Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Jan 3.
Published in final edited form as: Psychosom Med. 2012 Jan 3;74(1):33–38. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182405ac4

Are Somatic Symptoms of Depression Better Predictors of Cardiac Events than Cognitive Symptoms in Coronary Heart Disease?

Robert M Carney 1, Kenneth E Freedland 1
PMCID: PMC3254789  NIHMSID: NIHMS342371  PMID: 22219384

Abstract

Several recent studies have found that somatic symptoms of depression predict cardiac events in patients with established CHD, but cognitive symptoms of depression do not. However, other studies have not supported this finding, and the research in this area is complicated by methodological differences and inconsistencies in the classification of “cognitive” and “somatic” symptoms. In addition, somatic symptoms are more common than cognitive symptoms in cardiac patients, and are often associated with more severe depression. These factors may confound the relationship between somatic symptoms and cardiac outcomes. Some reasons why somatic symptoms may be more common than cognitive symptoms in cardiac patients are considered, as well as whether somatic symptoms are likely to be symptoms of depression or of medical illness. Finally, some directions for future research are proposed.

Keywords: Behavioral symptoms, depression, depressive disorder, myocardial ischemia, myocardial revascularization, prognosis


Several recent studies have reported that somatic symptoms of depression predict cardiac events in patients with coronary heart disease (CHD) or heart failure, but cognitive symptoms do not (18). For example, de Jonge and his colleagues (1) found that somatic/affective but not cognitive/affective depression symptoms predicted cardiac events following acute myocardial infarction, even after adjusting for left ventricular ejection fraction and other major prognostic medical variables. Similar findings have been reported in women undergoing angiography for suspected myocardial ischemia (4), in patients with documented, medically stable CHD (3), and in patients with chronic heart failure (7). Even in studies of individuals initially free of clinically significant CHD, somatic but not cognitive symptoms of depression predict subclinical atherosclerotic progression (9).

Does this mean, as some of these reports suggest, that “depression” may not be the same disorder in cardiac patients as it is in psychiatric patients? That when “depression” questionnaires are administered to cardiac patients, their responses are driven not only by depression but also by the symptoms of heart disease? Or that depressed cardiac patients need treatments that selectively target the somatic symptoms of depression? We acknowledge that studies of specific symptoms in relation to cardiac outcomes may help to clarify the relationship between depression and cardiac mortality. However, this body of research requires careful scrutiny before meaningful conclusions can be drawn from it.

Although it is possible that somatic symptoms of depression are more predictive of cardiac events than are cognitive symptoms, it is important to be aware that some studies have not found this to be the case (1017). In one of the earliest studies comparing somatic and cognitive symptoms of depression, Barefoot et al. (10) found that only negative affect independently predicted survival in patients with angiographically diagnosed coronary artery disease when included in a model with somatic and other depression symptoms. On the other hand, some studies have found that both cognitive and somatic symptoms predict cardiac outcomes (12, 13), whereas others have found that cognitive but not somatic symptoms are predictive (11, 16, 17). Thus, some findings do not support the dominance of somatic over cognitive and affective symptoms as predictors of cardiac outcomes. Table 1 presents a representative sample of the studies that have compared the predictive values of various depression symptom subsets in post-ACSI, revascularization, and stable CHD patient populations.

Table 1.

Studies of Somatic vs. Cognitive/Affective Depression Symptoms and Cardiac Events

Source Sample Size Depression Measure Symptom Classification Method Follow-up Duration Outcome Results
Symptom Subset Risk Statistic p
Acute Coronary Syndrome
Martens et al., 2010 437 BDI-1 Factor analysis 2.8 years Cardiac death or recurrent MI Somatic 1.31 0.04
Cognitive/Affective Not reported NS
Roest et al., 2010 913 BDI-1 Principal components analysis 1 year All-cause mortality Somatic 1.92 0.001
Cognitive/Affective 1.07 0.73
Smolderen et al., 2009 2,347 PHQ-9 Item face validity 4 years Hospitalizations Somatic 1.16 0.01
Cognitive/Affective Not reported NS
All-cause mortality Somatic 1.07 0.30
de Jonge et al., 2006 494 BDI-1 Factor analysis 2.5 years Readmission Somatic 1.30 <0.10
Cognitive/Affective 1.05 <0.74
Appetitive 1.09 <0.43
Cardiac mortality Somatic 3.91 <0.001
Cognitive 0.40 <0.04
Appetitive 0.94 <0.83
Frasure-Smith & Lespérance, 2003 896 BDI-1 Principal components analysis 5 years Cardiac mortality Somatic 1.51 <0.001
Cognitive 1.34 0.002
Irvine et al., 1999 671 BDI-1 Item face validity 2 years Sudden cardiac death Somatic1 1.00†† 0.95
Cognitive1 1.09†† 0.06
Lespérance et al., 1996 222 DIS DSM-III 1.5 years Cardiac mortality MDD2 3.96 0.008
MDD3 4.76 0.003

Revascularization
Tully et al., 2011 226 BDI-II Factor analysis 4.9 years Cardiovascular events Somatic 1.18 0.38
Cognitive 1.36 0.04
Affective 0.79 0.09
Connerey et al., 2007 309 BDI-1 Item face validity 9.3 years Cardiac mortality Somatic 1.07 0.12
Cognitive 1.10 0.007
Pedersen et al., 2007 534 Maastricht Questionnaire (MQ) Principal components/item analysis 2 years All-cause mortality and nonfatal MI Fatigue 1.35 0.42
Depression4 2.69 0.007
Hopelessness 1.9 0.001

Known or Suspected Stable Coronary Heart Disease
Hoen et al., 2010 1,019 PHQ-9 Item face validity 6.1 years Cardiovascular morbidity/mortality Somatic 1.14 0.002
Cognitive 1.08 0.09
Linke et al., 2009 550 BDI-I Principal components analysis 5.0 years Cardiovascular morbidity/mortality Somatic 1.39 0.01
Cognitive 0.81 0.09
Barefoot et al., 2000 1,250 Zung Depression Scale Factor analysis 15.2 years Cardiac deaths Somatic 1.31†† 0.003
Well Being 1.26†† 0.001
Negative Affective5 1.42†† 0.001
Hopelessness 1.50†† 0.015

Adjusted Odds Ratio (OR)

Adjusted Hazard Ratio (HR)

††

Adjusted Relative Risk (RR)

1

Placebo group only

2

MDD with sleep/appetite disturbance

3

MDD without sleep/appetite disturbance

4

6 of 7 symptoms are cognitive

5

Only negative affect remained in the model with all symptom groups

Inconsistencies in Methodology

Evaluation of this research is complicated by numerous inconsistencies among studies as to whether particular symptoms are classified as “cognitive” or “somatic”. First, two different approaches have been used to classify symptoms. Some investigators have depended on the “face validity” of the items. Others have utilized factor analysis or principal component analysis of depression questionnaires to produce factor or component scores. These statistical approaches complicate this area of research in a number of ways. First, different analyses of the same questionnaire can, and often do, produce different factor structures, because of differences in the samples, analytic procedures, and/or decision rules. For example, one investigator might choose to include an item on a “somatic” factor only if it loads highly on that factor but not on any other factor, but another investigator might count the same item on multiple factors if it loads highly on multiple factors. The latter decision rule makes it possible for a single item to be counted both as a “somatic” symptom and as a “cognitive” symptom within the same study.

Second, the items that are called “somatic” or “somatic/affective” based on factor analyses often differ from the items that are classified as somatic in studies that are based on face validity. In the de Jonge et al. (1), and Martens et al. (5) studies, for example, “dissatisfaction” (item 4) on the BDI loaded higher on the “somatic/affective” factor (0.69 and 0.72 respectively) than on the “cognitive” factor (.49 and .38), yet dissatisfaction is generally considered to be a cognitive item based on its face validity. Similarly, “indecisiveness” was the item with the second highest loading on the “somatic/affective” factor in the de Jonge et al. study, yet it would ordinarily be classified as a cognitive symptom. Interestingly, none of the “face valid” somatic symptoms (BDI items 15–21) except for “work difficulty” loaded as highly on the somatic/affective factor in either study as did “dissatisfaction”. On the other hand, none of the traditional somatic symptoms loaded on the “cognitive” factor, and the cognitive symptoms of guilt, punishment, self-dislike, and self-accusation loaded only on the cognitive factor. Nevertheless, these factors clearly represent something other than either purely cognitive or purely somatic depressive symptoms.

Third, both the somatic and the cognitive/affective items on well-validated depression questionnaires are indicators of a single, underlying depression factor. For example, by applying an advanced technique known as hierarchical factor analysis to data obtained from a sample of primary care patients, Arnau et al. found that the first-order “cognitive” and “somatic” factors on the BDI-II reflected a single, underlying, second-order “depression” factor (18). Thus, both the cognitive and somatic items on the BDI-II are indicators of depression, even in medical patients, and “cognitive depression” and “somatic depression” are forms of “generic depression”, not two distinct constructs. Furthermore, when these two highly correlated factors are pitted against each other in the same statistical model, multicollinearity may be present, and this may be responsible for some of the contradictory findings in this area (19).

Frequency and Severity of Cognitive and Somatic Depression Symptoms

If somatic symptoms turn out to better predictors of cardiac events than cognitive symptoms, a possible reason is that somatic symptoms are more common in relatively severe depression than they are in milder cases (20). Thus, the dominance of somatic symptoms in some predictive models may simply be an artifact of a more fundamental relationship between the severity of depression and the risk of cardiac events (21). For example, Smolderen et al. (8) used the upper quartiles of scores on cognitive and somatic subscales of the Patient Health Questionnnaire (PHQ-9) (22) to define “cognitive” and “somatic” patient subgroups. This method ensured that the cognitive and somatic subgroups were approximately the same size. However, the somatic group scored an average of 2 points higher on the PHQ-9 total score than did the cognitive group.

In addition to reflecting more severe depression, somatic symptoms may be more prevalent than cognitive/affective symptoms in patients with CHD (23). In the Hoen et al. study (3), for example, cognitive/affective symptoms predicted cardiovascular events in age-adjusted analyses (p=0.006), but the effect was no longer significant after adjustment for additional covariates (p=0.09). Somatic symptoms remained significant even after covariate adjustment (p=0.002). However, fewer patients reported cognitive than somatic symptoms. Also, the confidence intervals around the hazard ratios overlapped considerably. Depressed mood, for example, was reported by 114 participants and was associated with a 32% increased risk of CV events that was not statistically significant. Fatigue was associated with an almost identical risk (34%), but 267 patients reported this symptom, yielding a narrower confidence interval and a significant effect. In women with suspected myocardial ischemia, Linke et al. (4) found that somatic symptoms predicted mortality whereas cognitive symptoms did not. However, somatic symptoms accounted for about three-fourths of the BDI total scores in a three factor solution, and two thirds of the BDI total scores in a two factor solution. Unfortunately, few reports have provided enough information to determine whether differential symptom frequency or severity may have contributed to the findings. These data should be reported in future studies and evaluated as potential explanations for the findings.

Reporting Bias or Actual Frequency?

Why do depressed CHD patients tend to report more somatic than cognitive symptoms? One possibility is that many depressed patients believe that cognitive and affective symptoms are less socially acceptable, more stigmatizing, or riskier to report than somatic symptoms (24). Men with heart disease often report fewer symptoms of depression than are observed by a spouse or friend (25, 26), and depressed cardiac patients more readily admit to irritability than to depressed mood (27). It is probably easier for many medical patients to admit to sleep or appetite disturbances than to thoughts of hopelessness or suicide, even when these cognitive symptoms are present. This sort of reporting bias would result in lower total scores from patients who selectively report somatic symptoms than in those who are willing to report a broader range of symptoms. Consequently, it may seem as though the former are less severely depressed than some of them actually are. This problem could complicate efforts to investigate whether differences in the overall severity of depression explain the differential predictive value of somatic symptoms.

Bias against reporting cognitive or affective symptoms may be especially problematic in studies of depressed cardiac patients who are not seeking treatment for depression. Martens and colleagues (23) found that cognitive symptoms are more frequently reported in depressed psychiatric patients than in depressed cardiac patients. The psychiatric patients in their study were being treated for depression at a psychiatric clinic. The cardiac patients, in contrast, were participants in an observational study of post-MI depression, not treatment-seeking depressed patients. Patients seeking treatment for depression are probably more likely to acknowledge that they are depressed, and they may be more willing to endorse statements such as “I am worthless,” or “I don’t deserved to be loved”, two items on the Cognition Checklist that were used to measure cognitive symptoms in the Martens et al. study (23).

Another explanation for why depressed cardiac patients may report fewer cognitive symptoms is that they may actually have fewer cognitive symptoms. Cognitive symptoms are often present in individuals who are “depressed about being depressed” (28). That is, depressed mood and other symptoms of depression may be viewed by depressed patients as evidence of their personal inadequacy. This may initiate a vicious cycle of guilt, hopelessness, self-criticism, and even suicidal ideation in some cases. In contrast, many cardiac patients may attribute their depressive symptoms to their heart disease or believe depression to be a “natural” and understandable reaction to a life-threatening cardiac event (29, 30). Consequently, these patients may be less vulnerable to becoming depressed about their depression, and this could minimize the cognitive and affective symptoms while making the somatic symptoms more salient.

One implication of the observation that patients are more likely to experience or report somatic than cognitive symptoms of depression is that some depression screening tools, including ones recommended by the American Heart Association (31) and a National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute advisory panel (32), may not be optimal for identifying these patients. For example, the PHQ-2 asks only about dysphoric mood and anhedonia, and therefore may fail to identify cardiac patients with predominantly somatic symptoms of depression. The full PHQ-9 or other screening tools that sample a wider range of symptoms should be considered when screening for depression in cardiac patients.

Symptoms of Depression, Heart Disease, or Something Else?

It has also been suggested that somatic symptoms predict cardiac outcomes because the BDI and similar depression questionnaires assess symptoms of heart disease in addition to depression. The primary somatic symptoms of depression include pervasive fatigue, sleep disturbances, and appetite disturbances. Of these, only fatigue is generally considered to be a symptom of heart disease, especially in heart failure patients with poor cardiac output, or in patients with a recent major cardiac event. Interestingly, in a recent study comparing depressed patients with heart failure and depressed psychiatric patients, those with heart failure reported less depressed mood and fewer feelings of worthlessness and guilt compared to the psychiatric patients, but the groups did not differ on any of the somatic depression symptoms, including fatigue (33).

Furthermore, most prognostic studies of the effects of depression in cardiac patients have adjusted for standard indices of cardiac disease severity and other risk factors for mortality. It is possible that some unmeasured or unknown component of heart disease that happens to produce the same somatic symptoms as depression may account for the effects of somatic symptoms on cardiac outcomes, or that the measures of illness severity and cardiac risk that have been used as covariates are inadequate. However, this concern applies to studies of other cardiac risk factors as well. Residual confounding is always a possible alternative explanation for any putative risk factor.

When considering explanations other than depression for the presence of somatic depression symptoms, it must be pointed out that these symptoms overlap with those of “vital exhaustion” and “sickness behavior” (34). Whether these constructs describe the same underlying disorder or have distinct etiologies and underlying biological processes has been subject of much discussion and debate in the literature (e.g. 34, 35, 36, 37, 38).

Covariate Adjustment

What about the observation that the cardiovascular risks that have been attributed to depression often diminish after covariate adjustment? Depression is often associated with other cardiac risk factors such as diabetes, obesity, smoking, and poor aerobic capacity (39). Consequently, the risk for cardiac events associated with depression often decreases when these factors are included as covariates in a covariate-adjusted model. However, that does not mean that depression is not the principal contributor to elevated depression scores in cardiac patients, or that depression itself is not a risk factor. Depression may be part of a broader network of cardiac risk factors, such as the metabolic syndrome (40, 41), and it may increase the risk for cardiac events by adding to the burden of the proinflammatory and procoagulant processes and the cardiovascular autonomic dysregulation that are associated with both depression and with other cardiac risk factors (39). Adjusting for the effects of other cardiac risk factors may mask important interactions. Zeigelstein and his colleagues, for example, found an interaction between a BDI score >10 and Killip class >1 in patients with a recent MI (42). The one year mortality rate in patients with both depression and Killip class >1 was significantly higher (17%) than in those with neither (4.6%), Killip class >1 alone (5.2%), or BDI > 10 alone (5.2%). It is also likely that behavioral patterns associated with depression, such as physical inactivity, poor adherence to medical treatment regimens, and disturbed sleep, worsen the effects of other cardiac risk factors

The principal mediators and moderators of depression as a cardiac risk factor have not been firmly established, and deserve further attention. Longitudinal studies that recognize the changing nature of these factors over time are especially needed

Depression Without Cognitive Symptoms?

Can somatic symptoms actually be symptoms of depression in the absence of any cognitive or affective symptoms? Dysphoric mood and anhedonia are the cardinal symptoms of major and minor depression. Unless at least one of these symptoms is present, a patient cannot meet the DSM-IV criteria for a major or minor depressive episode (43). However, individuals who do not meet the diagnostic criteria for major depression but who have somatic symptoms of depression also tend to have family and personal histories of mood disorders, shortened REM latency, and an increased proportion of sleep time spent in REM, all of which are factors associated with major depressive disorder (44). Somatic symptoms of depression, especially disturbed sleep, often herald major depressive episodes and are better predictors of episodes than are cognitive or affective symptoms (4548). In patients whose depression is in partial remission, residual somatic symptoms are more common than cognitive or affective symptoms, and they are risk factors for recurrent or chronic depression (4951). Thus, isolated somatic symptoms of depression may reflect an inter-episode phase of major depressive disorder, they may eventually evolve into a recurrent episode of major depression, or they may reflect a chronic, subthreshold mood disorder. Patients with a recent acute coronary syndrome who have residual depression symptoms following treatment are at higher risk for mortality (52), so perhaps the goal of treatment should not be to target somatic symptoms per se, but rather to treat each patient to complete remission.

Summary and Conclusions

Although some studies have reported that somatic symptoms of depression are better predictors of cardiac events than are cognitive symptoms, this question is far from settled. Future studies should address the potentially confounding effects of differences in the frequency and severity of the different types of symptoms, as well as those of the overall burden of depression. Furthermore, factor analytic techniques should be carefully evaluated, especially when a decision is made to include symptoms in more than one factor, or when traditionally cognitive or affective symptoms are included in a “somatic” factor or vice versa. Researchers should consider reporting both factor analytic and face valid methods in the same study to facilitate comparisons among studies and to evaluate the sensitivity of the findings to differences in methodology. It would also be helpful to move away from reliance on unreplicated exploratory factor analyses and to depend instead on more rigorous methods of confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) (53).

Finally, it seems remarkable that after determining an ejection fraction, establishing the severity of coronary artery stenosis, and documenting a history of acute coronary events, heart failure, diabetes, renal function, etc. from medical records or by direct evaluation of patients, it is possible to identify an additional two fold or greater risk of death in post-MI patients merely by asking about symptoms of general fatigue, disturbed sleep, increased or decreased appetite, and perhaps dysphoric mood and irritability (e.g. 54). Researchers who suspect that these symptoms do not represent depression, but instead some aspect of heart disease or medical comorbidity that is being overlooked in routine cardiovascular evaluations, should be working to discover their true cause. In the meantime, depression remains the most plausible explanation for the increased risk of mortality in patients who report symptoms of depression.

Acknowledgments

This manuscript was supported in part by Grant No. RO1 HL08933601A2 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, and by the Lewis and Jean Sachs Charitable Lead Trust.

Abbreviations

ACS

Acute coronary syndrome

BDI

Beck Depression Inventory

CHD

coronary heart disease

MI

myocardial infarction

PHQ-9

Patient Health Questionnnaire

Reference List

  • 1.de Jonge P, Ormel J, van den Brink RH, van Melle JP, Spijkerman TA, Kuijper A, van Veldhuisen DJ, van den Berg MP, Honig A, Crijns HJ, Schene AH. Symptom dimensions of depression following myocardial infarction and their relationship with somatic health status and cardiovascular prognosis. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(1):138–44. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.1.138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Doyle F, Conroy R, McGee H, Delaney M. Depressive symptoms in persons with acute coronary syndrome: specific symptom scales and prognosis. J Psychosom Res. 2010;68(2):121–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hoen PW, Whooley MA, Martens EJ, Na B, van Melle JP, de Jonge P. Differential associations between specific depressive symptoms and cardiovascular prognosis in patients with stable coronary heart disease. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2010;56(11):838–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2010.03.080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Linke SE, Rutledge T, Johnson BD, Vaccarino V, Bittner V, Cornell CE, Eteiba W, Sheps DS, Krantz DS, Parashar S, Bairey Merz CN. Depressive symptom dimensions and cardiovascular prognosis among women with suspected myocardial ischemia: A report from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute-sponsored Women’s Ischemia Syndrome Evaluation. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(5):499–507. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Martens EJ, Hoen PW, Mittelhaeuser M, de Jonge P, Denollet J. Symptom dimensions of post-myocardial infarction depression, disease severity and cardiac prognosis. Psychol Med. 2010;40(5):807–14. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709990997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Roest AM, Thombs BD, Grace SL, Stewart DE, Abbey SE, de Jonge P. Somatic/affective symptoms, but not cognitive/affective symptoms, of depression after acute coronary syndrome are associated with 12-month all-cause mortality. J Affect Disord. 2010 doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.11.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Schiffer AA, Pelle AJ, Smith OR, Widdershoven JW, Hendriks EH, Pedersen SS. Somatic versus cognitive symptoms of depression as predictors of all-cause mortality and health status in chronic heart failure. J Clin Psychiatry. 2009;70(12):1667–73. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Smolderen KG, Spertus JA, Reid KJ, Buchanan DM, Krumholz HM, Denollet J, Vaccarino V, Chan PS. The association of cognitive and somatic depressive symptoms with depression recognition and outcomes after myocardial infarction. Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes. 2009;2(4):328–37. doi: 10.1161/CIRCOUTCOMES.109.868588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Stewart JC, Janicki DL, Muldoon MF, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Kamarck TW. Negative emotions and 3-year progression of subclinical atherosclerosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(2):225–33. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.2.225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Barefoot JC, Brummett BH, Helms MJ, Mark DB, Siegler IC, Williams RB. Depressive symptoms and survival of patients with coronary artery disease. Psychosom Med. 2000;62(6):790–5. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200011000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Connerney I, Sloan RP, Shapiro PA, Bagiella E, Seckman C. Depression is associated with increased mortality 10 years after coronary artery bypass surgery. Psychosom Med. 2010;72(9):874–81. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181f65fc1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Frasure-Smith N, Lesperance F. Depression and other psychological risks following myocardial infarction. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60(6):627–36. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.6.627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Hoen PW, Conradi HJ, Denollet J, Martens EJ, de Jonge P. Interview-based ratings of somatic and cognitive symptoms of depression and their impact on cardiovascular prognosis. Psychother Psychosom. 2010;79(5):319–20. doi: 10.1159/000319528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Irvine J, Basinski A, Baker B, Jandciu S, Paquette M, Cairns J, Connolly S, Roberts R, Gent M, Dorian P. Depression and risk of sudden cardiac death after acute myocardial infarction: testing for the confounding effects of fatigue. Psychosom Med. 1999;61(6):729–37. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199911000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lesperance F, Frasure-Smith N, Talajic M. Major depression before and after myocardial infarction: its nature and consequences. Psychosom Med. 1996;58(2):99–110. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199603000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Pedersen SS, Denollet J, Daemen J, van de Sande M, de Jaegere PT, Serruys PW, Erdman RA, van Domburg RT. Fatigue, depressive symptoms, and hopelessness as predictors of adverse clinical events following percutaneous coronary intervention with paclitaxel-eluting stents. J Psychosom Res. 2007;62(4):455–61. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Tully PJ, Winefield HR, Baker RA, Turnbull DA, de Jonge P. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Beck Depression Inventory-II and the association with cardiac morbidity and mortality after coronary revascularization. J Health Psychol. 2011;16(4):584–95. doi: 10.1177/1359105310383604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Arnau RC, Meagher MW, Norris MP, Bramson R. Psychometric evaluation of the Beck Depression Inventory-II with primary care medical patients. Health Psychol. 2001;20(2):112–9. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.20.2.112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Thombs BD, Grace SL, Ziegelstein RC. Do symptom dimensions of depression following myocardial infarction relate differently to physical health indicators and cardiac prognosis? Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(7):1295–6. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.7.1295a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Sunderajan P, Gaynes BN, Wisniewski SR, Miyahara S, Fava M, Akingbala F, DeVeaugh-Geiss J, Rush AJ, Trivedi MH. Insomnia in patients with depression: a STAR*D report. CNS Spectr. 2010;15(6):394–404. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900029266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Lesperance F, Frasure-Smith N, Talajic M, Bourassa MG. Five-year risk of cardiac mortality in relation to initial severity and one-year changes in depression symptoms after myocardial infarction. Circulation. 2002;105(9):1049–53. doi: 10.1161/hc0902.104707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB. Validation and utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD: the PHQ primary care study. Primary Care Evaluation of Mental Disorders. Patient Health Questionnaire. JAMA. 1999;282(18):1737–44. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.18.1737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Martens EJ, Denollet J, Pedersen SS, Scherders M, Griez E, Widdershoven J, Szabo B, Bonnier H, Appels A. Relative lack of depressive cognitions in post-myocardial infarction depression. J Affect Disord. 2006;94(1–3):231–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.04.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Tanaka-Matsumi J, Kameoka VA. Reliabilities and concurrent validities of popular self-report measures of depression, anxiety, and social desirability. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1986;54(3):328–33. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.54.3.328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ketterer MW, Huffman J, Lumley MA, Wassef S, Gray L, Kenyon L, Kraft P, Brymer J, Rhoads K, Lovallo WR, Goldberg AD. Five-year follow-up for adverse outcomes in males with at least minimally positive angiograms: importance of “denial” in assessing psychosocial risk factors. J Psychosom Res. 1998;44(2):241–50. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(97)00206-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ketterer MW, Denollet J, Chapp J, Thayer B, Keteyian S, Clark V, John S, Farha AJ, Deveshwar S. Men deny and women cry, but who dies? Do the wages of “denial” include early ischemic coronary heart disease? J Psychosom Res. 2004;56(1):119–23. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00501-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Freedland KE, Lustman PJ, Carney RM, Hong BA. Underdiagnosis of depression in patients with coronary artery disease: the role of nonspecific symptoms. Int J Psychiatry Med. 1992;22(3):221–9. doi: 10.2190/YF10-H39R-NY6M-MT1G. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Teasdale JD. Psychological treatments for depression: how do they work? Behav Res Ther. 1985;23(2):157–65. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(85)90024-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Cameron LD, Petrie KJ, Ellis C, Buick D, Weinman JA. Symptom experiences, symptom attributions, and causal attributions in patients following first-time myocardial infarction. Int J Behav Med. 2005;12(1):30–8. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm1201_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lesperance F, Frasure-Smith N. Depression in patients with cardiac disease: a practical review. J Psychosom Res. 2000;48(4–5):379–91. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(99)00102-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Lichtman JH, Bigger JT, Jr, Blumenthal JA, Frasure-Smith N, Kaufmann PG, Lesperance F, Mark DB, Sheps DS, Taylor CB, Froelicher ES. Depression and coronary heart disease: recommendations for screening, referral, and treatment: a science advisory from the American Heart Association Prevention Committee of the Council on Cardiovascular Nursing, Council on Clinical Cardiology, Council on Epidemiology and Prevention, and Interdisciplinary Council on Quality of Care and Outcomes Research: endorsed by the American Psychiatric Association. Circulation. 2008;118(17):1768–75. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.108.190769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Davidson KW, Kupfer DJ, Bigger JT, Califf RM, Carney RM, Coyne JC, Czajkowski SM, Frank E, Frasure-Smith N, Freedland KE, Froelicher ES, Glassman AH, Katon WJ, Kaufmann PG, Kessler RC, Kraemer HC, Krishnan KR, Lesperance F, Rieckmann N, Sheps DS, Suls JM. Assessment and treatment of depression in patients with cardiovascular disease: National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute working group report. Ann Behav Med. 2006;32(2):121–6. doi: 10.1207/s15324796abm3202_9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Holzapfel N, Muller-Tasch T, Wild B, Junger J, Zugck C, Remppis A, Herzog W, Lowe B. Depression profile in patients with and without chronic heart failure. J Affect Disord. 2008;105(1–3):53–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kop WJ, Gottdiener JS. The role of immune system parameters in the relationship between depression and coronary artery disease. Psychosom Med. 2005;67 (Suppl 1):S37–S41. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000162256.18710.4a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kopp MS, Falger PR, Appels A, Szedmak S. Depressive symptomatology and vital exhaustion are differentially related to behavioral risk factors for coronary artery disease. Psychosom Med. 1998;60(6):752–8. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199811000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kudielka BM, von KR, Gander ML, Fischer JE. The interrelationship of psychosocial risk factors for coronary artery disease in a working population: do we measure distinct or overlapping psychological concepts? Behav Med. 2004;30(1):35–43. doi: 10.3200/BMED.30.1.35-44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.van DR, Appels A. Vital exhaustion and depression: a conceptual study. J Psychosom Res. 1991;35(4–5):535–44. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(91)90048-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Wojciechowski FL, Strik JJ, Falger P, Lousberg R, Honig A. The relationship between depressive and vital exhaustion symptomatology post-myocardial infarction. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2000;102(5):359–65. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2000.102005359.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Carney RM, Freedland KE. Depression in patients with coronary heart disease. Am J Med. 2008;121(11 Suppl 2):S20–S27. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2008.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Vogelzangs N, Kritchevsky SB, Beekman AT, Brenes GA, Newman AB, Satterfield S, Yaffe K, Harris TB, Penninx BW. Obesity and onset of significant depressive symptoms: results from a prospective community-based cohort study of older men and women. J Clin Psychiatry. 2010;71(4):391–9. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04743blu. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Vogelzangs N, Beekman ATF, Boelhouwer IG, Bandinelli S, Milaneschi Y, Ferrucci L, Penninx BWJH. Metabolic depression: A chronic depressive subtype? Findings from the InCHIANTI Study of Older Persons. J Clin Psychiatry. 2011 doi: 10.4088/JCP.10m06559. Online ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ziegelstein RC, Thombs BD, Parakh K, Stewart DE, Abbey SE, Grace SL. The brain and the heart: independent or interactive? J Am Coll Cardiol. 2007;49(17):1824–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2007.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.American Psychiatric Association, American Psychiatric Association, Task Force on DSM-IV. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV. 4. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Akiskal HS, Judd LL, Gillin JC, Lemmi H. Subthreshold depressions: clinical and polysomnographic validation of dysthymic, residual and masked forms. J Affect Disord. 1997;45(1–2):53–63. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(97)00059-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Breslau N, Roth T, Rosenthal L, Andreski P. Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders: a longitudinal epidemiological study of young adults. Biol Psychiatry. 1996;39(6):411–8. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00188-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Cho HJ, Lavretsky H, Olmstead R, Levin MJ, Oxman MN, Irwin MR. Sleep disturbance and depression recurrence in community-dwelling older adults: a prospective study. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(12):1543–50. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07121882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Ford DE, Kamerow DB. Epidemiologic study of sleep disturbances and psychiatric disorders. An opportunity for prevention? JAMA. 1989;262(11):1479–84. doi: 10.1001/jama.262.11.1479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Chang PP, Ford DE, Mead LA, Cooper-Patrick L, Klag MJ. Insomnia in young men and subsequent depression. The Johns Hopkins Precursors Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1997;146(2):105–14. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Nierenberg AA, Husain MM, Trivedi MH, Fava M, Warden D, Wisniewski SR, Miyahara S, Rush AJ. Residual symptoms after remission of major depressive disorder with citalopram and risk of relapse: a STAR*D report. Psychol Med. 2010;40(1):41–50. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709006011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Pigeon WR, Hegel M, Unutzer J, Fan MY, Sateia MJ, Lyness JM, Phillips C, Perlis ML. Is insomnia a perpetuating factor for late-life depression in the IMPACT cohort? Sleep. 2008;31(4):481–8. doi: 10.1093/sleep/31.4.481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Silverstein B, Patel P. Poor response to antidepressant medication of patients with depression accompanied by somatic symptomatology in the STAR*D Study. Psychiatry Res. 2011;187(1–2):121–4. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.12.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Carney RM, Freedland KE. Treatment-resistant depression and mortality after acute coronary syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166(4):410–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08081239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Brown TA. Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: Guilford Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Carney RM, Blumenthal JA, Catellier D, Freedland KE, Berkman LF, Watkins LL, Czajkowski SM, Hayano J, Jaffe AS. Depression as a risk factor for mortality after acute myocardial infarction. Am J Cardiol. 2003;92(11):1277–81. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2003.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES