Skip to main content
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
. 2011 Dec 29;5:169. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00169

Practice of Contemporary Dance Promotes Stochastic Postural Control in Aging

Lena Ferrufino 1,2,*, Blandine Bril 1, Gilles Dietrich 1,3, Tetsushi Nonaka 1,4, Olivier A Coubard 2,*
PMCID: PMC3247757  PMID: 22232582

Abstract

As society ages and the frequency of falls increases, counteracting gait and posture decline is a challenging issue for countries of the developed world. Previous studies have shown that exercise and hazard management help to improve balance and/or decrease the risks for falling in normal aging. Motor activity based on motor-skill learning, particularly dance, can also benefit balance and decreases falls with age. Recent studies have suggested that older dancers have better balance, posture, or gait than non-dancers. Additionally, clinical or laboratory measures have shown improvements in some aspects of balance after dance interventions in elderly trainees. This study examined the impact of contemporary dance (CD) and of fall prevention (FP) programs on postural control of older adults. Posturography of quiet upright stance was performed in 41 participants aged 59–86 years before and after 4.4-month training in either CD or FP once a week. Though classical statistic scores failed to show any effect, dynamic analyses of the center-of-pressure displacements revealed significant changes after training. Specifically, practice of CD enhanced the critical time interval in diffusion analysis, and reduced recurrence and mathematical stability in recurrence quantification analysis, whereas practice of FP induced or tended to induce the reverse patterns. Such effects were obtained only in the eyes open condition. We suggest that CD training based on motor improvisation favored stochastic posture inducing plasticity in motor control, while FP training based on more stereotyped behaviors did not.

Keywords: aging, postural control, motor activity, contemporary dance, flexibility, plasticity

Introduction

Balance is a complex function achieved by (i) multi-sensory integration of visual, vestibular, and somesthetic afferences, (ii) central motor control, and (iii) context-specific response generation (Nashner, 1976). In aging, changes occur in visual (Lord, 2006), vestibular (Kristinsdottir et al., 2001), proprioceptive and exteroceptive inputs (Famula et al., 2008), central processing (Horak, 2006), and muscular effectors (Schultz, 1995). Reduced sensory cue congruency, increased visual dependency and motor tone cause decreased balance and unsteady gait (Judge, 2003). Balance and gait disorders are the second risk factor for falls in aging with dramatic morbidity and mortality consequences (Rubenstein, 2006). Developing strategies to prevent falls is thus a major issue of public health to preserve a successful aging (Judge, 2003).

Several programs have been proposed to improve balance and reduce falls in aging: exercise, environmental inspection, and hazard management (Day et al., 2002; Rubenstein, 2006). Home- or center-based exercise interventions have focused on the training of lower limb strength and of balance (Robertson et al., 2001), walking and stair climbing with weights, joint reinforcement, functional balance (King et al., 2002), or flexed posture (Benedetti et al., 2008), and their efficacy has been assessed using clinical (Berg et al., 1995; Rossiter-Fornoff et al., 1995) or laboratory (Benedetti et al., 2008) measures. Tai Chi and dance have also been suggested to be promising programs to develop balance and prevent falls in older adults (American Geriatrics Society, British Geriatrics Society, and American Academy of Orthopaedic Surgeons Panel on Falls Prevention, 2001; Judge, 2003).

Cross-sectional studies have shown that older social dancers (i.e., ballroom, in line, or pairs) have better balance, a more stable walking pattern (Verghese, 2006), faster leg reaction time, and better postural stability (Zhang et al., 2008) than non-dancers. In an intervention study, Shigematsu et al. (2002) showed that 20 women aged 72–87 years trained to 36 sessions of dance-based aerobic exercise had better balance in single-leg stance and functional reach, and higher locomotion in walking around two cones, compared to untrained controls. After only six sessions of a Laban-based movement program, Hamburg and Clair (2003) observed that 36 adults aged 63–86 years increased their balance in time up-and-go and standing toe/heel lifts, and their velocity and cadence gait. Alpert et al. (2009) reported a progressive balance improvement in the Sensory Organization Test in 13 women aged 52–88 years performing jazz dance for 15 weeks. Hui et al. (2009) compared 52 adults aged 68 years in average trained to low impact aerobic dance (cross and Cha-cha steps) to 42 untrained controls. After 24 sessions, dancers had improved their dynamic balance in time up-and-go, but not their static balance as assessed by the Physical Performance Battery.

The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of two motor activities sought to improve balance, contemporary dance (CD), and fall prevention (FP), on postural control of older adults. To achieve this goal, we used a force platform and measured the center-of-pressure (CoP) displacements in upright stance, eyes open, and eyes closed, before and after 4.4 months of training. In addition to classical statistic scores (length, areas, mean, and variance velocities, Romberg quotient, fractal dimension), we examined the dynamics of CoP displacements using stabilogram diffusion analysis (SDA; Collins and De Luca, 1993) and recurrence quantification analysis (RQA; Riley et al., 1999). In SDA, Collins and De Luca (1993) have shown that the trajectories of the mean square displacement plotted as a function of time interval are different from those expected for a Brownian motion. Indeed, the SD plot changes slope after a critical point thus exhibiting short- and long-term regions which, from a physiological viewpoint, respectively approximate open- and closed-loop control mechanisms. RQA is a non-linear method that yields, among others, the degree of autocorrelation measured by recurrence (%REC), determinism vs. randomness (%DET), and mathematical stability measured by maximal line diagonal (MAXL). RQA is an empirical method rather than a theoretical approach. Thus RQA measures are not considered as absolute but must be taken with respect to the levels of a manipulated data (e.g., before and after a training; Riley et al., 1999).

Regarding the influence of the sensory context (i.e., vision), SDA has shown two patterns. Visual input has caused either a decrease or an increase in stochastic activity, which has been interpreted in both schemes to serve to decrease the stiffness of the musculoskeletal system (Collins and De Luca, 1995). In RQA, the deterministic structure of CoP displacements increase with the eyes closed as compared to eyes open (Riley et al., 1999). With regards to age, critical mean square displacement and critical time interval increase with age in SDA (Collins et al., 1995). While no systematic study has been done on the aging effect on RQA measures (Riley et al., 1999), %REC, %DET, and MAXL are known to increase with decreasing behavioral flexibility (Riley et al., 1999; Webber and Zbilut, 2005). Consistent, Parkinson’s disease yield higher values of %REC, %DET, and MAXL compared to healthy controls (Schmit et al., 2006), while ballet dancers have lower RQA values than track athletes (Schmit et al., 2005).

In this study, we expected the CD program to favor stochastic posture in SDA and postural flexibility in RQA in higher proportion than the FP program. As both training programs were mostly performed eyes open, such effects were expected to be observed in the eyes open condition. To justify this hypothesis, we suggest that normal aging is accompanied by inflexibility as a result of decreasing motor and cognitive control with age. Indeed, attentional control show more precocious and larger decline than other functions in older adults (Bherer et al., 2004), consistent with an early decline of prefrontal areas of the brain (Rajah and D’Esposito, 2005; Raz and Rodrigue, 2006). As a correlate, motor control has been shown to decline with age with pejorative consequences for gait and posture (de Bruin and Schmidt, 2010; Theill et al., 2011). In a recent study, Coubard et al. (2011) showed that the practice of CD improved switching attention (i.e., cognitive flexibility), which was not the case of that of FP or of Tai Chi Chuan. As CD focused on motor improvisation while FP and Tai Chi Chuan taught motor routines, it was suggested that CD may have worked as a training for change, thus inducing plasticity in flexible attention (Coubard et al., 2011). Yet the causal relationship between motor and cognitive dimensions remain under debate (Coubard, 2011), we suggest that such a motor correlate may be observed for posture. In other words, we expected CD to improve motor flexibility (in other words to reduce motor stiffness) as compared to FP, which should take the form of higher stochastic activity in SDA and reduced determinism in RQA.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Forty-one French natives participated in the study, which was approved by the local ethics committee (Ecole des Hautes Etudes en Sciences Sociales, Paris). They were right-handed, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, no known neurological disorders, and were unaware of the goal of the experiment. Table 1 details the participants’ sex, age, body mass index (BMI, defined as the weight divided by the squared height in kg m−2), years of education, and their score in the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for cognitive status (Folstein et al., 1975).

Table 1.

Number (gender) or mean ± SD (age, BMI, education, MMSE) for the groups of participants (CD, contemporary dance; FP, fall prevention).

CD FP
Sex (women/men) 16/0 23/2
Age (years) 73.7 ± 5.5 72.9 ± 7.2
BMI (kg m−2) 26.9 ± 4.8 24.6 ± 3.3
Education (years) 9.8 ± 4.4 7.8 ± 3.9
MMSE (/30) 27.4 ± 1.4 27.2 ± 2.1

Sixteen participants aged 64–83 years made up the CD group, and 25 participants aged 59–86 years made up the FP group. The two groups were matched in sex (Chi2 < 1), age (t < 1), BMI (t39 = 1.84, P > 0.05), education (t39 = 1.38, P > 0.05), and their score in the MMSE (t < 1).

The two groups were matched in past physical activity as appreciated by the reported years of practice, except for gymnastics. One participant had done 1 year of FP in the past, eight participants had done 4.0 ± 5.4 years of aqua gymnastics (no between-group difference, Mann–Whitney test, Z < 1), and 19 of them had done 4.7 ± 4.5 years of gymnastics, with higher practice for the FP group (Mann–Whitney, Z = −2.68, P < 0.01).

Apparatus

Static posture was examined using a Techno-Concept platform (Céreste, France), which consisted of two dynamometric clogs, one for each foot, embedded in a board so that the angle made by the feet was 30°. The displacements of the CoP were recorded for 51.2 s and digitized at 40 Hz using a 16 bit analogical–digital converter.

Training programs

Participants were trained to CD or FP for 4.42 months in average. Participants did not have a choice in the training in which they were enrolled, which was set by the district where they lived in the Ile-de-France region. The training was conducted by a professional instructor and supervised by a senior teacher, each in their specialty (CD or FP). The frequency of the training was once a week, and each session lasted 1 h. Music could be used during up to 50% of the session duration. Participants did not take part in other motor programs during the intervention period.

Contemporary dance

Contemporary dance focused on motor improvisation. (1) Opening was adapted to the needs of the group (e.g., variations of the action of walking, free dance on a popular music). (2) Warm-up and preparation to dance. Body was awakened by passive and active movements of joints, and movements of muscular stretching promoting coordination, link between breath and movement, and body positioning and alignment. (3) Improvisation. Based on a theme or constraint (word, action, idea, object, music, or location), it was organized around four steps: (i) individual exploration of the theme; (ii) exploration in pair or more taking into account the others’ presence; (iii) each group presented the work developed in (i) and (ii); (iv) participants improvised in a solo and developed a natural movement to express their own sensations. At steps (i) and (ii), the instructor suggested dance tools to favor exploration and use of each individual and of the exercise resources. (4) Closure. Cooling down by breath and massages. For a session, 5, 20, 30, and 5 min were respectively dedicated to the stages 1–4.

Fall prevention

Fall prevention focused on balance and the development of lower limbs. A session was organized as follows. (1) Warm-up and stretching. (2) Development of visual, vestibular, kinaesthetic, and proprioceptive functions, through specific exercises, to optimize each function. (3) Workshops were organized around objects: as example, participants stepped over obstacles, walked on foam rubbers, on small bags of sand, on a rope, etc. The training emphasized motor skills ensuring postural stability, as well as accuracy and amplitude of movements. (4) Cooling down and stretching. The training was performed individually and in pairs. For a session, 10, 20, 20, and 10 min were respectively dedicated to the stages 1–4.

Postural recording

Participants underwent postural recordings before and after the training intervention. In a quiet normal illuminated room, they stood in an upright posture on the platform, barefoot, and their arms comfortably at their sides, with the instruction to breathe normally and keep relaxed. Participants underwent four conditions: in the eyes closed condition, they wore a mask in front of their eyes enabling darkness; in the eyes open conditions, they fixated a black circular surface at eye level subtending 1.5° of visual angle, at a distance of 600, 150, or 40 cm (Coubard, 2011). When necessary, participants wore their usual spectacle correction.

Postural measurements

Raw data provided by the manufacturer software were pre-processed using home-made scripts under matlab 7.0 (The MathWorks, USA). First and last of the 2048 samples were discarded as they exhibited artifacts due to respectively onset and offset of the recording, and only positions of the CoP in mediolateral (x) and anteroposterior (y) planes were kept for further analysis. An example of CoP displacements is illustrated in Figure 1A. We calculated statistic scores and performed dynamic analyses using home-made scripts under R (www.r-project.org).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

(A)Statokinesigram of center-of-pressure (CoP) displacements. Anteroposterior displacements of the CoP (Yp) are plotted against mediolateral displacements (Xp) in millimeters (mm). The convex hull area is shown in dotted line. (B) Stabilogram-diffusion plot. We show the mean square CoP displacement (<Δr2>) in mm2 as a function of time interval (Δt) in seconds. Experimental data is shown in full line, and theoretical fitting for the first and second regressions is shown in dotted line. The critical point between the two regressions is indicated by its coordinates (emphasized by semi-dotted lines) on the x axis (Δtrc) and on the y axis (<Δr2>c). (C) Recurrence quantification analysis plot. In this example, the phase space is shown for mediolateral fluctuations (CPx) for a radius of 3%. Thus, for a sphere of radius equal to 3% of the mean distance between data points of the phase space, we calculated for i = 1 − N and j = 1 − N (where N is the total number of data points) the distance between data points x(i) and x(j), and darkened every recurrent point for which the distance was below the radius. For i = j, the distance is zero resulting in the central diagonal line.

Statistic scores

We measured the length (in millimeters, mm), the confidence ellipse area (in mm2) that includes 90% of the positions of the CoP, and the mean and variance velocities (in mm s−1) of the CoP displacements. We also calculated the convex hull area (in mm2) including 100% of the positions of the CoP (Andrew, 1979; see Figure 1A), the Romberg quotient as the surface confidence ellipse eyes closed divided by the average one in the eyes open conditions, and the fractal dimension ratio (Chiari et al., 2000).

Diffusion analysis

We plotted the planar mean square displacement of the CoP <Δr2> in mm2 (where r is the sum of the displacements in the mediolateral and anteroposterior planes, and the brackets means the average over time) as a function of time interval Δt in seconds (see Figure 1B). We calculated the diffusion coefficients DS and DL (in mm2 s−1) for respectively the short- and long-term regions (before and after the critical point) from the slopes of linear-linear plots of <Δr2> vs. Δt curves, and calculated the corresponding scaling exponents HS and HL from the log–log plots of such curves. We measured the coordinates of the critical point separating short- and long-term regions, defined as the critical time interval Δtrc (in s) on the x axis and the critical mean square displacement <Δr2>c (in mm2) on the y axis (see Figure 1B).

Recurrence quantification analysis

We examined the local recurrence of data points in the reconstructed phase space. Following input parameters were chosen according to Webber and Zbilut’s (2005) recommendations: time lag was set to 50 ms corresponding to two samples; the embedding dimension was set to 12; the radius was restricted to the values 2–3% of the mean distance between data points since 1% or 4–5% provided respectively not enough and too many recurrent points; the number of successive points to define a diagonal line segment was set to 3. We measured the percentage of %REC and of %DET, and the MAXL. We calculated the three measures separately for the x and y planes of the CoP fluctuations, and for radii equal to 2 and 3%. An example of RQA plot is shown in Figure 1C.

Statistical analysis

All measures were submitted to analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with Group (two levels: CD vs. FP) as between-participant factor, Period (two levels: pre-test vs. post-test), and Eye (two levels: eyes closed, eyes open) as within-participant factors. The training duration was entered as a covariate in all statistical analyses since it was higher in the CD group compared to the FP group (5.06 ± 0.92 vs. 4.01 ± 0.85 months, respectively; t39 = 3.73, P < 0.001). Post hoc tests were calculated using Fisher’s least significant difference (LSD) method. Critical results were corroborated by calculating more conservative post hoc tests using Newman–Keuls (NK) method. For critical results, we also calculated effects sizes using Cohen’s measure defined as (mexp − mctrl)/[(σexp + σctrl)/2], where m and σ are respectively mean and SD for experimental (CD) and control (FP) groups. We used statistica 7.0 (StatSoft, USA) for all analyses. Distributional information was given by standard errors (SE).

Results

Statistic scores

Results are detailed in Table 2. Three-way ANOVAs with Group, Period, and Eye as factors showed neither main effect nor interaction for the length, the confidence ellipse, and convex hull areas, the mean and variance velocities, and the Romberg quotient. For fractal dimension, only the Group × Eye interaction was statistically significant (F1,38 = 6.02, P < 0.05), due to a lower mean value eyes open in the CD group (LSD, P < 0.01).

Table 2.

Mean ± SE of statistic scores for the groups of participants (CD, contemporary dance; FP, fall prevention).

Eyes closed
Eyes open
CD FP CD FP F P
LENGTH (mm)
Pre-test 703.7 ± 109.0 789.5 ± 86.6 496.6 ± 55.5 538.4 ± 56.0
Post-test 695.9 ± 74.4 709.8 ± 63.5 440.0 ± 29.6 501.7 ± 35.1 <1
ELLIPSE AREA (mm2)
Pre-test 345.3 ± 168.4 308.6 ± 67.9 308.6 ± 145.3 155.6 ± 38.9
Post-test 346.8 ± 136.7 262.8 ± 58.2 162.9 ± 21.7 160.9 ± 28.7 1.04 0.314
HULL AREA (mm2)
Pre-test 527.8 ± 260.7 497.1 ± 126.5 402.3 ± 179.6 231.3 ± 67.0
Post-test 445.6 ± 139.4 389.1 ± 94.7 199.6 ± 24.8 235.8 ± 43.7 1.61 0.212
MEAN VELOCITY (mm s−1)
Pre-test 13.8 ± 2.13 15.4 ± 1.69 9.70 ± 1.09 10.5 ± 1.09
Post-test 13.6 ± 1.45 13.9 ± 1.24 8.60 ± 0.58 9.80 ± 0.69 <1
VARIANCE VELOCITY (mm s−1)
Pre-test 138.5 ± 50.8 172.2 ± 56.9 61.6 ± 22.1 62.9 ± 17.7
Post-test 133.4 ± 39.9 100.9 ± 17.7 33.9 ± 4.38 49.3 ± 8.24 <1
FRACTAL DIMENSION
Pre-test 1.68 ± 0.04 1.70 ± 0.02 1.64 ± 0.03 1.72 ± 0.02
Post-test 1.69 ± 0.03 1.68 ± 0.02 1.64 ± 0.03 1.68 ± 0.02 <1
ROMBERG QUOTIENT EYES CLOSED/EYES OPEN
Pre-test 1.08 ± 0.12 2.03 ± 0.18
Post-test 2.12 ± 0.79 1.70 ± 0.18 1.80 0.187

ANOVAs’ F and P values are those of the third-order interaction (except for the Romberg quotient for which F and P values are those of the second-order interaction).

Diffusion analysis

Results are provided in Table 3. Three-way ANOVAs with Group, Period, and Eye as factors showed neither main effect nor interaction for the diffusion coefficient DS, the scaling exponents HS and HL, and the critical mean square displacement <Δr2>c. For the diffusion coefficient DL, we found a main effect of Period (1.48 vs. 1.78 mm2 s−1 in pre- and post-test periods, respectively; F1,38 = 4.57, P < 0.05).

Table 3.

Mean ± SE of diffusion analysis for the groups of participants (CD, contemporary dance; FP, fall prevention).

Eyes closed
Eyes open
CD FP CD FP F P
DS (mm2 s−1)
Pre-test 44.7 ± 23.2 46.6 ± 14.3 22.6 ± 11.3 18.5 ± 6.80
Post-test 30.8 ± 8.08 32.9 ± 8.43 11.1 ± 1.37 14.6 ± 3.02 <1
DL (mm2 s−1)
Pre-test 1.48 ± 0.80 1.30 ± 0.30 1.90 ± 0.69 1.24 ± 0.25
Post-test 2.09 ± 0.91 1.85 ± 0.59 1.69 ± 0.24 1.49 ± 0.25 <1
HS
Pre-test 0.688 ± 0.02 0.731 ± 0.018 0.648 ± 0.019 0.657 ± 0.015
Post-test 0.689 ± 0.019 0.714 ± 0.016 0.652 ± 0.019 0.665 ± 0.012 <1
HL
Pre-test 0.034 ± 0.011 0.039 ± 0.008 0.081 ± 0.013 0.085 ± 0.009
Post-test 0.044 ± 0.011 0.053 ± 0.010 0.095 ± 0.009 0.104 ± 0.007 <1
r2>c (mm2)
Pre-test 108.7 ± 53.0 80.2 ± 19.3 74.7 ± 42.1 32.9 ± 9.66
Post test 88.4 ± 35.9 61.3 ± 12.4 32.4 ± 4.92 27.0 ± 5.17 <1
Δtrc (s)
Pre-test 1.41 ± 0.16 1.19 ± 0.13 1.53 ± 0.15 1.16 ± 0.11
Post test 1.32 ± 0.16 1.19 ± 0.12 1.65 ± 0.22* 1.02 ± 0.06 4.21 0.047

ANOVAs’ F and P values are those of the third-order interaction. Asterisk only indicates statistical significant difference (LSD, P < 05) between pre- and post-test periods.

For the critical time interval Δtrc, we observed a main effect of Group (1.48 vs. 1.14 s in the CD and FP groups, respectively; F1,38 = 6.84, P < 0.05). Critical for our hypothesis, the Group × Period × Eye interaction was significant (F1,38 = 4.21, P < 0.05), with eyes open an increase between the pre- and post-test periods in the CD group (1.53 vs. 1.65 s; LSD, P = 0.030; NK, P = 0.045) vs. a tendency for a decrease in the FP group (1.16 vs. 1.02 s; LSD, P = 0.059). With respect to effect sizes, Cohen’s d values in the eyes open condition were 2.93 and 4.75 in pre- and post-test periods, respectively (see Figure 2A).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

(A) Mean critical time interval Δtrc in seconds from the diffusion analysis. (B) Mean percentage of recurrence (%REC) and (C) mean maximal diagonal line (MAXL) for mediolateral fluctuations (CPx) for a radius of 2% from the recurrence quantification analysis. Results are shown as a function of pre-test (Pre) and post-test (Post) periods, and of eyes closed (left panels) and eyes open (right panels) conditions, for the two training programs: contemporary dance (CD) in full lines, and fall prevention (FP) in dotted lines. Vertical bars are SE. Asterisks only indicate statistical significant difference (LSD, P < 0.05) between pre- and post-test periods within a group.

Recurrence quantification analysis

Table 4 shows detailed results. In the mediolateral plane, three-way ANOVAs with Group, Period, and Eye as factors showed a main effect of Group for %REC and a radius of 2% (F1,38 = 9.90, P < 0.01), for %REC-3% (F1,38 = 9.66, P < 0.01), for MAXL-2% (F1,38 = 11.8, P < 0.01), and for MAXL-3% (F1,38 = 6.95, P < 0.05) with higher mean values in the CD group, and a main effect of Period for %REC-2% (F1,38 = 5.41, P < 0.05) with a higher value in the pre-test period. We observed a Group × Eye interaction for all measures except MAXL-2% due to systematic higher mean values eyes open in the CD group (LSD, P < 0.05), and a Period × Eye interaction for %REC-2% (F1,38 = 5.65, P < 0.05), and %REC-3% (F1,38 = 4.90, P < 0.05) with only a tendency for higher mean values eyes open in the post-test period (LSD, P > 0.05).

Table 4.

Mean ± SE of recurrence quantification analysis for the groups of participants (CD, contemporary dance; FP, fall prevention).

Eyes closed
Eyes open
CD FP CD FP F P
%REC x2
Pre-test 0.392 ± 0.116 0.418 ± 0.070 1.08 ± 0.227 0.269 ± 0.040
Post-test 0.658 ± 0.183 0.253 ± 0.063 0.589 ± 0.122* 0.425 ± 0.089 20.8 0.000
%REC x 3
Pre-test 1.16 ± 0.257 1.39 ± 0.187 2.71 ± 0.463 0.955 ± 0.104
Post-test 1.80 ± 0.415 0.880 ± 0.152 1.62 ± 0.253* 1.34 ± 0.201 19.6 0.000
%DET x 2
Pre-test 69.0 ± 3.25 69.8 ± 2.64 78.1 ± 2.18 65.5 ± 2.49
Post-test 74.1 ± 2.58* 68.6 ± 1.65 74.1 ± 2.35 70.2 ± 2.14* 16.7 0.000
%DET x3
Pre-test 75.8 ± 2.59 77.4 ± 1.57 83.5 ± 1.76 74.5 ± 1.52
Post-test 79.5 ± 2.20 75.2 ± 1.28 80.4 ± 1.69 77.5 ± 1.57 14.3 0.000
MAXL x 2
Pre-test 16.8 ± 6.19 12.9 ± 2.45 34.4 ± 5.39 9.88 ± 1.77
Post-test 19.6 ± 4.64 9.45 ± 2.05 23.1 ± 4.67* 15.0 ± 2.89 9.58 0.004
MAXL x 3
Pre-test 33.4 ± 7.46 41.1 ± 5.59 66.0 ± 6.96 32.8 ± 4.00
Post-test 40.9 ± 7.08 26.3 ± 4.02* 55.3 ± 8.01 39.8 ± 4.61 9.28 0.004
%REC y 2
Pre-test 0.420 ± 0.120 0.332 ± 0.069 0.690 ± 0.218 0.308 ± 0.054
Post-test 0.353 ± 0.080 0.389 ± 0.076 0.538 ± 0.124 0.389 ± 0.056 1.08 0.304
%REC y 3
Pre-test 1.22 ± 0.268 1.01 ± 0.170 1.72 ± 0.483 0.941 ± 0.128
Post-test 1.02 ± 0.181 1.15 ± 0.177 1.43 ± 0.284 1.16 ± 0.130 <1
%DET y 2
Pre-test 73.9 ± 1.65 72.8 ± 1.12 73.2 ± 2.64 72.1 ± 1.12
Post-test 70.1 ± 3.34 73.6 ± 1.47 75.1 ± 2.06 72.8 ± 1.38 2.27 0.140
%DET y 3
Pre-test 76.7 ± 2.13 76.6 ± 1.12 77.4 ± 2.41 75.5 ± 1.13
Post-test 76.2 ± 2.13 77.3 ± 1.27 78.9 ± 1.89 77.3 ± 1.02 <1
MAXL y 2
Pre-test 15.7 ± 3.70 12.5 ± 2.39 23.7 ± 5.90 12.0 ± 1.85
Post-test 14.5 ± 3.75 18.2 ± 3.31 22.0 ± 4.98 15.6 ± 2.32 <1
MAXL y 3
Pre-test 32.8 ± 6.43 28.3 ± 4.27 45.1 ± 8.45 33.7 ± 4.35
Post-test 29.6 ± 4.49 35.5 ± 4.33 49.5 ± 7.01 36.1 ± 4.10 2.20 0.146

ANOVAs’ F and P values are those of the third-order interaction. Asterisks only indicate statistical significant difference (LSD, P < 05) between pre- and post-test periods.

With regard to our hypothesis, we found a Group × Period interaction for %REC-2% (F1,38 = 4.34, P < 0.05), which was due to a tendency for a decrease between pre- and post-test periods in the CD group (respectively 0.737 and 0.623; LSD, P > 0.05) whereas no change occurred in the FP group (0.343 and 0.339; LSD, P > 0.05). The Group × Period × Eye interaction was significant for all measures (P < 0.05), with a systematic pattern that is illustrated in Figures 2B,C. In the eyes open condition, the mean value decreased in the CD group for all measures (%REC-2%, LSD, P = 0.002, NK, P = 0.002; %REC-3%, LSD, P = 0.005, NK, P = 0.006; MAXL-2%, LSD, P = 0.012, NK, P = 0.006), whereas it increased in the FP group for all measures (LSD, P < 0.05 only for %DET-2%; see Figures 2B,C). To illustrate effect sizes, Cohen’s d values for %REC-2% in the eyes open condition were 6.57 and 1.54 in pre- and post-test periods, respectively (see Figure 2B). For MAXL-2%, Cohen’s d values eyes open were 7.27 and 2.17 in pre- and post-test periods (see Figure 2C). Eyes closed, the pattern was the reverse: an increase between the two periods in the CD group (LSD, P < 0.05 only for %DET-2%) vs. a decrease in the FP group (LSD, P < 0.05 only for MAXL-3%).

In the anteroposterior plane, three-way ANOVAs with Group, Period, and Eye as factors showed a main effect of Eye for %REC-2% (F1,38 = 4.19, P < 0.05) and %REC-3% (F1,38 = 4.16, P < 0.05) with higher mean values eyes open. As in the mediolateral plane, we observed a Group × Eye interaction for all measures as a result of higher mean values eyes open in the CD group (LSD, P < 0.05 except for %REC-3% and %DET-3%).

With respect to our expectancy, a Group × Period interaction was found for %REC-2% (F1,38 = 4.10, P < 0.05), as a result of a tendency for a decrease vs. an increase in mean values between the pre- and post-test periods in the CD vs. the FP groups, respectively (the difference between the two periods failed to reach significance for the two groups). The Group × Period interaction was also significant for MAXL-2% (F1,38 = 5.50, P < 0.05), for which the decrease in the CD group was insignificant contrary to the increase in the FP group (LSD, P < 0.05). Finally, there was no third-order interaction for any measure.

Discussion

The main findings of this study were that the practice of CD in older adults enhanced the critical time interval in SDA, and reduced recurrence and mathematical stability in RQA, as compared to practice of FP which tended to induce reverse patterns. One limitation of this study was that the two groups did not perform equally in the pre-test period. The initial higher level in gymnastics in the FP group may have participated in between-group disparity and further research is needed to corroborate our observations.

In aging, previous cross-sectional studies have suggested that social dance may benefit balance, postural stability, and walking pattern (Verghese, 2006; Zhang et al., 2008), and intervention studies have evidenced how aerobic (Shigematsu et al., 2002; Hui et al., 2009), Laban-based (Hamburg and Clair, 2003), or jazz (Alpert et al., 2009) dance improve balance using clinical (Shigematsu et al., 2002; Hamburg and Clair, 2003; Hui et al., 2009) or laboratory (Alpert et al., 2009) measures. Our study is the first one to examine the effects of CD on motor control using posturography and dynamic analyses of static posture before/after the intervention, which enabled us to provide further insight into the underlying mechanisms by which dance influences postural control (Judge, 2003).

For Collins and De Luca (1993), human being in quiet standing is viewed not as an inverted pendulum but a pinned-polymer whose CoP displacements result from a blend of stochastic and deterministic processes. Over short-term interval, the postural control system would utilize open-loop mechanisms dominated by randomness, before closed-loop mechanisms would be called into play with a prevalence of deterministic control. Here we showed that CD delayed the point at which the postural system switches from open- to closed-loop control, suggesting that CD enlarged the initial temporal window for motor stochastic processes. A previous study reported age-related increased critical mean square displacement and critical time interval resulting in a short-term interval higher slope, which the authors interpreted as enhanced postural stiffness with age (Collins et al., 1995). In our CD group, post-test increased critical time interval without any change in critical mean square displacement resulted in lower short-term interval slope, which suggests reduced postural stiffness after CD training.

Further insight was provided by RQA approach (Riley et al., 1999) allowing us to quantify the degree of recurrence, determinism, and mathematical stability of CoP displacements. CD reduced both recurrence and mathematical stability suggesting that CoP displacements were less likely to repeat themselves over time and that their dynamics were more flexible, whereas FP yielded the opposite tendency. Such a pattern was almost visible for mediolateral displacements of the CoP, which we explain from a biomechanical viewpoint by the fact that the base of support for upright stance is wider in the mediolateral plane than in the anteroposterior one.

Taken together, we propose that CD promoted stochastic postural control of older adults, by providing more time to random postural processes, decreasing repeatability and increasing flexibility of postural oscillations. In both SDA and RQA, the effects were observed eyes open, which may be due to the fact that motor activities were mostly practiced eyes open. Since we measured static posture, we suggest that CD influenced the flexibility of the central postural system per se, resulting in higher complexity in the mathematical sense, i.e., higher adaptability in the physiological sense. Such effect may have been caused by improvisation which favored creativity and constant adaptation to constraints in space, time, interaction with other dancers, whereas FP based on more stereotyped behaviors tended to produce opposite effects.

Suggesting that CD improves postural flexibility in older adults, the present study completes a previous report (Coubard et al., 2011) showing that CD improves cognitive flexibility in aging. How motor and cognitive dimensions interact to yield parallel effects needs to be further investigated by measuring motor and attentional control in the same participants undergoing such training programs. In the meanwhile, we suggest a cortical-subcortical loop hypothesis to account for the improvement of postural flexibility. Motor control involves extensive areas of the central nervous system from the spinal cord to the cerebral cortex: globus pallidus, putamen, caudate nucleus, thalamus, substantia nigra, subthalamic nucleus, cerebellum, reticular formation, vestibular nuclei. At a higher level, the supplementary motor cortex, the frontal eye fields, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex play a supramotor role in, respectively, preparing (Jenkins et al., 2000), monitoring (Schall, 2004), and controlling (Rowe et al., 2000) the movement to be produced by the primary motor cortex.

It is likely that this motor network together with cortical–subcortical loops linking the cerebral cortex to basal ganglia (Alexander et al., 1986) may be involved in motor activities such as CD and FP. However, CD may require higher attentional demand than FP due to the practice of improvisation. In such a way, the prefrontal–subcortical interaction may have been recruited with higher frequency and intensity, resulting in enhanced motor flexibility. Taken together with the report by Coubard et al. (2011), this study suggests that CD induces changes at both postural and attentional levels, which may share common characteristics.

To conclude, the results of this study suggested that CD practice favors flexible postural control in older adults. Taken together with good acceptance, adherence, and moderate intensity associated with this practice, we recommend CD to develop motor plasticity not only in normal aging but also in pathological conditions with motor stiffness (Schmit et al., 2006).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Results were presented at the 6th International Conference on the Arts in Society (Berlin, Germany, 2011). The authors thank ADAL (Paris) for allowing the assessment of some participants.

References

  1. Alexander G. E., DeLong M. R., Strick P. L. (1986). Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 357–381 10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alpert P. T., Miller S. K., Wallmann H., Havey R., Cross C., Chevalia T., Gillis C. B., Kodandapari K. (2009). The effect of modified jazz dance on balance, cognition, and mood in older adults. J. Am. Acad. Nurse Pract. 21, 108–115 10.1111/j.1745-7599.2008.00392.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Geriatrics Society, British Geriatrics Society, and American Academy of Orthopaedic Surgeons Panel on Falls Prevention (2001). Guideline for the prevention of falls in older persons. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 49, 664–672 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.49115.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Andrew A. M. (1979). Another efficient algorithm for convex hulls in two dimensions. Inf. Process. Lett. 9, 216–219 10.1016/0020-0190(79)90072-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Benedetti M. G., Berti L., Presti C., Frizziero A., Giannini S. (2008). Effects of an adapted physical activity program in a group of elderly subjects with flexed posture: clinical and instrumental assessment. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 5, 32. 10.1186/1743-0003-5-32 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Berg K., Wood-Dauphinee S., Williams J. I. (1995). The balance scale: reliability assessment with elderly residents and patients with an acute stroke. Scand. J. Rehabil. Med. 27, 27–36 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bherer L., Belleville S., Hudon C. (2004). Le déclin des fonctions exécutives au cours du vieillissement normal, dans la maladie d’Alzheimer et dans la démence frontotemporale. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 2, 181–189 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chiari L., Cappello A., Lenzi D., Della Croce U. (2000). An improved technique for the extraction of stochastic parameters from stabilograms. Gait Posture 12, 225–234 10.1016/S0966-6362(00)00086-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Collins J. J., De Luca C. J. (1993). Open-loop and closed-loop control of posture: a random-walk analysis of center-of-pressure trajectories. Exp. Brain Res. 95, 308–318 10.1007/BF00229788 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Collins J. J., De Luca C. J. (1995). The effects of visual input on open-loop and closed-loop postural control mechanisms. Exp. Brain Res. 103, 151–163 10.1007/BF00241972 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Collins J. J., De Luca C. J., Burrows A., Lipsitz L. A. (1995). Age-related changes in open-loop and closed-loop postural control mechanisms. Exp. Brain Res. 104, 480–492 10.1007/BF00231982 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Coubard O. A. (2011). Les neurones des mouvements des yeux. Saarbrücken: Editions Universitaires Européennes [Google Scholar]
  13. Coubard O. A., Duretz S., Lefebvre V., Lapalus P., Ferrufino L. (2011). Practice of contemporary dance improves cognitive flexibility in aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 3:13. 10.3389/fnagi.2011.00013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Day L., Fildes B., Gordon I., Fitzharris M., Flamer H., Lord S. (2002). Randomised factorial trial of falls prevention among older people living in their own homes. BMJ 325, 128. 10.1136/bmj.325.7356.128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. de Bruin E. D., Schmidt A. (2010). Walking behaviour of healthy elderly: attention should be paid. Behav. Brain Funct. 6, 59. 10.1186/1744-9081-6-59 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Famula A., Nowotny-Czupryna O., Brzek A., Nowotny J., Kita B. (2008). Telereceptive and proprioceptive control of balance vs. body stability in elderly people. Ortop. Traumatol. Rehabil. 10, 384–395 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Folstein M. F., Folstein S. E., McHugh P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state.” A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hamburg J., Clair A. A. (2003). The effects of a laban-based movement program with music on measures of balance and gait in older adults. Activ. Adapt. Aging 28, 17–33 10.1300/J016v28n01_02 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Horak F. B. (2006). Postural orientation and equilibrium: what do we need to know about neural control of balance to prevent falls? Age Ageing 35, ii7–ii11 10.1093/ageing/afl077 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hui E., Chui B. T., Woo J. (2009). Effects of dance on physical and psychological well-being in older persons. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 49, e45–e50 10.1016/j.archger.2008.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jenkins I. H., Jahanshahi M., Jueptner M., Passingham R. E., Brooks D. J. (2000). Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. II. The effect of movement predictability on regional cerebral blood flow. Brain 123, 1216–1228 10.1093/brain/123.6.1216 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Judge J. O. (2003). Balance training to maintain mobility and prevent disability. Am. J. Prev. Med. 25, 150–156 10.1016/S0749-3797(03)00178-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. King M. B., Whipple R. H., Gruman C. A., Judge J. O., Schmidt J. A., Wolfson L. I. (2002). The performance enhancement project: improving physical performance in older persons. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 83, 1060–1069 10.1053/apmr.2002.33653 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kristinsdottir E. K., Fransson P. A., Magnusson M. (2001). Changes in postural control in healthy elderly subjects are related to vibration sensation, vision and vestibular asymmetry. Acta Otolaryngol. 121, 700–706 10.1080/000164801300366624 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lord S. R. (2006). Visual risk factors for falls in older people. Age Ageing 35, ii42–ii45 10.1093/ageing/afl088 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Nashner L. M. (1976). Adapting reflexes controlling the human posture. Exp. Brain Res. 26, 59–72 10.1007/BF00235249 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Rajah M. N., D’Esposito M. (2005). Region-specific changes in prefrontal function with age: a review of PET and fMRI studies on working and episodic memory. Brain 128, 1964–1983 10.1093/brain/awh608 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Raz N., Rodrigue K. M. (2006). Differential aging of the brain: patterns, cognitive correlates and modifiers. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 730–748 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.07.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Riley M. A., Balasubramaniam R., Turvey M. T. (1999). Recurrence quantification analysis of postural fluctuations. Gait Posture 9, 65–78 10.1016/S0966-6362(98)00044-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Robertson M. C., Gardner M. M., Devlin N., McGee R., Campbell A. J. (2001). Effectiveness and economic evaluation of a nurse delivered home exercise programme to prevent falls. 2: controlled trial in multiple centres. BMJ 322, 701–704 10.1136/bmj.322.7299.1430 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Rossiter-Fornoff J. E., Wolf S. L., Wolfson L. I., Buchner D. M. (1995). A cross-sectional validation study of the FICSIT common data base static balance measures. Frailty and injuries: cooperative studies of intervention techniques. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 50, M291–M297 10.1093/gerona/50A.6.M291 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Rowe J. B., Toni I., Josephs O., Frackowiak R. S., Passingham R. E. (2000). The prefrontal cortex: response selection or maintenance within working memory? Science 288, 1656–1660 10.1126/science.288.5471.1656 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Rubenstein L. Z. (2006). Falls in older people: epidemiology, risk factors and strategies for prevention. Age Ageing 35, ii37–ii41 10.1093/ageing/afl084 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Schall J. D. (2004). On the role of frontal eye field in guiding attention and saccades. Vision Res. 44, 1453–1467 10.1016/j.visres.2003.10.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Schmit J. M., Regis D. I., Riley M. A. (2005). Dynamic patterns of postural sway in ballet dancers and track athletes. Exp. Brain Res. 163, 370–378 10.1007/s00221-004-2185-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Schmit J. M., Riley M. A., Dalvi A., Sahay A., Shear P. K., Shockley K. D., Pun R. Y. (2006). Deterministic center of pressure patterns characterize postural instability in Parkinson’s disease. Exp. Brain Res. 168, 357–367 10.1007/s00221-005-0094-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Schultz A. B. (1995). Muscle function and mobility biomechanics in the elderly: an overview of some recent research. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 50, 60–63 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Shigematsu R., Chang M., Yabushita N., Sakai T., Nakagaichi M., Nho H., Tanaka K. (2002). Dance-based aerobic exercise may improve indices of falling risk in older women. Age Ageing 31, 261–266 10.1093/ageing/31.4.261 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Theill N., Martin M., Schumacher V., Bridenbaugh S. A., Kressig R. W. (2011). Simultaneously measuring gait and cognitive performance in cognitively healthy and cognitively impaired older adults: the Basel motor-cognition dual-task paradigm. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 59, 1012–1018 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03429.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Verghese J. (2006). Cognitive and mobility profile of older social dancers. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 54, 1241–1244 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2006.00808.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Webber C. L., Zbilut J. P. (2005). “Reccurrence quantification analysis of nonlinear dynamical systems,” in Tutorials in Contemporary Nonlinear Methods for the Behavioral Sciences, eds Riley M. A., Van Orden G. C. 26–94 Available at: http://www.nsf.gov/sbe/bcs/pac/nmbs/nmbs.jsp
  42. Zhang J. G., Ishikawa-Takata K., Yamazaki H., Morita T., Ohta T. (2008). Postural stability and physical performance in social dancers. Gait Posture 27, 697–701 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2007.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Human Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES