Abstract
Three theories offer different explanations to the understanding of male drink-driving. In order to test road safety education, deterrence, and social capital theories, logistic regression analysis was applied to predict respondents’ statements of having or not having engaged in actual drink-driving (DD). Variable for road safety education theory was whether a driver had graduated from a professional driving school or not. Deterrence theory was operationalized with a variable of whether a driver had been issued a traffic ticket or not. Social capital theory was operationalized with two variables, having children or not and having religion identification or not. Since both variables ‘years of formal education’ and ‘years of driving experience’ have been reported to be correlated to alcohol consumption and DD respectively, these were introduced as controls. In order to assess the significance of each variable statistically, Wald tests were applied in seven models. Results indicate on the one hand that road safety education variable is not statistically significant; and on the other, deterrence theory variable and social capital theory variable ‘having children’ were both statistically significant at the level of .01. Findings are discussed in reference to Chile’s context. Data were taken from the “Road Users Attitudes and Behaviors towards Traffic Safety” survey from the National Commission of Road Safety of the Government of Chile (2005). The sample size was reported to be 2,118 (N of male drivers was 396). This survey was representative of Chile’s Metropolitan Region road users' population.
INTRODUCTION
In industrial nations drink-driving (DD) has been comprehensively researched by disciplines such as transports engineering, epidemiology, psychology, economics and sociology [Chi et.al., 2010; Impinen et al., 2010; Clinard and Meier, 2008:274–276; Erke, 2009; Fiorentino, Berger and Ramirez, 2007; Shaffer et al., 2007; Winfree, Giever, and Maupin, 2007; Hingson and Winter, 2003; Williams, 2003; Ross, 1993; Arnett, 1990; Gusfield 1998]. Within this literature a common finding has been the association between DD and male drivers [Elvik et al., 2009; Leadbeater B J, Foran K, and Grove-White A, 2008; Schwartz J, 2008]. There are at least three theories which can offer different explanations to the understanding of what social circumstances may trigger or inhibit the association between DD and male drivers: road safety education, deterrence, and social capital theories.
Firstly, supporters of road safety education argue that individuals exposed to road safety education, i.e courses in primary or secondary levels and professional driving schools, learn knowledge which helps them to distinguish which road behaviors are either risky or safe [Montoro, 2011; McKenna, 2007]i. Specifically, Thomson et al. (1996) have proposed a set of principles where the link between road safety knowledge and road safety behavior can be met. In sociological terms, this would imply that processes of secondary socialization led by formal institutions such as primary or secondary schools, or professional driving schools should have a positive impact on road users’ safety behaviors.
According to Christie (2001) few studies have analyzed the association between road safety education and road users’ behaviors. Particularly, in the area of driving training, some studies suggest that this type of education is not significantly associated to road safety behaviors or road crashes [Elvik et al., 2009; Ker et al., 2005; Lund, Williams and Zador, 1986]. Even two studies state that road safety education can have negative effects on road safety [Morrison, Petticrew and Thomson, 2003; Elvik et al., 2009:787–789]. It is important to acknowledge, however, that individuals who register in professional driving schools do it because they aim at obtaining a driver’s license rather than preventing crashes [Elvik et al., 2009:793]. Therefore, negative or lack of association may not be regarded as a driving school’s inefficacy per se. A potential explanation to understand why professional driving school methods may not be effective, particularly when targeting male students, can be related to its inadequate approach to tackle masculinity in relation to driving, as Walker, Butland and Connell suggest (2001).
Secondly, deterrence theory argues that both formal threats of sanctions and sanctions themselves decrease risky behaviors [Leal, Barry and King 2007]. Specifically, these social structures operate as ‘constrain potential choices of would-be offenders’ [Pratt et al., 2009:370]. Application of this frame on DD has produced two contradictory types of findings. Some suggest that formal sanctions are effective on deterring and decreasing DD [Asbridge et al., 2004; Berger and Marelich, 1997]. Conversely other studies indicate that the association between formal threats and DD is weakened when variables representing informal threats are included into the analysis [Green, 1998; Tibbets 1997].
Thirdly, the concept of social capital generally refers to the assets that reside within social individuals’ relationships [Bebbington, 2009]. These assets are understood as constituting an important part of people’s identities, livelihood, interests and aspirations. This theory indicates that if social capital is strong at the individual level, risky behavior is likely to be inhibited [Coleman, 1990; Wright, Cullen and Miller, 2001]. If family relationships are one dimension of how social capital can be measured at the individual level, application of this theory states that certain family roles (marriage and having children) are significantly associated to the promotion of health behavior, which ultimately affects mortality [Umberson, 1987]. When religion is another dimension of how social capital is measured, findings indicate that individuals who belong to religious groups are less likely to engage in road risky behavior [O’Malley and Johnston, 1999; Nazif and López, 2006].
Grosso modo the last two theories agree on the importance of control as a means of inhibiting risky social actions. However, these approaches analytically differentiate two answers for the question of where control is socially situated in order to avoid DD. For the former, control is conceived to be in state institutions and its manifestation can be either formal threats of sanctions (i.e. public announcement of patrolling, patrolling, and threats of monetary fines, driver’s license suspension, and imprisonment, among others) or execution of sanctions (i.e incapacitation of driving by either license suspension or imprisonment). Whereas for the latter, control can be situated in certain type of families or organizations such as sports clubs or churches, among others, since individuals who belong to these groups are more likely to be inclined to consider whether their actions can impact their immediate social environments.
Both theories are ultimately complementary, because under certain social contexts these types of control can coexist and also reinforce each other when a given social norm is shared. Furthermore, a mechanism which both theories can also share is the feeling of shame of potential offenders [Braithwaite, 1989; Green, 1989; Tibbets 1997].
Since research on DD has been mainly carried out in industrial nations, and there has not been an attempt to test which of these three theories would seem to be most appropriate to the understanding of males and DD, the objective of this paper is twofold: Firstly, to advance in the area of the sociology of road safety by studying a setting (Chile’s Metropolitan Region) characterized by higher traffic fatality rates and weaker road safety policies when compared to advanced countries [Nazif, 2011]. Secondly, to enhance knowledge of the DD’s area, by testing, and contrasting road safety education, deterrence and social capital theories in a representative sample of male drivers from a Latin-American highly urbanized city.
METHODS
Data
Data were taken from the “Road Users Attitudes and Behavior towards Traffic Safety” Survey of the National Commission of Traffic Safety of the Government of Chile (2005). This data was gathered from a representative sample of Chile’s Metropolitan Region road user population aged 16 and over. Information was obtained from 2,118 respondents. Table 1 describes the main statistical variables of the sample.
Table 1.
Variables | Answer | N | % |
---|---|---|---|
Type of road user | Drivers | 549 | 25.1 |
Pedestrians or passengersii | 1569 | 74.9 | |
Gender | Male | 1053 | 48.1 |
Female | 1135 | 51.9 | |
Age | 16–29 | 704 | 32.2 |
30–49 | 867 | 39.6 | |
Over 49 | 617 | 28.2 | |
n = 2.118 |
The sub-sample of drivers (n= 549) had 137 females (24.9%) and 412 males (75.1%). A sub-sample composed of individuals who were male drivers and had responded to all questions regarding the variables analyzed here was taken in order to carry out the analysis (n = 396). In other words, there were 16 male drivers in the sub-sample of male drivers (n=412) who were not incorporated into the analysis because they had not responded to questions regarding road safety education, deterrence theory, social capital theory, years of education, years of driving experience and/or age.
Dependent Variable
In order to classify whether a male driver had engaged in DD, statements from the question ‘how many days per week do you drive after having drunk alcohol?’ were coded into two categories: ‘never’ as (0), and ‘only once per week’, ‘twice per week’ and ‘more than twice per week’ as (1). Rationale of this operationalization is as follows: a male who drinks alcohol and decides to drive in this condition once per week or more is nonetheless a source of road risk. Therefore in order to capture this road risky behavior properly, categories of having DD were collapsed into only one group. Table 1 summarizes descriptive statistics of the dependent variable ‘DD’.
Independent and Control Variables
Independent and control variables are operationalized to test each theory previously discussed.
1. Road safety education theory (RSET)
In order to classify whether a male driver had graduated from a professional driving school or not, statements from the question ‘where did you learn to drive?’ were coded into two categories: ‘parents’, ‘another relative’, ‘friend’, ‘autodidact’ and ‘other’ as (0) and ‘professional driving school’ as (1).
2. Deterrence theory (DT)
In order to classify whether a male driver had been subject of effective traffic controls or not, a variable from the question ‘have you ever been issued a traffic ticket?’ was operationalized. Statements from this question were coded into two categories: ‘no’ as (0), and ‘yes’ as (1).
3. Social capital theory (SCT)
SCT was operationalized with two dichotomous variables:
SCTi: In order to classify whether a male driver had children or not, statements from the question ‘do you have children?’ were coded into two categories: ‘no’ as (0), and ‘yes’ as (1).
SCTii: In order to classify whether a male driver had any religious identification or not, statements from the question ‘which of the following religions do you identify with?’ were coded into two categories: ‘without religion’ as (0), and ‘Catholic’, ‘Protestant’, ‘Jehovah’s Witnesses’ and ‘Other religion’ as (1).
4. Years of formal education (YFE control variable)
The variable YFE was introduced as control because it has been positively associated to alcohol consumption (Ministry of Health of Chile, 2010). Years of education come from the question ‘what is your highest level of education’.
5. Years of driving experience (YDE control variable)
Since the variable YDE has been reported to be a strong predictor in the literature (Elvik et al., 2009; Williamson, 2003), this was introduced as control. Information to build this variable was taken firstly from the question ‘When did you obtain your driver's license?’. Secondly, in order to know specifically how many years of driving experience a person had, values from this question were subtracted from the year when the survey was taken (2005). Table 2 summarizes descriptive statistics of independent and control variables.
Table 2.
Dependent variable | Answers | % |
---|---|---|
DD (How many days per week do you drive after having drunk alcohol?) | (0) Never | 77.0 |
(1) More than once | 23.0 | |
n = 396 |
Statistical method
Since the dependent variable was dichotomous, a logistic regression technique was applied.
The logistic regression models to test each theory were as follow:
(1) |
(2) |
(3) |
Where p is the probability of a male driver having engaged in DD; α the intercept; βxi the slope parameters of the independent and control variables (coefficients); RSET i represents a variable of road safety education theory which measures whether a male driver had graduated from a professional driving school or not; DT is the variable representing deterrence theory, which measures whether a male driver had been issued a traffic ticket or not; SCTi represents a variable of social capital theory which measures whether a male driver had children or not; SCTii represents a social capital variable which measures whether a male driver had religion identification or not; YFE represents a control variable which measures years of formal education and YDE is the control variable which measures how many years of driving experience a male driver had.
These three first models are set to test whether each independent variable representing each theory is associated to DD. In a second stage other models will be run in order to test the statistical significance of each variable while introducing variables from other competing theories. The control variables YFE and YDE however will be introduced in every model. Results are reported as coefficient exponents (odds ratio) and standard errors (s.e.) of the parameters. X2 and Wald test are applied to test the statistical significance of the models and coefficients respectively. Statistical significance was established at three different values p< .05, .01 and .001.
RESULTS
Results of the three models are presented in table 3. In this table, we firstly observe that models 1, 2 and 3 are statistically significant. In other words, the group of variables introduced into the models is statistically associated to the distribution of the odds ratio of variable DD, since their p values are lower than 0.001. In order to understand whether each variable is significantly associated to DD each model needs to be assessed independently.
Table 3.
Variable | Answers | % | Mean | S.D. |
---|---|---|---|---|
RSET | (0) No | 84.3 | ||
(1) Yes | 15.7 | |||
DT (had been issued a traffic ticket) | (0) No | 28.0 | ||
(1) Yes | 72.0 | |||
SCTi (having children) | (0) No | 26.3 | ||
(1) Yes | 73.7 | |||
SCTii (having religion identification) | (0) No | 19.9 | ||
(1) Yes | 81.1 | |||
YFE (control) | 13 | 2.405 | ||
YDE (control) | 20.21 | 12.762 | ||
n = 396 |
In model 1 we observe that RSET’s positive direction would indicate that a male driver who has graduated from a professional driving school is more likely to engage in DD than a male driver who learnt to drive under an informal structure, however this association is not significant. Secondly, YFE is positively and significantly associated to DD, which would suggest that the more years of education a male driver has, the chances of engaging in DD increase. Thirdly, when analyzing the second control variable YDE we observe that it is statistically significant, therefore values of this variable are associated to the odds ratio distribution of engaging in DD. Specifically, an increase of years in driving experience is associated to a decrease of the odds ratio of engaging on DD.
In model 2 we observe that DT is statistically significant, however, unlike what is commonly assumed the direction of the parameter is positive. This suggests that a male driver who has not been issued a traffic fine is less likely to engage in DD than a male driver who has been issued a traffic fine. Secondly, both control variables are statistically significant, and therefore model 1’s interpretation is also valid for mode 2.
We observe in model 3 that both variables SCTi and SCTii are negatively associated to DD. This means that a male driver who had children or identifies with a religious group is less likely to be associated to DD than a male driver without children or without religious identification. However, when assessing the statistical significance of both variables, only SCTi proves to be significant since its p value is < .001. We observe that the introduction of both SCT’s variables has ultimately decreased the statistical significance of the control variables YFE and YDE.
Four more models are going to be run in order to identify whether or not associations to DD remain statistically significant when a variable from a competing theory is introduced. The models are as follow:
(4) |
(5) |
(6) |
(7) |
Models 4, 5 and 6 are designed to compare only two theories among each other (RSET vs. DT; RSET vs. SCT; and DT vs. SCT). Model 7 is designed to compare these three theories (RSET vs. DT vs. SCT). This method allows seeing which model better fits the distribution of odds ratio of DD for the sub-sample analyzed, and which variables could be statistically associated to DD.
As we can observe in table 4 all the models are statistically significant. Model 7 fits the data better since its X2 has the highest value. However, this finding must be taken rather cautiously. Even though the introduction of RSET helped to increase this value to 35.854, its individual association to the odds ratio of DD is not statistically significant and the fit model measure by the Pseudo R2 Nagelkerke remains the same when compared to model 6.
Table 4.
Models | |||
---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | |
RSET | 1.132 (.324) | ----- | ----- |
DT | ----- | 2.740** (.314) | ----- |
SCTi | ----- | ----- | −.377** (.298) |
SCTii | ----- | ----- | −.656 (296) |
YFE (control) | 1.117* (.055) | 1.116* (.055) | 1.053 (.058) |
YDE (control) | −.971** (.011) | −.959*** (.011) | −.991 (.012) |
Constant | .120** (.759) | −.072*** (.789) | −.485 (.870) |
n=396 | |||
L(0) | 426.914 | 426.914 | 426.914 |
L(θ) | 412.642 | 402.152 | 400.322 |
X2 | 14.271*** | 24.762*** | 26.592*** |
Pseudo R2 Nagelkerke | .054 | .092 | .098 |
Coeficientes exp (B) and s.e between brackets. Level of significance
p < .05 /
p < .01 /
p < .001.
In model 4 we observe that variables RSET and DT are positive. This implies that a male driver who either had not graduated from a professional driving school or had not been issued a traffic ticket is less likely to engage in DD, than a male driver who had graduated from a professional driving school and had been issued a traffic ticket. However, between these two variables only DT is statistically significant associated to the odds ratio of DD. This association remains significant even with the introduction of two control variables which are also statistically significant. Specifically, both a decrease of YFE and an increase on YDE are associated to a decrease of the odds ratio of engaging on DD.
In model 5 we observe that RSET is not statistically significant and therefore its association to DD remains at best unknown. SCTi is, on the other hand, statistically significant, and therefore, male drivers who had children are less likely to engage in DD than male drivers who did not. Comparable to model 3, control variables YFE and YDE are not statistically associated to DD, and therefore these factors need to be reconsidered when analyzing this type of risky behavior. In other words, having introduced variable SCTi in the model implies that the statistical power of YFE and YDE has decreased significantly.
When variables are introduced in model 6 we observe that both DT and SCTi are statistically significant. Control variables YFE and YDE are not statistically associated to DD since its p value is > 0.05. Like models 2 and 4, variable DT has a positive direction and therefore, a male driver who has been issued a traffic fine is more likely to engage in DD than a male driver who has not been issued a ticket. SCTi behaves like models 3 and 5. Since its direction is negative the same interpretation for previous models can be applied i.e. that male drivers who have children are less likely to engage in DD than those who do not have children.
In model 7 we can appreciate whether each of these three variables are statistically significant when associated to DD. Like model 6 DT and SCTi, both are significantly associated to DD. In other words, male drivers who had been issued a traffic ticket are more likely to engage in DD, than those male drivers who had not been issued a traffic ticket. Secondly, male drivers who have children are less likely to engage in DD than male drivers who do not have children. Regarding variable RSET we also see that its introduction is not statistically associated to DD since its p is > .05. Ultimately, control variables YFE and YDE are not significant, and therefore their statistical power is not appropriate to either reject or confirm the distribution of DD’s odds ratio in this sub-sample.
Lastly, figure 1 allows us to see how the Pseudo R2 (Nagelkerke) of each model evolves once variables of each theory here discussed, were introduced. As expected, this value improves when a new variable is added. However, in sociological terms, only DT and SCT seem to be more appropriate to the understanding of DD. In other words, even though model 7 is built with three variables of each sociological theory, while controlling by YFE and YDE, it is model 6 which seems to describe more accurately the association of DT and SCT with the odds ratio distribution of DD. In sum these two variables remain statistically significant and the Pseudo R2 value of the model does not change when compared to model 7.
DISCUSSION
After testing RSET, DT and SCT independently (models 1 to 3), findings indicate, on the one hand, that while variables RSET and SCTii are not statistically associated to the odds ratio of DD, variables DT and SCTi are. On the other, control variables are statistically significant when RSET and DT are tested, but not with SCTi. Then when the three theories were compared to each other (models 4 to 7), the main finding indicates that both DT and SCTi are the only variables which remain significantly associated to the odds ratio of DD. I proceed now to discuss each of these findings by considering key elements of the Chilean society.
Firstly, since RSET is not statistically associated to DD, and like most literature on road safety education suggests, this finding reinforces the notion that an association between road safety knowledge and road safety behavior is hard to establish. This interpretation moreover would need to be taken cautiously because three elements should be at least researched further: firstly, whether Chilean professional driving schools explicitly teach the risks associated to DD. Secondly, whether or not male drivers who have graduated from professional driving schools behave significantly different (while controlling for significant variables) than females who have also graduated from professional driving school. Thirdly, as Walker, Butland and Connell (2001) suggest whether professional driving schools develop curricula and approaches which tackle the association between risky driving and the social identity of masculinity. In sum in order to either reject or accept hypotheses regarding the association between professional driving schools and DD in Chile more research is necessary.
Even though other studies suggest that religion is likely to be associated to specific road safety attitudes and behavior [O’Malley and Jonhston, 1999; Nazif and López, 2006; Factor, Mahalel and Yair, 2008], in this study religion (as a proxy of social capital) and DD is at best unknown. In order to consider more attentively the relation between religion identification and DD, studies would have to introduce aspects which come from the sociology of religion. Specifically, by studying how much an individual is involved in a religious group and how religious practices influence alcohol consumption. For instance, in the case of Chile it would be required to see how restriction of alcohol consumption is an essential part of the social construction of masculinities in orthodox evangelical groups [Montecinos, 2002; Mansilla 2007].
It is noteworthy that the introduction of SCTi cancelled out the effects of both control variables. In theoretical and methodological terms, this suggests that sociological variables need to be fully integrated in road safety analyses because it allows us to expand our understanding of this phenomenon. Specifically, since SCTi had more statistical power than years of driving experience -a traditional variable of transport engineering- analyses of DD will benefit enormously if sociological approaches complement engineering perspectives. The finding associated to variable YFE is particularly interesting for the case of Chile. When SCTi was not part of the models, this variable had a very similar performance, as it was reported in the literature, i.e. an increase in level of education was positively associated to an increase in alcohol consumption. However, when SCTi is introduced YEF is no longer significantly associated to DD. This finding has two important implications. Firstly, DD needs to be investigated on its own merit because male drivers constitute a group with their own social characteristics, which for instance go beyond their inner difference in terms of level of education. Secondly, alcohol consumption might also be associated to social capital and therefore assessment of when and how much males drink alcohol should be reassessed; this might ultimately affect the effect of level of education.
When analyzing DT we observe that it has a positive and significant direction on the odds ratio of DD. This suggests that a male driver who had not been issued a traffic ticket has fewer chances of engaging in DD than a male driver who had been issued one. This also suggests that, a driver who had been issued a traffic ticket is less likely to declare that he has never DD, than a male who had not been issued one. Before analyzing one important methodological issue of this finding, I will discuss this association by describing the Chilean context of police enforcement and traffic law regarding DD.
Firstly, Frühling observes that Chilean police - Carabineros de Chile- is perceived as one of the most trustworthy police institutions in Latin America; in fact the probability of negotiating with a Chilean police officer to get out of a ticket was found to be the second lowest among 18 countries of the region (2009:234). Secondly, when Carabineros’ traffic control performance is assessed, more than 60% of the population believe it is good or very good [Interior Office and National Institute of Statistics, 2003]. Regarding DD it is also important to consider how Chile’s traffic law sanctions this behavior. Firstly, the legal status of driving while intoxicated (Blood Alcohol Concentration (BAC) ≥ .10) is a crime, and therefore sanctions may include imprisonment, costly fines and driver’s license suspension. Secondly, the legal status of driving under the influence of alcohol (.05 ≤ BAC < .10) is a misdemeanor, and therefore, sanctions may include fines and driver's license suspension. According to this context two complementary hypotheses could explain the association between DD and DT. Firstly, one group of male drivers is not likely engaging in DD because being controlled—which is expressed in effective policing—along with the nature of punishment—which is the sanctions established in Chile’s traffic law—might be perceived to be both high and effective, and therefore this group is deterred from engaging in DD. However, there is a group of male drivers who is not likely to be deterred, even though they had been effectively ticketed. One potential explanation for this association can be found in the Problem-Theory Behavior (PBT). PBT states that three major systems: the perceived-environment system, the personality system, and the behavior system, serve either as instigations for engaging in problem behavior or controls against involvement in problem behavior (Jessor, Donovan and Costa, 1991). If having been fined (i.e the perceived-environment system) is not deterring DD (i.e. behavior system), we could therefore assume that a disruption at the personality system of the offenders may be occurring.
However, an important methodological limitation of this finding is how DT has been operationalized. Thus, in order to advance more precisely in the understanding between control and male’s DD in Chile further research should include elements such as whether an individual had been subject to alcohol controls, what the result of the alcohol test was, had been issued a traffic fine due to DD, his attitudes towards the legal graduation of the BAC limits, his perception of how intense traffic patrolling had been and information regarding their personality.
Regarding SCTi we observe that male drivers who have children are less likely to engage in DD than male drivers without children. In order to understand the association between this indicator of social capital and DD, I suggest two hypotheses which complement each other. Firstly, engaging in DD has at least two types of effects which can be analytically distinguished i) road crashes with fatal or severe injury consequences, and ii) legal sanctions (imprisonment, suspension of driver's license, monetary fines, among others), male drivers with children perceive that both consequences have also unintended effects on their children’s development. In other words, neither fatal, severe injuries nor the legal graduation of engaging in DD is taken unambiguously by a male driver with children because both not only can affect his own well-being and/or his driving record, but also his social capital. Specifically, road crash consequences and legal sanctions might bring a variety of side effects whereby restriction of household incomes (in case of long term disability and/or high monetary sanction [Ritchey and Nicholson-Crotty, 2011; Pérez-Nuñez et al. 2011; van Beeck, van Roijen and Mackenbach, 1997]) or absence of the father in the household (in case of death or imprisonment [Wildeman, 2010; Murray and Farrington, 2008]) also impact on how the children are educated and how the relationship father-child is weakened.
Lastly, Chile’s evidence indicates that self-perception of lack of time is higher in men with children than men without children [Valenzuela and Herrera, 2006:283]. If time limitations impact the behavior of these two groups differently, then engagement in DD can also be associated to this constraint. In this case the second hypothesis is: perception of having less time in male drivers with children limits heavily their willingness to engage in DD since many other activities may be regarded as more important. However, in order to assess whether this factor, in reference to the first hypothesis, is interactive or not, more research would be required.
CONCLUSION
In this study I tested and contrasted three different theories in order to understand why male drivers engage in DD. Seven models of logistic regression were run and findings indicate that only variables from DT and SCT are significantly associated to DD. Evidence for RSET suggests that either a radical change in how RSE is designed needs to take place in order to be effective, or better methodologies should be applied in order to capture its actual impact. A finding indicates that ticketing seems to be effective in deterring a specific group of male drivers, however deficient to deter other. However, since the variable applied is insufficient in measuring police control, new information, including the perception of legal sanctions of DD, frequency of police patrolling, actual results of blood-alcohol concentration in drivers and personality of the individuals should be added. The introduction of one indicator of social capital allowed us to see that one constraint of male drivers for engaging in DD is whether they are or not parents. Male driver mechanisms of self-regulation can be associated to foreseen side effects of road crashes consequences and legal sanctions on children and the relationship father-child.
Since ground transportation is greatly affected by social conditions of the individuals, more sociological research would be necessary to identify and explain some of these characteristics. For instance by studying which social conditions and social mechanisms trigger DD, we may shed light on whether a group of male drivers is structural to the functioning of society or is an aggregated sum of individuals who engage in DD when certain opportunities are present.
Lastly, these findings suggest at least two countermeasures. Firstly, a parallel increase of both controlling and sanctioning should be implemented. Ultimately, only a combination of these two strategies can tackle a male group who seem resilient to driving safely. Specifically, lowering the legal BAC limit from 0.1 to .05 g/100m, and changing this offence from regulatory to criminal and increasing the potential sanctions for engaging in DD should be studied carefully. Secondly, public campaigns, which address the challenge of DD, should also consider social capital elements of the population in order to disseminate messages more effectively. In other words, public campaign messages of DD need to appeal to male drivers who do not have children, because this group of drivers is more likely to engage in DD than those who have children.
Table 5.
Models | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | |
RSET | 1.059 (.332) | 1.092 (.330) | ----- | 1.026 (.340) |
DT | 2.730 *** (.315) | ----- | 2.779*** (.322) | 2.775*** (.322) |
SCTi | ----- | −.371 ** (.297) | −.366*** (.303) | −.366*** (.304) |
YFE (control) | 1.116* (.055) | 1.067 (.058) | 1.064 (.088) | 1.064 (.088) |
YDE (control) | −.960 *** (.011) | −.988 (.012) | −.977 (.012) | −.977 (.013) |
Constant | −.071 ** (.792) | −.310 (.821) | −.189 (.846) | −.188 (.850) |
n=396 | ||||
L(0) | 426.914 | 426.914 | 426.914 | 426.914 |
L(θ) | 402.122 | 402.234 | 391.065 | 391.059 |
X2 | 24.791*** | 24.680*** | 35.849*** | 35.854*** |
Pseudo R2 Nagelkerke | .091 | .092 | .131 | .131 |
Coeficientes exp (B) and s.e between brackets. Level of significance
p < 0.05 /
p < 0.01 /
p < 0.001.
Acknowledgments
I would like to thank the National Commission of Road Safety of the Government of Chile for having facilitated the data set of the “Road Users Attitudes and Behaviors towards Traffic Safety” survey. Christine Wickens and two anonymous reviewers provided extraordinarily helpful comments on earlier versions of this paper. I am responsible for the mistakes that remain.
Footnotes
Chris suggests (2001) to analytically distinguish between road safety education and driver training, since both systems have different methods. While I agree with this distinction in this paper both concepts are used synonymously since both systems aim at improving road users’ safety behaviours.
Respondents could identify themselves as pedestrians and/or passengers.
REFERENCES
- Arnett J. Drunk driving, sensation seeking, and egocentrism among adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences. 1990;11(6):541–546. [Google Scholar]
- Asbridge M, Mann R, Flam-Zalcman R, Stoduto G. The Criminalization of Impaired Driving in Canada: Assessing the Deterrent Impact of Canada's First Per Se Law. J Stud. Alcohol. 2004;65:450–459. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2004.65.450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bebbington A. Social Capital International Encyclopedia of Human Geography. Elsevier Ltd; 2009. pp. 165–170. [Google Scholar]
- Berger DE, Marelich WD. Legal and Social Control of Alcohol-Impaired Driving in California: 1983–1994. J Stud. Alcohol. 1997 Sept.58(5):518–523. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1997.58.518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braithwaite J. Crime, Shame and Reintegration. Cambridge University Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Clinard MB, Meier RF. Sociology of Deviant Behavior. New York, Holt: Rinehart and Winston; [1968] 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Chi G, Zhou X, McClure TE, Gilbert PA, Cosby AG, Zhang L, Robertson AA, Levinson D. Gasoline prices and their relationship to drunk-driving crashes. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 2010 Aug;27(4):523–549. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2010.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christie R. The effectiveness of driver training as a road safety measure: a review of the literature RACV. Noble Park, Victoria: 2001. Literature Report No 01/03. [Google Scholar]
- Coleman J. Foundations of Social Theory. Cambridge/London: The Belnap Press of Harvard University Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- De Boni R, Leukefeld C, Pechansky F. Young people’s blood alcohol concentration and the alcohol consumption city law, Brazil. Rev. Saúde Pública. 2008 Dec;42(6):1101–1104. doi: 10.1590/s0034-89102008005000054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elvik R. Why some road safety problems are more difficult to solve than others. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 2010 Jul;42(4):1089–1096. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2009.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elvik R, Hoye A, Vaa T, Sorensen M. The Handbook of Road Safety Measures. Emerald Group Publishing Limited; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Erke A, Goldenbeld C, Vaa T. The effects of drink-driving checkpoints on crashes—A meta-analysis. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 2009 Aug;41(5):523–549. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2009.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Factor L, Mahalel D, Gad Y. Inter-group differences in road-traffic crash involvement. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 2008 Nov;40(6):349–357. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2008.08.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiorentino D, Berger D, Ramirez J. Drinking driving among high-risk young Mexican-American men. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 2007 Aug.39(1):16–21. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2006.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frühling H. Violencia y Policía en América Latina. FLACSO; Quito, Ecuador: 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Green D. Measures of Illegal Behavior in Individual-Level Deterrence Research. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 1989 Aug;26(3):253–275. [Google Scholar]
- Gusfield J. The Control of Drinking-Driving in the United States. A Period in Transition? In: Laurcence M, Snortun J, Zimring F, editors. Social Control of Drinking and Driving. The University of Chicago Press; 1988. pp. 109–135. [Google Scholar]
- Hingson R, Winter M. Epidemiology and consequences of drinking and driving. Alcohol Res Health. 2003;27(1):63–78. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Impinen A, Mäkelä P, Karjalainen K, Rahkonen O, Lintonen T, Lillsunde P, Ostamo A. High mortality among people suspected of drunk-driving. An 18-year register-based follow-up. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2010 Jul.110(1–2):80–84. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Interior Office and National Institute of Statistics . National Survey on Public Safety. Government of Chile; Santiago: 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Jessor R, Donovan J, Costa FM. Beyond adolescence: Problem behavior and young adult development. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Ker K, Roberts I, Collier T, Beyer F, Bunn F, Frost C. Post-licence driver education for the prevention of road traffic crashes: a systematic review of randomised controlled trials. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 2005 Mar.37(2):305–313. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2004.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lourens PF, Vissers JJ, Jessurun M. Annual mileage, driving violations, and accident involvement in relation to drivers’ sex, age, and level of education. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 1999 Sept.31(5):593–597. doi: 10.1016/s0001-4575(99)00015-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leadbeater BJ, Foran K, Grove-White A. How much can you drink before driving? The influence of riding with impaired adults and peers on the driving behaviors of urban and rural youth. Addiction. 2008 Apr.103(4):629–637. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02139.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leal NL, Watson, Barry WC, King M. Hooning offenders and offences: Who and what are we dealing with?. Australian Road Safety Research, Policing and Education Conference; 17–19 October; Melbourne, Australia. 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Lund AK, Williams AF, Zador P. High school driver education: Further evaluation of the Dekalb County study. Accident Analysis and Prevention. 1986 Aug;18(4):349–357. doi: 10.1016/0001-4575(86)90048-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mansilla M. Phobia to Canutos in Chile: The socio-cultural factors of evangelists discrimination. Ciencias Sociales Online. 2007 Mar.IV(1):48–61. Paper in Spanish. http://www.uvm.cl/csonline/2007_1/pdf/canutofobia.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- McKenna FP. The Perceived Legitimacy of intervention. A Key Feature for Road Safety. Prepared for the American Automobile Association Foundation for Traffic Safety; 2007. http://www.aaafoundation.org/pdf/McKenna.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Ministry of Health . Encuesta Nacional Salud Chile 2009–2010. Government of Chile; http://www.redsalud.gov.cl/portal/url/item/99c12b89738d80d5e04001011e0113f8.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Montecinos S. A new type of feminity and masculinity:looking at gender in the evangelicalworld of la pintana. Estudios Públicos translated into English by John Bell Nº87. 2002:1–31. [Google Scholar]
- Montoro L. Road safety education and drivers behavior. European Magazine for Traffic. Transport and Road Safety. 2011;3(8):3–4. [Google Scholar]
- Morrison DS, Petticrew M, Thomson H. What are the most effective ways of improving population health through transport interventions? Evidence from systematic reviews. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57:327–333. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.5.327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray J, Farrington D. Parental imprisonment: Long-lasting effects on boys’ internalizing problems through the life course. Development and Psychopathology. 2008;20:273–290. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nazif JI, López A. Why do they avoid pedestrian bridges? Explaining jaywalkers trough the Haddon matrix. Ciencias Sociales Online. 2006 Jul.III(2):71–86. Paper in Spanish http://www.uvm.cl/csonline/2006_2/pdf/nazif.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Nazif JI. UN-Economic Commission for Latin America and the Caribbean Natural Resources and Infrastructure Series. Chile United Nations Publication; 2011. Development of Road Safety in Latin America and the Caribbean: Towards a “Nunca más / Nunca mais / Never again” Road Safety Vision. Forthcoming. [Google Scholar]
- O’Malley PM, Jonhston LD. Drinking and Driving Among US High School Seniors, 1984–1997. American Journal of Public Health. 1999;89(5):678–684. doi: 10.2105/ajph.89.5.678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pérez-Núñez R, Ávila-Burgos L, Híjar-Medinas M, Pelcastre-Villafuerte B, Celis A, Salinas-Rodríguez A. A Economic impact of fatal and non-fatal road traffic injuries in Guadalajara Metropolitan Area and Jalisco, Mexico. Inj Prev. 2011 Apr 12; doi: 10.1136/ip.2010.027995. ip.2010.027995 Published Online First: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pratt T, Cullen F, Blevins K, Daigle L, Madensen T. The empirical status of deterrence theory: A Meta-Analysis. In: Cullen F, Wright J, Blevins K, editors. Taking Stock: The Status of Criminological Theory. Transaction Publishers; 2009. pp. 367–395. [Google Scholar]
- Ritchey M, Nicholson-Crotty S. Deterrence Theory and the Implementation of Speed Limits in the American States. The Policy Studies Journal. 2011;39(2):329–346. [Google Scholar]
- Ross HL. Confronting Drunk Driving: Social Policy for Saving Lives. Yale University Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Ross CE, Wu C. The Links Between Education and Health. American Sociological Review. 1995 Oct.60(5):719–745. [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz J. Gender differences in drunk driving prevalence rates and trends: A 20-year assessment using multiple sources of evidence. Addictive Behaviors. 2008 Sept.33(9):1217–1222. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaffer H, Nelson SE, LaPlante D, LaBrie R, Albanese M, Caro G. The Epidemiology of Psychiatric Disorders Among Repeat DUI Offenders Accepting a Treatment-Sentencing Option. J of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2007;75(5):795–804. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.5.795. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tibbets S. Shame and Rational Choice in Offending Decisions. Criminal Justice and Behavior. 1997;24(2):234–255. [Google Scholar]
- Thomson JA, Tolmie A, Foot HC, McLaren B. Child Development and the Aims of Road Safety Education: A Review and Analysis. London: Department of Transport; 1996. Road Safety Research Report No.1. [Google Scholar]
- Umberson D. Family status and health behaviors: social control as a dimension of social integration. J Health Soc Behav. 1987 Sep;28(3):306–319. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valenzuela E, Herrera MS. Tiempo, trabajo y familia. In: Valenzuela JM, Tironi E, Scully TR, editors. El Eslabón Perdido. Santiago, Chile: 2006. pp. 265–286. Taurus. [Google Scholar]
- van Beeck EF, van Roijen L, Mackenbach JP. Medical Costs and Economic Production Losses due to Injuries in the Netherlands. J of Trauma-Injury Infection & Critical Care. 1997 Jun.42(6):1116–1123. doi: 10.1097/00005373-199706000-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wildeman C. Paternal Incarceration and Children's Physically Aggressive Behaviors: Evidence from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study. Social Forces. 2010 Sep;89(1):285–309. [Google Scholar]
- Williams AF. Patterns of risk and the teenage driver. J Safety Research. 2003;34(1):5–15. doi: 10.1016/s0022-4375(02)00075-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams AF. Young driver risk factors: successful and unsuccessful approaches for dealing with them and an agenda for the future. Injury Prevention. 2006;12(Suppl I):i4–i8. doi: 10.1136/ip.2006.011783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williamson A. Update of literature review: literature 2000 to 2003” NSW Injury Risk, Management Research Center. University of New South Wales; 2003. Why are young drivers over represented in crahses? Summary of the issues. http://www.maa.nsw.gov.au/pdfs/-young_divers_crashes.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Winfree L, Jr, Giever D, Maupin J. Midwest Drunk Driving and the Prediction of Analogous Behavior: A Longitudinal Test of Social Learning and Self-Controlled Theories. Victims & Offenders. 2007;2(4):327–349. [Google Scholar]
- Wright P, Cullen F, Miller J. Family social capital and delinquent involvement. J of Criminal Justice. 2001 Jan-Feb;29(1):1–9. [Google Scholar]
- Walker L, Butland D, Connell RW. Boys on the Road: Masculinities, Car Culture, and Road Safety Education. J of Men’s Studies. 2000;8(2):153–69. [Google Scholar]