Skip to main content
Learning & Memory logoLink to Learning & Memory
. 2012 Feb;19(2):67–72. doi: 10.1101/lm.024752.111

Methylphenidate enhances extinction of contextual fear

Antony D Abraham 1,1, Christopher L Cunningham 1, K Matthew Lattal 1
PMCID: PMC3262973  PMID: 22251891

Abstract

Methylphenidate (MPH, Ritalin) is a norepinephrine and dopamine transporter blocker that is widely used in humans for treatment of attention deficit disorder and narcolepsy. Although there is some evidence that targeted microinjections of MPH may enhance fear acquisition, little is known about the effect of MPH on fear extinction. Here, we show that MPH, administered before or immediately following extinction of contextual fear, will enhance extinction retention in C57BL/6 mice. Animals that received MPH (2.5–10 mg/kg) before an extinction session showed decreased freezing response during extinction, and the effect of the 10 mg/kg dose on freezing persisted to the next day. When MPH (2.5–40 mg/kg) was administered immediately following an extinction session, mice that received MPH showed dose-dependent decreases in freezing during subsequent tests. MPH administered immediately after a 3-min extinction session or 4 h following the first extinction session did not cause significant differences in freezing. Together, these findings demonstrate that MPH can enhance extinction of fear and that this effect is sensitive to dose, time of injection, and duration of the extinction session. Because MPH is widely used in clinical treatments, these experiments suggest that the drug could be used in combination with behavioral therapies for patients with fear disorders.


Following the experience of an unconditioned stimulus (US) such as footshock in a certain context (conditioned stimulus, CS), an animal will associate those contextual cues with the US. If reexposed to those cues without the shock, the fearful reaction evoked by those cues will be suppressed. This suppression occurs through an extinction process that is a form of behavioral and neural adaptation that allows an organism to modify responding in the presence of conditioned cues (Myers and Davis 2007).

At the clinical level, extinction learning is an important aspect of treatment in therapies for a variety of diseases including drug addiction, phobias, and fear disorders such as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Quirk and Mueller 2008). Behavioral treatments for these disorders often include exposure therapy, in which a patient is exposed to a fear-arousing stimulus in the absence of an aversive stimulus in order to reduce fear response. Pharmacological methods for enhancing extinction are of value for clinicians to increase exposure therapy efficacy. In particular, drugs that enhance prefrontal cortical activity are of interest due to demonstrations of a correlation between decreased activation of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) with increased PTSD symptom severity (Shin et al. 2006) and to demonstrations of a role of the mPFC in inhibiting learned fear responses (Vidal-Gonzalez et al. 2006). Thus, developing pharmacological approaches that target prefrontal function may be useful for treatments that are designed to promote extinction (Quirk et al. 2006).

Several experiments have shown that psychostimulants can alter prefrontal cortex function. For example, acute cocaine or amphetamine administration increases extracellular availability of dopamine in the mPFC (Sorg and Kalivas 1993; Mazei et al. 2002). Although there is some evidence for fear extinction enhancements with intra-mPFC infusions of amphetamine (Pezze et al. 2003), the literature is generally mixed, with no effect (Mueller et al. 2009; Carmack et al. 2010) or even extinction-impairing effects of systemic administration of amphetamine or cocaine (Miczek and Luttinger 1978; Borowski and Kokkinidis 1998). These studies have examined psychostimulant effects on extinction following pre-session injections, which result in large locomotor effects that may interfere with the animal's ability to retrieve or express fear memory.

Methylphenidate hydrochloride (MPH) is a dopamine (DA) and norepinephrine (NE) transporter blocker that has been approved for use in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and narcolepsy under the trade name Ritalin. Deficits in prefrontocortical functioning are thought to underlie ADHD and may be related to decreased catecholamine terminals in the prefrontal cortex of ADHD adults (Arnsten and Dudley 2005). Berridge et al. (2006) demonstrated that low doses of MPH (0.25–2.0 mg/kg) preferentially increased DA and NE extracellular levels in the prefrontal cortex and improved spatial working memory in the delayed alternation task in Sprague-Dawley rats. Zheng et al. (2008) found that a single post-training session infusion of MPH into the anterior cingulate or the basolateral amygdala augmented fear memory consolidation in a step through inhibitory avoidance task but did not see effects of systemically injected MPH (0.5 mg/kg and 5 mg/kg). Recently, acute administration of MPH in rats has been shown to facilitate cortico-amygdalar plasticity following a cue–reward learning task (Tye et al. 2010). Because the prefrontal cortex is important for fear extinction (Myers and Davis 2007), there is a strong likelihood that MPH may enhance the development and persistence of extinction through preferential activation of dopamine and norepinephrine receptors in the prefrontal cortex.

The following experiments examine the effects of acute systemic MPH administration in contextual fear extinction before, immediately after, or 4 h after a single longer (12-min) extinction session or following a relatively shorter (3-min) extinction session. The persistence of MPH effects was tested for 3 d after the first extinction session. The use of pre- and post-session injections allows us to specifically examine effects of MPH on acquisition and consolidation of the learning that occurs during extinction.

Results

Experiment 1: Pre-extinction session administration of MPH reduces freezing during extinction and testing

This experiment examined the effect of MPH (0, 2.5, 5, or 10 mg/kg) on extinction acquisition through the use of a single pre-session administration immediately before extinction.

Figure 1 shows that pre-session administration of MPH caused a dose-dependent reduction in freezing during the extinction session (Ext 1 in Fig. 1). A one-way between-subjects ANOVA found significant effects of MPH on freezing (F(3,28) = 12.7, P < 0.0005). Post hoc comparisons with a Dunnett's test showed that saline was significantly different from 5 mg/kg (P = 0.001) and 10 mg/kg (P < 0.0005) but not from 2.5 mg/kg (P = 0.305). This effect was likely due to the locomotor effects of MPH, because doses of 10 and 20 mg/kg MPH showed significant differences in locomotor activity compared with saline (Supplemental Fig. 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Effects of MPH (black arrow) administered immediately before a 12-min extinction session. Mice that received methylphenidate showed differences in freezing response during the Extinction session (5 and 10 mg/kg) and during Test Day 1 (10 mg/kg). Error bars indicate SEM. (*) P < 0.05 significant difference compared with saline (Dunnett's test).

Test day analyses indicated that the effects of MPH persisted to the first drug-free test, when methylphenidate-treated (10 mg/kg) animals froze less than saline-treated animals (Test 1 in Fig. 1). This effect did not persist to Tests 2 and 3. These effects were confirmed by a Dose × Test Day ANOVA that found no main effect of Dose (F(3,28) = 0.6, P = 0.633) but did find a significant main effect of Test Day (F(2,56) = 7.5, P = 0.001), as well as a reliable interaction for Dose × Test Day (F(5,43) = 2.5, P = 0.049). Post hoc analyses of individual test days did not show significant effects for doses, but there was a trend (F(3,28) = 2.4, P = 0.085) on Test Day 1 that was not seen on other test days. A Dunnett's test confirmed that there was a significant effect of MPH at 10 mg/kg compared with saline on Test Day 1 (P = 0.048) when administered before an extinction session.

Experiment 2: Post-extinction session administration of MPH dose-dependently enhances retention of extinction

Experiment 2 tested the effects of MPH (0, 2.5, 5, 10, 20, or 40 mg/kg) on extinction consolidation using a single post-session administration of MPH immediately following Extinction.

Figure 2 indicates that there was a persistent effect of methylphenidate administration that caused lower freezing across test days. Animals that received 20 mg/kg MPH showed reliably lower freezing compared with the saline group, while animals that received other MPH doses did not show reliable differences across test days. Statistical analysis confirmed a significant main effect of Dose (F(5,82) = 3.5, P = 0.006) and Test Day (F(2,130) = 17.0, P < 0.0005). There was no significant Dose × Test Day interaction (F(8,130) = 0.607, P < 0.770). Post hoc analysis with Dunnett's test showed an overall significant difference between saline and 20 mg/kg (P = 0.007).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Between-session effects of MPH (black arrow) administered immediately after a 12-min extinction session. Error bars indicate SEM. (*) P < 0.05 significant difference compared with saline.

To examine the test effects more closely, we analyzed within-session differences between each dose of MPH and saline during each test session. Figure 3 shows the results of each test session plotted in 3-min time blocks. There were no differences across tests between saline and the 2.5 mg/kg dose (Fig. 3A). Post-extinction administration of 5 mg/kg MPH (Fig. 3B) caused a transient difference in freezing during the first 3 min of Test 1 (t(32) = 2.7, P = 0.012). Post-extinction administration of 10 mg/kg MPH (Fig. 3C) caused decreased freezing in Blocks 1 and 2 of Test 1 compared with saline (t(42) = 2.8, P = 0.008, and t(42) = 2.0, P = 0.049, respectively), as well as an overall effect of 10 mg/kg MPH for Test 1 (t(42) = 2.2, P = 0.036). Post-extinction administration of 20 mg/kg MPH (Fig. 3D) caused decreased freezing during Test 1 (t(44) = 2.6, P = 0.012), Test 2 (t(44) = 3.3, P = 0.002), and Test 3 (t(44) = 3.1, P = 0.004). Post-extinction administration of 40 mg/kg MPH (Fig. 3E) trended toward decreased freezing compared with saline during Test 1 (t(32) = 2.0, P = 0.051), with the second and third blocks showing significant differences (t(32) = 2.7, P = 0.011, and t(32) = 2.4, P = 0.02, respectively). The 40 mg/kg MPH dose (Fig. 3E) was significantly different from saline during Test 3 (t(32) = 2.237, P = 0.032), with reliable differences during the second block (t(32) = 3.8, P = 0.001).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Within-session effects of MPH administered immediately after a 12-min extinction session. Comparisons of methylphenidate doses (2.5, 5, 10, 20, and 40 mg/kg, panels AE, respectively) to saline during test sessions revealed differences during particular 3-min blocks for all doses higher than 2.5 mg/kg. Error bars indicate SEM. (*) P < 0.05 significant difference compared with saline. (Asterisk [*] above black line) Significant difference compared with saline over total test session.

To assess potential nonspecific effects of these doses on locomotion, we conducted another experiment that was identical to Experiment 2, except that there was no shock in Day 1 (i.e., post-session injections following Day 2 and subsequent testing on Days 3–5). We found no effects of these doses on locomotor behavior during any of the sessions (Supplemental Fig. 2). Thus, although there are clear acute locomotor effects of MPH (Supplemental Fig. 1), these effects do not persist to further test sessions, indicating that the effects on freezing in Figures 2 and 3 are likely not due to simple persistent effects on activity.

Experiment 3: Four-hour delayed administration of MPH following an extinction session has no effect on retention of extinction

To test whether the extinction enhancement effects seen in Experiments 1 and 2 were due to the temporal proximity of the injection to the extinction session, animals were administered a single dose of MPH 4 h following an extinction session. If MPH targets extinction memory consolidation, then a 4-h post-session injection should have little effect. Animals were returned to their homecage during the 4-h interval before being administered methylphenidate (0, 20, or 40 mg/kg).

Although Figure 4 visually suggests a dose-dependent effect of MPH on extinction, there was no significant effect of MPH treatment on freezing during test days. Statistical analysis confirmed that there was no main effect of Dose (F(2,29) = 0.7, P = 0.503) but there was a significant effect of Test Day (F(2,58) = 23.4, P < 0.0005). There was no Dose × Test Day interaction (F(4,58) = 1.341, P = 0.266). Within-session analysis of test days confirmed that there were no test days or 3-min blocks significantly different from saline at 20 mg/kg or 40 mg/kg MPH.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Effects of MPH (black arrow) administered 4 h after a 12-min extinction session. Mice that received methylphenidate (0, 20, and 40 mg/kg) 4 h following extinction did not show significant differences in freezing response during test days. Error bars indicate SEM.

Experiment 4: Brief extinction immediately followed by administration of MPH has no effect on extinction retention

This experiment examined the effects of MPH (0, 10, 20 mg/kg) on extinction under conditions of reduced within-session extinction. Figure 5 shows no effect of MPH on freezing during test days. Statistical analysis confirmed that there was no main effect of Dose (F(2,21) = 0.416, P = 0.665), but there was a significant effect of test days (F(2,42) = 20.7, P < 0.0005) and no Dose × Test Day interaction (F(4,42) = 0.924, P = 0.459).

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Effects of MPH (black arrow) administered immediately after a 3-min extinction session. Mice that received methylphenidate (0, 10, and 20 mg/kg) following brief (3-min) extinction did not show significant differences in freezing response during test days. Error bars indicate SEM.

Discussion

These experiments demonstrate that extinction can be enhanced by MPH under certain conditions. Pre-session injections of MPH (10 mg/kg) promoted extinction on Test Day 1, but this effect was complicated by locomotor activation during extinction (Experiment 1). Experiment 2 showed that a single post-extinction session administration of MPH caused extinction enhancement on subsequent test days. This effect was most obvious with the 20 mg/kg MPH dose but other doses (5, 10, and 40 mg/kg) also revealed enhancements. The extinction enhancements were not observed when MPH was administered 4 h following extinction (Experiment 3). No extinction or fear memory enhancement occurred when the animal received a brief extinction trial followed by MPH administration (Experiment 4). These results are consistent with literature demonstrating the importance of dopaminergic and noradrenergic signaling for extinction learning (Bernardi and Lattal 2010; Holtzmann-Assif et al. 2010).

Previous studies that have examined DAT and NET blockers such as cocaine or amphetamine have generally used a pre-session administration paradigm to determine whether these drugs may have effects on fear acquisition or extinction (e.g., Pezze and Feldon 2004). One challenge in interpreting results of experiments with pre-session injections is disentangling the contribution of locomotor effects from effects on learning. Another challenge is that the interoceptive state induced by these drugs may impair attention, which would create a non-specific deficit in extinction learning. Administering MPH (10 mg/kg) immediately preceding an extinction session (Experiment 1) enhanced extinction on the following day, but the effects of MPH (10 mg/kg) on locomotor performance during the extinction session complicate interpretation of the long-term effects. It is possible, for example, that the animal may superstitiously learn that increased locomotor activity prevents the occurrence of shock and would engage this behavior upon return to the context (Skinner 1948). Thus, there are several interpretational challenges associated with pre-session administration of MPH.

By administering MPH immediately after extinction, we avoided some of the issues associated with pre-session injections. This approach ensures that all groups experience extinction under common conditions, and this treatment resulted in differences during test days after extinction. The persistence of the attenuation of freezing after a single injection is particularly notable, because MPH is generally prescribed as a chronic treatment. The changes in fear behavior here cannot be attributed to alterations in the acquisition of extinction or interference of fear memory retrieval and are most likely related to the effects of MPH on consolidation of extinction. It is unlikely that the effect is simply due to locomotor activation (Supplemental Fig. 2), and previous research has demonstrated that MPH's locomotor-activating effects do not persist to days when the drug is absent (Gaytan et al. 1997). The extinction enhancement effect did not occur when injections followed extinction by 4 h (Experiment 3) or when extinction sessions were short (Experiment 4). The absence of an effect with brief extinction trials may suggest that some within-session extinction is required for MPH to have enhancing effects (Weber et al. 2007). Future experiments could clarify these effects by using targeted microinjections of MPH into the prefrontal cortex (PFC), amygdala, or hippocampus, because these are regions that may differentially mediate aspects of fear retrieval and extinction (Maren and Quirk 2004; Busti et al. 2011).

Many studies that have examined MPH have been interested in the drug's effects on attention while MPH is active within the animal (Arnsten and Dudley 2005; Berridge et al. 2006; Tye et al. 2010). Most of these studies have used rat models rather than mice, which show different pharmacokinetic properties with MPH (Faraj et al. 1974), such as longer times for drug metabolism in rats compared with mice. Comparisons between mouse dosing and human dosing are difficult to quantify due to the differences in affinity that are shown by mouse DAT and human DAT to MPH, with MPH inhibiting human DAT fourfold more potently than mouse DAT (Wu and Gu 1999). Mouse NET and human NET show similar affinities to MPH (Han and Gu 2006). In attention or learning tasks with drug present, a locomotor effect from MPH would be detrimental to measuring behavior. However, in the context of enhancing extinction consolidation in mice, it is possible that higher doses of MPH may be necessary to create sufficiently large signaling changes from baseline levels for detectable differences in learning. Future studies could test human subjects undergoing exposure therapy while receiving MPH at clinically prescribed doses ranging up to 1.5 mg/kg (Arnsten and Dudley 2005), which may provide comparable extinction enhancements in humans. Although the dosing is quite different between mice and humans, the mechanism of action for extinction enhancements is likely to be closely related.

Activation of the prefrontal cortex by methylphenidate (Berridge et al. 2006) is one candidate for the mediation of the extinction enhancements reported in this study. The infralimbic region of the prefrontal cortex is thought to modulate consolidation and expression of fear extinction (Vidal-Gonzalez et al. 2006; Burgos-Robles et al. 2007), and Marsteller et al. (2002) demonstrated that when MPH is paired with a mild stressor, there is a large increase in dopamine release in the medial prefrontal cortex compared with MPH administered with no stressor or 2 h following a stressor. Arnsten and Dudley (2005) demonstrated that MPH actions are mediated in part by D1 receptors, and Hikind and Maroun (2008) showed that blockade of infralimbic cortex D1 receptors impairs consolidation of extinction. The interaction between stress-induced norepinephrine release and methylphenidate is less clear (Marsteller et al. 2002), although previous studies in our laboratory have shown the importance of adrenergic receptors to fear and drug memory consolidation following extinction (Bernardi et al. 2009; Bernardi and Lattal 2010). These previous studies, in combination with our present study, suggest that methylphenidate is likely to be specifically involved with enhancing consolidation of an extinction memory through dopaminergic and noradrenergic mechanisms when administered immediately following extinction.

An alternative explanation for the effects seen with post-extinction methylphenidate may be related to counterconditioning of the fearful context. Using an aversive-to-appetitive transfer procedure, Bouton and Peck (1992) showed that cues previously associated with an aversive outcome can be trained to predict a rewarding outcome. Bouton (1993) proposed that the counterconditioning effect was not due to incompatible behavioral responses, but rather a central interaction between a rewarding memory and an aversive memory. Methylphenidate is known to induce conditioned place preference (CPP) (Nolley and Kelley 2007) at the dose range (20 mg/kg) that was effective for enhancing extinction in this study, indicating that an interaction between reward networks and the fear extinction circuit may allow for extinction enhancement. Because fear and drug extinction circuitry overlap in the prefrontal cortex (Peters et al. 2009), MPH's rewarding properties and unique pharmacology (Volkow et al. 1995; Berridge et al. 2006) could be quite useful for behavioral therapies involving extinction learning.

In these experiments, it appears that dopamine and norepinephrine transporter blockade following an extinction session caused enhanced consolidation. These findings have implications for disorders such as ADHD and PTSD. As discussed by Johansen et al. (2002), ADHD is associated with dysfunctional dopaminergic signaling, along with failures in reinforcement and extinction learning. Treatment with MPH may have several different beneficial effects on behavior, including restoration of extinction processes that are deficient in ADHD, and weakening of PTSD symptoms in veterans (Houlihan 2010). This study demonstrates that MPH treatment in combination with fear extinction will enhance extinction retention and suggests a possible pharmacological treatment that could be useful in combination with exposure therapy for patients with fear disorders.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Male C57BL/6 mice (n = 240) ranging from 7 to 11 wk of age (28 ± 5 g) were used in these experiments. Animals were purchased from Jackson Laboratory (Bar Harbor, ME) and given at least 7 d in the vivarium prior to experimental use. For 2 d before conditioning, mice were handled and given a 0.2-mL saline (0.9% NaCl) injection each day to habituate to injections and handling. Animals were housed four to a cage, and all animals within a cage received the same experimental conditions. Polycarbonate cages were held in a Thoren rack, and animals were given access to food and water ad libitum. Vivarium and experiment room temperatures were maintained at 22°C ± 1°C, and subjects were maintained on a 12-h light–dark cycle (lights on 0600 h–1800 h). Animals were moved from the vivarium to the experiment room 60 min before the start of an experiment, and experiments were conducted between 1100 and 1700 h. All experimental procedures were approved by the OHSU Institutional Animal Use and Care Committee and were conducted in accordance with National Institutes of Health (NIH) “Principles of Laboratory Animal Care” (NIH Publication No. 86-23, revised 1985).

Drugs

Methylphenidate HCl (Sigma-Aldrich) was dissolved in saline (0.9% NaCl) at concentrations of 2.5, 5, 10, 20, and 40 mg/kg. Drug was administered intraperitoneally (i.p.) in a volume of 10 mL/kg. Doses were selected based on previous studies indicating MPH enhancement of cognitive function and fear memory (Arnsten and Dudley 2005; Nolley and Kelley 2007; Zheng et al. 2008).

Apparatus

Fear conditioning

Four Coulbourn Instruments mouse-conditioning chambers (H10-11M-TC) were used. Chambers were housed in sound and light-attenuating cabinets with a fan providing 70 dB of background noise. A Plexiglas cylinder, 21.5 cm in diameter and 23 cm in height, was placed on a grid floor in the chamber. The grid floor consisted of stainless steel rods, 3.2 mm in diameter, spaced 6.4 mm apart. Scrambled shock (2 sec, 0.35 mA) was delivered to the grid floor by a computer controlled shock generator (Coulbourn H13-15). Above the Plexiglas cylinder, an automated infrared activity monitor (Coulbourn H24-61) recorded activity in Graphic State 3.01 software.

Behavioral procedures

General conditioning procedure

On Day 1 (Acquisition), subjects received a 12-min exposure to the context with four unsignaled shocks, delivered at 2.5, 5, 9, and 11.5 min. In all experiments, groups were matched following acquisition to ensure equal terminal freezing levels across MPH dose assignments. On Day 2, mice received a 12-min (Experiments 1, 2, and 3) or a 3-min (Experiment 4) nonreinforced exposure to the context (Extinction). In all post-session injection experiments (Experiments 2, 3, and 4), groups were matched within experiments to ensure equal levels of terminal freezing before MPH injections. Animals with higher freezing response during the final 3-min block compared with the initial 3-min block on Day 2 were excluded from data analysis due to their failure to extinguish fear responding in Experiments 2 and 3. On Days 3–5 (Test Days 1–3), animals received 12-min nonreinforced exposures to the context on each day.

Experiment 1: Pre-extinction administration of MPH

This experiment tested the effects of acute MPH given before an extinction session to determine whether MPH could enhance acquisition and retention of extinction. On Day 1, animals (n = 8 per group) received an injection of saline, followed by acquisition training. On Day 2, animals received either MPH (2.5, 5, 10 mg/kg) or saline, followed immediately by a 12-min extinction session (Fig. 1). On Days 3–5, animals received an injection of saline, followed by additional 12-min extinction sessions.

Experiment 2: Post-extinction administration of MPH

This experiment examined the effect of acute MPH administered immediately after an extinction session. The post-session administration allowed animals to be given drug or saline under common conditions during a period of memory consolidation (Abel and Lattal 2001). On all days of conditioning and testing, animals received injections post-session. On Day 1, animals received an acquisition session with administration of saline. On Day 2, animals received either MPH or saline immediately following extinction. On Days 3, 4, and 5, animals received injections of saline following 12-min nonreinforced sessions (Fig. 2). The doses of MPH that were given to the animals following extinction were distributed across three separate replications. Experiment 2a consisted of doses 0, 2.5, 5, and 10 mg/kg (n = 8 per group). Experiment 2b consisted of doses 0, 10, and 20 mg/kg (n = 16 per group). Experiment 2c consisted of doses 0, 20, and 40 mg/kg (n = 8 per group). Doses that were replicated were pooled for final analysis following statistical confirmation that there was no effect of replication on any dose. Data for Test Days 1–3 for each dose compared with saline are shown in 3-min blocks in Figure 3. Six animals were removed from both the saline and 10 mg/kg groups, and four animals were removed from the 20 mg/kg group because they failed to meet extinction criterion.

Experiment 3: Four-hour delayed administration of MPH

In this experiment, the end of the extinction session and the administration of MPH were separated by 4 h. The temporal separation tested whether drug administration, unpaired from extinction training, would affect freezing response on the following days (Burgos-Robles et al. 2007). The animals (n = 12 per group) still received saline injections on all days following exposure to the context in order to ensure equivalence to treatment conditions in Experiment 2. However, in this experiment, following the extinction session, all animals received an injection of saline and 4 h later received either MPH (20 or 40 mg/kg) or saline (Fig. 4). In accordance with exclusion criteria, one animal was removed from both saline and 40 mg/kg MPH groups, and two animals were removed from the 20 mg/kg MPH group for final analysis.

Experiment 4: Brief extinction followed by administration of MPH

This experiment tested whether MPH can facilitate extinction under conditions of reduced within-session extinction. A brief extinction period should ensure that animals only undergo partial extinction and may increase the detection of MPH enhancements. Animals (n = 8 per group) received injections of saline on Days 1, 3, 4, 5, and 26. On Day 2, animals received either MPH (10, 20 mg/kg) or saline following a brief (3-min) extinction period (Fig. 5).

Data analysis

Fear memory expression was determined by freezing response within the context. Freezing was defined as an episode of at least 3 sec of inactivity. Total freezing time was divided by 12 min to calculate percentage of time freezing in each day. Data analyses were performed with SPSS version 17.0. Data for Acquisition, Extinction, and Brief Extinction were analyzed using analysis of variance (ANOVA). Test Days 1–3 were analyzed using Repeated Measures ANOVA with Day as a within-subjects factor and Dose as a between-subjects factor. When sphericity could not be assumed for data, reported degrees of freedom reflect a Greenhouse-Geisser estimate. All post hoc comparisons of Repeated Measures ANOVA data were performed using a Dunnett's test. Two-tailed t-tests were used for post hoc analysis of 3-min blocks for each post-session dose compared with saline. Equal variance was assumed based on nonsignificant Levene's tests for each dose at reported time points. For all statistical tests, the α was set to 0.05.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grants from the NIH (NIDA T32DA007262, NIDA R01DA025922, NIMH R01MH077111).

Footnotes

[Supplemental material is available for this article.]

References

  1. Abel T, Lattal KM 2001. Molecular mechanisms of memory acquisition, consolidation and retrieval. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11: 180–187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arnsten AFT, Dudley A 2005. Methylphenidate improves prefrontal cortical cognitive function through α-2 adrenoceptor and dopamine D1 receptor actions: Relevance to therapeutic effects in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Behav Brain Funct 1: 2–11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bernardi RE, Lattal KM 2010. A role for α-adrenergic receptors in extinction of conditioned fear and cocaine conditioned place preference. Behav Neurosci 124: 204–210 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bernardi RE, Ryabinin AE, Berger SP, Lattal KM 2009. Post-retrieval disruption of a cocaine conditioned place preference by systemic and intrabasolateral amygdala β2- and α1-adrenergic antagonists. Learn Mem 16: 777–789 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Berridge CW, Devilbiss DM, Andrzejewski ME, Arnsten AFT, Kelley AE, Schmeichel B, Hamilton C, Spencer RC 2006. Methylphenidate preferentially increases catecholamine neurotransmission within the prefrontal cortex at low doses that enhance cognitive function. Biol Psychiatry 60: 1111–1120 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Borowski TB, Kokkinidis L 1998. The effects of cocaine, amphetamine, and the dopamine D1 receptor agonist SKF 38393 on fear extinction as measured with potentiated startle: Implications for psychomotor stimulant psychosis. Behav Neurosci 112: 952–965 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bouton ME 1993. Context, time and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychol Bull 114: 80–99 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bouton ME, Peck CA 1992. Spontaneous recovery in cross-motivational transfer (counterconditioning). Anim Learn Behav 20: 313–321 [Google Scholar]
  9. Burgos-Robles A, Vidal-Gonzalez I, Santini E, Quirk GJ 2007. Consolidation of fear extinction requires NMDA receptor-dependent bursting in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Neuron 53: 871–880 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Busti D, Geracitano R, Whittle N, Dalezios Y, Mańko M, Kaufmann W, Sätzler K, Singewald N, Capogna M, Ferraguti F 2011. Different fear states engage distinct networks within the intercalated cell clusters of the amygdala. J Neurosci 31: 5131–5144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carmack SA, Wood SC, Anagnostaras SG 2010. Amphetamine and extinction of cued fear. Neurosci Lett 468: 18–22 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Faraj BA, Israili ZH, Perel JM, Jenkins ML, Holtzman SG, Cucinell SA, Dayton PG 1974. Metabolism and disposition of methylphenidate-14C: Studies in man and animals. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 191: 535–547 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gaytan O, al-Rahim S, Swann A, Dafny N 1997. Sensitization to locomotor effects of methylphenidate in the rat. Life Sci 61: 101–107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Han DD, Gu HH 2006. Comparison of the monoamine transporters from human and mouse in their sensitivities to psychostimulant drugs. BMC Pharmacol 6: 6 10.1186/1471-2210-6-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hikind N, Maroun M 2008. Microinfusion of the D1 receptor antagonist, SCH23390 into the IL but not the BLA impairs consolidation of extinction of auditory fear conditioning. Neurobiol Learn Mem 90: 217–222 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Holtzmann-Assif O, Laurent V, Westbrook RF 2010. Blockade of dopamine activity in the nucleus accumbens impairs learning extinction of conditioned fear. Learn Mem 17: 71–75 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Houlihan DJ 2010. Psychostimulant treatment of combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. J Psychopharmacol 25: 1568–1572 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Johansen EB, Aase H, Meyer A, Sagvolden T 2002. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder behaviour explained by dysfunctioning reinforcement and extinction processes. Behav Brain Res 130: 37–45 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Maren S, Quirk GJ 2004. Neuronal signaling of fear memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 5: 844–852 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Marsteller DA, Gerasimov MR, Schiffer WK, Geiger JM, Barnett CR, Schaich Borg J, Scott S, Ceccarelli J, Volkow ND, Molina PE, et al. 2002. Acute handling stress modulates methylphenidate-induced catecholamine overflow in the medial prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology 27: 163–170 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Mazei MS, Pluto CP, Kirkbride B, Pehek EA 2002. Effects of catecholamine uptake blockers in the caudate putamen and subregions of the medial prefrontal cortex of the rat. Brain Res 936: 58–67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Miczek KA, Luttinger D 1978. Differential attenuation of two kinds of conditioned suppression by d-amphetamine and pentobarbital. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 205: 282–290 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Mueller D, Olivera-Figueroa LA, Pine DS, Quirk GJ 2009. The effects of yohimbine and amphetamine on fear expression and extinction in rats. Psychopharmacology 204: 599–606 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Myers KM, Davis M 2007. Mechanisms of fear extinction. Mol Psychiatry 12: 120–150 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. National Institutes of Health 1985. NIH Publication 86-23 Revised. In Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals. Public Health Service, National Institutes of Health [Google Scholar]
  26. Nolley EP, Kelley BM 2007. Adolescent reward system perseveration due to nicotine: Studies with methylphenidate. Neurotoxicol Teratol 29: 47–56 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Peters J, Kalivas PW, Quirk GJ 2009. Extinction circuits for fear and addiction overlap in the prefrontal cortex. Learn Mem 16: 279–288 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Pezze MA, Feldon J 2004. Mesolimbic dopaminergic pathways in fear conditioning. Prog Neurobiol 74: 301–320 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Pezze MA, Bast T, Feldon J 2003. Significance of dopamine transmission in the rat medial prefrontal cortex for conditioned fear. Cereb Cortex 13: 371–380 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Quirk GJ, Mueller D 2008. Neural mechanisms of extinction learning and retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology 33: 56–72 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Quirk GJ, Garcia R, Gonzalez-Lima F 2006. Prefrontal mechanisms in extinction of conditioned fear. Biol Psychiatry 60: 337–343 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Shin LM, Rauch SL, Pitman RK 2006. Amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and hippocampal function in PTSD. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1071: 67–79 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Skinner BF 1948. Superstition in the pigeon. J Exp Psychol 38: 168–172 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Sorg BA, Kalivas PW 1993. Effects of cocaine and footshock stress on extracellular dopamine levels in the medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience 53: 695–703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Tye KM, Tye LD, Cone JJ, Hekkelman EF, Janak PH, Bonci A 2010. Methylphenidate facilitates learning-induced amygdala plasticity. Nature 13: 475–481 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Vidal-Gonzalez I, Vidal-Gonzalez B, Rauch SL, Quirk GJ 2006. Microstimulation reveals opposing influences of prelimbic and infralimbic cortex on the expression of conditioned fear. Learn Mem 13: 728–733 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Volkow ND, Ding YS, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Logan J, Gatley JS, Dewey S, Ashby C, Liebermann J, Hitzemann R, et al. 1995. Is methylphenidate like cocaine? Studies on their pharmacokinetics and distribution in the human brain. Arch Gen Psychiatry 52: 456–463 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Weber M, Hart J, Richardson R 2007. Effects of d-cycloserine on extinction of learned fear to an olfactory cue. Neurobiol Learn Mem 87: 476–482 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Wu X, Gu H 1999. Molecular cloning of the mouse dopamine transporter and pharmacological comparison with the human homologue. Gene 233: 163–170 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Zheng XL, Liu F, Wu XW, Li BM 2008. Infusion of methylphenidate into the basolateral nucleus of amygdala or anterior cingulate enhances fear memory consolidation in rats. Sci China C Life Sci 51: 808–813 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Learning & Memory are provided here courtesy of Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

RESOURCES