Skip to main content
Pediatrics logoLink to Pediatrics
. 2012 Jan;129(1):132–140. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-0972

Quality of Early Maternal–Child Relationship and Risk of Adolescent Obesity

Sarah E Anderson a,, Rachel A Gooze b, Stanley Lemeshow c, Robert C Whitaker b,d
PMCID: PMC3255468  PMID: 22201144

Abstract

Objectives:

The goal of this study was to determine whether obesity in adolescence is related to the quality of the early maternal–child relationship.

Methods:

We analyzed data from 977 of 1364 participants in the Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development. Child attachment security and maternal sensitivity were assessed by observing mother–child interaction at 15, 24, and 36 months of age. A maternal–child relationship quality score was constructed as the number of times across the 3 ages that the child was either insecurely attached or experienced low maternal sensitivity. Adolescent obesity was defined as a measured BMI ≥95th percentile at age 15 years.

Results:

Poor-quality maternal–child relationships (score: ≥3) were experienced by 24.7% of children compared with 22.0% who, at all 3 ages, were neither insecurely attached nor exposed to low maternal sensitivity (score: 0). The prevalence of adolescent obesity was 26.1%, 15.5%, 12.1%, and 13.0% for those with risk scores of ≥3, 2, 1, and 0, respectively. After adjustment for gender and birth weight, the odds (95% confidence interval) of adolescent obesity was 2.45 (1.49–4.04) times higher in those with the poorest quality early maternal–child relationships (score: ≥3) compared with those with the highest quality (score: 0). Low maternal sensitivity was more strongly associated with obesity than insecure attachment.

Conclusions:

Poor quality of the early maternal–child relationship was associated with a higher prevalence of adolescent obesity. Interventions aimed at improving the quality of maternal–child interactions should consider assessing effects on children’s weight and examining potential mechanisms involving stress response and emotion regulation.

KEY WORDS: attachment security, maternal sensitivity, parenting, obesity, BMI, Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development, children, adolescents, prospective


What’s Known On This Subject:

The quality of the relationship between mother and child affects the child’s neurodevelopment, emotion regulation, and stress response. Extreme or sustained stress responses are associated with dysregulation of physiologic systems involved in energy balance, which could lead to obesity.

What This Study Adds:

The prevalence of obesity in adolescence was more than twice as high among those youth who in early childhood had poor-quality relationships with their mothers compared with those with better relationships.

Most childhood obesity prevention strategies are focused on energy balance1 and target behaviors and environments that directly affect energy intake or expenditure, such as increasing physical activity, reducing sedentary behavior, or limiting intake of energy-dense foods and beverages.2 The limited success of these strategies35 underscores the importance of considering new approaches.

We have proposed that insecure attachment may be a risk factor for obesity in preschool-aged children.6 The mechanism underlying this association is uncertain. However, attachment security reflects the development of children’s emotion regulation and stress response.7,8 These capacities could influence adiposity through their effects on appetite, sleep, and activity.911 Despite the potential of attachment security to affect the neurodevelopment of physiologic systems regulating weight, no studies have examined the association between attachment security and obesity beyond the preschool age.

Assessment of attachment security is based on a child’s behaviors during interactions with a primary caregiver, usually the mother. Secure attachment is 1 indicator of the quality of the mother–child relationship. Specifically, it reflects the child’s awareness that the mother can be used as a “secure base” from which to explore and that returning to the mother after a stressful experience will be comforting.12,13 Maternal sensitivity, another indicator of maternal–child relationship quality, refers to the mother’s capacity to recognize the child’s emotional state and respond with comfort, consistency, and warmth.14 Although a child’s secure attachment is more likely to develop within the context of maternal sensitivity,15,16 additional factors such as the child’s temperament, innate capacity for self-regulation, relationships with other caregivers, or the household environment may also influence attachment security.17,18

Evidence suggests that obesity is more prevalent among adults who have been abused or neglected as children,19,20 but, to the best of our knowledge, few studies have examined the relationship between the quality of early maternal–child relationships and obesity. Two prospective studies suggest that greater maternal sensitivity during early childhood is associated with lower risk of obesity later in childhood,21,22 but this was not found in another study.23

Both attachment security and maternal sensitivity may be linked to obesity through development of children’s capacity to modulate their responses to internal emotional states, such as those that occur with stress.24,25 These stress responses can be both physiologic (eg, increased cortisol levels) and behavioral (eg, increased food consumption), and may lead to obesity if the stress is extreme or sustained.26,27 Although healthy emotion regulation may be a mechanism for protection against development of obesity, young children’s capability for emotion regulation is difficult to directly assess in epidemiologic studies.28 In the absence of such data, we examined how obesity in adolescence is related to quality of the early maternal–child relationship using direct observation of 2 factors that reflect emotional regulation—attachment security and maternal sensitivity.

Methods

Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development

We used data from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD), a prospective cohort study of children born in 1991 that was designed to examine the impact of nonmaternal care on children’s developmental outcomes.29 To achieve a sample of children from families with diverse sociodemographic characteristics, investigators recruited children at birth from 24 hospitals located in 9 US states.30,31 Exclusion criteria included maternal age <18 years, nonsingleton birth, lack of English fluency, postbirth hospitalization for >7 days, or plans for adoption.30,31 Study protocols were approved by the institutional review boards of participating universities.

Study Subjects

Our study examined 977 children, which is 71.6% of the original cohort (n = 1364). To be included in our analyses, subjects required data on BMI assessed between 12.0 and 15.9 years of age.

Study Variables

Adolescent Obesity

Adolescent obesity was defined as a gender-specific BMI ≥95th percentile of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention growth reference.32 We calculated BMI (kilograms per meters squared) by using height and weight measurements obtained in a laboratory setting using a standardized protocol.33 To maximize the number of youth included in our analyses, we defined adolescent obesity relative to the BMI percentile at the 15-year assessment. For those missing BMI data at 15 years, we used the BMI measured at the oldest age after 12.0 years.

Early Maternal–Child Relationship Quality

At 15, 24, and 36 months of age, child attachment security and maternal sensitivity were assessed by direct observation of mother–child interaction. We combined information from these assessments to characterize the quality of the early maternal–child relationship.

Maternal sensitivity was coded from a standardized, videotaped, 15-minute play session conducted in the home (15 months) or a child development laboratory (24 and 36 months). Mothers were instructed to play with their child using the contents of 3 bags, each containing a different toy. Videotapes were coded at a central location by trained coders who were unaware of other information about the child’s family. Coders met regularly with an investigator who ensured they maintained consistent expertise.31 Maternal sensitivity was computed as the sum of ratings on 3 aspects of observed maternal behavior toward the child. At 15 and 24 months, these aspects were sensitivity to nondistress, intrusiveness (reverse coded), and positive regard; each was rated using a 4-point scale where 1 = not at all characteristic and 4 = very characteristic. At 36 months, a 7-point scale was used, and the 3 aspects of maternal behavior rated were supportive presence, respect for autonomy, and hostility (reverse coded).34 Maternal sensitivity scores were skewed toward high values; we used the lowest quartile to define low maternal sensitivity (scores ≤8 at 15 and 24 months, and scores ≤15 at 36 months).

At 15 and 36 months, attachment security was assessed in the laboratory using the Strange Situation procedure,31,35 which involved observation and coding of the child’s behavior during a standardized separation from and reunion with the mother.12,36 Based on the Strange Situation procedure, children were classified as securely or insecurely attached. Attachment security was assessed at 24 months using the Attachment Q-sort (AQS).37 Mother and child were observed in their home for ∼2 hours by a trained observer from the SECCYD research staff. After this, the observer completed the AQS by sorting 90 statements about behaviors children may exhibit relative to how well each statement described the behavior of the child; the AQS security score reflects the correlation of the child’s score (range: –1.0 to 1.0) with that of a “prototypically secure” child.37 The validity of the AQS has been established.38 For interpretability and comparability with our previous work,6 we defined insecure attachment at 24 months as the lowest quartile of the AQS security score, which in this sample was <0.16. The Strange Situation procedure and AQS provide related but complementary information.39,40

To describe the maternal–child relationship experience across early childhood, we created a maternal–child relationship quality score using 2 measures (attachment and maternal sensitivity) at each of 3 time points (15, 24, and 36 months). We created this score as an aggregate measure of the relationship experience because we conceptualized insecure attachment and low maternal sensitivity as overlapping but complementary risk factors41 and because neither would necessarily be expected to track strongly during early childhood.40 The score was based on a count of the number of times over the 3 assessments that the child was characterized as insecurely attached or the mother displayed low levels of sensitivity, and had a possible range of 0 to 6; we defined poor maternal–child relationship quality as a score ≥3, which was approximately the lowest quartile.

Additional Variables

At enrollment, mothers reported their educational attainment and their child’s gender and racial-ethnic group. Birth weights of children were recorded from birth certificates. At the 24-month interview, mothers reported household size and income, which were used to determine the household income-to-poverty line ratio.42 When the children were 15 years old, mothers self-reported their current height and weight, and we used these data to assess maternal obesity (BMI ≥30).

Statistical Analysis

By using χ2 tests, we compared the characteristics of children in our analytic sample with those not included due to missing data on adolescent obesity. For each of the 3 early childhood time periods, we used logistic regression43 to estimate odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for adolescent obesity associated with insecure attachment, low maternal sensitivity, and their combination. We examined the association between sociodemographic characteristics and the prevalence of both adolescent obesity and of poor maternal–child relationship quality. We used logistic regression to calculate the odds (95% CI) of adolescent obesity associated with maternal–child relationship scores of ≥3, 2, and 1 relative to scores of 0. In a separate analysis, we calculated the odds (95% CI) of adolescent obesity associated with the number of times the child had insecure attachment and the number of times the mother displayed low sensitivity. We present both sets of regression analyses with and without adjusting for 2 potentially confounding variables (gender and birth weight). Birth weight was modeled as a continuous variable after confirming that it was linear in the logit.43 We also present analyses adjusted for maternal obesity and sociodemographic characteristics. However, we did not consider these as our primary analyses because these variables may be part of a causal chain or pathway leading to adolescent obesity that also involves insecure attachment and/or low maternal sensitivity.44

Results

There were some differences in characteristics between children in our analysis (n = 977) and those (n = 387) not included due to missing data on adolescent obesity (Table 1). However, at each time period neither the prevalence of low maternal sensitivity nor insecure attachment was significantly different between the children in the analytic sample and those not in the sample (data not shown).

TABLE 1.

Characteristics of Subjects in the Analytic Sample Compared With Subjects Not Included in the Analytic Sample

Characteristic Analytic Samplea Not in Analytic Sampleb Pc
Gender
 Female 492 (50.4) 167 (43.2) .02
 Male 485 (49.6) 220 (56.8)
Racial-ethnic group
 White 788 (80.7) 309 (79.8) .73
 Nonwhite 189 (19.3) 78 (20.2)
Birth weight, g
 2000–2999 159 (16.3)d 65 (16.8)
 3000–3999 673 (68.9) 261 (67.4) .87
 ≥4000 145 (14.8) 61 (15.8)
Maternal education
 Graduate degree 153 (15.7) 45 (11.7)
 Bachelor degree 219 (22.4) 65 (16.8)
 Some college or associate degree 324 (33.2) 131 (33.9) <.001
 High school degree or equivalent 201 (20.6) 86 (22.3)
 Less than high school degree 80 (8.2) 59 (15.3)
Household income-to-poverty line ratio
 ≥5.00 216 (23.2) 57 (22.4)
 3.00–4.99 262 (28.1) 81 (31.9)
 1.86–2.99 205 (22.0) 47 (18.5) .69
 1.00–1.85 139 (14.9) 37 (14.6)
 <1.00 111 (11.9) 32 (12.6)
Adolescent obesitye
 BMI <95th percentile 816 (83.5) NA NA
 BMI ≥95th percentile 161 (16.5)

Values are given as number (%). Percentages may not total to 100% due to rounding. NA, not applicable.

a

Participants in analytic sample (n = 977). Of these, 44 were missing data on household income.

b

Participants not included in analytic sample because of missing information on adolescent obesity (n = 387). Of these, 133 were missing data on household income and 1 was missing data on maternal education.

c

P values from χ2 tests comparing characteristics of participants in the analytic sample with those not in the analytic sample.

d

Preterm birth (<37 weeks’ gestation) was an exclusion criterion for the SECCYD; 24 of these 159 children weighed between 2000 and 2500 g.

e

Gender-specific BMI-for-age percentile from height and weight measured at a mean ± SD age of 14.9 ± 0.6 years. BMI was measured between 15.0 and 15.9 years for 605 (61.9%) participants, between 14.0 and 14.9 years for 287 (29.4%) participants, and between 12.0 and 13.9 years for 85 (8.7%) participants.

Low maternal sensitivity at 15, 24, and 36 months was associated with an increased odds of adolescent obesity (Table 2). At 24 months, insecure attachment was associated with increased odds for adolescent obesity but not at 15 or 36 months. At 24 and 36 months, the combination of insecure attachment and low maternal sensitivity was associated with greater odds of adolescent obesity than either was alone.

TABLE 2.

Maternal Sensitivity and Child Attachment Security at 15, 24, and 36 Months in Relation to Adolescent Obesity

Factor N (%)a Adolescent Obesity,%b Unadjusted OR (95% CI)c
15 months
 Maternal sensitivity
  Normal 732 (76.6) 14.5 1 (Referent)
  Low 224 (23.4) 22.8 1.74 (1.20–2.53)
 Attachment securityd
  Secure 555 (59.6) 16.4 1 (Referent)
  Insecure 376 (40.4) 16.8 1.03 (0.72–1.46)
 Low sensitivity and insecure attachment
  Neither 436 (47.1) 14.0 1 (Referent)
  1 only 391 (42.2) 18.2 1.36 (0.94–1.98)
  Both 99 (10.7) 21.2 1.66 (0.95–2.88)
24 months
 Maternal sensitivity
  Normal 686 (74.1) 14.6 1 (Referent)
  Low 240 (25.9)f 22.9 1.74 (1.21–2.52)
 Attachment securitye
  Secure 705 (75.2) 15.0 1 (Referent)
  Insecure 232 (24.8)g 21.1 1.51 (1.04–2.21)
 Low sensitivity and insecure attachment
  Neither 539 (58.6) 13.4 1 (Referent)
  1 only 296 (32.2) 19.6 1.58 (1.08–2.31)
  Both 85 (9.2) 25.9 2.27 (1.31–3.91)
36 months
 Maternal sensitivity
  Normal 731 (79.3) 14.4 1 (Referent)
  Low 191 (20.7) 24.6 1.95 (1.32–2.87)
 Attachment securityd
  Secure 566 (62.2) 15.9 1 (Referent)
  Insecure 344 (37.8) 17.7 1.14 (0.80–1.63)
 Low sensitivity and insecure attachment
  Neither 475 (52.4) 14.5 1 (Referent)
  1 only 328 (36.3) 16.5 1.16 (0.79–1.71)
  Both 101 (11.2) 26.7 2.15 (1.29–3.57)
a

Of the 977 participants, the number missing information for maternal sensitivity, attachment security, and the combination of these 2 variables was n = 21, 46, and 51, respectively at 15 months; 51, 40, and 57 at 24 months; and 55, 67, and 73 at 36 months. Percentages may not total to 100% due to rounding.

b

Gender-specific BMI-for-age percentile ≥95th percentile.

c

ORs and 95% CIs from logistic regression.

d

Coded from Strange Situation procedure.

e

Lowest quartile of AQS security score.

f

Of the 240 children experiencing low maternal sensitivity at 24 months, 101 experienced it at 15 months, and 104 at 36 months.

g

Of the 232 children who were insecurely attached at 24 months, 106 were insecure at 15 months, and 103 at 36 months.

Based on a maternal–child relationship quality score of ≥3, a total of 241 children (24.7%) were classified as having a poor relationship during early childhood. Of these children, 215 experienced low maternal sensitivity and insecure attachment at least once, whereas only 19 never experienced low maternal sensitivity and only 7 were never insecurely attached. Lower household income and maternal education were related to adolescent obesity and to relationship quality (Table 3).

TABLE 3.

Associations Between Participant Characteristics, the Quality of the Early Maternal–Child Relationship, and Adolescent Obesity

Characteristic Poor Maternal-Child Relationship Quality,a % Pb Adolescent Obesity,c % Pb
Gender
 Female 21.7 .09 13.8 .02
 Male 27.6 19.2
Racial-ethnic group
 White 18.8 <.001 14.8 .005
 Nonwhite 49.2 23.3
Birth weight, g
 2000–2999 34.0 14.5
 3000–3999 23.6 .009 15.3 .03
 ≥4000 19.3 24.1
Maternal education
 Graduate degree 10.5 9.8
 Bachelor degree 11.0 10.0
 Some college or associate degree 23.5 <.001 18.5 <.001
 High school degree or equivalent 42.8 20.4
 Less than high school degree 48.8 28.8
Household income-to-poverty line ratio
 ≥5.00 12.0 7.4
 3.00–4.99 19.8 14.9
 1.86–2.99 22.9 <.001 17.1 <.001
 1.00–1.85 31.7 21.6
 <1.00 56.8 30.6
Maternal obesityd
 BMI <30 21.1 .005 9.6 <.001
 BMI ≥30 30.0 33.9
a

Poor maternal–child relationship quality defined as score ≥3 based on the number of times in early childhood (15, 24, and 36 months) child was classified as insecurely attached or mother displayed low sensitivity.

b

P values from χ2 tests.

c

Gender-specific BMI-for-age percentile ≥95th percentile.

d

Maternal BMI ≥30 based on maternal self-report of height and weight when youth was 15 years of age, n = 888.

The prevalence of obesity in adolescence was 26.1% among children who experienced poor early maternal–child relationships (score: ≥3) and was 15.5%, 12.1%, and 13.0% for children with better relationships (scores of 2, 1, and 0, respectively) (upper section of Table 4). After adjustment for gender and birth weight (model 2), the odds (95% CI) of adolescent obesity were 2.45 (1.49–4.04) times higher for those with the poorest relationships (score: ≥3) compared with those with the best relationships (score: 0). With additional adjustment for race/ethnicity, maternal education, and household income-to-poverty line ratio, the OR (95% CI) was attenuated to 1.56 (0.90–2.73), and with inclusion of maternal obesity to 1.42 (0.76–2.63). Low maternal sensitivity was more strongly related to adolescent obesity than was insecure attachment (lower section of Table 4).

TABLE 4.

Association of Maternal–Child Relationship Quality and Adolescent Obesity

Predictor Variable(s) N (%) Prevalence of Adolescent Obesity,
% (95% CI)a Model 1, Crude OR (95% CI) Model 2, Adjusted OR (95% CI)b Model 3, Adjusted OR (95% CI)c Model 4, Adjusted OR (95% CI)d
Maternal–child relationship quality scoree
 0 215 (22.0) 13.0 (8.5–17.5) 1 (Referent) 1 (Referent) 1 (Referent) 1 (Referent)
 1 315 (32.2) 12.1 (8.5–15.7) 0.92 (0.54–1.54) 0.93 (0.55–1.57) 0.88 (0.51–1.52) 0.96 (0.53–1.74)
 2 206 (21.1) 15.5 (10.6–20.5) 1.23 (0.71–2.12) 1.20 (0.69–2.09) 1.05 (0.59–1.87) 1.00 (0.53–1.90)
 ≥3f 241 (24.7) 26.1 (20.6–31.7) 2.36 (1.45–3.86) 2.45 (1.49–4.04) 1.56 (0.90–2.73) 1.42 (0.76–2.63)
Maternal sensitivity and insecure attachment simultaneously
No. of time periods with low maternal sensitivityg
 0 557 (57.0) 11.9 (9.2–14.5) 1 (Referent) 1 (Referent) 1 (Referent) 1 (Referent)
 1 237 (24.3) 20.7 (15.5–25.8) 1.91 (1.27–2.88) 1.88 (1.25–2.84) 1.50 (0.97–2.33) 1.26 (0.77–2.05)
 ≥2 183 (18.7) 25.1 (18.9–31.4) 2.43 (1.58–3.74) 2.47 (1.59–3.83) 1.47 (0.88–2.44) 1.48 (0.84–2.62)
No. of time periods with insecure attachmentg
 0 321 (32.9) 13.4 (9.7–17.1) 1 (Referent) 1 (Referent) 1 (Referent) 1 (Referent)
 1 417 (42.7) 17.5 (13.9–21.2) 1.29 (0.85–1.94) 1.29 (0.85–1.95) 1.36 (0.88–2.09) 1.23 (0.77–1.97)
 ≥2 239 (24.5) 18.8 (13.9–23.8) 1.20 (0.75–1.93) 1.23 (0.76–1.97) 1.10 (0.67–1.81) 0.86 (0.49–1.51)
a

Gender-specific BMI-for-age percentile ≥95th percentile.

b

Adjusted for gender and birth weight (grams), n = 977.

c

Adjusted for gender, birth weight (grams), nonwhite race/ethnicity, maternal education, and household income-to-poverty line ratio, n = 933.

d

Adjusted for gender, birth weight (grams), nonwhite race/ethnicity, maternal education, household income-to-poverty line ratio, and maternal obesity, n = 850.

e

Number of times child was insecurely attached or mother displayed low maternal sensitivity at 15, 24, or 36 months.

f

Of the 241 participants with a poor maternal–child relationship (score ≥3), 134 (55.6%) had scores of 3, 66 (27.4%) had scores of 4, 32 (13.3%) had scores of 5, and 9 (3.7%) had scores of 6. Seventy-four of these children (30.7%) were both insecurely attached and experienced low maternal sensitivity at ≥2 time periods, 69 (28.6%) were insecurely attached at ≥2 time periods and experienced low maternal sensitivity at 1 time period, 72 (29.9%) were insecurely attached at 1 time period and experienced low maternal sensitivity at ≥2 time periods, 19 (7.9%) were insecurely attached at all 3 time periods but never experienced low maternal sensitivity, and 7 (2.9%) experienced low maternal sensitivity at all 3 time periods but were never insecurely attached.

g

ORs from models including both predictor variables: number of time periods the child experienced low maternal sensitivity, and number of time periods the child was insecurely attached.

Discussion

In these prospective analyses, we found that children who experienced poor-quality early relationships with their mothers, as measured by insecure attachment and low levels of maternal sensitivity, had a greater risk of obesity in adolescence. This conclusion is consistent with our previous finding in a larger and nationally representative cohort that insecure attachment at 24 months was associated with obesity at preschool age.6

Children’s ability to regulate their emotions and cope with stress is developed in the context of their early interactions with their parents.45 Although not the sole determinant, sensitive parenting increases the likelihood that a child will have a secure pattern of attachment and develop a healthy response to stress.7,46,47 The areas of the brain that govern energy balance are also involved with stress response and emotion regulation, and extreme and/or sustained stress is associated with dysregulation of these areas of the brain.10,26,48 Animal studies have shown that stress preferentially increases consumption of highly palatable foods, and eating these foods acts to calm the stress-perceiving areas of the brain.4951 Maternal sensitivity could protect against obesity by improving children’s ability to modulate their physiologic and behavioral responses to stress. Children whose stress response is well regulated may be less likely, for example, to eat in response to emotional distress, and may have longer sleep duration, which could also affect their risk for obesity.52,53

We found that adolescent obesity was related to insecure attachment based on the AQS at 24 months but not to insecure attachment based on the Strange Situation procedure at 15 or 36 months. There may be several explanations. Compared with the Strange Situation procedure, the AQS involves a period of mother–child observation that is longer, occurs in the home, and does not explicitly involve a stress paradigm. Therefore, the AQS may yield different information about the maternal–child relationship than the Strange Situation procedure. There is evidence that low scores on the AQS more strongly predict child outcomes, such as behavior problems, than does insecurity as assessed by using the Strange Situation procedure.34 As reported by others examining the SECCYD data,34 we found that a child’s attachment security status was not consistent between 15, 24, and 36 months, and the reasons for this finding have been debated.40

Few studies have examined the association between maternal sensitivity and childhood obesity, and most have used data from SECCYD based on direct observation of maternal-child interaction. Rhee et al found that low maternal sensitivity at 4.5 years of age was associated with a greater risk of obesity in first grade,21 while Wu et al22 reported that low maternal sensitivity at 6 months of age was associated with higher BMI in preadolescence. Also in the SECCYD, a cross-sectional analysis of 15-year-olds found that obesity was associated with low maternal sensitivity as coded from videotaped conversations of adolescents discussing a topic of conflict with their mother.54 To our knowledge, the only other study of maternal sensitivity and childhood obesity was a cross-sectional analysis of 4- and 5-year-old Australian children.23 Maternal warmth was not associated with child obesity, but warmth was not assessed by direct observation of mother–child interaction.23

The sensitivity that a mother displays in interacting with her child may be influenced by factors she cannot necessarily control. For example, it is challenging for caregivers to respond sensitively to children who have innate difficulties with self-regulation or who are temperamentally predisposed toward negative emotionality and reactivity.18 This situation may be particularly true for caregivers who are stressed by various hardships arising from their socioeconomic circumstances.17,55,56

Our results are suggestive of a cumulative effect of the poor quality of the early maternal–child relationship on a child’s obesity risk. Although maternal sensitivity was a stronger predictor than insecure attachment, the combination of both seemed to be a greater risk than either alone. In practice, any obesity prevention strategies that aim to alter either maternal sensitivity or attachment security are likely to affect both.

The causes of childhood obesity are multiple and interact with one another. Our findings suggest that consideration be given to obesity prevention strategies that do not focus exclusively on energy balance. Interventions are effective in increasing maternal sensitivity and enhancing young children’s attachment security and ability to regulate their emotions,57,58 but to our knowledge, the effect of these interventions on children’s weight status has not been investigated. However, improving the quality of the maternal–child relationship may require addressing broader social determinants of health.59 Poverty has broad effects on children’s well-being.6062 Parenting in the context of poverty is particularly difficult59 and, as our data suggest, children living in poverty are more likely to be insecurely attached.15

We found that sociodemographic factors were related both to early maternal–child relationship quality and adolescent obesity. The strength of the association between a poor-quality maternal–child relationship and obesity was attenuated after adjustment for sociodemographic factors. This finding is consistent with a causal pathway going from these sociodemographic factors to maternal–child relationship quality to obesity. Alternatively, these sociodemographic factors may confound the association between relationship quality and obesity. An observational study cannot distinguish between these possibilities.

Our research has limitations. Causality cannot be established from observational studies, but reverse causality is unlikely because of the temporal separation between our assessment of exposure and outcome. We chose to control these analyses for a limited number of variables to avoid underestimating the risk relationship by controlling for factors potentially on the causal pathway. However, in doing so, we may have overestimated the risk of adolescent obesity associated with insecure attachment and low maternal sensitivity. Our measure of maternal obesity was limited because it was assessed when youth were adolescents rather than in early childhood, and data were missing for ∼10% of participants. In our analyses, maternal obesity was associated both with adolescent obesity and having a poor early maternal–child relationship. However, after controlling for sociodemographic variables, further adjustment for maternal obesity changed the model estimates only slightly. We used logistic regression to estimate ORs; when an outcome is not rare, ORs will be farther from 1 than the equivalent risk ratio.63 Finally, because we did not have adolescent obesity data on the entire cohort, we cannot exclude the possibility of selection bias.

Conclusions

Obesity is affecting even preschool-aged children, and we lack effective approaches for prevention.3 We provide evidence that the quality of the early maternal–child relationship is associated with risk for adolescent obesity. Decades of research indicate that having a high-quality maternal–child relationship contributes to the cognitive, social, and emotional outcomes that most parents want for their children. The quality of this relationship could prevent obesity through its influence on the child’s capacity for emotion-regulation and response to stress.7,26,46 If future research confirms these mechanisms, obesity prevention interventions could incorporate more emphasis on the quality of maternal–child relationships. This strategy might be more acceptable to parents than interventions focused on energy balance, and would offer additional benefits to children’s health and well-being aside from maintaining a healthy weight.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grant R01DK088913 from the National Institutes of Health. The SECCYD was conducted by the NICHD Early Child Care Research Network supported by NICHD through a cooperative agreement that calls for scientific collaboration between grantees and the NICHD staff. The Ohio State University and Temple University have restricted data use agreements to analyze the SECCYD data.

We are grateful to Khushi Malhotra for technical assistance and to Margaret T. Owen, PhD, who provided helpful comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript.

Glossary

AQS

Attachment Q-sort

CI

confidence interval

NICHD

Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development

OR

odds ratio

SECCYD

Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development

Footnotes

All authors approved the final version of the article, and have participated sufficiently in the work to take public responsibility for appropriate portions of the content. Dr Anderson was involved in conception and design, acquisition of funding, analysis and interpretation of data, drafting of the article, and critical revision for important intellectual content; Ms Gooze was involved in conception and design, analysis and interpretation of data, and critical revision of the article for important intellectual content; Dr Lemeshow was involved in analysis and interpretation of data and critical revision of the article for important intellectual content; and Dr Whitaker was involved in conception and design, acquisition of funding, analysis and interpretation of data, drafting of the article, and critical revision of the article for important intellectual content.

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

Funded by the National Institutes of Health (NIH).

References

  • 1.Institute of Medicine Preventing Childhood Obesity: Health in the Balance. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2005 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Flynn MAT, McNeil DA, Maloff B, et al. Reducing obesity and related chronic disease risk in children and youth: a synthesis of evidence with ‘best practice’ recommendations. Obes Rev. 2006;7(suppl 1):7–66 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Ciampa PJ, Kumar D, Barkin SL, et al. Interventions aimed at decreasing obesity in children younger than 2 years: a systematic review. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164(12):1098–1104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hesketh KD, Campbell KJ. Interventions to prevent obesity in 0-5 year olds: an updated systematic review of the literature. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2010;18(suppl 1):S27–S35 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Whitlock EP, O'Connor EA, Williams SB, Beil TL, Lutz KW. Effectiveness of weight management interventions in children: a targeted systematic review for the USPSTF. Pediatrics. 2010;125(2). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/125/2/e396 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Anderson SE, Whitaker RC. Attachment security and obesity in US preschool-aged children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2011;165(3):235–242 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Schore AN. Effects of a secure attachment relationship on right brain development, affect regulation, and infant mental health. Infant Ment Health J. 2001;22(1–2):7–66 [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Waters SF, Virmani EA, Thompson RA, Meyer S, Raikes HA, Jochem R. Emotion regulation and attachment: unpacking two constructs and their association. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2010;32(1):37–47 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Calabrese F, Molteni R, Racagni G, Riva MA. Neuronal plasticity: a link between stress and mood disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(suppl 1):S208–S216 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(6):434–445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Vgontzas AN, Lin HM, Papaliaga M, et al. Short sleep duration and obesity: the role of emotional stress and sleep disturbances. Int J Obes (Lond). 2008;32(5):801–809 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ainsworth MD, Blehar M, Waters E, Wall S. Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1978 [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss, Vol. 1: Attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1969/1982 [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ainsworth M, Bell S, Stayton D. Infant-mother attachment and social development: socialization as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In: Richards M, ed. The Integration of a Child Into a Social World. London, England: Cambridge University Press; 1974:99–135 [Google Scholar]
  • 15.De Wolff MS, van Ijzendoorn MH. Sensitivity and attachment: a meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Dev. 1997;68(4):571–591 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Isabella RA. Origins of attachment: maternal interactive behavior across the first year. Child Dev. 1993;64(2):605–621 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Kochanska G. Children's temperament, mother's discipline, and security of attachment: multiple pathways to emerging internalization. Child Dev. 1995;66(3):597–615 [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Schmid G, Schreier A, Meyer R, Wolke D. Predictors of crying, feeding and sleeping problems: a prospective study. Child Care Health Dev. 2011;37(4):493–502 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Williamson DF, Thompson TJ, Anda RF, Dietz WH, Felitti V. Body weight and obesity in adults and self-reported abuse in childhood. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2002;26(8):1075–1082 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lissau I, Sørensen TI. Parental neglect during childhood and increased risk of obesity in young adulthood. Lancet. 1994;343(8893):324–327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rhee KE, Lumeng JC, Appugliese DP, Kaciroti N, Bradley RH. Parenting styles and overweight status in first grade. Pediatrics. 2006;117(6):2047–2054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Wu T, Dixon WE, Jr, Dalton WT, III, Tudiver F, Liu X. Joint effects of child temperament and maternal sensitivity on the development of childhood obesity. Matern Child Health J. 2011;15(4):469–477 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Wake M, Nicholson JM, Hardy P, Smith K. Preschooler obesity and parenting styles of mothers and fathers: Australian national population study. Pediatrics 2007;120(6). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/120/6/e1520 [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 24.Braungart-Rieker JM, Garwood MM, Powers BP, Wang X. Parental sensitivity, infant affect, and affect regulation: predictors of later attachment. Child Dev. 2001;72(1):252–270 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Calkins SD, Leerkes EM. Early attachment processes and the development of emotional self-regulation. In: Vohs KD, Baumeister RF, eds. Handbook of Self-Regulation. 2nd ed. New York, NY: The Guilford Press; 2011:355–373 [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Dallman MF. Stress-induced obesity and the emotional nervous system. Trends Endocrinol Metab. 2010;21(3):159–165 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Roemmich JN, Smith JR, Epstein LH, Lambiase M. Stress reactivity and adiposity of youth. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2007;15(9):2303–2310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: a theme in search of definition. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 1994;59(2–3):25–52 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.NICHD Early Child Care Research Network Child care and child development: the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. In: Friedman SL, Haywood HC, eds. Developmental Follow-up: Concepts, Domains, and Methods. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1994:377–396 [Google Scholar]
  • 30.National Institutes of Health, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development. Study overview. Available at: www.nichd.nih.gov/research/supported/seccyd/overview.cfm Accessed March 13, 2011
  • 31.NICHD Early Child Care Research Network Child care and mother-child interaction in the first 3 years of life. Dev Psychol. 1999;35(6):1399–1413 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Guo SS, et al. 2000 CDC Growth Charts for the United States: methods and development. Vital Health Stat 11. 2002;(246):1–190 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development. Chapter 5.5, Physical examination. In : Operations Manual, Phase IV. Research Triangle Park, NC: RTI, International; 2006 [Google Scholar]
  • 34.McCartney K, Owen MT, Booth CL, Clarke-Stewart A, Vandell DL. Testing a maternal attachment model of behavior problems in early childhood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45(4):765–778 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.NICHD Early Child Care Research Network The effects of infant child care on infant-mother attachment security: results of the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Child Dev. 1997;68(5):860–879 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Cassidy, J, Marvin, RS and Attachment Working Group of the MacArthur Network on the Transition from Infancy to Early Childhood (1987, 1992). Attachment organization in three- and four-year-olds: Coding guidelines. Unpublished manual, Psychology Department, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA
  • 37.Waters E, Deane KE. Defining and assessing individual-differences in attachment relationships: Q-methodology and the organization of behavior in infancy and early-childhood. Monogr Soc Res Child. 1985;50(1–2):41–65 [Google Scholar]
  • 38.van IJzendoorn MH, Vereijken CMJL, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Riksen-Walraven JM. Assessing attachment security with the Attachment Q Sort: meta-analytic evidence for the validity of the observer AQS. Child Dev. 2004;75(4):1188–1213 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Van Bakel HJ, Riksen-Walraven JM. AQS security scores: what do they represent? A study in construct validation. Infant Ment Health J. 2004;25(3):175–193 [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Thompson RA. Measure twice, cut once: attachment theory and the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development. Attach Hum Dev. 2008;10(3):287–297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kraemer HC, Stice E, Kazdin A, Offord D, Kupfer D. How do risk factors work together? Mediators, moderators, and independent, overlapping, and proxy risk factors. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(6):848–856 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.US Census Bureau. Poverty thresholds 1993. Available at: www.census.gov/hhes/www/poverty/data/threshld/thresh93.html Accessed February 24, 2011
  • 43.Hosmer DW, Lemeshow S. Applied Logistic Regression. 2nd ed. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc; 2000 [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Belsky J, Houts RM, Fearon RM. Infant attachment security and the timing of puberty: testing an evolutionary hypothesis. Psychol Sci. 2010;21(9):1195–1201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Thompson RA, Meyer S. The socialization of emotion regulation in the family. In: Gross J, ed. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2007:249–268 [Google Scholar]
  • 46.McEwen BS. Understanding the potency of stressful early life experiences on brain and body function. Metabolism. 2008;57(suppl 2):S11–S15 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Raikes HA, Thompson RA. Attachment security and parenting quality predict children’s problem-solving, attributions, and loneliness with peers. Attach Hum Dev. 2008;10(3):319–344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Schwartz MW, Woods SC, Porte D, Jr, Seeley RJ, Baskin DG. Central nervous system control of food intake. Nature. 2000;404(6778):661–671 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Dallman MF, Pecoraro N, Akana SF, et al. Chronic stress and obesity: a new view of “comfort food”. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100(20):11696–11701 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Pecoraro N, Reyes F, Gomez F, Bhargava A, Dallman MF. Chronic stress promotes palatable feeding, which reduces signs of stress: feedforward and feedback effects of chronic stress. Endocrinology. 2004;145(8):3754–3762 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Peters A, Pellerin L, Dallman MF, et al. Causes of obesity: looking beyond the hypothalamus. Prog Neurobiol. 2007;81(2):61–88 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Snell EK, Adam EK, Duncan GJ. Sleep and the body mass index and overweight status of children and adolescents. Child Dev. 2007;78(1):309–323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Touchette E, Petit D, Tremblay RE, et al. Associations between sleep duration patterns and overweight/obesity at age 6. Sleep. 2008;31(11):1507–1514 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Neal Davis R, Ashba J, Appugliese DP, et al. Adolescent obesity and maternal and paternal sensitivity and monitoring. Int J Pediatr Obes. 2011;6(2–2):e457–e463 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Frick PJ, Morris AS. Temperament and developmental pathways to conduct problems. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2004;33(1):54–68 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Collins WA, Maccoby EE, Steinberg L, Hetherington EM, Bornstein MH. Contemporary research on parenting. The case for nature and nurture. Am Psychol. 2000;55(2):218–232 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. Less is more: meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychol Bull. 2003;129(2):195–215 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Berlin LJ, Zeanah CH, Lieberman AF. Prevention and intervention programs for supporting early attachment security. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, eds. Handbook of Attachment. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2008:745–761 [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Klebanov PK, Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ. Does neighborhood and family poverty affect mothers' parenting, mental health, and social support? J Marriage Fam. 1994;56(2):441–455 [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ. The effects of poverty on children. Future Child. 1997;7(2):55–71 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Evans GW. The environment of childhood poverty. Am Psychol. 2004;59(2):77–92 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Hackman DA, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13(2):65–73 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Cummings P. The relative merits of risk ratios and odds ratios. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(5):438–445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Pediatrics are provided here courtesy of American Academy of Pediatrics

RESOURCES