Abstract
Behavioural and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) are present in the course of the illness in up to 90% of patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD). They are the main source of caregiver burden and one of the major factors contributing to early institutionalization. The involvement of a genetic component in BPSD aetiology seems beyond controversy, though the exact significance of particular polymorphisms is uncertain in the majority of cases. Multiple genes have been assessed for their putative influence on BPSD risk. In this paper we review the behavioural genetics of AD, particularly the importance, with respect to BPSD risk, of genes coding for apolipoprotein E and proteins involved in the process of neurotransmission: serotonin receptors, serotonin transporter, COMT, MAO-A, tryptophan hydroxylase and dopamine receptors. A general conclusion is the striking inconsistency of the findings, unsurprising in the field of psychiatric genetics. The potential reasons for such discrepancy are exhaustively discussed.
Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, behavioural disturbances, behavioural and psychological symptoms of dementia, genetics, polymorphisms
Introduction
Dementing disorders can usually be characterized by impairments in cognition and behaviour with resulting subsequent decline in activities of daily living (ADL). In the more advanced stages cognitive function disturbances are usually accompanied by mood disorders, anxiety, apathy, dysphoria, psychotic symptoms (delusions, hallucinations), aggression or agitation. These symptoms, alongside other behavioural disturbances (wandering, inappropriate sexual behaviours), are often clustered together as behavioural and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD). In the course of the illness BPSD can be present in as many as 60-98% of demented individuals, with an average of around 80% in subjects with Alzheimer's disease (AD) [1]. The presence of BPSD, as an exponent of the more aggressive disease course, is usually followed by particularly significant prognostic implications: a more rapid decline in both cognitive functions and ADL [2], increased mortality [3] as well as early institutionalization of individuals with AD [4]. The dramatic functional decline of the patients leading to autonomy loss and rapidly progressive caregiver dependence is much more a consequence of peracute psychiatric pathology rather than cognitive impairment per se. The presence of BPSD and consequently the patient’s lack of self-containment are also the main sources of caregiver burden and major contributors to the increased prevalence of depression in this population [5]. In the context of the clinical, economical and social significance of BPSD, discovering mechanisms implicated in their pathogenesis is among the top-priority challenges of modern old-age psychiatry. However, our knowledge on the aetiology of these symptoms is still incomplete. Behavioural and psychological symptoms of dementia have been hypothesized to be caused by unmet needs (for example, needs for social contact, activity, relief of pain, or hunger), by the inability of the patient to communicate those needs, and by the inability of the environment to meet those needs [6]. Biological theories have focused on the role of various neurotransmitter systems, including serotonin [5-T] (aggression, depression, anxiety, psychosis) [7], dopamine [DA] (aggression, psychosis) [8], norepinephrine [NA] (aggression) [9], acetylcholine [ACh] (psychosis, apathy) [10] and γ-aminobutyric acid [GABA] [11]. Neurochemical BPSD theories have led to pharmacological treatments targeting specific neurotransmitters [12]. A wealth of evidence (although conflicting at times) associates behavioural symptoms with the progression of neuropathological changes [13], abnormalities seen in neuroimaging procedures [14], or the results of electrophysiological studies [15]. Furthermore, there are accruing data on the association between genetic factors and behavioural abnormalities in AD. In this paper we review the available evidence on the behavioural genetics of AD.
Behavioural genetics of Alzheimer’s disease
One of the most popular hypotheses regarding genetic risk factors states that common complex disorders, besides the small proportion of familial cases with known causative mutations, are governed by DNA variants prevalent in the general population [16]. These variants, such as single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), tend to increase disease risk but are insufficient to actually cause a specific disorder. Unfortunately, a typical feature of gene polymorphisms is their relatively weak effect. It is therefore quite common to be confused by conflicting results of published reports, some of them demonstrating risk-conferring or protective properties of a particular allele, others showing no effect whatsoever. To overcome the potential sources of bias discussed below, a meta-analytic approach has been proposed. Single nucleotide polymorphisms might not only affect the risk of developing the disease, they may also have an impact on particular disease phenotypes or treatment results. The involvement of a genetic component in BPSD aetiology seems beyond controversy, though the exact significance of particular polymorphisms is uncertain in the majority of cases. Multiple genes, coding for proteins involved in various neurotransmitter systems, have been assessed for their putative influence on BPSD risk. Due to significant clinical and aetiological diversity of symptoms typically ascribed to the BPSD construct, in research it is usually broken down into isolated behavioural symptoms or behavioural clusters (e.g. psychosis, apathy, disinhibition, etc.). In the following paragraphs the results of studies assessing the putative contribution of genetic variants to BPSD aetiology in AD will be reviewed.
Search strategy
PubMed and other databases accessible at our University (Blackwell Synergy, EbscoHost, Karger, Ovid, Proquest and Science Direct) were browsed through using multiple queries built from words such as Alzheimer, polymorphism, gene, behaviour/behavioural, BPSD, and specific behavioural symptoms, for example psychosis, delusions, hallucinations, aggression, depression, anxiety, etc., used in various combinations. Query results were examined in the search for relevant papers on BPSD genetic background. The selected papers were then manually reviewed to look for additional references.
Apolipoprotein E
The apolipoprotein E (ApoE) locus is located on the long arm of chromosome 19. The three most common SNPs in the APOE gene lead to changes in the coding sequence and result in three common isoforms of apoE: apoE2 (cys112, cys158), apoE3 (cys112, arg158), and apoE4 (arg112, arg158); although they differ by single amino acids, these differences profoundly alter apoE structure and function. The apolipoprotein E plays a key role in lipoprotein metabolism and cholesterol transport in plasma and the nervous system. To date, it is the only unanimously accepted genetic risk factor for the development of sporadic AD. It has been documented that harbouring the ε4 allele dose-dependently increases the risk of developing AD; it is also associated with an earlier age at onset – subjects homozygous for ApoE ε4 almost always develop AD by the age of 80 [17]. ApoE is a molecule implicated in all the biochemical disturbances characteristic of AD: β-amyloid (Aβ) aggregation, deposition and clearance, neurofibrillary tangle (NFT) formation, neurotoxicity, neuroinflammation, loss of synaptic plasticity, and cholinergic dysfunction [18].
The genetic significance of ApoE inspired research on its potential association with the psychiatric manifestations of AD – psychosis, depression, aggression, anxiety. We have identified 37 studies evaluating this concept [19-55]. The results of these studies are summarized in Table I. The initial conclusion that could be drawn is the striking inconsistency of the findings; yet this is unsurprising in the field of psychiatric genetics. Regardless of the BPSD symptoms studied, the ε4 allele has been found to increase the risk for a given behavioural pathology by some authors (with an obvious notion that it might somehow be involved in the pathogenesis of the symptoms), while others found the ApoE genotype insignificant in this regard. In a recent study by our group performed in a population of carefully selected AD subjects (very stringent inclusion and exclusion criteria to increase the homogeneity of the participants) ApoE ε4 carriers had a 7-fold increased risk for exhibiting delusions and 4.5-fold increased risk for agitation/aggressive behaviours [50]. The potential explanations of these discrepancies will be discussed in a separate paragraph, as they are largely independent of the particular polymorphism studied.
Table I.
Reference | No. of participants | Effect of ApoE genotype on BPSD |
---|---|---|
Lehtovirta et al., 1996 [19] | 58 | No effect of genotype on psychosis or depression |
Ramachandran et al., 1996 [20] | 46 | ε4 increases risk for psychosis and depression |
Holmes et al., 1996 [21] | 164 | ε2 increases risk for depression |
Holmes et al., 1997 [22] | 232 | ε2 increases risk for depression |
Holmes et al., 1998 [23] | 210 | ε2 increases risk for depression and delusions |
Ballard et al., 1997 [24] | 51 | ε4 increases risk for psychosis, lowers risk for depression |
Cacabelos et al., 1997 [25] | 207 | No effect of genotype on behavioural disturbances |
Cantillon et al., 1997 [26] | 162 | No effect of genotype on depression |
Forsell et al., 1997 [27] | 184 (out of 806 studied) | No effect of genotype on depression |
Forsell et al., 1998 [28] | 225 (out of 668 studied) | No effect of genotype on psychosis |
Lopez et al., 1997 [29] | 194 | No effect of genotype on psychotic symptoms |
Lyketsos et al., 1997 [30] | 120 | No effect of genotype on psychosis or depression |
Murphy et al., 1997 [31] | 77 | ε4 increases risk for behavioural disturbances |
Hirono et al., 1998 [32] | 228 | No effect of genotype on psychotic symptoms |
Hirono et al., 1999 [33] | 175 | No effect of genotype on behavioural disturbances |
Harwood et al., 1999 [34] | 501 | ε4 increases risk for psychosis |
Levy et al., 1999 [35] | 605 | No effect of genotype on behavioural disturbances |
Weiner et al., 1999 [36] | 97 | ε4 marginally associated with delusions and hallucinations |
Gabryelewicz et al., 2002 [37] | 139 | No effect of genotype on behavioural disturbances |
Scarmeas et al., 2002 [38] | 87 | ε4 increases risk for delusions |
Sweet et al., 2002 [39] | 316 | Genotype does not predict time to onset of psychosis |
Chang et al., 2004 [40] | 135 | ε4 increases risk for psychosis |
Craig et al., 2004 [41] | 400 | ε4 increases risk for agitation/aggression |
Craig et al., 2005 [42] | 404 | No effect of genotype on depression |
Robertson et al., 2005 [43] | 125 | ε4 increases the level of anxiety |
Borroni et al., 2006 [44] | 234 | No effect of genotype on psychosis |
Borroni et al., 2006 [45] | 232 | No effect of genotype on behavioural disturbances |
Craig et al., 2006 [46] | 426 | No effect of genotype on sleep disruption |
Engelborghs et al., 2006 [47] | 186 | No effect of genotype on behavioural disturbances |
Spalletta et al., 2006 [48] | 171 | ε4 increases risk for delusions |
Pritchard et al., 2007 [49] | 388 | ε4 increases level of anxiety. No effect of genotype on behavioural disturbances after correction for multiple testing |
Sobow et al., 2007 [50] | 44 | ε4 increases risk for delusions and agitation/aggression |
Zdanys et al., 2007 [51] | 266 | ε4 increases risk for psychosis |
Borroni et al., 2009 [52] | 264 | No effect of genotype on depression |
Grünblatt et al., 2009 [53] | 72 | No effect of genotype on depression |
Quaranta et al., 2009 [54] | 148 | No effect of genotype on psychosis |
Woods et al., 2009 [55] | 36 | ε4 increases mean behavioural scores in nursing home patients |
The mechanism by which ApoE may increase the risk for BPSD is unclear. ApoE has been shown to promote the deposition of neuropathological features of AD – Aβ plaques and NFTs [18]. There are reports associating the degree of AD pathology with behavioural symptoms, e.g. NFT burden, particularly in the anterior cingulate, with agitation [56] or apathy [13]. ApoE4 AD carriers show a more profound loss in cholinergic activity in the hippocampus and the cortex [18]; decreased acetylcholine levels have in turn been implicated in the pathogenesis of BPSD [10]. In neuroimaging studies the presence of the ApoE ε4 allele was associated with a greater rate of hippocampal, cortical and whole-brain atrophy [57], while a link between atrophy, hypoperfusion or hypometabolism of various brain structures and BPSD symptoms has repeatedly been demonstrated [58, 59].
Serotonin receptors
Genes involved in neurotransmitter systems are usually considered one of the primary choices in candidate-gene association studies in psychiatric genetics, as the neurochemicals and their receptors are also targets for treatment of psychiatric disorders (including BPSD). Strong evidence supports the presence of a substantial disruption in global serotonergic neurotransmission in dementia. In vitro and in vivostudies provide evidence (although inconsistent) to link 5-HT dysfunction with aggressive behaviours, psychotic symptoms, anxiety and depression in AD [7]. The actions of 5-HT are mediated by 14 distinct subtypes of receptors [60]. Polymorphic variations in serotonergic receptors have already been implicated in the pathogenesis of many psychiatric ailments, including schizophrenia, mood disorders, anxiety disorders and eating disorders [61]. Two of these receptors, 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C, have also been examined as possible susceptibility factors for various BPSD symptoms in AD. We have identified 11 studies evaluating the role of 5-HT2A T102C polymorphism [62-72] and 4 studies on cys23ser polymorphism in 5-HT2C [62, 64, 66, 71], as well as one study on 5-HT6 [73] in the pathogenesis of BPSD. The results of the studies are summarized in Table II.
Table II.
Reference | No. of participants | Effect of 5-HT receptor genotype on BPSD |
---|---|---|
5-HT2A receptors (T102C polymorphism) | ||
Holmes et al., 1998 [62] | 211 | C allele increases risk for hallucinations |
Nacmias et al., 2001 [63] | 275 | CC genotype and C allele increase risk for psychosis |
Holmes et al., 2003 [64] | 158 | CC and TT genotypes increase risk for depression |
Rocchi et al., 2003 [65] | 135 | CC genotype increases risk for psychosis |
Assal et al., 2004 [66] | 96 | T allele increases risk for delusions and agitation/aggression |
Wa Lam et al., 2004 [67] | 87 | CC genotype increases risk for delusions, agitation, apathy and aberrant motor behaviour |
Micheli et al., 2006 [68] | 208 | No effect of genotype on depression |
Craig et al., 2007 [69] | 406 | No effect of genotype on psychosis |
Wilkosz et al., 2007 [70] | 324 | No effect of genotype on time to psychosis onset or depression |
Pritchard et al., 2008 [71] | 393 | No effect of genotype on behavioural disturbances (increase in CC genotype and C allele in psychosis, not significant) |
Angelucci et al., 2009 [72] | 80 | T allele increases risk for delusions and treatment-resistance to second generation antipsychotics |
5-HT2C receptors (cys23ser polymorphism) | ||
Holmes et al., 1998 [62] | 211 | C allele increases risk for hallucinations and hyperphagia (in females) |
Holmes et al., 2003 [64] | 158 | CC genotype increases risk for depression |
Assal et al., 2004 [66] | 96 | No effect of genotype on behavioural disturbances |
Pritchard et al., 2008 [71] | 394 | C allele increases risk for anxiety in females |
5-HT6 receptors | ||
Liu et al., 2001 [73] | 145 | No effect of genotype on depression |
Carrying the C allele of the 5-HT2A T102C SNP was associated with psychosis [62, 63, 65, 67], agitation [67], apathy [67], aberrant motor behaviour [67] and depression [64], while possession of the T allele was found to be associated with delusions [66], agitation [66], and depression [65]. According to the most recent of papers, T allele carriers could also suffer from diminished antipsychotic efficacy of second generation antipsychotics [72]. However, 4 large recent studies – contrary to all others – found no significant associations of this polymorphism with psychosis, depression, or any behavioural disturbances [68-71] (although in the latest one, distortions in allele frequencies were observed, with a similar, albeit insignificant increase of the C allele in the BPSD group [71]). No biologically plausible explanation of the discordant findings has been proposed as yet. T102C is a synonymous (silent) change with no consequence for the amino acid sequence. The gene, however, is highly polymorphic and this polymorphism may be in linkage disequilibrium (LD) with other SNPs in the gene – potential true susceptibility variants – such as the –1438 G/A promoter polymorphism or the His452Tyr polymorphism. Some conflicting reports could stem from different LD patterns in the evaluated cohorts. Whilst synonymous SNPs have no direct consequences for the protein structure, their importance should not be discounted. The T102C polymorphism might predispose AD patients to various BPSD phenomena via altering gene transcription, RNA stability, editing or splicing, or translational efficiency, or it may influence the post-translational modification of 5-HT2A [61]. The C allele of the T102C variation seems to be associated with reduced 5-HT2A receptor densities in brain tissues (the temporal cortex in particular) [74], a finding typical for AD itself as well. This could lead to worse dopaminergic modulation mediated by the serotonergic system and – taken together with the dopaminergic hypothesis of psychosis – might putatively be responsible for the association of this genetic variant with the psychotic spectrum [75].
The serine allele of the cys23ser polymorphism of the 5-HT2C gene has previously been found to be associated with hallucinations [62], hyperphagia (in females only) [62], and depression [64]. In the most recent study by Pritchard et al., the same C allele was significantly associated with anxiety in females; however, the effects of C allele-containing genotypes (CC and GC) did not reach statistical significance [71]. 5-HT2C is one of the pivotal serotonin receptors highly expressed in multiple brain areas. On a general level it is one of the key elements of the serotonergic inhibitory modulation of the dopaminergic tone, playing a crucial role in a wide range of psychiatric disorders, as diverse as eating disorders, anxiety and mood disorders (which could provide a rationale for the aforementioned results of AD-BPSD studies), and addiction, as well as influencing antipsychotic efficacy and side effects [76]. Unfortunately, the biological relevance of 5-HT2C for psychiatric phenotypes is contradictory to the poor, inconsistent results obtained by genetic investigations. Apart from the incomplete coverage of the gene, with the above described possibility of an LD with yet undiscovered pathogenetically influential SNPs, and some universal pitfalls described in detail in a later paragraph, there are other intriguing confounding factors unique to the 5-HT2C gene and receptor protein, often called “the fine-tuning machinery”. These include the modulatory influences of 5-HT2C-expressing glutamatergic and GABAergic interneurons exerting regionally specific, frequently different actions, as well as complicated 5-HT2C receptor mRNA editing patterns that can modify its affinity for the binding of serotonin and its efficiency to activate the second messenger cascade [77]. Via a specific editing profile the machinery will probably counteract the effects of genetic variations – more efficiently in some subjects, completely blunting the genetic background, reducing the penetrance of mutations, and making most investigations underpowered, but less efficiently in other subjects, to the point that the variation in the HTR2C turns out to be sufficiently relevant to be associated with a psychiatric disorder [76].
Serotonin transporter
The 5-HT transporter (5-HTT) is central to the control of brain 5-HT neurotransmission by regulating the magnitude and duration of the serotonergic response through the reuptake of 5-HT at the synapse. Furthermore, 5-HTT is a target for several types of pharmacological interventions in psychiatric disorders (e.g. SSRIs). Investigations of possible associations between 5-HTT and BPSD have produced inconsistent findings. Two different variable number tandem repeats (VNTR) polymorphisms in the SERTgene (located on chromosome 17q) have been examined in this context; the results are summarized in Table III.
Table III.
Reference | No. of participants | Effect of 5-HT receptor genotype on BPSD |
---|---|---|
LPR (linked polymorphic region) | ||
Li et al., 1997 [79] | 196 | No effect of genotype on depression |
Sukonick et al., 2001 [80] | 137 | L allele and LL/LS genotypes increase risk for aggression |
Sweet et al., 2001 [81] | 332 | L allele and LL/LS genotypes increase risk for psychosis and aggression |
Rocchi et al., 2003 [65] | 135 | No effect of genotype on psychosis |
Assal et al., 2004 [66] | 96 | No effect of genotype on behavioural disturbances |
Ha et al., 2005 [82] | 65 | No effect of genotype on delusions and aggression |
Borroni et al., 2006 [44] | 234 | SS genotype and S allele increase risk for psychosis |
Borroni et al.2006 [45] | 232 | LL genotype decreases risk for “psychosis” endophenotype |
Micheli et al., 2006 [68] | 208 | No effect of genotype on depression |
Pritchard et al., 2007[83] | 367 | L allele increases risk for irritability. No effect of genotype on behavioural disturbances after correction for multiple testing |
Ueki et al., 2007 [84] | 200 | No effect of genotype on behavioural disturbances |
Albani et al., 2009 [85] | 235 | No effect of genotype on behavioural disturbances |
Grünblatt et al., 2009 [53] | 72 | No effect of genotype on depression |
Quaranta et al., 2009 [54] | 148 | L allele increases risk for psychosis in a dose-dependent fashion |
STin2 VNTR (variable number of tandem repeats) | ||
Li et al., 1997 [79] | 196 | 12-repeat allele non-significantly (p = 0.07) associated with depression |
Assal et al., 2004 [66] | 96 | No effect of genotype on behavioural disturbances |
Pritchard et al., 2007 [83] | 367 | 10-repeat allele increases risk for psychosis. No effect of genotype after correction for multiple testing |
Ueki et al., 2007 [84] | 200 | 10-repeat allele increases risk for behavioural disturbances and aggression |
Firstly, there is a functional polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR); the long (L) and short (S) alleles are defined by differing numbers of a 44-base pair repetitive sequence. The homozygous S/S genotype has been associated with an increased risk of unipolar and bipolar depression, anxiety, substance abuse, and a predisposition to suicide or depression following stressful life events [78]. Conversely, the homozygous L/L genotype was associated with a predisposition to obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and increased intensity of hallucinations in individuals with schizophrenia [78]. In AD, 5-HTTLPR polymorphism has been evaluated in 14 studies [44, 45, 53, 54, 65, 66, 68, 79-85]. Eight of them proved negative, finding no association whatsoever. Even in the remaining six, the results are conflicting. Four papers, in accordance with the preclinical BPSD aetiological hypotheses suggesting a lower level of synaptic 5-HT, demonstrated a detrimental effect of the L allele on aggression, psychosis, and irritability in AD patients [54, 80, 81, 83]. In one of the studies, however, the result did not remain significant after correction for multiple testing [83]. The putative association between 5-HT depletion and psychosis could further be strengthened by the demonstrated efficacy of a selectively serotonergic antidepressant – citalopram – in the treatment of psychotic disturbances in demented individuals [86]. In contrast, Borroni et al. in 2 other studies observed an association between the S allele and SS genotype with psychotic symptoms in AD, and a protective effect of the LL genotype against “psychotic” endophenotype (described above) [44, 45]. The authors provide no attempt to explain these observations on a biological basis. However, in a recent meta-analysis, the S allele carriers demonstrated significantly greater amygdala activation in response to neutral environmental stimuli when compared to the L allele harbouring subjects, providing an indirect clue for the comprehension of the S allele’s involvement in BPSD pathogenesis [87].
The numerous studies aiming to reveal associations between polymorphisms in the 5-HTT gene and various behavioural traits are based on the assumption that these polymorphisms, or polymorphisms linked to them, influence the binding capacity or function of 5-HTT in the human brain. However, some recent studies exploring a possible association between polymorphisms in SERT and the expression of 5-HTT protein or 5-HT binding in the brains of healthy controls or patients did not reveal any genotype-dependent differences, blurring the “classic”, coherent picture [78, 88]. Drawing conclusions is further complicated by studies on other SNPs within the gene, verifying the existence of subgroups within the dichotomous S/L paradigm, for example converting the activity associated with the L allele to that expected for the S allele [89]. Considering the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism as tri-allelic possibly alters the interpretations of previous association studies based on a simple “L vs S” dichotomy model. Another confounding factor potentially leading to non-replication of results may be a failure to control for epistatic effects. The SERTgene is a classical example of the necessity to control for gene-environment interactions, with numerous studies consistently proving that exposure to adverse life events strengthens the association between 5-HTTLPR and a given behavioural phenotype (particularly depression- and anxiety-related) [90].
Another functional polymorphism studied was the VNTR in intron 2 of the 5-HTT gene comprising 9, 10 or 12 copies of a 16/17-base pair element (frequently termed STin2.9, STin2.10 and STin2.12 VNTR, respectively). Association studies have linked this polymorphism to several behavioural phenotypes (as well as antidepressant efficacy), although without consistency [78]. Only 4 research groups have evaluated the significance of STin2 VNTR polymorphism in BPSD in AD [66, 79, 83, 84]. Two of the studies were negative, while in the other two STin2.10 was associated with aggression, psychosis and the total level of behavioural psychopathology (one of those insignificant after correction for multiple testing). The STin2 VNTR displays functionality both in vitro and in vivo[78, 88]. In expression studies, the 12-repeat allele was found to be a stronger enhancer of transcriptional activity than the 10-repeat allele. Moreover, apart from the number, the primary structure of the repeats could also affect the transcription of the gene. In transgenic mice, STin2 VNTR genotype was demonstrated to exert an important regulatory role in development of the serotonergic system. Transient alterations in fetal 5-HT homeostasis can modify the wiring of brain connections leading to permanent changes in adult behaviour. It is therefore possible that the effects of both 5-HTT polymorphisms are more pronounced during embryogenesis and development [91].
Catechol-O-methyltransferase
The catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene is a major enzyme in synaptic DA catabolism with a critical role in the prefrontal cortex because of the relative lack of DA transporters in this region. It contains a functional common polymorphism characterized by G to A transition at codon 108/158 (soluble/membrane-bound COMT) resulting in a valine-to-methionine substitution, giving rise to a significant, three-to-fourfold reduction in its enzymatic activity. The presence of valine (H allele = high activity) in the coding sequence corresponds dose-dependently with reduced prefrontal DA levels, subsequently leading to the upregulation of striatal dopamine activity (via increased tyrosine hydroxylase expression, the rate-limiting enzyme in DA synthesis) [92]. This reciprocal association could therefore explain both poorer cognitive scores (lower prefrontal DA) and an increased risk for schizophrenia (higher midbrain DA) [93] in COMT Val158 carriers [94]. The role of COMT genetic variants as BPSD risk modifiers has been analysed in five published papers [44, 45, 95-97], four of which were conducted by Borroni et al. The Met158Val polymorphism was evaluated either separately or as part of a four-loci haplotype. Contrary to other genes evaluated in AD behavioural genetics the results on the role of COMT are convergent in demonstrating the COMT*H genotype as a culprit implicated in the aetiology of psychosis in AD (however, the studies by Borroni et al. evaluate – at least in part – the same population, although in various aspects). The relevance of the HH genotype or H allele for AD psychosis has been observed both in isolation [44, 45, 95] and in interaction with three other loci forming a haplotype [96, 97]. Furthermore, in one of the studies COMT*H decreased the risk for disinhibition and euphoria grouped together as a “frontal” endophenotype. The susceptibility to a cognitively impaired/psychotic phenotype in COMT 158Val carriers might be explained by the above described modulating role of COMT in DA metabolism with a genotype-dependent inverse balance in striatal/frontal DA activity regulation. This mechanism is of course not restricted to schizophrenia alone, possibly translating into a universally increased proneness to psychosis and cognitive decline, both crucial aspects of a psychotic AD phenotype. However, one has to bear in mind that the reported associations between COMT Met158Val and cognition or psychosis were relatively weak (or even inconsistent on a meta-analytic level [98]); therefore – despite a uniform correlation pattern in AD-BPSD studies – the aetiological significance of this polymorphism alone should not be overestimated.
Dopamine receptors and transporter
The dopaminergic system plays a role in many aspects of human behaviour, including aggression, psychosis, depression, elation and the control of movement. The action of DA is mediated by five distinct receptor subtypes, DRD1-DRD5, and the dopamine transporter (DAT). Dopamine receptors are major targets for antipsychotic agents, one of the most important classes of medications used in the treatment of BPSD, and for drugs of abuse eliciting behavioural and psychological changes. Polymorphisms in DRD1-DRD4 and DAT have been evaluated in various psychiatric disorders, typically with mixed results [99]. Contrary to primarily psychotic disorders, the development of psychotic symptoms in AD does not appear to be associated with brain or plasma concentrations of dopamine or its metabolites (homovanillic acid). Nevertheless, the effects of dopamine in the synapse are dependent not only on its concentrations, but also on receptor densities (as well as post-receptor signal transduction mechanisms). Accordingly, striatal DRD2 and DRD3 availability has recently been proven to increase in AD patients with delusions [100]. The variants DRD1 (A-48G), DRD2 (ser311cys), DRD3 (ser9gly), DRD4 (VNTR) and DAT (3’-UTR) have been investigated in 7 studies on BPSD [8, 53, 101-105], as usual with contradictory results (summarized in Table V). DRD1 A allele, either in homo- (A/A) [8] or heterozygosity (A/G) [101], was found to be associated with psychosis and aggression [8] or aggression and hallucinations (but not delusions) [101]. However, in the latest and largest study, the DRD1 genotype did not correlate with behavioural disturbances [103]. The inconsistent direction of effect suggests spurious associations or LD of the A-48G variant with a true susceptibility factor. Investigations on DRD2 demonstrated no associations of the ser311cys polymorphism with any behavioural symptom [8, 103]. Results on the hypothetically most relevant DRD3 ser9gly variant are conflicting. All possible variants – Ser/Ser or Gly/Gly homozygosity, Ser/Ser genotype, or possession of Gly allele – have been postulated to play a role in the aetiology of delusions or psychosis [8, 101, 104]. However, in other studies DRD3 showed no association with psychosis risk whatsoever [102, 103]. In another recent study ser9gly homozygosity increased the risk for elation mania [103], although the result did not remain significant after multiple testing. The discordance of the findings might suggest that the observed associations were accidental. For the same reason, the DRD4 VNTR polymorphism seems to play a minor (if any) role in BPSD pathogenesis [8, 53, 103]. DAT1 3’UTR variants were tested in AD patients in a single study, with the 9-repeat allele increasing risk for irritability, while the 10-repeat allele showed an association with aberrant motor behaviour [105]. However, 9- and 10-repeat alleles made up > 97% of all the observed alleles and significance was lost after Bonferroni correction [105].
Table V.
Reference | No. of participants | Studied gene | Effect of genotype on BPSD | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Sweet et al., 1998 [8] | 275 | DRD1, DRD2, DRD3, DRD4 | DRD1 A/A genotype increases risk for psychosis and aggression in whites. DRD3 homozygosity (Ser/Ser or Gly/Gly) increases risk for psychosis in whites. No effect of DRD3 genotype on aggression. No effect of DRD2 or DRD4 genotypes on psychosis or aggression | |
Holmes et al., 2001 [101] | 134 | DRD1, DRD3 | DRD1 A/G genotype increases risk for aggression and hallucinations. No effect of DRD1 on delusions. DRD3 Ser/Ser genotype increases risk for delusions compared with Gly/Gly. No effect of DRD3 on aggression or hallucinations | |
Craig et al., 2004 [102] | 416 | DRD3 | No effect of genotype on psychosis | |
Grunblatt et al., 2009 [53] | 72 | DRD4 | No effect of genotype on depression | |
Pritchard et al., 2009 [103] | 395 | DRD1, DRD2, DRD3, DRD4 | DRD3 homozygosity (Ser/Ser or Gly/Gly) increases risk for elation. DRD4 7-repeat allele increases risk for agitation/aggression, decreases risk for depression. DRD4 4-repeat allele increases risk for depression, decreases risk for agitation/aggression. No effect of genotype after correction for multiple testing. No effect of genotype on psychosis. No effect of DRD1 or DRD2 genotypes on behavioural disturbances | |
Sato et al., 2009 [104] | 210 | DRD3 | DRD3 Gly allele increases risk for delusions. No effect of DRD3 on other behavioural disturbances | |
Pritchard et al., 2008 [105] | 395 | DAT | 9-repeat allele increases risk for irritability. 10-repeat allele increases risk for aberrant motor behaviour. No effect of genotype after correction for multiple testing. No association observed for psychosis, depression, agitation/aggression |
DRD1-4 - dopamine receptors D1-D4, DAT - dopamine transporter
Other genes
The significance of several other genes has been assessed in single studies. Their detailed analysis is hardly available due to editorial constraints. Two research groups tested the hypothesis that interleukin-1β (IL-1β) might act as a BPSD modifier gene. Craig et al. found an association between the CC genotype or C allele (responsible for the diminished production of IL-1α) with both delusions and hallucinations in AD patients [106], while McCulley et al. associated depressive symptoms with the possession of a T allele, thus with raised IL-1α levels [107]. To establish a possible relationship between oxidative stress and the non-cognitive symptoms of AD, polymorphic sites in genes coding for heat-shock proteins (HSP) and glutathione-S-transferases (GST) have been evaluated in AD subjects [108, 109]. Clarimon et al. reported an allele dose-dependent increase in NPI-measured level of behavioural pathology in carriers of the A2 allele of the HSPA1B gene (involved in the stress response) [108]. Spalletta et al. observed an association between genetic variants in the GST polymorphic sites and AD age of onset or rate of progression; however, no effect of GST polymorphisms on behavioural symptom severity was found [109]. Following the track of neurotransmitter-associated candidate genes Craig et al. in two separate studies tested the significance of polymorphisms in tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH) and monoamine oxidase-A genes in relation to AD behavioural pathology [46, 110]. The TPH is the rate-limiting enzyme in the biosynthesis of serotonin catalyzing the conversion of tryptophan to 5-HT. It was observed that male AD participants with a history of agitation/aggression were significantly more likely to possess C allele-containing genotypes [110]. In another paper, the authors hypothesized that the risk of sleep disturbance in AD may, at least in part, be influenced by the availability of serotonin used for melatonin synthesis secondary to MAO-A VNTR polymorphic variation. A quantitative sleep disturbance score was significantly higher in the patients possessing MAO-A 4-repeat allele genotypes [46]. Go et al. evaluated the significance of neuregulin-1 (NRG1) SNP in conferring extra risk for psychotic symptoms in AD [111]. There is strong evidence from several studies that genetic variation in NRG1 has a substantial impact on schizophrenia risk. Genetic linkage studies in patients with AD with psychosis revealed multiple suggestive peaks, including 8p, within the chromosomal region of NRG1. Go et al. observed an association between NRG1 SNP, both in isolation and as part of a 3-loci haplotype, and a psychotic phenotype in AD [111]. Another effort to dissect the genetic background of BPSD focused on the IDE gene coding for insulin-degrading enzyme protein [112]. The IDE plays a key role in degrading several important peptides, including Aα. A correlation was observed between carrying the C allele of the IDE gene and the risk for affective disturbances in AD, with no effect on other behavioural symptoms [112].
In the most recent study an association between brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) genetic variants and depression in AD was evaluated [52]. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor is an important regulator of neuronal plasticity and survival. A functional Val66Met SNP in the coding region of the BDNF gene (located on chromosome 11p14) has previously been associated with major depression, geriatric depression, and the risk of AD itself, although with conflicting results. In their study, Borroni et al. observed a dose-dependent correlation between presence of the A allele (coding for methionine) and the risk of depression comorbid with AD [52].
Potential sources of between-study variations
One of the major obstacles in the field of psychiatric genetics concerns problems with the consistency of the results, which obviously hinders their interpretation considerably. Typical association studies test the significance of given polymorphisms as risk-conferring or protective factors for a specific disorder in a case-control manner. An important drawback inherent to this strategy is the recruitment process solely based on clinical diagnoses (phenotypes). As complex (multifactorial) disorders suffer from both genetic and phenotypic heterogeneity, simply relying on the ICD-10- or DSM-IV-based symptomatic, biologically undetermined criteria will frequently obscure the genetic signals. In the field of neurodegenerative disorders even accurate application of the criteria leaves some space for uncertainty. Subjects asymptomatic during the interview might have been developing brain pathology for several years, though the clinical symptoms threshold had not yet been reached; the differential diagnosis of dementia without autopsy confirmation can be a source of substantial difficulties as well. In this regard it is beneficial to remember that the same symptoms can have completely diverse pathogenetic backgrounds in different dementing disorders. This applies to the genetic studies as well, e.g. in a study by Engelborghs et al. ApoE genotype had no effect on BPSD in AD subjects, in frontotemporal dementia (FTD) patients; however, a dose-dependent effect of the ε4 allele on aggressiveness and the total level of behavioural pathology was identified [47].
Several means of circumventing the issue are usually considered, one of which is substituting diagnoses with endophenotypes – traits biologically and genetically simpler, mediating between susceptibility genes and full expression of the disorder. The studies on AD behavioural genetics follow this line of thought, narrowing the entire phenotype to patients with comorbid behavioural abnormalities, symptom clusters or finally isolated symptoms. Unfortunately, as can be seen from the aforementioned results, the clarity and consistency of the findings did not improve much, still not allowing for firm conclusions. Several sources of such ongoing disparity can be taken into consideration.
Variability in general study design
The BPSD symptoms are rarely very stable phenomena in AD patients. Notwithstanding the general impression that the overall level of psychopathology increases with dementia severity, they have a tendency to wax and wane, their severity fluctuating with time. Not surprisingly, the results of genetic studies will therefore heavily rely on the average disease stage of the AD participants. In a recent study on ApoE and psychosis in AD, Zdanys et al. found that the ε4 detrimental effect was statistically significant only for the severe-stage patients [51], a result already seen in some [34] but not all [37] studies. Consequently, an association can be missed if mild-to-moderate stage patients predominate [33, 35]. A significant choice in this context is that of a cross-sectional versus longitudinal study design. Cross-sectional studies can omit episodes that occur outside the assessment period. If patients are being followed over a period of time, a higher frequency of symptoms can be detected, significantly influencing the attribution of patients to predefined study groups.
Diagnostic criteria employed
A variety of methods have been used by the researchers to identify behavioural abnormalities in AD participants. Some older studies relied only on general clinical examination and descriptive assessments, while others used rating scales, or a combination. However, even with rating scales it is difficult to reconcile contradictory reports due to a wide range of scales having been used for any symptom. Some diagnostic tools assess the symptoms qualitatively (simply defining the presence or absence of a given BPSD symptom), while others provide quantitative measures (e.g. the popular Neuropsychiatric Inventory – NPI), permitting the choice of different thresholds of severity and allowing for the inclusion of patients only with clinically significant psychopathology.
Choice of symptoms
The range of symptoms evaluated has to be precisely defined, e.g. in the papers one can find “psychosis”, delusions and hallucinations analysed together or separately, or persecutory delusions on their own. In most cases AD patients exhibit more than one behavioural symptom. The isolated symptoms are frequently interrelated, e.g. the presence of delusions could likely be associated with hallucinations, agitation/aggression or sleep disturbances, while depression could increase the chance of comorbid anxiety, sleep and appetite disturbances. This constitutes one of the major limitations for the correct evaluation of BPSD pathogenesis. Therefore, some of the authors propose using clusters of symptoms – behavioural endophenotypes – supposedly linked to specific neurotransmitter abnormalities or even sharing a common genetic basis [45]. In one of the studies carrying the COMT*H allele was not significantly correlated with a “psychotic” phenotype (defined as NPI items delusions + hallucinations + night-time disturbances), but proved to be significantly associated with hallucinations alone [45]. Two different approaches may thus be considered – relating the polymorphic variations to isolated symptoms or symptom clusters.
Selection bias
Apart from the clinical characteristics of the investigated population, the results of genetic studies can heavily depend on its ethnicity or even its genetic homogeneity within one race. Some populations are considered genetically homogeneous, e.g. Northern Ireland inhabitants or Sephardic Jews. This increases the consistency of the findings. Such populations, however, are prone to the founder effect (loss of genetic variation due to shared ancestry). Selection bias can also manifest itself through the choice of setting for patient recruitment: nursing home dwellers, inpatients or patients treated in an ambulatory setting. The most inclusive population-based studies recruit “real-life” patients, making the results much more practical, although at a price of numerous medical, environmental, and drug-related confounders, particularly troublesome in genetic studies.
Statistical power
Commonly, the problem with divergent findings of genetic studies lies with inadequate statistical power owing to an insufficient number of study participants. More studies on larger cohorts with appropriate assessment tools as well as using a meta-analytic approach are potential solutions to overcome the problem.
Another issue worth mentioning in the context of statistical methods is the correction for multiple testing. Some of the observed associations might be spurious as the majority of studies are subject to considerable multiple testing. Employing proper statistical methods can nullify an initially statistically significant result [49].
Carrier status versus dose
Interpretation of genetic studies requires even more caution as some authors evaluate genetic associations dichotomously (carrier vs non-carrier status of a particular allele) [51], while others examine the effect of allele “dose” in relation to BPSD symptoms [38]. Sometimes only a homozygous genotype for a particular allele increases BPSD risk [67]; in other cases it is only the carrier status that matters [63].
Inherent limitations of genetic studies
Genetic studies on complex traits are prone to several limitations. One has to remember that the aetiology of BPSD is multifactorial, with an interplay of genetic, other biological, environmental and social factors, the individual influence of which is rather difficult to disentangle. Multiple genes are usually involved in the pathogenesis of a complex trait, often with a contradictory effect (susceptibility genes, protective genes, modifying genes); moreover, the effects of individual polymorphisms are usually weak, requiring large cohorts to demonstrate. Some of the SNPs are statistically associated with each other in the form of a haplotype increasing the complexity of epistatic (gene-gene) interactions. The discrepant findings might also relate to the fact that some of the polymorphisms studied are not true risk factors themselves but only represent markers located near some unidentified genes associated with AD or BPSD. Importantly, drawing final conclusions and comparing association results is often hampered as genes usually contain many polymorphic sites, whereas the majority of studies only offer analyses of single polymorphisms, sometimes different ones in successive studies (it should be noted that testing all polymorphisms present in a particular gene of interest is usually unfeasible for practical reasons).
Conclusions
The last couple of years have witnessed an unprecedented struggle in the search for the genetic correlates of behavioural symptoms in AD. The strategy of narrowing the studied phenotype to clinically – and hopefully genetically – distinct and homogeneous populations has been becoming increasingly popular as a way to circumvent typical problems of psychiatric genetics (or genetics of most complex traits, more generally speaking). The results of this tactic have been and still are awaited with ongoing expectations. Unfortunately, as one can see from this review, definite answers are hardly available. The reports still mainly fuel discussions on the potential sources of discrepancies rather than providing a stimulus for finding practical applications of purely scientific observations. Such a gloomy perspective could, however, derive from unrealistic expectations. The influence of genetic polymorphisms on BPSD profile might simply be weak enough to suffer from a particular vulnerability to potential confounders, so difficult to control for in genetic association studies, or simply lack of adequate statistical power. Another explanation, not mutually exclusive with the former, could focus our attention on the still limited technical possibilities and their rapid evolution in recent years. With whole-genome scans, the use of microarrays and chips allowing a simultaneous study of thousands of genes and their interactions, and an increasing awareness of the significance of haplotypes, rather than isolated SNPs, the perspectives of genetics should probably be sketched in brighter colours. Finally, interpreting the outcome of genetic research separately from neurochemical, neuropathological, neuroimaging or electrophysiological studies seems an undesired oversimplification. Fusing the available multidisciplinary data can result in discoveries important also in practical terms. Probably the most important lesson from studies on BPSD is that the pathogenesis of the same symptoms (e.g., psychotic or depressive) can be totally divergent in different psychiatric disorders, e.g. the biological basis for the psychotic symptoms in AD seems to be more closely associated with cholinergic-serotonergic imbalance than with dopamine, traditionally given priority in schizophrenia neurobiology. The same is true even for various types of dementia, albeit more closely related at first glance. It is therefore unwise to mechanically project knowledge on “general” psychiatry to neuropsychiatry, on one type of dementia to another, or to rely solely on atheoretical, strictly symptomatic classifications in neurobiological research. From this point on, only one step separates us from translating those conclusions into highly expected, pharmacological applications. One should bear in mind that this has already happened, as in the previously cited study by Pollock et al. on the efficacy of serotonergic citalopram in the treatment of psychosis in AD [86], or in a study of the 5-HT agonist tandospirone effective in reducing agitation/aggression, irritability, anxiety and depression in AD patients [113].
Table IV.
Reference | No. of participants | Effect of COMT genotype on BPSD |
---|---|---|
Borroni et al., 2004 [95] | 181 | HH genotype and H allele increase risk for psychosis |
Sweet et al., 2005 [96] | 373 | A four-locus haplotype increases the risk for psychosis |
Borroni et al., 2006 [44] | 234 | HH genotype and H allele increase risk for psychosis |
Borroni et al., 2006 [45] | 232 | HH genotype and H allele decrease risk for “frontal” endophenotype and increase risk for hallucinations |
Borroni et al., 2007 [97] | 246 | HH genotype and H allele increase risk for psychosis. Alleles at four loci (haplotype) interact to influence psychosis risk |
Table VI.
Reference | No. of participants | Studied gene | Effect of genotype on BPSD |
---|---|---|---|
Craig et al., 2004 [106] | 406 | IL-1β promoter (-511) | CC genotype and C allele increase risk for psychosis |
McCulley et al., 2005 [107] | 133 | T allele increases risk for depression | |
Clarimon et al., 2003 [108] | 77 | HSP70-2 (HSPA1B) | A2 allele increases risk for behavioural (HSPA1B) disturbances in a dose-dependent fashion |
Craig et al., 2004 [110] | 396 | TPH | CC genotype and C allele increase aggression |
Go et al., 2005 [111] | NRG1 | NRG1 SNP increases the risk for psychosis. A 3-SNP haplotype with NRG1 increases the risk for psychosis | |
Craig et al., 2006 [46] | 426 | MAO-A | 4-repeat allele increases risk for sleep disruption |
Spalletta et al., 2007 [109] | 99 | GST | No effect of genotype on behavioural disturbances |
Sato et al., 2008 [112] | 207 | IDE | C allele increases risk for “affective disturbances”. No association with other behavioural symptoms |
Borroni et al., 2009 [113] | 264 | BDNF | A (Met) allele increases risk for depression in a dose-dependent fashion |
GST - glutathione S-transferase, HSP - heat shock protein, IL-1β - interleukin 1β, MAO-A - monoamine oxidase A, NRG - neuregulin, SNP - single nucleotide polymorphism, TPH - tryptophan hydroxylase
Acknowledgments
This work was funded from the Medical University of Lodz grant no. 502-18-856.
References
- 1.Hartm DJ, Craig D, Compton SA, et al. A retrospective study of the behavioural and psychological symptoms of mid and late phase Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2003;18:1037–42. doi: 10.1002/gps.1013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Lopez OL, Wisniewski SR, Becker JT, Boller F, DeKosky ST. Psychiatric medication and abnormal behavior as predictors of progression in probable Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 1999;56:1266–72. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.10.1266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Moritz DJ, Fox PJ, Luscombe FA, Kraemer HC. Neurological and psychiatric predictors of mortality in patients with Alzheimer disease in California. Arch Neurol. 1997;54:878–85. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1997.00550190066016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ferris SH, Steinberg G, Shulman E, Kahn R, Reisberg B. Institutionalization of Alzheimer's disease patients: reducing precipitating factors through family counseling. Home Health Care Serv Q. 1987;8:23–51. doi: 10.1300/J027v08n01_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Clyburn LD, Stones MJ, Hadjistavropoulos T, Tuokko H. Predicting caregiver burden and depression in Alzheimer’s disease. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2000;55:S2–13. doi: 10.1093/geronb/55.1.s2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Cohen-Mansfield J, Mintzer JE. Time for change: the role of nonpharmacological interventions in treating behavior problems in nursing home residents with dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2005;19:37–40. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000155066.39184.61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lanctôt KL, Herrmann N, Mazzotta P. Role of serotonin in the behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2001;13:5–21. doi: 10.1176/jnp.13.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Sweet RA, Nimgaonkar VL, Kamboh MI, Lopez OL, Zhang F, DeKosky ST. Dopamine receptor genetic variation, psychosis and aggression in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 1998;55:1335–40. doi: 10.1001/archneur.55.10.1335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Herrmann N, Lanctôt KL, Khan LR. The role of norepinephrine in the behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2004;16:261–76. doi: 10.1176/jnp.16.3.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Minger SL, Esiri MM, McDonald B, et al. Cholinergic deficits contribute to behavioral disturbance in patients with dementia. Neurology. 2000;55:1460–7. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.10.1460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lanctôt KL, Herrmann N, Mazzotta P, Khan LR, Ingber N. GABAergic function in Alzheimer's disease: evidence for dysfunction and potential as a therapeutic target for the treatment of behavioural and psychological symptoms of dementia. Can J Psychiatry. 2004;49:439–53. doi: 10.1177/070674370404900705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Herrmann N, Lanctôt KL. Pharmacologic management of neuropsychiatric symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease. Can J Psychiatry. 2007;52:630–46. doi: 10.1177/070674370705201004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Marshall GA, Fairbanks LA, Tekin S, Vinters HV, Cummings JL. Neuropathologic correlates of apathy in Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2006;21:144–7. doi: 10.1159/000090674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Lee DY, Choo IH, Kim KW, et al. White matter changes associated with psychotic symptoms in Alzheimer's disease patients. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;18:191–8. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2006.18.2.191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Edwards-Lee T, Cook I, Fairbanks L, Leuchter A, Cummings JL. Quantitative electroencephalographic correlates of psychosis in Alzheimer disease. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 2000;13:163–70. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Tanzi RE. A genetic dichotomy model for the inheritance of Alzheimer's disease and common age-related disorders. J Clin Invest. 1999;104:1175–9. doi: 10.1172/JCI8593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Corder EH, Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, et al. Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer's disease in late onset families. Science. 1993;261:921–3. doi: 10.1126/science.8346443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kim J, Basak JM, Holtzman DM. The role of apolipoprotein E in Alzheimer's disease. Neuron. 2009;63:287–303. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.06.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lehtovirta M, Soininen H, Helisalmi S, et al. Clinical and neuropsychological characteristics in familial and sporadic Alzheimer’s disease Relation to apolipoprotein E polymorphism. Neurology. 1996;46:413–9. doi: 10.1212/wnl.46.2.413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ramachandran G, Marder K, Tang M, et al. A preliminary study of apolipoprotein E genotype and psychiatric manifestations of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1996;47:256–9. doi: 10.1212/wnl.47.1.256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Holmes C, Levy R, McLoughlin DM, Powell JF, Lovestone S. Apolipoprotein E non-cognitive symptoms and cognitive decline in late onset Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1996;61:580–3. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.61.6.580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Holmes C, McLoughlin DM, Powell J, Lovestone S. Reply. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1997;63:273–4. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Holmes C, Russ C, Kirov G, et al. Apolipoprotein E depressive illness depressive symptoms and Alzheimer's disease. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;43:159–64. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(97)00326-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ballard C, Massey H, Lamb H, Morris C. Apolipoprotein E: non-cognitive symptoms and cognitive decline in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr. 1997;63:273–4. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.63.2.273b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Cacabelos R, Rodriguez B, Carrera C, Beyer K, Lao JI, Sellers MA. Behavioral changes associated with different apolipoprotein E genotypes in dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 1997;11:S27–34. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Cantillon M, Harwood D, Barker W, et al. No association between apolipoprotein E genotype and late-onset depression in Alzheimer's disease. Biol Psychiatry. 1997;41:246–8. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(96)00422-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Forsell Y, Corder EH, Basun H, Lannfelt L, Viitanen M, Winblad B. Depression and dementia in relation to apolipoprotein E polymorphism in a population sample age 75+ Biol Psychiatry. 1997;42:898–903. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00468-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Forsell Y, Basun H, Corder EH, Lannfelt L, Winblad B. Psychotic symptoms and apolipoprotein E genotypes in an elderly population. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44:139–40. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(97)00352-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Lopez OL, Kamboh MI, Becker JT, Kaufer DI, DeKosky ST. The apolipoprotein E e4 allele is not associated with psychiatric symptoms or extrapyramidal signs in probable Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1997;49:794–7. doi: 10.1212/wnl.49.3.794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Lyketsos CG, Baker L, Warren A, et al. Depression, delusions and hallucinations in Alzheimer’s Disease No relationship to apolipoprotein E genotype. J Neuropsychiatr Clin Neurosci. 1997;9:64–7. doi: 10.1176/jnp.9.1.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Murphy GM Jr, Taylor J, Tinklenberg JR, Yesavage JA, et al. The apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 allele is associated with increased behavioral disturbance in Alzheimer's disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1997;5:88–9. doi: 10.1097/00019442-199700510-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Hirono N, Mori E, Yasuda M, et al. Factors associated with psychotic symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr. 1998:648–52. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.64.5.648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hirono N, Mori E, Yasuda M, et al. Lack of effect of apolipoprotein E E4 allele on neuropsychiatric manifestations in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;11:66–70. doi: 10.1176/jnp.11.1.66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Harwood DG, Barker WW, Ownby RL St. George-Hyslop PH, Duara R. Apolipoprotein-E (APO-E) genotype and symptoms of psychosis in Alzheimer's disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatr . 1999;7:119–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Levy ML, Cummings JL, Fairbanks LA, Sultzer DL, Small GW. Apolipoprotein E genotype and noncognitive symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatr. 1999;45:422–5. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00041-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Weiner MF, Vega G, Risser RC, et al. Apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 other risk factors and course of Alzheimer's disease. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45:633–8. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00222-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Gabryelewicz T, Religa D, Styczynska M, et al. Behavioural pathology in Alzheimer's disease with special reference to apolipoprotein E genotype. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2002;14:208–12. doi: 10.1159/000066020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Scarmeas N, Brandt J, Albert M, et al. Association between the APOE genotype and psychopathologic symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 2002;58:1182–8. doi: 10.1212/wnl.58.8.1182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Sweet R, Kamboh MI, Wisniewski SR, et al. Apolipoprotein E and alpha-1-antichymotrypsin genotypes do not predict time to psychosis in Alzheimer’s disease. J Geriatr Psychiatr Neurol. 2002;15:24–30. doi: 10.1177/089198870201500106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Chang JB, Wang PN, Chen WT, et al. ApoE epsilon4 allele is associated with incidental hallucinations and delusions in patients with AD. Neurology. 2004;63:1105–7. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000138612.24301.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Craig D, Hart D, McCool K, McIlroy S, Passmore A. Apolipoprotein E e4 allele influences aggressive behaviour in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2004;75:1327–30. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.032276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Craig D, Hart DJ, McIlroy SP, Passmore AP. Association analysis of apolipoprotein E genotype and risk of depressive symptoms in Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2005;19:154–7. doi: 10.1159/000082887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Robertson J, Curley J, Kaye J, Quinn J, Pfankuch T, Raber J. APOE isoforms and measures of anxiety in probable AD patients and apoe -/- mice. Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26:637–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Borroni B, Grassi M, Agosti C, et al. Cumulative effect of COMT and 5-HTTLPR polymorphisms and their interaction with disease severity and comorbidities on the risk of psychosis in Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14:343–51. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000192491.50802.c3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Borroni B, Grassi M, Agosti C, et al. Genetic correlates of behavioral endophenotypes in Alzheimer disease: role of COMT 5-HTTLPR and APOE polymorphisms. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27:1595–603. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.09.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Craig D, Hart DJ, Passmore AP. Genetically increased risk of sleep disruption in Alzheimer's disease. Sleep. 2006;29:1003–7. doi: 10.1093/sleep/29.8.1003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Engelborghs S, Deraut B, Mariën P. Dose dependent effect of APOE epsilon4 on behavioral symptoms in frontal lobe dementia. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27:285–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Spalletta G, Bernardini S, Bellincampi L, Federici G, Trequattrini A, Caltagirone C. Delusion symptoms are associated with ApoE epsilon4 allelic variant at the early stage of Alzheimer's disease with late onset. Eur J Neurol. 2006;13:176–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2006.01165.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Pritchard AL, Harris J, Pritchard CW, et al. The effect of the apolipoprotein E gene polymorphisms and haplotypes on behavioural and psychological symptoms in probable Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007;78:123–6. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.092122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Sobow T, Kloszewska I, Flirski M, Golanska E, Liberski PP. Apoliprotein E genotype and its relation to behavioral and psychological symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease [Polish] Post Psych Neurol. 2007;16:215–220. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Zdanys KF, Kleiman TG, MacAvoy MG, et al. Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele increases risk for psychotic symptoms in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:171–9. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Borroni B, Archetti S, Costanzi C, et al. ITINAD Working Group. Role of BDNF Val66Met functional polymorphism in Alzheimer's disease-related depression. Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30:1406–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.11.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Grünblatt E, Zehetmayer S, Bartl J, et al. Genetic risk factors and markers for Alzheimer's disease and/or depression in the VITA study. J Psychiatr Res. 2009;43:298–308. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Quaranta D, Bizzarro A, Marra C, et al. Psychotic symptoms in Alzheimer's disease and 5-HTTLPR polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene evidence for an association. J Alzheimers Dis. 2009;16:173–80. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2009-0950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Woods DL, Bushnell B, Kim H, Geschwind D, Cummings J. Apolipoprotein epsilon4 status is associated with behavioral symptoms in nursing home residents with dementia. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009;21:722–8. doi: 10.1017/S1041610209009235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Tekin S, Mega MS, Masterman DM, et al. Orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortex neurofibrillary tangle burden is associated with agitation in Alzheimer disease. Ann Neurol. 2001;49:355–61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Cherbuin N, Leach LS, Christensen H, Anstey KJ. Neuroimaging and APOE genotype a systematic qualitative review. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007;24:348–62. doi: 10.1159/000109150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Rosen HJ, Allison SC, Schauer GF, Gorno-Tempini ML, Weiner MW, Miller BL. Neuroanatomical correlates of behavioural disorders in dementia. Brain. 2005;128:2612–25. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Lanctôt KL, Herrmann N, Nadkarni NK, Leibovitch FS, Caldwell CB, Black SE. Medial temporal hypoperfusion and aggression in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2004;61:1731–7. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.11.1731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Barnes N, Sharp T. A review of central 5HT receptors and their functions. Neuropharmacology. 1999;38:1083–152. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(99)00010-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Norton N, Owen M. HTR2A association and expression studies in neuropsychiatric genetics. Ann Med. 2005;37:121–9. doi: 10.1080/07853890510037347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Holmes C, Arranz M, Powell J, Collier D, Lovestone S. 5HT2A and 5HT2C receptor polymorphisms and psychopathology in late onset Alzheimer’s disease. Hum Mol Genet. 1998;7:1507–9. doi: 10.1093/hmg/7.9.1507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Nacmias B, Tedde A, Forleo P, et al. Association between 5HT2A receptor polymorphism and psychotic symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:472–5. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01114-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Holmes C, Arranz M, Collier D, Powell J, Lovestone S. Depression in Alzheimer’s disease the effect of serotonin receptor gene variation. Am J Med Genet (Neuropsychiatric Genet) 2003;119B:40–3. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.10068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Rocchi A, Micheli R, Ceravolvo M, et al. Serotonergic polymorphisms (5-HTTLPR and 5-HT2A) association studies with psychosis in Alzheimer’s disease. Genet Test. 2003;7:309–14. doi: 10.1089/109065703322783662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Assal F, Alarcon M, Soloman E, Masterman D, Geshwind D, Cummings J. Association of the serotonin transporter and receptor gene polymorphisms in neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol. 2004;61:1249–53. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.8.1249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Wa Lam L, Tang N, Ma S, Zhang W, Chiu K. 5-HT2A T102C receptor polymorphism and neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatric Psychiatry. 2004;19:523–6. doi: 10.1002/gps.1109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Micheli D, Bonvicini C, Rocchi A, et al. No evidence for allelic association of serotonin 2A receptor and transporter gene polymorphisms with depression in Alzheimer disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2006;10:371–8. doi: 10.3233/jad-2006-10405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Craig D, Donnelly C, Hart D, Carson R, Passmore P. Analysis of the 5HT-2A T102C receptor polymorphism and psychotic symptoms in Alzheimer's disease. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2007;144B:126–8. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Wilkosz PA, Kodavali C, Weamer EA, et al. Prediction of psychosis onset in Alzheimer disease the role of depression symptom severity and the HTR2A T102C polymorphism. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2007;144B:1054–62. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Pritchard AL, Harris J, Pritchard CW, et al. Role of 5HT 2A and 5HT 2C polymorphisms in behavioural and psychological symptoms of Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2008;29:341–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Angelucci F, Bernardini S, Gravina P, et al. Delusion symptoms and response to antipsychotic treatment are associated with the 5-HT2A receptor polymorphism (102T/C) in Alzheimer's disease a 3-year follow-up longitudinal study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2009;17:203–11. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2009-1031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Liu HC, Hong CJ, Liu CY, et al. Association analysis of the 5-HT6 receptor polymorphism C267T with depression in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2001;55:427–9. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2001.00886.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Polesskaya OO, Sokolov BP. Differential expression of the C and T alleles of the 5-HT2A receptor gene in the temporal cortex of normal individuals and schizophrenics. J Neurosci Res. 2002;67:812–22. doi: 10.1002/jnr.10173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Serretti A, Drago A, De Ronchi D. HTR2A gene variants and psychiatric disorders: a review of current literature and selection of SNPs for future studies. Curr Med Chem. 2007;14:2053–69. doi: 10.2174/092986707781368450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Drago A, Serretti A. Focus on HTR2C: a possible suggestion for genetic studies of complex disorders. Am J Med Genet Part B. 2009;150B:601–37. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Berg KA, Clarke WP, Cunningham KA, Spampinato U. Fine-tuning serotonin2c receptor function in the brain: molecular and functional implications. Neuropharmacology. 2008;55:969–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Haddley K, Vasiliou AS, Ali FR, Paredes UM, Bubb VJ, Quinn JP. Molecular genetics of monoamine transporters: relevance to brain disorders. Neurochem Res. 2008;33:652–67. doi: 10.1007/s11064-007-9521-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Li T, Holmes C, Sham PC, et al. Allelic functional variation of serotonin transporter expression is a susceptibility factor for late onset Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroreport. 1997;8:683–6. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199702100-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Sukonick DL, Pollock BG, Sweet RA. The 5-HTTPR*S/*L polymorphism and aggressive behavior in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2001;58:1425–8. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.9.1425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Sweet RA, Pollock BG, Sukonick DL, et al. The 5-HTTPR polymorphism confers liability to a combined phenotype of psychotic and aggressive behavior in Alzheimer disease. Int Psychogeriatr. 2001;13:401–9. doi: 10.1017/s1041610201007827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Ha TM, Cho DM, Park SW, et al. Evaluating associations between 5-HTTLPR polymorphism and Alzheimer's disease for Korean patients. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2005;20:31–4. doi: 10.1159/000085071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Pritchard AL, Pritchard CW, Bentham P, Lendon CL. Role of serotonin transporter polymorphisms in the behavioural and psychological symptoms in probable Alzheimer disease patients. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007;24:201–6. doi: 10.1159/000107081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Ueki A, Ueno H, Sato N, Shinjo H, Morita Y. Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism and BPSD in mild Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2007;12:245–53. doi: 10.3233/jad-2007-12306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Albani D, Prato F, Tettamanti M, et al. The serotonin transporter promoter polymorphic region is not a risk factor for Alzheimer's disease related behavioral disturbances. J Alzheimers Dis. 2009;18:125–30. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2009-1129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Pollock BG, Mulsant BH, Rosen J, et al. Comparison of citalopram, perphenazine and placebo for the acute treatment of psychosis and behavioral disturbances in hospitalized demented patients. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:460–5. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.3.460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Munafo` MR, Brown SM, Hariri AR. Serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) genotype and amygdala activation: a meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:852–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.08.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.D'Souza UM, Craig IW. Functional genetic polymorphisms in serotonin and dopamine gene systems and their significance in behavioural disorders. Prog Brain Res. 2008;172:73–98. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00904-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Hu XZ, Lipsky RH, Zhu G, et al. Serotonin transporter promoter gain-of-function genotypes are linked to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Hum Genet. 2006;78:815–26. doi: 10.1086/503850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Luddington NS, Mandadapu A, Husk M, El-Mallakh RS. Clinical implications of genetic variation in the serotonin transporter promoter region: a review. Prim Care Companion J Clin Psychiatry. 2009;11:93–102. doi: 10.4088/pcc.08r00656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Parsey RV, Hastings RS Oquendo MA, et al. Effect of a triallelic functional polymorphism of the serotonin-transporter-linked promoter region on expression of serotonin transporter in the human brain. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:48–51. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.1.48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Akil M, Kolachana BS, Rothmond DA, Hyde TM, Weinberger DR, Kleinman JA. Catechol-O-methyltransferase genotype and dopamine regulation in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2003;23:2008–13. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-06-02008.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Glatt S, Faraone SV, Tsuang MT. Association between a functional catechol-O-methyltransferasegene polymorphism and schizoprenia meta-analysis of case–control and family-based studies. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:469–76. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.3.469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Tan HY, Callicott JH, Weinberger DR. Dysfunctional and compensatory prefrontal cortical systems genes and the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex. 2007;Suppl 1:i171–81. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Borroni B, Agosti C, Archetti S, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase gene polymorphism is associated with risk of psychosis in Alzheimer Disease. Neurosci Lett. 2004;370:127–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Sweet RA, Devlin B, Pollock BG, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase haplotypes are associated with psychosis in Alzheimer disease. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10:1026–36. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Borroni B, Grassi M, Costanzi C, et al. Haplotypes in cathechol-O-methyltransferase gene confer increased risk for psychosis in Alzheimer disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2007;28:1231–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.05.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Barnett JH, Scoriels L, Munafo` MR. Meta-analysis of the cognitive effects of the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene Val158/108Met polymorphism. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64:137–44. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Jönsson EG, Kaiser R, Brockmöller J, Nimgaonkar VL, Crocq MA. Meta-analysis of the dopamine D3 receptor gene (DRD3) Ser9Gly variant and schizophrenia. Psychiatr Genet. 2004;14:9–12. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200403000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Reeves S, Brown R, Howard R, Grasby P. Increased striatal dopamine (D2/D3) receptor availability and delusions in Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2009;72:528–34. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000341932.21961.f3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Holmes C, Smith H, Ganderton R, et al. Psychosis and aggression in Alzheimer's disease the effect of dopamine receptor gene variation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2001;71:777–9. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.71.6.777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Craig D, Hart DJ, Carson R, McIlroy SP, Passmore AP. Psychotic symptoms in Alzheimer's disease are not influenced by polymorphic variation at the dopamine receptor DRD3 gene. Neurosci Lett. 2004;368:33–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.06.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Pritchard AL, Ratcliffe L, Sorour E, et al. Investigation of dopamine receptors in susceptibility to behavioural and psychological symptoms in Alzheimer's disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2009;24:1020–5. doi: 10.1002/gps.2214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Sato N, Ueki A, Ueno H, Shinjo H, Morita Y. Dopamine D3 receptor gene polymorphism influences on behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia (BPSD) in mild dementia of Alzheimer's type. J Alzheimers Dis. 2009;17:441–8. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2009-1054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Pritchard AL, Pritchard CW, Bentham P, Lendon CL. Investigation of the role of the dopamine transporter in susceptibility to behavioural and psychological symptoms of patients with probable Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;26:257–60. doi: 10.1159/000160958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Craig D, Hart DJ, McCool K, McIlroy SP, Passmore AP. The interleukin 1beta gene promoter polymorphism (-511) acts as a risk factor for psychosis in Alzheimer's dementia. Ann Neurol. 2004;56:121–4. doi: 10.1002/ana.20120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.McCulley MC, Day IN, Holmes C. Association between interleukin 1-beta promoter (-511) polymorphism and depressive symptoms in Alzheimer's disease. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2004;124B:50–3. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Clarimón J, Bertranpetit J, Boada M, Ta`rraga L, Comas D. HSP70-2 (HSPA1B) is associated with noncognitive symptoms in late-onset Alzheimer's disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2003;16:146–50. doi: 10.1177/0891988703256051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Spalletta G, Bernardini S, Bellincampi L, et al. Glutathione S-transferase P1 and T1 gene polymorphisms predict longitudinal course and age at onset of Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007;15:879–87. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3180547076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Craig D, Hart DJ, Carson R, McIlroy SP, Passmore AP. Allelic variation at the A218C tryptophan hydroxylase polymorphism influences agitation and aggression in Alzheimer's disease. Neurosci Lett. 2004;363:199–202. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.02.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Go RC, Perry RT, Wiener H, et al. Neuregulin-1 polymorphism in late onset Alzheimer's disease families with psychoses. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005;139B:28–32. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Sato N, Ueki A, Ueno H, Shinjo H, Morita Y. IDE Gene Polymorphism influences on BPSD in mild dementia of Alzheimer's type. Curr Gerontol Geriatr Res. 2008;00:858759. doi: 10.1155/2008/858759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Sato S, Mizukami K, Asada T. A preliminary open-label study of 5-HT1A partial agonist tandospirone for behavioural and psychological symptoms associated with dementia. Int J Neuropsychopharm. 2007;10:281–3. doi: 10.1017/S1461145706007000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]