive mating groups. Such groups then have the potential to genetically diverge and hence speciate. After 20,000 generations the random mating alleles were allowed to mutate to assortative mating, as well as back again to random. After 80,000 generations, we switched off the mutations to get rid off phenotypes continuously generated by mutations. This permits us to get beyond the initial conditions and study the evolutionary process of the emergence of fittest niche-preference alleles over the first 20,000 generation, evaluate the additional complexities of the action of habitat-linked mating genes over the next 60 thousand generations, and finally confirm the genotypes formed by selection alone (without mutations) over the final 20,000 generations.
Analysis of genetic data
We used principle components analysis (PCA) to verify the existence of haplotypes (Fig. 4). Specifically, we used a Matlab® princomp algorithm to identify clusters and then plotted the haplotypes visually demonstrating that clustering had occured. Evolving phenotypes were classified to the nearest integer response array Ψ. Hence 16 phenotype classes are possible using such binary array representation. Thus, for example, [0,0,1,0] phenotypes are specialists phenotype on niche III while [1,1,1,1] are the non-discriminating generalists.
Test of mutation robustness in phenotypes
Since evolution can be driven by selection for genetic robustness [40], [41] we sampled our simulation for exploiter individuals every 1000 generations on the interval 15,000–20,000 when the population was still under enforced random mating, and on the interval 27,000–32,000 to when assortative mating had evolved. After sorting individuals obtained from the above samples into genotypes, the proportion of phenotypes that were unfit was then evaluated, where unfit phenotypes are defined as those that show a preference for one or more unfavourable niches (with detrimental or non-viable resources). To assess the effects of mutations, K mutants were generated by randomly picking with replacement (i.e. returning sampled individuals to the pool unaltered) an individual from the phenotype group of interest and subjected it to a mutation (a weight perturbation as in the simulation) at a random locus. The sample size K = P(P−1), where P is the size of the phenotype pool, means that many individuals were resampled but the mutations are likely to differ. We then evaluated the number of unfit phenotypes among the K so produced. The percentage was multiplied by P and rounded off to the nearest integer to represent the expected number of unfit individuals in a genotype after being subjected to mutations. The increase in unfit individuals is expressed as a percentage of the original number.
Results
Evolving gene complex for resource selection
The initial 500 perceptrons at the start of each new simulation were assigned genetic values (i.e. node weightings and biases) at random, which accounts for the initial lack of clustering of haplotypes (Fig. 4a) and genotypes (Fig. 4b). Twenty thousand generations later, under random mating, two haplotypes (Fig. 4c) have emerged, creating three diploid genotypes in four clusters (Fig. 4d; clusters AB and BA represent the same genotypes but are clustered differently because the allele vectors are aligned in two ways). In the depicted case, haplotypes A and B constitute a stable polymorphism in which the homozygote genotype AA and the heterozygote genotype AB (Fig. 4d) express phenotype P1, a specialist on resource 1 (i.e. exploits niche I only) (Fig. 1), whereas homozygote genotype BB expresses phenotype P2, a specialist on resource 3 (i.e. exploits niche III only).
During the simulation, four SAs (super-alleles, see Methods, the genetic model) d 1, d 2, d 3 and m emerged in association with the SGs (super genes), arranged as haplotype A = md 10 and B = d 3 d 20, where 0 is a marker for alleles not involved with discrimination. These give rise to the three genotypes AA = mm d 1 d 1 00, BB = d 3 d 3 d 2 d 2 00, and AB = md 3 d 1 d 2 00. In homozygous form the SAs d 1, d 2, and d 3 respectively facilitate discrimination of niche I from the others, niches III and IV from the others, and niches I–III from IV. In homozygous form, the SA m does not facilitate discrimination (Fig. 1); but in heterozygous form SA m with SA d 3 (genotype AB) modifies the hidden neuron response to favour niches I over II–IV (as a homozygote SA d 1 would do). SAs d 1 and d 2 are mutually neutralized when together in heterozygote form (genotype AB) because d 1 excites while d 2 inhibits the output neuron (neuron 2, wout 2, Fig. 3). Thus, there are SAs that can be functionally ‘silenced’, meaning that they are not involved in any discrimination. In the polymorphism with random mating, the consequence of ‘silencing’ is that mutations are not expressed in either SG1 of the AA genotype or in SG2 of the AB genotype. Mutations can thus accumulate each generation until the mutated allele becomes expressed by rearrangement in F1 or later generations: only then is the defective allele removed by selection (Fig. 5).
Out of ten repeated independent simulations checked at generation 20,000, six developed the type of polymorphism just described (the heterozygote and one of the homozygotes code for the same phenotype). The other four either developed a polymorphism with the two homozygotes expressing P1 and the heterozygote P2 (or the reverse; two simulations), or developed a monomorphic population of a discriminant generalists (phenotype [1,0,1,0]; two simulations). All of the six type cases of polymorphism evolved into homozygote specialists. In this presentation we focus on these type-cases to get deeper insights to the underlying mechanisms. The reader interested in the evolution of generalists and specialists is referred to our previous studies on asexual populations [37].
Evolving assortative mating
In the most common situation, after an often short but variable lag phase from the onset of mutations in the mating gene at generation 20,000, the assortative mating allele rapidly invades the population (Fig. 6b). The two haplotypes A and B (Fig. 4e) still code for the same genotypes in diploid form (Fig. 4f), but the heterozygote numbers are reduced due to assortative mating (Fig. 4f). This low proportion of heterozygotes are maintained as a result of mutational noise on the parental genes causing interbreeding among the homozygote lineages. The frequency of heterozygotes persists in mutation-selection equilibrium (Fig. 6a). In simulations with allele r dominant over a, this equilibrium exhibits a higher proportion of heterozygotes. Together with a less rapid invasion of a, these are the only discernable differences when dominance in the mating alleles is reversed (Table S1). When mutations in our simulations are arrested after 80,000 generations, selection removes the heterozygotes completely and creates perfect homozygote specialists mating assortatively (Fig. 6). The simulation thus demonstrates that two specialist phenotypes can arise in an environment with two favourable niches, first as stable polymorphism and then as two genetically distinct and reproductively isolated populations, provided mating takes place within niche-specific locations.
Selection mechanism for assortative mating
The exploiters' fitness is determined by their accuracy in selecting viable (non-detrimental) resources and competition within the niches for these resources (eq. 2). In the polymorphism prior to when assortative mating is permitted, the proportion of heterozygotes that exhibit unfit phenotypes (i.e. exhibit preference for at least one detrimental resource) (11.4%) is slightly less than that of homozygotes (AA: 13.9%, and BB: 16.3%, Table 1). Thus we obtain the important insight that assortative mating is able to evolve for reasons other than selection against inferior heterozygotes, which is the case in other models of speciation. The total number of unfit phenotypes in the population as a whole was significantly lower after assortative mating was introduced (14.0%, vs. 11.6%, ; ML χ 2 = 5.97, p = 0.015, Table 1), thereby indicating selection for assortative mating. Since the mutation rate is kept constant, there appear to be only two possible explanations for this observation: (i) either assortative-mating phenotypes are more resistant than random-mating phenotypes to change in phenotypic expression from genomic point mutations or (ii) the removal rate of unfit phenotypes is higher in assortative mating than in random mating populations. To distinguish between these two possibilities, we subjected phenotypes selected from both randomly and assortatively mating populations to random mutations (see methods). The increase in the number of unfit phenotypes resulting from these mutational perturbations was the same for both populations (13.0% vs. 12.9% ; ML χ 2 = 0.01, n.s., Table 1) leading us to conclude that the evolution of assortative mating is associated with the benefits from a higher removal rate of unfit phenotypes.
Table 1. Comparison of genetic robustness in randomly (rand) and assortatively (asst) mating populations.
Genotype | AA | AB | BB | Total | ||||||||
Mating | rand | asst | rand | asst | rand | asst | rand | asst | ||||
Unfit phenotypes (%) | 13.9 | 7.4 | ** | 11.4 | 31.1 | *** | 16.3 | 11.4 | *** | 14.0 | 11.6 | * |
Increase (%) in unfit phenotypes | 7.9 | 9.5 | n.s. | 8.2 | 0.0 | *** | 16.5 | 17.6 | n.s. | 13.0 | 12.9 | n.s. |
N | 187 | 913 | 962 | 206 | 1077 | 1127 | 2226 | 2246 |
Statistics are maximum likelihood χ2 values: n.s. = not significant,
*p<0.05,
**p<0.01,
***p<0.001.
Pairs of columns show the proportion of unfit phenotypes (those responding to one or more detrimental resources) for the labeled genotypes (AA, AB, and BB), as well as the population as a whole (Total). The first row of results pertains to the proportion of unfit phenotypes just before (rand) or soon after (asst) the invasion of the assortative mating gene (Methods). The second row of results pertains to the percentage increase in unfit phenotypes after the genotypes have been subject to single point mutations (Methods). Statistics indicate significant differences in the proportion of the functional phenotypes before and after the invasion of assortative mating, indicated for each row of results.
In a population of two assortatively mating homozygote specialists AA and BB, mutations in the functional SGs are immediately expressed and subject to selection. It is due to the lower selection rate against harmful mutations that the population when mating randomly expresses a higher number of unfit phenotypes compared with the population when mating assortatively. Mutations in the silent SG (mm) of genotype AA will accumulate mutations over many generations as long as these genotypes keep mating assortatively. A post-zygotic reproductive barrier thus builds up, and incidental hybridization will more likely result in inferior offspring.
Discussion
The prevailing view of sympatric speciation is that it acts through disruptive selection on adaptive quantitative traits (e.g. morphology of feeding apparatuses or on physiological systems related to detoxification of plant defensive compounds). Here we propose a complementary view in which selection acts against mutations accumulating on epistatically-acting niche-preference alleles that are only conditionally expressed in a random mating population. The two views are different with regard to adaptability, the genetic mechanisms involved, and how the selection process acts. In the prevailing view, under adaptive trait selection individuals are morphologically or physiologically canalized to perform better in one niche than the other [17], whereas in our model no such canalization exists: the niche arises purely through preference imposed by the individual's perceptual system. From a functional perspective alone, many species appear to be far more specialized than they need to be [42]. This observations suggests that niche width itself is under selection [37] probably driven by frequency-dependent emergent competition for resources (i.e. emergent in the sense of acting on the population level in contrast to the individual level). If niche preference is also a trait under disruptive selection in sympatric speciation then one would expect it to be the only trait differentiated in newly formed species. In this light, the sister species in which host choice is the only character correlated with genetic differentiation is an intriguing observation [28], [43]. We examined the sensitivity of the evolution of specialists to parameter settings in a previous study [38]. Specialists readily evolves when the range of mutational perturbations are 1/3 of the selected value. Specialists still evolves but with a lower probability even at 1/50 of the selected value, if combined with a higher mutation rate. Cross-overs at rather low rates, however, promote the evolution of a uniform population of discrimating generalists [1010] rather than two specialists. We also know that speciation can evolve in an upscaled environment with six niches, although a longer evolutionary time is required (Table S1) to see this occur.
Current speciation models typically employ additive genetics [17], [44]. Mutations in additive genes cause limited change on the expressed trait (at least in the models referred to above), and hybrids express phenotypes intermediate to their parents. In contrast, when there is disruptive selection on non-additive or epistatic genes, polymorphism readily evolves [45]. Host recognition in species such as herbivorous insects is determined by the interaction of many genes coding for receptor proteins with varying specificity [46]–[49], where individuals typically respond to ratios of signal components rather than signal strength per se [49], [50] over a moderate range of concentrations. Mutational changes can have large effects on phenotype expression with a high degree of freedom for the genotype to express phenotypes. Hybrids among genetic lineages are rarely expressing intermediate traits, as seen in tephritid flies [48]. On the contrary, they can express functional phenotypes, so that evolutionary branching can occur under random mating and establish a polymorphism (Fig. 6).
The selection mechanism in our model, ultimately acting against accumulating mutations, is different from existing theories that demonstrate how reinforcement-like selection on inferior heterozygotes selects for assortative mating [16], [17]. In both these and our models, genes for assortative mating must evolve, and as such they must also be linked to ecological or niche preference genes. A gene for mating within a specific niche, as we have included, does however, not need to be physically linked to resource preference genes if these are under disruptive selection [20]. It should also be noted that the initial polymorphism (and the following speciation) would not have evolved in case the environment had been arranged in a way that enables our ANNs to categorize the two viable niches as one superniche [37] (i.e. the opportunity exists for two kinds specialist versus one kind of generalist to emerge in our two viable niche environment). In some cases in real systems the discrimination task is trivial, e.g. to distinguish the odour of two potential host-plants that signal using compounds that do not overlap with respect to the input sensory channels (if they do overlap then ratios of compounds become important, thereby posing a greater challenge to discrimination: e.g. see [51]). Some resources, like prey, may mimic their environment being cryptic, or mimicking a noxious prey being involved in an arms race with their exploiter driven by the exploiters perceptual system [52], [53]. Evolution of asexual haploids in an environment with varying non-zero resource values, resulted in a guild with non-discriminating generalists and specialist whose population numbers matched the resource values [37]. It remains to be investigated whether or not this latter result holds for sexually reproducing organisms.
In conclusion, our model demonstrates a new feasible process for sympatric speciation in diploid sexual organisms (Fig. 7). A non-discriminating, random mating, ancestral population appears in an environment of two niches (niche 1 and niche 2 denoted by green areas in Fig. 7) that requires sensory recognition by the exploiters. Alternatively the ancestral population initially inhabits one niche and a novel niche appears in the environment. Failure to recognize any of the two niches is detrimental to the phenotype (grey area Fig. 7). This selects for two phenotypes specialized on each resource, acting on the diploid genome with epistatic regulation of the sensory trait (1 in Fig. 7). The result is a polymorphism in which genotypes AA, and AB utilize niche 1 and genotype BB utilize niche 2, but other genotype-phenotype matching cannot be ruled out as likely until a large number of simulations have been undertaken (it will still be a challenge to calculate the likelihood of the different outcomes). Four alleles evolve in two loci (m, d1, d2 and d3), which are silenced when paired mm in homozygote AA or d1d2 in the heterozygote (see Fig. 5). This kind of allelic interaction readily evolves when mapping three genotypes onto two niches, most likely aided by the assumed codominance of homologous alleles. It is noteworthy that SG3 has not evolved any functional alleles at all, which may suggest that allelic interaction is not due to a size-constraint of the ANN and the genome. Silenced alleles accumulate harmful mutations (an example is marked with a black oval around the allele) in two or more generations (2 in Fig. 7). Disrupted alleles are eventually expressed in the offspring, which becomes inferior with regard to utilize an available niche (3 in Fig. 7; see Fig. 5 for a full crossing scheme). In this scenario, there is selection for assortative mating since it will reduce the proportion of unfit offspring. An allele for mating in association with the niche (a) is introduced to the population (4 in Fig. 7), which will spread in the population. In an assortatively mating population, the heterozygotes will be reduced by 50% by outcrossing in each generation, resulting in two genetic populations of pre-zygotically isolated (by assortatively mating) homozygote specialists. A Bateson-Dobzhansky-Muller incompatibility [25], [26] builds up by the undisturbed accumulation of mutations in genotype AA (5 in Fig. 7; marked by black encircling). Should an incidental hybridization occur (6 in Fig. 7), the hybrid is a heterozygote, more certain to have severely reduced vigor (7 in Fig. 7).
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
Eva Kisdi and eight anonymous reviewers provided valuable comments on the manuscript.
Footnotes
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Funding: This work was funded by a James S. McDonnell Foundation 21st Century Science Initiative Award (2003016, http://www.jsmf.org/grants/d.php?id=2003016) to WMG. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Fitzpatrick BM, Fordyce JA, Gavrilets S. Pattern, process and geographic modes of speciation. J Evolution Biol. 2009;22:2342–2347. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01833.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Ryan PG, Bloomer P, Moloney CL, Grant TJ, Delport W. Ecological Speciation in South Atlantic Island Finches. Science. 2007;315:1420–1423. doi: 10.1126/science.1138829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Savolainen V, Anstett M-C, Lexer C, Hutton I, Clarkson JJ, et al. Sympatric speciation in palms on an oceanic island. Nature. 2006;441:210–213. doi: 10.1038/nature04566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Barluenga M, Stöltig KN, Salzburger W, Muschick M, Meyer A. Sympatric speciation in Nicaraguan crater lake cichlid fish. Nature. 2006;439:719–723. doi: 10.1038/nature04325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Friesen VL, Smith AL, Gómez-Diaz E, Bolton M, Furness RW, et al. Sympatric speciation by allochrony in a seabird. PNAS. 2007;104:18589–18594. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0700446104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Taylor EB, McPhail JD. Historical contingency and ecological determinism interact to prime speciation in sticklebacks, Gasterosteus. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2000;267:2375–2384. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hohenlohe PA, Bassman S, Etter PD, Stiffler N, Johnson EA, et al. Population Genomics of Parallel Adaptation in Threespine Stickleback using Sequenced RAD Tags. PLoS Genetics. 2010;6:e1000862. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1000862. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1000862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bell MA, Aguirre WE, Buck NJ. Twelve years of contemporary armor evolution in a threespine stickleback population. Evolution. 2004;814 doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2004.tb00414.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Panova M, Hollander J, Johannesson K. Site-specific genetic divergence in parallel hybrid zones suggests nonallopatric evolution of reproductive barriers. Mol Ecol. 2006;15:4021–4031. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2006.03067.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Coyne JA, Orr HA. The evolutionary genetics of speciation. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B. 1998;353:287–305. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1998.0210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Levene H. Genetic equilibrium when more than one ecological niche is available. Am Nat. 1953;87:331–333. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Maynard Smith J. Sympatric speciation. Am Nat. 1966;100:637–650. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Felsenstein J. Skepticism towards Santa Rosalia, or why are there so few kinds of animals. Evolution. 1981;35:124–138. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1981.tb04864.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gavrilets S. The Maynard Smith model of sympatric speciation. J theor Biol. 2006;239:172–182. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2005.08.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Getz WM, Kaitala V. Ecogenetic models, competition, and heteropatry. Theoret Pop Biol. 1989;36:34–58. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Geritz SAH, Kisdi E. Adaptive dynamics in diploid, sexual populations and the evolution of reproductive isolation. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2000;267:1671–1678. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Dieckmann U, Doebeli M. On the origin of species by sympatric speciation. Nature. 1999;400:354–357. doi: 10.1038/22521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Rundle HD, Nosil P. Ecological speciation. Ecol Lett. 2005;8:336–352. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Gavrilets S. Perspective: Models of speciation: What have we learned in 40 years? Evolution. 2003;57:2197–2215. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2003.tb00233.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bolnick DI, Fitzpatrick BM. Sympatric speciation: models and empirical evidence. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 2007;38:459–487. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Turelli M, Barton NH, Coyne JA. Theory and speciation. Trends Ecol Evol. 2001;16:330–343. doi: 10.1016/s0169-5347(01)02177-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Rueffler C, Van Dooren TJM, Leimar O, Abrams PA. Disruptive selection and then what? Trends Ecol Evol. 2006;21:238–245. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2006.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Van Dooren TJM. The evolutionary ecology of dominance-recessivity. J theor Biol. 1999;198:519–532. doi: 10.1006/jtbi.1999.0929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Van Dooren TJM. Protected polymorphism and evolutionary stability in peliotropic models with trait-specific dominance. Evolution. 2006;60:1991–2003. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Fierst JL, Hansen TF. Genetic architecture and postzygotic reproductive isolation: evolution of Bateson-Dobzhansky-Muller incompatibilities in a polygenic model. Evolution. 2009;64:675–693. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2009.00861.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Orr HA, Turelli M. The evolution of postzygotic isolation: accumulating Dobzhansky-Muller incompatibilities. Evolution. 2001;55:1085–1094. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2001.tb00628.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Bickford D, Lohman DJ, Sodhi NS, Ng PKL, Meier R, et al. Cryptic species as a window on diversity and conservation. Trends Ecol Evol. 2007;22:148–155. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2006.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Smith MA, Rodriguez JJ, Whitfield JB, Deans AR, Janzen DH, et al. Extreme diversity of tropical parasitoid wasps exposed by iterative integration of natural history, DNA barcoding, morphology, and collections. P Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:12359–12364. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805319105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hebert PDN, Penton EH, Burns JM, Janzen DH, Hallwachs W. Ten species in one: DNA barcoding reveals cryptic species in the neotropical skipper butterfly Astraptes fulgerator. P Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:14812–14817. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0406166101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Bush GL. Sympatric speciation in phytophagous parasitic insects. In: Price PW, editor. Evolutionary strategies of parasitic insects and mites. New York: Plenum; 1975. pp. 187–206. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Diehl SR, Bush GL. The role of habitat preference in adaptation and speciation. In: Otte D, Endler J, editors. Speciation and its consequences. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates; 1989. pp. 345–365. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Fry JD. Multilocus models of sympatric speciation: Bush versus Rice versus Felsenstein. Evolution. 2003;57:1735–1746. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2003.tb00582.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Tosh C, Ruxton GD. Modelling perception with artificial neural networks. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ryan MJ, Getz WM. Signal decoding and receiver evolution: an analysis using a neural network. 2000. unpublished. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 35.Haykin S. Neural networks, a comprehensive foundation. New York: MacMillan College Publishing Company; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Doebeli M. A quantitative genetic competition model for sympatric speciation. J evol Biol. 1996;9:893–909. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Holmgren NMA, Getz WM. Evolution of host plant selection in insects under perceptual constraints: a simulation study. Evol Ecol Res. 2000;2:81–106. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Holmgren NMA, Norrström N, Getz WM. Artificial neural networks in models of specialization, guild evolution and sympatric speciation. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2007;362:431–440. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1970. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Getz WM. A hypothesis regarding the abruptness of density dependence and the growth rate of populations. Ecology. 1996;77:2014–2026. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bornholdt S, Sneppen K. Robustness as an evolutionary principle. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2000;267:2281–2286. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Azevedo RBR, Lohaus R, Srinivasan S, Dang KK, Burch CL. Sexual reproduction selects for robustness and negative epistasis in artificial gene networks. Nature. 2006;440:87–90. doi: 10.1038/nature04488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Dethier VG. Chemical insect attractants and repellents. Philadelphia: Blakiston Co; 1947. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Smith MA, Woodley NE, Janzen DH, Hallwachs W, Hebert PDN. DNA barcodes reveal cryptic host-specificity within the presumed polyphagous members of a genus of parasitoid flies (Diptera: Tachinidae). Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103:3657–3662. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0511318103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Dobzhansky T. Genetics and the Origin of Species. Columbia University Press; 1937. [Google Scholar]
- 45.ten Tusscher KHWJ, Hogeweg P. The role of genome and gene regulatory network canalization in the evolution of multi-trait polymorphisms and sympatric speciation. BMC Evolutionary Biology. 2009;9:159–179. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-9-159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ray A, van Naters WvdG, Carlson JR. A regulatory code for neuron-specific odor receptor expression. PLOS Biology. 2008;6:1069–1083. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Fishilevich E, Vosshall LB. Genetic and functional subdivision of the Drosophila antennal love. Curr Biol. 2005;15:1548–1553. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.07.066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Olsson SB, Linn CE, Jr, Michel A, Dambroski HR, Berlocher SH, et al. Receptor expression and sympatric speciation: unique olfactory receptor neuron responses in F1 hybrid Rhagoletis populations. J exp Biol. 2006;209:3729–3741. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Said I, Tauban D, Renou M, Mori K, Rochat D. Structure and function of the antennal sensilla of the palm weevil Rhynchophorus palmarum (Coleoptera, Curculionidae). J Insect Physiol. 2003;49:857–872. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1910(03)00137-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Visser JH. Host odor perception in phytophagous insects. Annu Rev Entomol. 1986;31:121–144. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Akers RP, Getz WM. Response of olfactory receptor neurons in honey bees to odorants and their binary mixtures. J Comp Physiol A. 1993;173:169–185. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Norrström N, Getz WM, Holmgren NMA. Coevolution of exploiter specialization and victim mimicry can be cyclic and saltational. Evolutionary Bioinformatics Online. 2006;2:1–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Holmgren NMA, Enquist M. Dynamics of mimicry evolution. Biol J Linn Soc. 1999;66:145–158. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.