Abstract
Early iron deficiency (ID) not only affects brain and behavioral function during the period of ID but long after treatment. The mechanisms include long-term alterations in dopamine metabolism, myelination and hippocampal structure and function. Recent studies demonstrate long-term genomic changes suggesting that the regulation of brain function is fundamentally altered.
Introduction
Iron deficiency (ID) is the most common micronutrient deficiency in the world. In early life, there are three peak times of risk for ID large based on perturbations in the balance between iron supply and iron demand. These times periods are late fetal/early neonatal life, toddlerhood, and adolescence, particularly in females. The clinical conditions that lead to ID during each of these time periods have been extensively characterized. Each is associated with poorer brain performance during the period of ID. Interestingly and importantly, however, treatment with iron appears to completely reverse cognitive symptoms only in the latter group. In contrast, early ID, defined as ID occurring in either fetal/neonatal or toddler time periods, results in long-term, and potentially permanent, neurobehavioral impairments.
It is not surprising that the brain does not function normally while it is iron deficient. Iron is absolutely necessary for normal neuronal and glial energy metabolism, neurotransmitter production and myelination1. A large number of studies in humans and in animal models demonstrate that early ID has a negative effect on these brain processes with concurrent behavioral abnormalities. It remains less clear, however, why relatively prompt early and complete iron repletion fails to reverse the genomic, neurochemical, structural and behavioral effects of early ID. This chapter reviews the evidence for long-term consequences of fetal/neonatal and toddler ID in humans and considers the animal data supporting the premise that these long-term consequences are due to improper development during critical periods of development.
Long-term Effects of Early ID in Humans
A striking aspect of early ID is the failure of relatively prompt treatment and recovery from ID to completely reverse the behavioral abnormalities1,2. Multiple studies demonstrate long-term motor, cognitive and socio-emotional behavioral deficits in children and young adults following a period of ID early in life (reviewed in 1). The majority of these long-term studies follow-up infants who were iron deficient during infancy and toddlerhood, although there is an increasing body of literature suggesting that fetal-neonatal ID also confers long term risks to brain function (reviewed in 3, 4).
Neonatal ID
Newborn ID occurs as a consequence of maternal gestational conditions that limit the iron supply or increase the iron demands of the fetus. Processes that limit iron availability to the fetus include severe maternal ID anemia, maternal hypertension that restricts placental nutrient flow, maternal cigarette smoking and premature birth5. Increased fetal iron demand occurs during pregnancies complicated by maternal diabetes because chronic intrauterine hypoxia driven by chronic fetal hyperglycemia and hyperinsulinemia augments erythropoiesis. Each additional gram of fetal hemoglobin that is synthesized requires 3.5 additional milligrams of iron6. Acute neurobehavioral effects of neonatal ID include altered temperament and child-mother interaction7, slower neural conduction velocity8, higher prevalence of abnormal neurologic reflexes9, and poorer discrimination memory10.
The recovery from neonatal ID has not been extensively defined although one small study showed that infants born with low cord serum ferritin concentrations have normal, although statistically lower, ferritin concentrations at 9 months of age11. Follow-up neurobehavioral studies of infants born with low iron stores have not assessed iron status at the time of behavioral testing. Nevertheless, a number of studies suggest that children born with low iron stores have abnormal neurobehavioral processing at follow-up12,13. Newborn infants with the lowest quartile of cord serum ferritin (<76 mcg/L) have more school performance problems than infants with ferritins from the middle quartiles12. Studying a cohort of infants of diabetic mothers and control infants at 3.5 years of age, Riggins et al (2009) demonstrated poorer immediate recall memory, delayed recall memory, and working memory inversely related to the cord serum ferritin concentration13. Enabling the infants during the memory tasks alleviated many of these deficits, suggesting that the learning deficits are not absolute and are amenable to alternative learning approaches13. High density electrophysiology neuroimaging of the infants during task performance using event related potentials (ERP) confirmed a relationship between brain and behavioral findings. Overall, although the body of the literature is small, the available data suggest that long-term general and cognitive behaviors remain affected well after the likely resolution of neonatal ID.
Postnatal ID
Term infants that are born with normal iron stores do not typically become iron deficient in the first 6 postnatal months because the stored iron combined with a small amount of dietary intake matches the needs of the growing infant14. After six months of age, infants’ risk for ID increases because the neonatal iron stores have been utilized, the low amount of iron in human milk is insufficient, and non-meat complimentary foods have limited iron bioavailability15. These factors may combine in certain populations with an increase in intestinal iron loss because of blood loss due to parasites or allergic response to cow milk protein to place the infant in significantly negative iron balance.
Neurodevelopmental studies of iron deficient infants and toddlers begin with assessments as early as 6 months of age16 and continue into early pre-school age. Over 50 studies now demonstrate that ID causes abnormal neurobehavioral processing while the infant is ID17. While many of these studies concentrate on the abnormalities of the currently iron deficient individual, several large and small-scale studies demonstrate that these abnormalities continue beyond the period of resolution of the ID. The studies highlight the effects of early ID on general functioning, cognitive ability, socio-emotional functioning, motor capacity, and brain electrophysiological function. The long-term outcome studies of infants and toddlers have assessed two fundamental questions; whether there is a positive developmental outcome related to iron treatment of anemic children and children at risk for ID and whether there are residual neurodevelopmental deficits in spite of iron treatment. The studies attempt to link the observed behaviors to neurologic processes that were likely to have been affected during the period of ID as predicted by animal models (see below). These processes include effects ascribable to alterations in dopamine metabolism, myelination and neuronal energy metabolism and are developmentally sensitive to how the effects of ID are modulated by the timing, dose and duration of ID2,18.
Studies that assess whether iron deficient children respond to iron typically show improvements in long-term outcomes in the treated group19. Similarly, most randomized studies that assess whether iron supplementation prevents ID also demonstrate improved iron status and developmental outcome in the supplemented group20,21. However, in spite of these positive findings, there is an interesting and robust body of literature that shows that early ID results in long-term brain and behavior consequences. Although it is difficult to control for all potential confounders, several of these studies are well designed in that ID appears to be the only risk factor to neurodevelopment 22. Moreover, the participants in one of these cohort studies are now in their 30’s which will allow for even longer-term follow-up. In terms of general cognitive performance, iron deficient toddlers not only have a lower mean developmental quotient at initial testing but have a lower intelligence quotient in adolescence23. Indeed the gap in general cognitive function widens between the iron sufficient and iron deficient group as they age23. More specifically, virtually all of the neurologic domains affected acutely by ID in toddlerhood show residual long-term negative effects even after treatment. For example, motor abnormalities are evident during the period of ID particularly during sleep24 and persist well after treatment25. In the cognitive domain, formerly iron deficient children demonstrate poorer mathematical and writing abilities consistent with long term alterations in hippocampal and higher cortical functions22. In the socio-emotional behavioral domain, currently iron deficient toddlers demonstrate more hesitancy and wariness particularly in novel situations26. In follow-up, formerly iron deficient infants show lower levels of physical activity, positive affect, and verbalization during structured tasks at 5 years of age, despite iron therapy that corrected their ID anemia in infancy27. Furthermore, formerly iron deficient children demonstrate more anxiety-depression symptoms at 11 to 14 years of age28. These findings are consistent with long-term alterations in striatal-nigral and striatal-frontal dopaminergic circuitry. Other frontal lobe-mediated functions are also affected since formerly iron deficient adolescents show greater attention problems, poorer planning ability and lack of inhibitory control22,23. The temporally-and anatomically-distant frontal lobe findings are particularly interesting from a neurodevelopmental perspective because, unlike the striatum and the hippocampus, the frontal lobes are not undergoing rapid development in the time period when the infants were iron deficient24. Thus, they should not be at particularly high risk from the lack of an early life nutritional substrate2,18,29. The poorer frontal lobe development in children with early ID may be due to suboptimal connections being formed from primary areas (eg, striatum, hippocampus) that in fact are profoundly affected by ID, instead of direct effects on the neurons and neurochemistry of the frontal lobes themselves. The frontal effects have been understudied in animal models and certainly deserve further exploration.
Beyond the behavioral assessments, electrophysiologic abnormalities also persist in these infants. Auditory brain stem evoked potential latencies are longer in iron deficient 6 month olds16 and in iron deficient preterm infants8. Slower conduction velocities in these studies have been ascribed to abnormalities in myelin formation. In follow-up, formerly iron deficient 4 year old children demonstrate longer latencies on visual evoked potentials than never anemic controls30; a finding potentially consistent with persistent myelination deficits. As with the long-term hippocampal, frontal lobe and monoamine effects implied by the behavioral studies, these electrophysiologic abnormalities are consistent with known ID neuropathologies uncovered in animal models.
Long-term Effects of Early Life ID in Model Systems
The acute and long-term neurologic effects of early ID have been explored a number of models including mice, rats and non-human primates, but the vast majority of the literature has been in rats. In most studies, early life ID is induced during early gestation by feeding the mother an iron deficient diet until either postnatal day 7 or postnatal day 21 after which they are placed on iron sufficient diets or by placing weaned pups on an iron deficient diet at day 21. The first approach models gestational ID without postnatal ID in the human since the rodent brain at postnatal day 7 approximates the maturity of a term human infant. The second models gestational-lactational ID while the third models ID in the post-weaned toddler. All model common time periods for ID in humans. While the timing, dose and duration regional vulnerablilty theory suggests that there may be differential regional effects on the rapidly developing brain, the fact is that there is a good deal of overlap among these paradigms because of the relatively long time that it takes to replete the brain after the period of ID. Thus, in reviewing the animal work, it is probably best to categorize the entire body of work as “early ID.” Studies using these experimental paradigms date back to the 1970’s and first described the long-term biochemical and behavioral abnormalities. The laboratories of Youdim, Dallman and others established three fundamental neural processes that were profoundly affected by early ID; dopamine metabolism, energy metabolism, and myelination31–33. While these effects are largely driven by the failure to incorporate iron post-translationally into iron containing enzymes and hemoproteins, more recently there has been more interest in potential effects of ID on long-term brain gene expression34–37.
The largest body of work on long-term effects lies within the domain of dopamine, and more generally monoamine, neurotransmitter metabolism. Seminal work by Youdim’s and Beard’s groups have demonstrated that these neurotransmitters systems are highly vulnerable to ID at least in part by affecting iron dependent synthetic enzymes such as tyrosine hydroxylase31,33. While most of the studies have concentrated on brain regions where iron is highly concentrated (eg, striatum, substantia nigra, ventral midbrain), dopamine is found in many areas of the brain driven through the meso-cortico-limbic pathway, the nigro-striatal pathway and the tuberohypophoseal pathway. Large regional differences in dopamine metabolism are seen in response to the timing and severity of ID. If iron treatment begins later than postnatal day 4 in the ID rat, long-term behavioral and neurochemical effect are seen38. Neurochemical affects include decreased striatal D2 receptors and alterations in striatal metabolism related to myelination and energy metabolism39,40. The findings reinforce that importance of timing of ID in conferring long-term behavioral risk. Long-term behavioral effects include less exploration and fear of novel situations; findings that map well onto similar behaviors in the human (see above)41. Non-human primate models, where more human-like behaviors can be assessed, show that prenatal IDA leads to more impulsive behavior while postnatal IDA results in more passive, withdrawn behavior that is reminiscent of the findings in humans42.
Myelin synthesis is iron dependent for a number of defined and likely several as yet undefined mechanisms. Myelin is synthesized by oligodendrocytes and begins prenatally in rodents and humans. Oligodendrocytes are highly metabolic cells. ID, which compromises cellular energy status, likely reduces their capacity to generate energy and thus restricts cellular capabilities. Moreover, iron containing enzymes are involved in the synthesis of fatty acids contained in myelin. Early ID alters the fatty acid profile of myelin43 as well as genes that code for structural proteins involved in myelin generation such as myelin basic protein34. Metabolomic analyses of the hippocampus and the striatum demonstrate long-term abnormalities in myelin pre-cursors40,44. The myelin effects on the metabolome are a primary driver of the abnormalities in striatally based procedural memory induced by early ID in rats44. These long-term myelin effects likely underlie the slower neural conduction speeds observed in children following recovery from early ID30.
Dallman’s group was instrumental in uncovering the effects of ID on metabolic function in the brain32. ID reduces cytochrome c concentrations in the brain32 and cytochrome c oxidase activity45. These findings led to the hypthesis that brain regions with high metabolic demands would be most affected by this “metabolic brown-out.” The brain is not metabolically homogenous with certain areas such as the hippocampus, prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulated cortex demonstrating greater iron dependent metabolic activity early in life than other areas45. This higher metabolic rate occurs during periods of rapid cellular differentiation; in the rat the hippocampus is rapidly differentiating in the early postnatal period46. The hippocampus affected by early ID anemia exhibits altered neurometabolism and gene expression, decreased energy availability and growth factor expression, abnormal dendritogenesis, decreased long-term potentiation (LTP), and abnormal hippocampus-based learning and memory35,44,45,47,48. The deficits persist into adulthood in spite of complete brain and blood iron repletion35,36, 49–52.
Although studies using the rat model have yielded many insights into the effects of early ID anemia, the model has limitations with respect to addressing the specific role of iron in these neural processes because of the potential wide-spread confounding effects of brain and body ID (e.g., hypoxia, uptake of other divalent metals, glucocorticoid activation, poor maternal care). We recently addressed this issue by generating a genetic mouse model in our laboratory; a non-anemic mouse with a hippocampal neuron-specific, late gestation knock-out of the gene for the iron transporter DMT-1 (Slc11a2). It shares multiple (but not all) of the phenotypic and genomic characteristics with the ID anemic rat53 suggesting that it is the specific lack of iron, as opposed to the anemia or other confounders, that is responsible for the abnormal recognition memory seen in iron deficient rodents. Indeed, in the wild-type control, the amount of iron transporter expression in the hippocampus was directly related to the difficulty of the task being learned53. Based on the findings of acute and long-term deficits exhibited by this non-anemic mouse model of ID, we concluded that iron has a critical role in normal learning and memory53. The long-term cognitive deficits seen in formerly iron deficient adolescents22 are likely in part due to the long-term changes in hippocampal structure and function seen in these models.
The new frontier in ID research is on its long-term effects on the genome. Important whole brain genomic effects 6 months after early ID anemia in the rat include reductions in myelin basic protein expression and microtuble associated protein-2, which codes for a scaffolding program important for cytoskeletal stability34. The effects appear to be regional as well. Early ID anemia acutely altered genes involved in synaptic efficacy, neurotransmitter release, determination of structure and neurotrophic factors35. All point to long-term dysregulation of brain processes that are critical for normal adult synaptic plasticity.
Finally, although it convenient to use animal models to isolate specific long-term pathologic processes induced by early ID, ultimately abnormal behaviors are a complex product of the integration of multiple brain processes that work in cooperation or in competition54. Early ID appears to have differential magnitudes of effect on the various systems involved in cognition, thus potentially disturbing the balance of their relative contributions to normal behavior. Unbalancing of memory systems can be induced by targeted genomic disruption of iron uptake in a single brain region (eg, hippocampus). When this is done, not only is hippocampal based recognition memory compromised53, but extra-hippocampal effects in the striatum are seen55. This “unbalancing” of developing brain systems is thought to potentially underlie later-appearing developmental psychopathologies56. For example, children born to iron deficient mothers are more likely to develop schizophrenia later in life in a dose-dependent manner related to the degree of maternal ID57.
Summary
A robust parallel literature in humans and animals models strongly supports the premise that early ID causes long-term neurobehavioral abnormalities in spite of relatively prompt diagnosis and treatment. The abnormalities span important neurologic domains, including dopamine metabolism, myelination and energy metabolism, whose proper function is critical for optimal brain health in adulthood. Adult neurobehavioral dysfunction not only serves as a personal behavioral risk in terms of educational achievement and job placement, but also a but also a risk to the following generation.
References
- 1.Lozoff B, Beard J, Connor J, Felt B, Georgieff M, Schallert T. Long-lasting nueral and behavioral effects of early iron deficiency in infancy. Nutr Rev. 2006;64:S34–S43. doi: 10.1301/nr.2006.may.S34-S43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Fugelstad A, Rao R, Georgieff MK. Handbook in Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2008. The Role of Nutrition in Cognitive Development; pp. 623–641. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Lozoff B, Georgieff MK. Iron deficiency and brain development. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2006;13:158–65. doi: 10.1016/j.spen.2006.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Rao R, Georgieff MK. Iron in fetal and neonatal nutrition. Semin Fetal Neonatl Med. 2007;12:54–63. doi: 10.1016/j.siny.2006.10.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Siddappa AJ, Rao R, Long JD, Widness JA, Georgieff MK. The assessment of newborn iron stores at birth: A review of the literature and standards for ferritin concentrations. Neonatology. 2007;92:73–82. doi: 10.1159/000100805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Nold J, Georgieff MK. Infants of Diabetic Mothers. In: Rademacher R, Kliegman R, editors. Pediatric Clinics of North America. Vol. 51. Philadelphia: WB Saunders; 2004. pp. 619–637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wachs TD, Pollitt E, Cueto S, Jacoby E, Creed-Kanashiro H. Relation of neonatal iron status to individual variablility in neonatal temperament. Dev Pscyhobiol. 2005;46:141–53. doi: 10.1002/dev.20049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Amin SB, Orlando M, Eddins A, MacDonald M, Monczynski C, Wang H. In utero iron status and auditory neural maturation in premature infants as evaluated by auditory brainstem response. J Pediatr. 2010;16:377–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2009.09.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Armony-Sivan R, Eidelman AI, Lanir A, Sredni D, Yehuda S. Iron status and neurobehavioral development of premature infants. J Perinatol. 2004;24:757–62. doi: 10.1038/sj.jp.7211178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Siddappa AM, Georgieff MK, Wewerka S, Worwa C, Nelson CA, deRegnier R-A. Auditory recognition memory in iron-deficient infants of diabetic mothers. Pediatric Research. 2004;55:1034–1041. doi: 10.1203/01.pdr.0000127021.38207.62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Georgieff MK, Wewerka SW, Nelson CA, deRegnier R-A. Iron status at 9 months of infants with low iron stores at birth. J Pediatr. 2002;141:405–409. doi: 10.1067/mpd.2002.127090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Tamura T, Goldenberg RL, Hou J, Johnston KE, Cliver SP, Ramey SL, Nelson KG. Cord serum ferritin concentrations and mental and psychomotor development of children at five years of age. J Pediatr. 2002;140:165–170. doi: 10.1067/mpd.2002.120688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Riggins T, Miller NC, Bauer PB, Georgieff MK, Nelson CA. Consequences of Low Neonatal Iron Status due to Maternal Diabetes Mellitus on Explicit Memory Performance in Childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2009;34:762–79. doi: 10.1080/87565640903265145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.American Academy of Pediatrics. Iron Deficiency. In: Kleinman R, editor. Pediatric Nutrition Handbook. AAP Press; Elk Grove Village, IL: 2004. pp. 299–312. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Krebs NF, Hambidge KM. Complementary feeding: clinically relevant factors affecting timing and composition. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;85:639S–645S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/85.2.639S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Roncagliolo M, Garrido M, Walter T, Peirano P, Lozoff B. Evidence of altered central nervous system development in infants with iron deficiency anemia at 6 mo: Delayed maturation of auditory brain stem responses. Am J Clin Nutr. 1998;68:683–690. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/68.3.683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Grantham-McGregor S, Ani C. A review of studies on the effect of iron deficiency on cognitive development in children. J Nutr. 2001;131:649S–668S. doi: 10.1093/jn/131.2.649S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kretchmer N, Beard JL, Carlson S. The role of nutrition in the development of normal cognition. Am J Clin Nutr. 1996;63:997S–1001S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/63.6.997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lozoff B, De Andraca I, Castillo M, Smith J, Walter T, Pino P. Behavioral and developmental effects of preventing iron-deficiency anemia in healthy full-term infants. Pediatrics. 2003;112:846–854. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Stoltzfus RJ, Kvalsvig JD, Chwaya HM, Montresor A, Albonico M, Tielsch JM, Savioli L, et al. Effects of iron supplementation and anthelmintic treatment on motor and language development of preschool children in Zanzibar: double blind, placebo controlled study. BMJ. 2001;323:1389–1393. doi: 10.1136/bmj.323.7326.1389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Black MM, Baqui AH, Zaman K, Ake PL, El Arifeen S, Le K, McNary SW, et al. Iron and zinc supplementation promote motor development and exploratory behavior among Bangladeshi infants. Am J Clin Nutr. 2004;80:903–910. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/80.4.903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lukowski AF, Koss M, Burden MJ, et al. Iron deficiency in infancy and neurocognitive functioning at 19 years : Evidence of long-term deficits in executive function and recognition memory. Nutr Neurosci. 2010;13 :54–70. doi: 10.1179/147683010X12611460763689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Hagen J, Mollen E, Wolf AW. Poorer behavioral and developmental outcome more than 10 years after treatment for iron deficiency in infancy. Pediatrics. 2000;105 :E51. doi: 10.1542/peds.105.4.e51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Angulo-Kinzler RM, Peirano P, Lin E, Garrido M, Lozoff B. Spontaneous motor activity in human infants with iron-deficiency anemia. Early Hum Dev. 2002;66 :67–79. doi: 10.1016/s0378-3782(01)00238-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Peirano PD, Algarin CR, Chamorro RA, et al. Sleep alterations and iron deficiency anemia in infancy. Sleep Med. 2010;11:637–42. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.03.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Lozoff B, Klein NK, Nelson EC, McClish DK, Manuel M, Chacon ME. Behavior of infants with iron-deficiency anemia. Child Dev. 1998;69:24–36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Corapci F, Smith J, Lozoff B. The role of verbal competence and multiple risk on the internalizing behavior problems of Costa Rican youth. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2006;1094:278–81. doi: 10.1196/annals.1376.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Corapci F, Calatroni A, Kaciroti N, Jimenez E, Lozoff B. Longitudinal evaluation of externalizing and internalizing behavior problems following iron deficiency in infancy. J Pediatr Psychol. 2010;35:296–305. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsp065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Thompson RA, Nelson CA. Developmental science and the media. Early brain development. Am Psychol. 2001;56:5–15. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.56.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Algarin C, Peirano P, Garrido M, Pizarro F, Lozoff B. Iron deficiency anemia in infancy: Long-lasting effects on auditory and visual systems functioning. Pediatr Res. 2003;53:217–223. doi: 10.1203/01.PDR.0000047657.23156.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Youdim MB, Green AR. Iron deficiency and neurotransmitter synthesis and function. Proc Nutr Soc. 1978;37:173–9. doi: 10.1079/pns19780022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Dallman PR. Biochemical basis for the manifestations of iron deficiency. Annual Review of Nutrition. 1986;6:13–40. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nu.06.070186.000305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Beard JL, Connor JR. Iron status and neural functioning. Ann Rev Nutr. 2003;23:41–58. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nutr.23.020102.075739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Clardy SL, Wang X, Zhao W, Liu W, Chase GA, Beard JL, Felt BT, et al. Acute and chronic effects of developmental iron deficiency on mRNA expression patterns in the brain. J Neural Trans. 2006;71:173–96. doi: 10.1007/978-3-211-33328-0_19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Carlson ES, Stead JDH, Neal CR, Petryk A, Georgieff MK. Perinatal iron deficiency results in altered developmental expression of genes mediating energy metabolism and neuronal morphogenesis in hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2007;17:679–91. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Tran PV, Fretham SJB, Carlson ES, Georgieff MK. Long-term reduction of hippocampal BDNF activity following fetal-neonatal iron deficiency in adult rats. Pediatr Res. 2009;65(5):493–498. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e31819d90a1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Brunette KE, Tran PV, Wobken JD, Carlson ES, Georgieff MK. Gestational and Neonatal Iron Deficiency Alters Apical Dendrite Structure of CA1 Pyramidal Neurons in Adult Rat Hippocampus. Dev Neurosci. 2010;32:238–48. doi: 10.1159/000314341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Pinero D, Jones B, Beard JL. Variations in dietary iron alter behavior in developing rats. J Nutr. 2001;131:311–318. doi: 10.1093/jn/131.2.311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Beard J, Erikson KM, Jones BC. Neonatal iron deficiency results in irreversible changes in dopamine function in rats. J Nutr. 2003;133:1174–9. doi: 10.1093/jn/133.4.1174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Ward KL, Tkac I, Jing Y, et al. Gestational and lactational iron deficiency alters the developing striatal metabolome and associated behaviors in young rats. J Nutrition. 2007;137:1043–9. doi: 10.1093/jn/137.4.1043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Felt BT, Beard JL, Schallert T, et al. Persistent neurochemical and behavioral abnormalities in adulthood despite early iron supplementation for perinatal iron deficiency anemia in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2006;171:261–70. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Golub MS. Recent studies of iron deficiency during brain development in nonhuman primates. Biofactors. 2010;36:111–116. doi: 10.1002/biof.86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Connor JR, Menzies SL. Relationship of iron to oligodendrocytes and myelination. Glia. 1996;17:89–93. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1136(199606)17:2<83::AID-GLIA1>3.0.CO;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Rao R, Tkac I, Townsend EL, Gruetter R, Georgieff MK. Perinatal iron deficiency alters the neurochemical profile of the developing rat hippocampus. J Nutr. 2003;133:3215–3221. doi: 10.1093/jn/133.10.3215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.de Ungria M, Rao R, Wobken JD, Luciana M, Nelson CA, Georgieff MK. Perinatal iron deficiency decreases cytochrome c oxidase activity in selective regions of neonatal rat brain. Pediatr Res. 2000;48:169–176. doi: 10.1203/00006450-200008000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Pokorny J, Yamamoto T. Postnatal ontogenesis of hippocampal CA1 area in rats development of dendritic arborization in pyramidal neurons. Brain Research Bulletin. 1981;7:113–20. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(81)90075-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.McEchron MD, Cheng AY, Liu H, Connor JR, Gilmartin MR. Perinatal nutritional iron deficiency permanently impairs hippocampus-dependent trace fear conditioning in rats. Nutritional Neuroscience. 2005;8:195–206. doi: 10.1080/10284150500162952. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Gewirtz JC, Hamilton KL, Babu MA, Wobken JD, Georgieff MK. Effects of gestational iron deficiency on fear conditioning in juvenile and adult rats. Brain Rsrch. 2008;1237:195–203. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.08.079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Felt BT, Lozoff B. Brain iron and behavior of rats are not normalized by treatment of iron deficiency anemia during early development. J Nutr. 1996;126:693–701. doi: 10.1093/jn/126.3.693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Jorgenson LA, Wobken JD, Georgieff MK. Perinatal iron deficiency alters apical dendritic growth in hippocampal CA-1 pyramidal neurons. Dev Neurosci. 2003;25:412–420. doi: 10.1159/000075667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Jorgenson LA, Sun M, O’Connor M, Georgieff MK. Fetal iron deficiency disrupts the maturation of synaptic function and efficacy in area CA1 of the developing rat hippocampus. Hippocampus. 2005;15:1094–1102. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Schmidt AT, Waldow KJ, Grove WM, Salinas JA, Georgieff MK. Dissociating the long-term effects of fetal/neonatal iron deficiency on three types of learning in the rat. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2007;121:475–82. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.3.475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Carlson ES, Tkac I, Magid R, O’Connor MB, et al. Iron is essential for neuron development and memory function in mouse hippocampus. J Nutrition. 2009;139:672–9. doi: 10.3945/jn.108.096354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.McDonald RJ, Devan BD, Hong NS. Multiple memory systems: the power of interactions. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2004;82:333–346. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Carlson ES, Fretham SJ, Unger EL, et al. Hippocampus specific iron deficiency alters competition and cooperation between developing memory systems. J Neurodev Disorders. 2010;2:133–140. doi: 10.1007/s11689-010-9049-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Lodge DJ, Grace AA. Aberrant hippocampal activity underlies the dopamine dysregulation in an animal model of schizophrenia. J Neurosci. 2007;27:11424–11430. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2847-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Insel BJ, Schaefer CA, McKeague IW, Susser ES, Brown AS. Maternal iron deficiency and the risk of schizophrenia in offspring. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:1136–1144. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.10.1136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]