Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2013 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2011 Dec 5;21(1):61–65. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-11-0552

Is the Accuracy of Self-Reported Colorectal Cancer Screening Associated with Social Desirability?

Sally W Vernon 1, Peter N Abotchie 1, Amy McQueen 3, Arica White 4, Jan M Eberth 2, Sharon P Coan 1
PMCID: PMC3268255  NIHMSID: NIHMS349120  PMID: 22144501

Abstract

Background

Self-reported cancer screening behaviors are often overreported and may lead to biased estimates of prevalence and of subgroup differences in screening. We examined whether the tendency to give socially desirable responses was associated with concordance between self-reported colorectal cancer (CRC) screening behaviors and medical records.

Methods

Primary care patients (n = 857) age 50 to 74 years completed a mail, face-to-face, or telephone survey that assessed CRC screening and social desirability measured by a short version of the Marlowe–Crowne scale. We used medical records to verify self-reports of fecal occult blood testing (FOBT), sigmoidoscopy, colonoscopy, and barium enema.

Results

Social desirability scores were lower for whites versus African Americans, college graduates, and patients reporting no prior screening tests; they were higher for telephone versus mail or face-to-face survey respondents. In univariable logistic regression analysis, social desirability scores were not associated with concordance for FOBT (OR = 1.03, 95% CI = 0.94–1.13), sigmoidoscopy (OR = 0.95, 95% CI = 0.86–1.04), or colonoscopy (OR = 0.99, 95% CI = 0.88–1.11); however, lower social desirability scores were associated with increased concordance for barium enema (OR = 0.87, 95% CI = 0.77–0.99). In multivariable analyses, no associations were statistically significant.

Conclusion

Social desirability as measured by the Marlowe–Crowne scale was not associated with accuracy of self-reported CRC tests in our sample, suggesting that other explanations for overreporting need to be explored.

Impact

By understanding sources of response bias, we can improve the accuracy of self-report measures.

Introduction

Population-based screening for breast, cervical, and colorectal (CRC) cancers has been shown to reduce morbidity and mortality (1). To assess whether efforts to promote cancer screening are successful, we need to measure adherence to screening guidelines. Two national surveys (2, 3) monitor screening rates at the population level, and both rely on self-reports. Likewise, many health promotion intervention trials, particularly those that are community or population based, use self-report to measure intervention outcomes. Thus, accurate self-report measures are needed to monitor progress toward increasing adherence to cancer screening guidelines.

Studies of the accuracy of self-report compared with medical record or administrative databases have found that cancer screening behaviors, including mammography, Pap tests, fecal occult blood tests (FOBT), and endoscopy, are overreported (48). Overreporting not only leads to inflated estimates of screening prevalence but also may differ systematically by population subgroups in ways that cause subgroup differences to be over- or underestimated (6). There also is some evidence that overreporting may differ by intervention group status such that those receiving an intervention are more likely to overreport mammography (9) and CRC screening behaviors (8) compared with a control group, thus inflating the estimate of intervention effectiveness.

A potential source of bias that has been hypothesized as a cause of overreporting in studies of cancer screening is social desirability (6, 1012). Social desirability has been defined as the tendency to respond to questions in socially or culturally sanctioned ways (13). Measures of social desirability have been shown to be associated with under-reporting total caloric intake in women (14) and overreporting measures of physical activity (15). The construct of social desirability has been less studied in relation to self-reported cancer screening behaviors, and the few studies that did examine its effects did not directly measure social desirability (10, 12). We sought to address this gap in the literature by answering the following questions: (i) Does social desirability vary by sociodemographic characteristics (age, gender, ethnicity, education, and marital status) or other study variables (number of prior CRC screening tests, family history of CRC, and survey mode)? (ii) Is social desirability associated with concordance between medical records and self-reported CRC screening with FOBT, sigmoidoscopy, colonoscopy, or barium enema? (iii) Does adjusting for covariates change the association between social desirability and concordance?

Methods

Background

We conducted a secondary analysis of data from a randomized controlled trial designed to assess the reliability and validity of a self-report questionnaire of CRC screening behaviors developed by a National Cancer Institute (NCI) workgroup (16). The trial was funded by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention to assess whether mode of survey administration (mail, telephone, or face-to-face) affected reliability or validity (7).

Trial participants were men and women 51 to 74 years of age who were primary care patients for at least 5 years at a large multispecialty group practice in Houston, Texas. Persons with a prior history of CRC were excluded. Of the 1,004 enrolled patients who were randomized to mail, telephone, or face-to-face mode of survey administration, 857 completed a baseline survey in 2005–2006. These 857 patients constituted the sample for this study. Self-reported CRC screening with FOBT, sigmoidoscopy, and colonoscopy was found to meet acceptable standards of test–retest reliability, concordance, sensitivity, and specificity for all survey modes; however, all were overreported as measured by the report-to-records ratio. Barium enema was underreported and showed only fair sensitivity. Additional details about the trial are reported elsewhere (7).

Measures

For this article, our primary outcome measure was concordance defined as agreement between self-report and the medical record, considered to be the “gold” standard. As described in Vernon and colleagues (7), respondents were classified as adherent or nonadherent for each test according to American Cancer Society screening guidelines in effect at the time of the study (17): FOBT within the past year or sigmoidoscopy, colonoscopy, or barium enema within the past 5 years. We used a 5-year interval for colonoscopy, rather than 10 as recommended by guidelines, because of the difficulty in identifying a sufficient number of patients who had received care at the clinic for 10 or more years. Patients could report multiple tests that occurred during the study period.

Social desirability was the primary independent variable of interest and was measured with a 10-item version of the Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability Scale (18). Items are rated true or false and are summed to obtain a score ranging from zero to 10 (Appendix). Higher scores reflect a greater tendency to give socially desirable responses. Validated 10-item short versions of the Marlowe–Crowne instrument have shown internal consistency reliability scores ranging from 0.49 to 0.70 in samples of undergraduate students (18, 19). In our sample, coefficient alpha was 0.60.

Covariates included age in years (continuous), gender (male/female), race/ethnicity (white/African American/other), education (≥ college/some college/≤ high school), marital status (married/single), family history of CRC (yes/no), number of CRC tests during the 5-year study period (0/1/2+), and mode of survey administration (face-to-face/mail/telephone).

Statistical analysis

One-way ANOVA was used to examine mean differences in social desirability scores by covariates (question i). Covariates that were statistically significant (P < 0.05) in univariable analysis were included in multivariable analyses using logistic regression to examine the association between social desirability and concordance, before and after adjusting for covariates (questions ii and iii). Patients with multiple tests within guidelines were included in the analyses for each test they had, that is, FOBT, colonoscopy, sigmoidoscopy, and barium enema. STATA 11 (StataCorp LP) was used to conduct the analyses.

Results

Description of the sample

The mean age of the sample was 59.2 years old (SD = 5.9). Approximately 59% were White and 26% were African American. Sixty-six percent were female, 74% were married, more than 50% had at least a college degree, and 11% had a CRC family history. Nineteen percent reported having 2 or more tests during the study period. According to medical records, 16% were screened with FOBT, 27% with colonoscopy, 26% with sigmoidoscopy, and 12% with barium enema. Concordance between medical record and self-reported CRC screening was 85% for FOBT, 91% for colonoscopy, 85% for sigmoidoscopy, and 92% for barium enema.

Mean differences in social desirability scores by covariates

Pairwise comparisons showed that whites had statistically significantly lower social desirability scores than African Americans and those categorized as “other” race/ethnicity (Table 1). Respondents with 4 or more years of college education had lower social desirability scores than those with less than 4 years of college. Patients who reported that they had no prior CRC screening tests had lower social desirability scores compared with those who reported 1 or more screening tests. Telephone respondents had statistically significantly higher social desirability scores than either mail or face-to-face respondents; there was no difference between mail and face-to-face respondents.

Table 1.

Mean differences in social desirability scores by study variables

N Mean SD F DF P
Age 857 6.81 2.01 1.18 0.254
Gender 2.51 1 0.114
 Male 293 6.66 2.08
 Female 564 6.88 1.98
Race/ethnicity 22.30 2 <0.001
 White 509 6.44 1.95
 African American 226 7.45 2.02
 Other 122 7.13 1.92
Education 4.78 2 0.009
 ≤HS/GED 115 7.09 2.07
 <College 267 7.03 1.86
 ≥College 468 6.62 2.08
Married 0.93 1 0.335
 Yes 636 6.77 1.99
 No 215 6.93 2.09
Family history of CRC 0.39 1 0.535
 Yes 97 6.67 2.10
 No 736 6.81 2.01
Number of prior CRC tests 3.70 2 0.025
 0 353 6.58 2.00
 1 344 6.97 2.04
 ≥2 160 6.94 1.96
Survey mode 3.95 2 0.020
 Face-to-face 280 6.64 1.95
 Mail 291 6.70 2.11
 Phone 286 7.08 1.96

NOTE: Higher scores on the 10-item Marlowe–Crowne (18) scale indicate more socially desirable responses.

Association between social desirability scores and concordance

In univariable logistic regression analysis, social desirability was not associated with concordance for FOBT (OR = 1.03, 95% CI = 0.94–1.13), sigmoidoscopy (OR = 0.95, 95% CI = 0.86–1.04), or colonoscopy (OR = 0.99, 95% CI = 0.88–1.11). Lower social desirability scores were associated with increased concordance for barium enema in univariable analysis (OR = 0.87, 95% CI = 0.77–0.99). The association between social desirability and concordance was not statistically significant for any CRC screening test in multivariable analyses. We also modeled the false positive rate; univariable results were generally similar to those for concordance. ORs were 0.98 (95% CI = 0.88–1.09) for FOBT, 1.01 (95% CI =0.90–1.14) for sigmoidoscopy, 1.01 (95% CI = 0.89–1.15) for colonoscopy, and 1.10 (95% CI = 0.89–1.34) for barium enema. Adjustment for covariates did not change these associations.

Discussion

Validity estimates such as concordance, sensitivity, and specificity provide useful information on the agreement between self-report and medical records, but they tell us little about factors that influence accuracy. We found that although several factors were associated with social desirability scores, social desirability was not associated with concordance for any of the CRC screening tests. Our findings were similar to those of Matthews and colleagues (11) who found that although social desirability was associated with race ethnicity, it was not associated with the accuracy of self-reported CRC screening. These findings are consistent with the view that overreporting of CRC screening may not be attributable to a tendency among survey respondents to present themselves in a favorable manner relative to perceived social norms.

Although social desirability was not measured directly, 2 studies evaluated strategies to reduce the influence of social desirability on self-reported cancer screening behaviors (10, 12). Although not statistically significant, Johnson and colleagues (12) found self-reports of mammography and Pap testing were more accurate (i.e., in agreement with medical records) when an intention question preceded questions about screening. Beebe and colleagues (10) also examined the effect of social desirability on self-reported CRC screening by asking a question about intention to get screened before or after asking about past screening behavior. They found that asking about intention before asking about screening resulted in lower reports of screening; however, self-reports were not validated against medical records. These findings indirectly support the view that social desirability influences responses to questions about cancer screening behaviors.

Although social desirability was not associated with the accuracy of self-reported CRC screening in our study, it is notable that higher social desirability scores were observed for some subgroups. Telephone survey respondents compared with mail or face-to-face respondents reported higher social desirability scores, suggesting that different modes of data collection may create different demand characteristics among respondents, a finding that deserves further investigation in future studies, particularly as new communication technologies, such as the Internet and smart phones, are used to collect survey data.

Limitations of our study are that the results may not generalize to other populations because study participants were a self-selected sample of relatively educated patients from one medical practice in a large urban area. Given our findings of subgroup differences in social desirability scores by race and education in this relatively homogeneously population, future studies should examine the effect of social desirability on the accuracy of self-reported CRC and other screening behaviors in more diverse populations. Despite general agreement that social desirability may influence the accuracy of self-reports, there is no consensus about how to measure it. Future studies should explore alternative ways to measure this construct. Nevertheless, social desirability, as measured by the Marlowe–Crowne scale, was not associated with accuracy of self-reported CRC tests in our sample, suggesting that other explanations for overreporting need to be explored. For example, telescoping, a cognitive memory error that occurs when an event is recalled as occurring more recently or more distally than it did in fact occur, also may lead to overreporting (20).

Our findings extend prior research by measuring social desirability using a validated scale and by assessing its association with sociodemographic and other variables and with the accuracy of self-reported CRC screening behaviors. Our findings also provide support for the use of survey measures, such as the one used in this study, to monitor the prevalence of screening and to evaluate intervention effects when medical records are unavailable.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Anthony Greisinger and the Kelsey-Seybold Clinic staff for supporting this project.

Grant Support

The project was funded by PRC SIP 19-04 U48 DP000057 from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (S.W. Vernon). P.N. Abotchie, A. White, and J.M. Eberth were supported by a pre/post-doctoral fellowship from the University of Texas School of Public Health Cancer Education and Career Development Program; National Cancer Institute Grant R25CA57712. A. McQueen is supported by an American Cancer Society Mentored Research Scholar Grant (08-222-01-CPPB).

Appendix

Social desirability scale (18)

  1. I never hesitate to go out of my way to help someone in trouble. (True)

  2. I have never intensely disliked anyone. (True)

  3. When I don’t know something I don’t at all mind admitting it. (True)

  4. I am always courteous, even to people who are disagreeable. (True)

  5. I would never think of letting someone else be punished for my wrong doings. (True)

  6. I sometimes feel resentful when I don’t get my way. (False)

  7. There have been times when I felt like rebelling against people in authority even though I knew they were right. (False)

  8. I can remember “playing sick” to get out of something. (False)

  9. There have been times when I was quite jealous of the good fortune of others. (False)

  10. I am sometimes irritated by people who ask favors of me. (False)

Footnotes

Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.

References

  • 1.Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ) The guide to clinical preventive services 2009. Recommendations of the US preventive services task force. 2009 [cited 2009 15 July]. Available from: www.ahrq.gov/clinic/cps3dix.htm#cancer.
  • 2.National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS) National Health Interview Survey. CDC/National Center for Health Statistics. 2009 [cited 2009 July 7] Available from: http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhis.htm.
  • 3.National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion. Behavioral risk factor surveillance system. 2009 [cited 2009 July 7]. Available from: http://www.cdc.gov/brfss/ [PubMed]
  • 4.Schenck AP, Klabunde CN, Warren JL, Peacock S, Davis WW, Hawley ST, et al. Evaluation of claims, medical records, and self-report for measuring fecal occult blood testing among Medicare enrollees in fee for service. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2008;17:799–804. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-07-2620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Partin MR, Grill J, Noorbaloochi S, Powell AA, Burgess DJ, Vernon SW, et al. Validation of self-reported colorectal cancer screening behavior from a mixed-mode survey of veterans. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2008;17:768–76. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-07-0759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Rauscher GH, Johnson TP, Cho YI, Walk JA. Accuracy of self-reported cancer screening histories: a meta-analysis. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2008;17:748–57. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-07-2629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Vernon SW, Tiro J, Vojvodic RW, Coan SP, Diamond P, Greisinger A, et al. Reliability and validity of a questionnaire to measure colorectal cancer screening behaviors: does mode of survey administration matter? Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2008;17:758–67. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-07-2855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Jones RM, Mongin SJ, Lazovich D, Church TR, Yeazel MW. Validity of four self-reported colorectal cancer screening modalities in a general population: differences over time and by intervention assignment. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2008;17:777–84. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-07-0441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Paskett E, Tatum C, Rushing J, Michielutte R, Bell R, Foley KL, et al. Randomized trial of an intervention to improve mammography utilization among a triracial rural population of women. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2006;98:1226–37. doi: 10.1093/jnci/djj333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Beebe TJ, Jenkins SM, Anderson KJ, Davern ME, Rockwood TH. The effects of survey mode and asking about future intentions on self-reports of colorectal cancer screening. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2008;17:785–90. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-07-2622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Matthews BA, Nattinger AB, Anderson RC. Accuracy and certainty of self-report for colorectal cancer screening among ambulatory patients. Psychol Health Med. 2005;10:1–15. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Johnson TP, O’Rourke DP, Burris JE, Warnecke RB. An investigation of the effects of social desirability on the validity of self-reports of cancer screening behaviors. Med Care. 2005;43:565–73. doi: 10.1097/01.mlr.0000163648.26493.70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Marlowe DA, Crowne DP. Social desirability and response to perceived situational demands. J Consult Psychol. 1961;25:109–15. doi: 10.1037/h0041627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hebert JR, Ma Y, Clemow L, Ockene IS, Saperia G, Stanek EJ, III, et al. Gender differences in social desirability and social approval bias in dietary self-report. Am J Epidemiol. 1997;146:1046–55. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Adams SA, Matthews CE, Ebbeling CB, Moore CG, Cunningham JE, Fulton J, et al. The effect of social desirability and social approval on self-reports of physical activity. Am J Epidemiol. 2005;161:389–98. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwi054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Vernon SW, Meissner HI, Klabunde CN, Rimer BK, Ahnen D, Bastani R, et al. Measures for ascertaining use of colorectal cancer screening in behavioral, health services, and epidemiologic research. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2004;13:898–905. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Smith RA, von Eschenbach A, Wender R, Levin B, Byers T, Rothenberger D, et al. American Cancer Society guidelines for the early detection of cancer: update of early detection guidelines for prostate, colorectal, and endometrial cancers. CA Cancer J Clin. 2001;51:38–75. doi: 10.3322/canjclin.51.1.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Strahan R, Gerbasi KC. Short, homogeneous versions of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. J Clin Psychol. 1972;28:191–3. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Reynolds WM. Development of reliable and valid short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. J Clin Psychol. 1982;38:119–25. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lavrakas PJ. Encyclopedia of survey research methods. Sage Publications, Inc; Thousand Oaks, CA: 2008. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES