Abstract
The GDI1 gene encodes αGDI, which retrieves inactive GDP-bound RAB from membranes to form a cytosolic pool awaiting vesicular release. Mutations in GDI1 are responsible for X-linked Intellectual Disability. Characterization of the Gdi1-null mice has revealed alterations in the total number and distribution of hippocampal and cortical synaptic vesicles, hippocampal short-term synaptic plasticity and specific short-term memory deficits in adult mice, which are possibly caused by alterations of different synaptic vesicle recycling pathways controlled by several RAB GTPases. However, interpretation of these studies is complicated by the complete ablation of Gdi1 in all cells in the brain throughout development. In this study, we generated conditionally gene-targeted mice in which the knockout of Gdi1 is restricted to the forebrain, hippocampus, cortex and amygdala and occurs only during postnatal development. Adult mutant mice reproduce the short-term memory deficit previously reported in Gdi1-null mice. Surprisingly, the delayed ablation of Gdi1 worsens the pre-synaptic phenotype at cortico-amygdala synaptic connections compared to Gdi1-null mice. These results suggest a pivotal role of αGDI via specific RAB GTPases acting specifically in forebrain regions at the pre-synaptic sites involved in memory formation.
Introduction
Human intellectual disability (ID), also referred to as Mental Retardation (MR), is a common human neurodevelopment disorder with onset early in postnatal life. This condition affects approximately 2–3% of the human population [1]. ID is classified based on intelligence quotient (IQ); the presence of other clinical features distinguishes syndromic ID (S-ID) from non-syndromic ID (NS-ID). Over the past 15 years, mutations in ∼ 40 genes have been associated with NS-ID and ∼ 80% of these are found on the X-chromosome. The identification and functional characterization of these genes have greatly enlarged our understanding of human cognition and intellect. Because NS-ID is characterized clinically only by intellectual impairment, the genes underlying this clinical condition are likely to be involved in learning and memory formation. Additionally, the functional study of these genes might help to understand neural development and provide treatment strategies for NS-ID.
The GDI1 gene encodes αGDI, a protein that controls the cycling of RAB GTPase [2]. RAB GTPases are proteins involved in the control of intracellular traffic; they act as molecular switches between active GTP-bound and inactive GDP-bound conformations. The role of αGDI is to bind the RAB in its GDP-bound state from membranes and to maintain a cytosolic pool of GDP-associated RAB. The discovery of mutations in the GDI1 [3] and RAB39B [4] genes in ID patients supports the importance of the intracellular trafficking mediated by the αGDI-RAB GTPase pathway in the development of cognitive function.
We have previously demonstrated that the lack of αGDI in Gdi1-null mice impaired hippocampal dependent forms of short-term memory, namely, the working and associative fear-related memory formation, and greatly reduced male aggression, thereby modifying social interaction [5].
We also showed that the absence of αGDI in the brain leads to alterations in the total number of hippocampal and cortical synaptic vesicles (SV) during synaptic differentiation at postnatal days (PND) 10 and 30. In adult mice at PND 90, the distribution of SV was altered at differentiated neuronal terminals, and the reserve pool (RP) appeared to be specifically affected [6]; this likely resulted in slow recovery after SV depletion and a short-term memory deficit in Gdi1-null mice.
Thus, the complete ablation of Gdi1 leads to a complex phenotype that is most likely the result of alterations in the temporal and spatial functioning of αGDI and some of the associated RABs. Our previous results suggest that these alterations might be due to the reduced availability of the endocytic RABs RAB4 and RAB5, which are involved in slow SV recycling and in alterations of RAB3A, which is the main RAB protein mediating SV exocytosis. Nevertheless, other RAB proteins may be involved in the Gdi1-null phenotype, particularly proteins that have recently been shown to be associated with SV membranes [7].
A major limitation for the study of αGDI function in the central nervous system (CNS) and the interpretation of all results obtained from the analysis of Gdi1-null mice is the fact that the complete ablation of Gdi1 occurs in all brain regions and all brain cell types throughout development in these mice. Moreover, the previously observed phenotype of the Gdi1-null mice may be due to alteration of several RAB GTPases and subsequent compensation.
To solve this problem, we used the CRE/lox P recombination system to restrict Gdi1 knockout both spatially and temporally to postmitotic neurons of the anterior forebrain. These conditional Gdi1 knockout mice were viable and developed without any obvious morphological defects. We show that the conditional Gdi1 mice phenocopy the cognitive deficit and showed greatly reduced male aggression, as was previously observed in the Gdi1-null mice, confirming the pivotal role of αGDI in hippocampal, cortical and amygdala synapses in the process of memory formation. Surprisingly, the electrophysiological comparison at cortico-amygdala synaptic connections between the Gdi1-null and conditional Gdi1 mice reveals a stronger pre-synaptic phenotype in conditional Gdi1 mice. Because significant levels of recombination do not start until the end of the third postnatal week, when many of the CNS neuronal pathways have already been established, these results suggest that the regulation of RABs by αGDI directly modulates complex behavior that is important for cognitive function.
Results
Inactivation of αGDI occurs in specific brain regions during postnatal life
To generate a conditional Gdi1 mouse model, we generated a floxed allele of Gdi1 by flanking exons 2 and 3 of the mouse Gdi1 gene with two lox P sites using homologous recombination in ES cells (Fig. 1A). One homologous recombinant clone with the highest percentage of normal karyotypes (90%) was analyzed by Southern blot (Fig. 1B and C) and was injected into blastocysts. Derived germline targeted females offspring carrying one floxed allele (Gdi1lox/X) were first crossed with transgenic male mice expressing the tgFlp recombinase to excise the neomycin cassette and subsequently backcrossed into a C57BL/6N genetic background. All subsequent work was performed using neo− mice.
To inactivate Gdi1 in the postnatal forebrain, we crossed heterozygous Gdi1lox/X female mice with the transgenic line CaMKII-Cre-159 [8] (provided from Dr. R. Brambilla, HSR, Milan-Italy) to generate CaMKII-Cre+-Gdi1flox/Y and CaMKII-Cre−-Gdi1lox/Y male mice (referred to as Gdi1flox/Y for Gdi1 knockout and Gdi1lox/Y for wild-type mice) for all subsequent work.
Western blot analysis was performed on several brain regions at different developmental stages to assess the regional and temporal pattern of αGDI inactivation driven by CaMKII-Cre-159 transgene activity in vivo (Fig. 2). At several postnatal days (ranging from PND 14 to 56), hippocampus (Hip), cerebral cortex (CC), olfactory bulbs (OB), cerebellum (CbC) and a mixture of the thalamus (T), hypothalamus (H) and amygdala (BLA) were dissected from Gdi1flox/Y and compared with Gdi1lox/Y littermate mice (PND 56) (Fig. 2). No difference in the αGDI protein level was observed in the OB (mean ± SD; Gdi1flox/Y 1.44±0.37; Gdi1lox/Y 1.5±0.38), CbC (mean ± SD; Gdi1flox/Y 4.14±0.97; Gdi1lox/Y 4.48±1) and T-H-BLA (mean ± SD; Gdi1flox/Y 1.2±0.33; Gdi1lox/Y 1.51±0.05) homogenates, confirming that recombination was not active in these brain regions, as previously reported [8]. At PND 28, αGDI was still detectable but down regulated by 50% in the Hip (t-test, p = 0.05) and by 65% in CC (t-test, p = 0.02). The down-regulation reached 80% at PND 56 in the Hip (t-test, p = 0.01) and 98% in CC (t-test, p = 0.005), as shown in Fig. 2B.
Additionally, double immunofluorescence for αGDI and NeuN on brain slices from Gdi1lox/Y, Gdi1flox/Y and Gdi1-null mice at PND 56 revealed that αGDI down-regulation in Gdi1flox/Y was specifically observed in the anterior forebrain, as previously reported [8] (Fig. 3B–E″). Surprisingly, double immunofluorescence for αGDI and glial markers such as S100β (Fig. 3B–E″) or GFAP (data not shown), revealed that αGDI was also down-regulated in S100β and GFAP positive glial cells in Gdi1flox/Y brain regions.
Gdi1flox/Y mice recapitulate the Gdi1-null cognitive and behavioral phenotypes
We previously showed that adult Gdi1-null mice did not have defects in emotional or exploratory behavior but were selectively impaired in hippocampal-dependent tasks important for acquisition of memory across short time intervals [5]. To assess whether the deletion of αGDI in post-natal life and in forebrain regions plays a key role in associative memory formation, Gdi1flox/Y and Gdi1lox/Y male littermates were subjected to a battery of behavioral tests at PND 60 in order to assess exploration, learning and social recognition, as previously preformed for Gdi1-null mice [5].
Exploration tests
Locomotors activity scores did not show significant genotype-dependent differences in the light-dark box, emergence test and the novelty test (data not shown), as previously observed for Gdi1-null mice [5].
Learning and memory tests
Water Maze
Gdi1flox/Y and Gdi1lox/Y male littermates behaved normally in the standard hidden-platform version of the water maze. No significant differences between genotypes were observed in all the variables analyzed, confirming that Gdi1flox/Y mice do not show alterations in spatial memory, as was previously shown for Gdi1-null mice [5].
Radial Maze
Gdi1flox/Y and Gdi1lox/Y male littermate mice showed altered spatial working memory as assessed by the radial maze test. The number of total errors declined over the ten days of training in both Gdi1flox/Y and Gdi1lox/Y mice, and no difference between genotypes was observed (ANOVA genotype by repeated exposure F[1,24] = 0.545; p = 0.467; Fig. 4A). Gdi1flox/Y and Gdi1lox/Y mice had significantly different performance in the position of the first repetition (ANOVA F[1,24] = 21.47, p = 0.0001). Although Gdi1lox/Y mice eventually reached nearly perfect performance (seven out of a maximum number of eight correct successive arm visits), Gdi1flox/Y mice barely scored above chance level (5.5 correct successive arm visits after 10 days of training) (Fig. 4B). In conclusion, the Gdi1flox/Y mice seem to have no defects in procedural learning but do have a specific and severe deficit in working memory, as was seen in Gdi1-null mice [5].
Fear Conditioning
Mice were also tested for contextual and trace fear conditioning. No differences were observed between Gdi1flox/Y and Gdi1lox/Y mice in the ability to associate the CS with the US during the training session (ANOVA genotype effect: F[1,40] = 0.208, p = 0.65) as well as 24 h later when testing the context (ANOVA genotype effect: F[1,40] = 0.008, p = 0.929) and cue memory test (ANOVA genotype effect: F[1,40] = 0.239, p = 0.628) in the contextual fear conditioning (data not shown). Instead, as previously shown for the Gdi1-null mice [5], there was a significant difference between genotypes in trace fear conditioning when animals were tested for freezing in the training session (ANOVA genotype effect: F[1,24] = 12.5, p = 0.001) and tested for context (ANOVA genotype effect: F[1,24] = 4.8, p = 0.004), as shown in Fig. 4C, D.
Resident-intruder test
Gdi1flox/Y and Gdi1lox/Y mice were tested in the resident-intruder test. Significant difference between genotypes was observed in the total aggression time (ANOVA genotype effect: F[1,8] = 24.4, p = 0.001), the number of attacks (ANOVA genotype effect: F[1,8] = 38.7, p = 0.0003) and in the time spent sniffing the head of the intruder (ANOVA genotype effect: F[1,8] = 7.1, p = 0.03) (Fig. 4E–G).
In conclusion, all of these data suggest that the down-regulation of αGDI in adult forebrain regions is sufficient to recapitulate the learning deficit previously shown in Gdi1-null mice [5]. These results suggest that αGDI acts via RAB GTPase availability to play a pivotal role in Gdi1-dependent short-term memory formation and maintenance.
A defect in excitatory transmission onto lateral amygdala principal cells in the absence of αGDI points to a defect in fast SV recycling
Glutamatergic lateral amygdala (LA) principal cells constitute the vast majority of neuronal cells in this amygdala nucleus [9] and can be easily identified by their electrophysiological properties [10]. They receive not only excitatory synapses from incoming projections from the cortex (cortico-LA synapses) and the medial division of the medial geniculate nucleus of the thalamus (MGm, thalamo-LA synapses) [11] but also input from neighboring principal cell collaterals. To investigate whether the emotional-related brain structures such as the amygdaloid nucleus may have a role in the fear-related behavior observed in the mutant mice, synaptic plasticity was analyzed at those synaptic connections.
To better define the previously described hippocampal short-term synaptic defect [6] and to provide a comparison between Gdi1flox/Y and Gdi1-null mice, the overall number of excitatory synaptic contacts impinging onto LA principal cells in juvenile (PND 28) Gdi1-null and Gdi1 WT littermates was first examined by measuring spontaneous and miniature EPSCs (in presence of 10 µM Tetrodotoxin) (Fig. 5A, B). In control conditions, we observed a strong reduction in both the frequency (Gdi1 WT: 7.75±0.85 Hz; Gdi1-null: 2.93±0.26 Hz, p<0.001, n = 10 cells in both conditions) and the amplitude of recorded EPSCs (Gdi1 WT: 10.33±0.80 pA; Gdi1-null: 7.02±0.39 pA, p<0.001, Fig. 5B, C2 ). Interestingly, a significant decrease in EPSC frequency persisted in presence of 10 µM TTX (mEPSCs: Gdi1 WT: 5.71±0.35 Hz, Gdi1-null: 3.08±0.63 Hz, p<0.001, Fig. 5B). In contrast, mean mEPSC amplitude was similar in control and mutant animals (Gdi1 WT: 7.44±0.11 pA; Gdi1-null: 6.96±0.13 pA, p>0.05, Fig. 5C2 ). Taken together, these results indicate that in Gdi1-null slices, there is little multi-vesicular release and little of spontaneous discharge activity of incoming excitatory axons.
A decrease in mEPSC frequency can result directly from a decrease in the size of the receptor field of LA principal cells, either due to a diminished dendritic tree or due to a lack of dendritic spines. To address these points, we first applied depolarizing steps to assess the cellular capacitance, but we failed to detect any difference in the cell size between genotypes (Capacitance: Gdi1 WT: 25.5±3.4 pF; Gdi1-null: 19.2±1.95 pF, p>0.05, n = 24 and 35 cells, respectively), suggesting that the main morphological properties were conserved and that probably the receptor field was similar in principal cells from Gdi1-null and control mice. Then, to directly visualize dendritic spines, we filled the recorded LA principal cells with 10 µM Alexa-488 while acquiring confocal fluorescence pictures of level III/IV dendrites using 2-photon laser scanning microscopy [12] but again did not observe any differences between Gdi1 WT and Gdi1-null mice (data not shown). These data suggest the presence of a lower release probability and/or a lower number of functional excitatory synapses onto LA principal cells but a normal quantal size in Gdi1-null mice.
Alteration of cortico-LA synaptic weight in the absence of αGDI
Cortical projections to the LA can be directly activated by positioning a stimulation electrode within the external capsule in coronal LA-containing acute slices of mouse brain (Fig. 6A) [13]. The application of a short (1 ms) current pulse elicits a monosynaptic EPSC whose amplitude increases progressively with the stimulation intensity (Fig. 6B). At the maximal stimulation intensity - determined as not being noxious for the incoming fibers, evoked EPSC (eEPSC) amplitude reaches a maximal value (eEPSCmax, Fig. 6B). Interestingly, the input/output (I/O) relationships obtained in Gdi1 WT and Gdi1-null animals were different. Indeed, the first eEPSC was obtained at similar stimulation intensities, indicating that the AP-threshold may be similar in both genotypes, while the amplitude of the eEPSC at supra-threshold intensities was systematically lower in null mice (at 10 mA*ms: Gdi1 WT: 328±67 pA; Gdi1-null: 110±41 pA, p<0.01, n = 8 and 14 cells, respectively, Fig. 6C1 ). Furthermore, when extrapolated, the I/O curves from both genotypes gave different eEPSCmax values, suggesting that the synaptic weight of cortico-LA synapses was altered in the absence of αGDI (eEPSCmax: Gdi1 WT: 491±74 pA; Gdi1-null: 217±46 pA, p<0.01).
In accordance with previous results, the quantal size (Q) at cortico-LA synapses was unaffected in the absence of αGDI. Indeed, by replacing part of the external Ca2+-concentration with strontium ions (Sr2+), a “loose” synchronization of vesicular fusion is usually obtained, allowing the analysis of currents associated with single fusion events in the tail of the main EPSC (Fig. 6D1 ). A careful analysis of these desynchronized events showed that mEPSC amplitude at cortico-LA synapses was comparable in both genotypes (Sr2+ mEPSC: Gdi1 WT: 7.33±0.8 pA; Gdi1-null: 7.22±0.48 pA, p>0.05, n = 4 cells in each condition) and resembled global miniature activity (Fig. 6D2 ). Thus, alteration of the I/O curve did not seem to result from a change in quantal size.
Modification of some pre-synaptic parameters at cortico-LA synapses absent in αGDI
Short-term synaptic plasticity is essentially determined by residual calcium in the synaptic terminal and by the rate at which release sites are refilled by fusion-competent synaptic vesicles [14]. The refilling rate can be assessed by high-frequency iterative stimulations: after the consumption of the pool of release-ready vesicles (RRP), neurotransmitter (NT) release usually stabilizes. The plateau level is then determined by the “refilling rate”, which is mostly dependent on vesicular recycling at the synaptic terminal [15].
Thus, we looked at vesicular pool dynamics by applying 20 Hz/4 sec stimulation trains at cortico-LA synapses from Gdi1 WT and Gdi1-null animals. At a typical WT synapse, NT release first facilitates and then progressively decreases to stabilize at ∼20% of the initial EPSC amplitude (Fig. 7B, C) [16]. The absence of αGDI led to major changes in the postsynaptic response to 20 Hz trains; after a first phase of facilitation due to calcium accumulation, the synaptic depression was exacerbated, and the final plateau level was significantly lower compared to Gdi1 WT littermates (Fig. 7B, C). Both RRP size and refilling rate were extracted from cumulative plots of EPSC amplitude (Fig. 7D, E1, E2 ) [15]. In the absence of αGDI, the refilling rate was strongly depressed (Gdi1 WT: 21.1±4% of initial EPSC; Gdi1-null: 9.4±1.7% of the initial EPSC, n = 9 and 11 cells, respectively, p<0.05, Fig. 7E2 ), whereas the RRP size was preserved (Gdi1 WT: 1503±136% of initial EPSC; Gdi1-null: 1498±257% of the initial EPSC, p>0.05, Fig. 7E1 ). Finally, we noticed that no post-tetanic potentiation was present at Gdi1-null synapses (data not shown), possibly because of the higher depression observed during high-frequency trains. The lack of potentiation indicated that synaptic plasticity was deeply affected in the absence of αGDI.
Conditional Gdi1 mutation indicates the potential pre-synaptic role of αGDI
As reported above, a progressive decrease in αGDI expression is observed in the conditional Gdi1 mouse after 4 weeks of age (PND 28) in the hippocampus, cerebral cortex and amygdala, whereas it remains elevated in the medial division of the medial geniculate nucleus (MGm, Fig. 3E-E″ and I-I″). Thus, the examination of synaptic physiology at thalamo-LA and cortico-LA synapses in 8–10 week-old Gdi1flox/Y animals, i.e., a time at which αGDI deletion is effective, directly examines the consequences of pre- and post-synaptic absence of αGDI at excitatory synapses.
In PND 100 animals, we first compared I/O curves at the two major excitatory entries to the LA by apposing stimulation electrodes in the external and internal capsules (Fig. 8A, D) [13]. In Gdi1 WT animals, the I/O curves reach an eEPSCmax of ∼2 mA at both stimulated pathways (cortico-LA: 2238±272 pA, thalamo-LA: 2345±279 pA, Fig. 8C, F). Similar to previous results in P100 Gdi1-null animals, both I/O curves were strongly affected (eEPSCmax: cortico-LA: 1063±164 pA, thalamo-LA: 995±224 pA, both p<0.001 as compared to Gdi1 WT). A normal I/O relationship was observed in P100 Gdi1flox/Y mice in the thalamo-LA pathway (Gdi1flox/Y: 2083±298 pA, p>0.05 as compared to Gdi1 WT, Fig. 8 F), indicating that postsynaptic αGDI was not crucial in determining synaptic efficacy at these synapses and that the deficit observed in the constitutive null mice was due to the pre-synaptic absence of αGDI. In contrast, the delayed absence of αGDI at cortico-LA synapses was associated with a strong alteration in the I/O curve (Gdi1flox/Y: 1155±103 pA, p<0.001 compared to Gdi1 WT, Fig. 8C), which mimicked the constitutive mutation. Thus, these results show that the presence of pre-synaptic αGDI is essential in maintaining normal excitatory projections to the LA.
Delayed absence of αGDI causes additional pre-synaptic deficits
We next compared short-term plasticity at cortico-LA synapses in PND 100 Gdi1 WT, Gdi1-null and Gdi1flox/Y mice using trains of 80 stimulations 20 Hz in magnitude (Fig. 8G). Post-synaptic responses to high frequency trains were affected at null synapses and were characterized by higher fatigue compared to WT synapses (Fig. 8G). However, analysis of cumulative EPSCs showed a major difference between both mutant mice: as seen in young animals (Fig. 7), a decrease in the refilling rate is present in not only PND 100 Gdi1-null mice but also Gdi1flox/Y PND 100 mice (Fig. 8I). More surprisingly, the RRP size of Gdi1flox/Y cortico-LA synapses was found to be significantly lower than in Gdi1 WT and Gdi1-null mice (Gdi1 WT: 2287±312% of initial EPSC, Gdi1-null: 2181±270%, Gdi1flox/Y: 1201±239%, p>0.05 for both, Fig. 8H). This unexpected observation may indicate the existence of cellular mechanisms that correct the synaptic dysfunction when the mutation occurs during early development.
Discussion
We previously showed that Gdi1-null mice had a specific hippocampus-dependent short-term memory impairment and overfriendly behavior [5]. We suggested that the memory deficit might be caused by alterations in the SV pool and short-term synaptic plasticity involving alterations of different trafficking pathways that are controlled by several RAB GTPase proteins [6].
A major limitation for the study of αGDI function in the central nervous system (CNS) is that Gdi1 is ubiquitously expressed and that complete ablation of Gdi1 occurs in all brain regions and all brain cell types throughout development in the null mice.
In this study, we used the CRE/lox P recombination system to spatially and temporally restrict knockout of the Gdi1 gene to postmitotic neurons and glia of the anterior forebrain.
Our results show that Gdi1flox/Y mice, in which Gdi1 is strongly reduced from PND 56 in the hippocampus, cortex and amygdala, were viable and fertile, and unlike the Gdi1-null mice, they did not present visible morphological or neuro-pathological phenotypes. The learning and memory and behavioral characterization of these mice showed no difference in emotion, explorative behavior, spatial memory or contextual fear conditioning. In contrast, Gdi1flox/Y mice suffered from a specific short-term memory deficit, as indicated by alterations in working memory in the radial maze test and associative fear-related memory assessed by trace fear conditioning. Moreover, we also observed loss of aggressive behavior, as previously demonstrated for the Gdi1-null mutants [5]. Taken together, these data suggest that postnatal inactivation of Gdi1 specifically in these brain regions is sufficient and recapitulates the cognitive and social deficits previously seen in Gdi1-null mice.
We then investigated the modifications in synaptic plasticity at different synaptic connections in the cortico-lateral amygdala (LA) of P30 Gdi1-null mice. Previous studies have reported that cortical projections to the LA contribute to emotional learning such as fear-related memory, providing qualitative information in addition to the highly responsive sub-cortical pathway [9]. Thus, we examined cortico-LA synaptic physiology in the absence of αGDI by evaluating synaptic depression in the cortico-amygdala pathway. Our results show a strong reduction in the overall synaptic weight at these synaptic contacts, associated with alterations of pre- but not post-synaptic parameters, suggesting that there are specific alterations in the refilling rate of release sites.
The examination of synaptic physiology at thalamo-LA and cortico-LA synapses in adult Gdi1flox/Y animals provides the opportunity to directly examine the consequences of pre- and post-synaptic absence of αGDI at excitatory synapses. The first important finding was that in contrast to Gdi1-null mutants, the maximal thalamo-LA EPSC was not changed in conditional Gdi1flox/Y mice (Fig. 8). Taken together with the results showing that the number of active cortico-LA synapses is lowered both in conditional and Gdi1-null mice, this suggests that presynaptic but not post-synaptic Gdi1 is important in maintaining the integrity of excitatory projections to the LA. Given the strong link between the physiology of excitatory projections to the LA and emotional learning, it is likely that these synaptic defects participate in the fear-related learning deficit that is seen in αGDI mutant mice. In addition to the functional disappearance of excitatory synapses, we also observed that the ready releasable pool size at the remaining cortico-LA synapses was significantly decreased in conditional but not Gdi1-null mutants (Fig. 8). This possibly suggests that if αGDI deletion occurs during the early phases of synaptogenesis, its absence may be overcame molecularly or functionally, which would no longer be possible in the adult consolidated synapses. Physiological experiments conducted on adult cortico-LA synapses suggest some functional differences. For example, we observed that the release probability at cortico-LA synapses, which is modified by classical fear conditioning procedures [17], is significantly higher in adult animals. Interestingly, this was observed together with a reduction in pre-synaptic expression of long-term plasticity (Y. Humeau F. Gambino, personal communication). Taken together, these observations suggest that during the life of the animal, experience-driven consolidation and maturation at excitatory synapses occur in the lateral amygdala, a phenomenon that depends on the presence of αGDI and possibly specific RAB GTPases involved in SV exo- and endocytosis.
We previously suggested that the lack of αGDI seems to perturb the functionality of different SV pools at the pre-synaptic site and may be due to alterations of at least two different classes of RAB GTPases, the SV-associated RAB3A and the endosomal RAB proteins RAB4 and RAB5 by altering cycling between the active GTP- and inactive GDP-bound state. Despite knowledge about the mechanisms by which RABs regulate intracellular exo- and endocitic events, it is not clear how many RABs are involved in these pathways and whether all of them are susceptible to αGDI-mediated regulation. Recently, Pavlos et al. [7], use d a combination of different techniques to conclude that SVs contained a set of RABs associated with SV membranes involved both in exocytosis and endosomal recycling, including RAB3A/B/C, RAB4B, RAB5A, RAB10, RAB11B, RAB14 and RAB27B. They also showed that RAB27B exhibits both overlapping and distinct features from RAB3.
In conclusion, our results point to a defect in pre-synaptic function that affects several parameters of short-term synaptic plasticity involved in hippocampus-related short-term memory deficits as well as in amygdala-related behavior. Stronger defects in short-term synaptic plasticity are present when Gdi1 is deleted specifically in the forebrain during adult life, suggesting a predominant role of Gdi1 via a subset of specific RAB GTPases acting specifically in these brain regions at the pre-synaptic sites and leading to learning and social alterations. In this regard, our results encourage further studies to identify RAB cell specificity and function in neurons and glial cells, which will most likely shed light on the RABs that determine the Gdi1 learning phenotype.
Materials and Methods
Animals
All animals were maintained on a 12 hrs light/darkness cycle at 22°–25°C. Food pellets and water were available ab libitum.
Experiments were done according to the animal protocols approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee San Raffaele (IACUC) (San Raffaele, Milan, Italy) and were approved by the National Ministry of Health, IACUC ID 470. All experiments were carried out in accordance with the guidelines established by the European Community Council Directive of 24 November 1986 on the use of animals in research (86/609/EEC). All efforts were made to minimize animal suffering and to use only the number of animals necessary to produce reliable results.
Behavioural tests
Dark/light box
A 20×30-cm lit chamber with transparent Perspex walls (20 cm high) and open top was connected to a 20×15×20-cm plastic dark box which was completely closed except for the 7.5×7.5-cm door connecting it to the lit chamber. Illumination was by direct room light (500 lx). Each mouse was released in the middle of the lit compartment and observed for 5 min.
Novel object test
Frames of non-reflective aluminium (37 cm high) were used to partition a round open field arena (diameter of 150 cm and 35-cm high walls) into four squares 50×50-cm arenas, allowing for concurrent observation of four animals. Illumination in the room was by indirect diffuse room light (4×40-W bulbs, 12 lx). The novel object was a 50-mL Falcon tube positioned vertically in the center of the arena. Each animal was observed for 30 min in the empty arena as pre-exposure. The novel object was then introduced and observation continued for another 30 min.
Emergence test
It is used the same arena used in the Novel object test. In the mouse cage the day before a box made of plastic (12×8×4 cm with opening of 8×4 cm) is inserted as home and during the test it is placed in a corner of the arena, at 5 cm from the nearest walls, with the opening facing away from the wall. The mice were observed for 30 min. For time course analysis, the total observation time was portioned into six periods of 10 min.
Water maze
The standard hidden-platform version of the water maze was done as previously described [5]. Briefly, the test included an acquisition phase (18 trials, six/day, inter-trial time 30–40 min) followed by a reversal phase during which the platform was moved to the opposite position (12 trials, six/day). For the analysis the trials were averaged in blocks of two trials.
Radial Maze
The apparatus consisted of eight arms (38 cm long, 7 cm wide) extending from an octagonal centre platform (diameter 18.5 cm) with 5-cm transparent plastic walls. The distance from the platform centre to the end of each arm was 47 cm. At the end of each arm is present a cup with a food pellet. Food-deprived mice (maintained at 85% of their free-feeding weight) were placed in the center platform and allowed to collect pellets placed at the end of each arm for 10 min. The animals were adapted to the maze for 1 day and then tested for 10 days. For each trial, the total number of arm choices, number of correct choices before the first error, total number of errors was recorded.
Fear conditioning test
Auditory trace fear conditioning were performed as previously described [5]. All mice were pre-exposed to the test chamber (Ugo Basile, Italy) for 10 minutes on the two days preceding conditioning. During the training session, trial started with the presentation of the CS (15 s), followed 15 s later by the presentation of the shock for 2 s. This was repeated 5 times with a 60 seconds inter-trial intervals (ITI).
Twenty-four hours after fear conditioning, mice were placed in the conditioning box again, measuring their freezing behavior in the context test (2 min without CS “contextual freezing”). During the test, animals were video-tracked using the ANY-maze system (Anymaze, Stoelting Co, Wood Dale, IL, USA, www.anymaze.com).
Resident-intruder test
Resident adult male mice were kept individually for 2 weeks prior to the test and were used as residents. C57BL/6N mice, housed in groups of 5, were used as intruders. After transfer of the intruder to the home cage of the resident the behavior was tape-recorded and the cumulative duration of aggressive behavior (biting attacks, tail rattling) and social behavior (sniffing head or back, social grooming) were determined.
Video tracking, data collection and statistical analysis
During all the tests, animals were video-tracked using the EthoVision 2.3 system (Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, the Netherlands, http://www.noldus.com) using an image frequency of 4.2/s. Raw data were transferred to Wintrack 2.4 (http://www.dpwolfer.ch/wintrack) for off-line analysis. Statistical computations were done using Statview 5.0 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA).
Gene targeting
The entire mouse sequence of Gdi1 was isolated from the 129/SvEv mouse genomic library and sequenced (accession number AF441240), as previously described [5]. The targeting vector was constructed as follow: the 5′ arm consists on a KpnI/NdeI 1625 bp fragment containing the 1st exon and part of the 1st intron of Gdi1 gene; lox P sites are inserted in the 1st and in the 3rd intron, in order to delete the Gdi1 gene between exon 2 and 3; the neomycin-resistance (neo) cassette, driven by promoter and 5′ UTR sequence from the mouse Pgk1 gene, flanked by frt sites was inserted in opposite orientation in the 3rd intron, immediately downstream of the 3′ lox P site; the 3′ arm of the targeting vector consists on EcoRV/EcoRI 7 kb containing the last 8 exons of Gdi1 and part of the 5′ region of the flanking gene.
After linearization with unique NotI restriction site and electroporation in AB1 embryonic stem (ES) cells, G418-resistant colonies were analyzed by PCR, from primers F4 and NEO1C with XL Expand Boheringher.
PCR positive clones were confirmed by Southern blot. The 5′ flanking probe S1 was an RT-PCR product of 320 bp containing exons 6 and 7 of the 16A 5′ flanking gene. A 590 bp probe containing the genomic region cloned between lox P sites was used to confirm the insertion of both sites. A neo probe, obtained by PCR from primers neo2 and neo6, was used to confirm the insertion of neo cassette. One of three homologous recombinant clones was injected into blastocysts by standard methods and chimera mice were generated. Chimera female mice were crossed to C57Bl/6N mice. Genotypes were determined by PCR from tail DNA biopsy using primers Lox1 and Lox2. The primer sequence is showed in table S1.
Generation of Gdi1flox mice
Heterozygote Gdi1lox/X female mice were crossed with transgenic SJL-TgN(ACTFLPe)9205Dym mice (Jackson Laboratory, Bar Harbor, ME, USA) expressing a variant of the S. cerevisiae FLP1 recombinase gene under the human b-actin promoter to delete the neo cassette. Mice were analyzed by PCR analysis of DNA extracted from the tail using primers Neo4 and Neo5, FlpF and FlpR. Heterozygote Gdi1lox/X female mice, neo−, were crossed with CaMKII-CRE-159 transgenic mouse [8], expressing the CRE recombinase under the control of the mouse αCaMKII promoter. Mice genotype was analyzed by PCR analysis of DNA extracted from the tail using primers Lox1 and Lox2, and cre1 and cre2. The primer sequence is showed in table S1.
Western blot analysis
Brain regions were dissected from Gdi1flox/Y (n = 3) and Gdi1lox/Y (n = 3) littermates. Proteins in sample buffer were fractionated in 10% SDS-PAGE, and Western blots were done using standard methods.
Immunofluorescence analysis
The animals were anesthetized with 2-2-2 Tribromoethanol (20 ml/gr) and transcardially perfused with 20 ml eparin in PBS1X (30 mg/100 ml), and brains were fixed in 4% (w/v) paraformaldehyde (Fluka, Buchs, Switzerland).
Brains were post-fixed in the same fixative overnight at 4°C. Frozen coronal sections (15 µm thick) were established for standard immunofluorescence. Briefly, sections were washed three times with PBS 1X and then incubated in blocking buffer (PBS1X, FBS 10%, BSA 1 mg/ml and Triton 0.1%). Primary antibodies were diluted in blocking buffer and applied on sections at at the following concentration: αGDI (1∶100, Zymed), NeuN (1∶1000, Chemicon), S100β (1∶100, SIGMA), GFAP (1∶1000, Dako), Primary antibodies were incubated over night at +4°C. Section were then washed three times in PBS 1X and secondary antibodies were applied accordingly manufacturer, instructions, nuclei were stained with Dapi. Images were acquired directly from slices using the Olimpus BX51 microscope and 20× objective.
Electrophysiology
Slice preparation
Standard procedures were used to prepare 330 µm thick coronal slices from Gdi1-null and Gdi1 WT, Gdi1lox/Y and Gdi1flox/Y male mouse brains following a protocol approved by the European and French guidelines on animal experimentation. Briefly, the brain was dissected in ice-cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF), mounted on an agar block and sliced with a vibratome (Leica VT1200s; Germany) at 4°C. Slices were maintained for 45 min at 35°C in an interface chamber containing ACSF equilibrated with 95% O2/5% CO2 and containing (in mM): 124 NaCl, 2.7 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 1.3 MgCl2, 26 NaHCO3, 0.4 NaH2PO4, 18 glucose, 4 ascorbate, and then for at least 45 min at room temperature before being transferred to a superfusing recording chamber. The same procedure was followed for manual brain area microdissections, the samples being immediately frozen in liquid nitrogen.
Recordings
Whole-cell recordings from LA principal neurons were performed at 30–32°C in a superfusing chamber as previously described [18]. Neurons were visually identified with infrared videomicroscopy using an upright microscope equipped with a 60× objective. Patch electrodes (3–5 MΩ) were pulled from borosilicate glass tubing and filled with a low-chloride solution containing (in mM): 140 Cs-methylsulfonate, 5 QX314-Cl, 10 HEPES, 10 phosphocreatine, 4 Mg-ATP, and 0.3 Na-GTP (pH adjusted to 7.25 with CsOH, 295 mOsm). For current-clamp experiments, Cs-methylsulfonate was replaced with equimolar K-gluconate. All experiments were performed in the presence of picrotoxin (100 µM). Monosynaptic EPSCs or EPSPs exhibiting constant 10–90% rise times and latencies were elicited by stimulation of afferent fibers with a bipolar twisted platinum/10% iridium wire (25 µm diameter). In all experiments, stimulation intensity was adjusted to obtain baseline EPSC amplitudes of ∼100.
Data acquisition and analysis
Data were recorded with a Multiclamp700B (Molecular Devices, USA), filtered at 2 kHz and digitized at 10 kHz. Data were acquired and analyzed with pClamp10.2 (Molecular Devices). In all experiments, series resistance was monitored throughout the experiment, and if it changed by more than 15%, the data were not included in the analysis. Spontaneous and miniature events were collected using a template-based detection run onto a 3–5 minute time period. For amplitude calculation, each event was then fitted using a bi-exponential equation leading to better estimation of event characteristics. For RRP and refilling rate measurements, the last 20 (on 80 in total) EPSC amplitude were used to generate the linear fit [15]. All values are given as means ± standard error of the mean (SEM). Mean values were compared between genotypes using either unpaired Student's t-test or Mann-Whitney (MW) test as appropriate.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We thank L. Wrabetz for helpful discussions, comments and critical reading of the manuscript, and R. Cassinari for graphic support.
Footnotes
Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Funding: This work was supported by grants from Telethon, Italy [TCP04015 to PD]; by Centre Nationale de la Recherche Scientifique and the Agence Nationale de la Recherche contract 06-003-Neuro-01 to YH, and by the European Neuroscience Institutes Network (Project Contract N° LSHM-CT-2005-019063), and Sanofi and the Région Aquitaine to YH. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Ropers HH. Genetics of intellectual disability. Curr Opin Genet Dev. 2008;18:241–250. doi: 10.1016/j.gde.2008.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Takai Y, Sasaki T, Matozaki T. Small GTP-binding proteins. Physiol Rev. 2001;81:153–208. doi: 10.1152/physrev.2001.81.1.153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.D'Adamo P, Menegon A, Lo Nigro C, Grasso M, Gulisano M, et al. Mutations in GDI1 are responsible for X-linked non-specific mental retardation. Nat Genet. 1998;19:134–139. doi: 10.1038/487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Giannandrea M, Bianchi V, Mignogna ML, Sirri A, Carrabino S, et al. Mutations in the small GTPase gene RAB39B are responsible for X-linked mental retardation associated with autism, epilepsy, and macrocephaly. Am J Hum Genet. 2010;86:185–195. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.D'Adamo P, Welzl H, Papadimitriou S, Raffaele di Barletta M, Tiveron C, et al. Deletion of the mental retardation gene Gdi1 impairs associative memory and alters social behavior in mice. Hum Mol Genet. 2002;11:2567–2580. doi: 10.1093/hmg/11.21.2567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bianchi V, Farisello P, Baldelli P, Meskenaite V, Milanese M, et al. Cognitive impairment in Gdi1-deficient mice is associated with altered synaptic vesicle pools and short-term synaptic plasticity, and can be corrected by appropriate learning training. Hum Mol Genet. 2009;18:105–117. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddn321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Pavlos NJ, Gronborg M, Riedel D, Chua JJ, Boyken J, et al. Quantitative analysis of synaptic vesicle Rabs uncovers distinct yet overlapping roles for Rab3a and Rab27b in Ca2+-triggered exocytosis. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2010;30:13441–13453. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0907-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Minichiello L, Korte M, Wolfer D, Kuhn R, Unsicker K, et al. Essential role for TrkB receptors in hippocampus-mediated learning. Neuron. 1999;24:401–414. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80853-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ehrlich I, Humeau Y, Grenier F, Ciocchi S, Herry C, et al. Amygdala inhibitory circuits and the control of fear memory. Neuron. 2009;62:757–771. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.05.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bissiere S, Humeau Y, Luthi A. Dopamine gates LTP induction in lateral amygdala by suppressing feedforward inhibition. Nature neuroscience. 2003;6:587–592. doi: 10.1038/nn1058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.LeDoux JE, Sakaguchi A, Iwata J, Reis DJ. Auditory emotional memories: establishment by projections from the medial geniculate nucleus to the posterior neostriatum and/or dorsal amygdala. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1985;444:463–464. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1985.tb37611.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Humeau Y, Herry C, Kemp N, Shaban H, Fourcaudot E, et al. Dendritic spine heterogeneity determines afferent-specific Hebbian plasticity in the amygdala. Neuron. 2005;45:119–131. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Humeau Y, Shaban H, Bissiere S, Luthi A. Presynaptic induction of heterosynaptic associative plasticity in the mammalian brain. Nature. 2003;426:841–845. doi: 10.1038/nature02194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Zucker RS, Regehr WG. Short-term synaptic plasticity. Annual review of physiology. 2002;64:355–405. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.64.092501.114547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Schneggenburger R, Sakaba T, Neher E. Vesicle pools and short-term synaptic depression: lessons from a large synapse. Trends in neurosciences. 2002;25:206–212. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(02)02139-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Fourcaudot E, Gambino F, Casassus G, Poulain B, Humeau Y, et al. L-type voltage-dependent Ca(2+) channels mediate expression of presynaptic LTP in amygdala. Nature neuroscience. 2009;12:1093–1095. doi: 10.1038/nn.2378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Tsvetkov E, Carlezon WA, Benes FM, Kandel ER, Bolshakov VY. Fear conditioning occludes LTP-induced presynaptic enhancement of synaptic transmission in the cortical pathway to the lateral amygdala. Neuron. 2002;34:289–300. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00645-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Humeau Y, Doussau F, Popoff MR, Benfenati F, Poulain B. Fast changes in the functional status of release sites during short-term plasticity: involvement of a frequency-dependent bypass of Rac at Aplysia synapses. The Journal of physiology. 2007;583:983–1004. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.139899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.