Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2012 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Behav Med. 2011 Apr 19;35(1):74–85. doi: 10.1007/s10865-011-9345-5

The Association Between Chronic Stress Type and C-Reactive Protein in the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA): Does Gender Make a Difference?

Smriti Shivpuri 1, Linda C Gallo 1, John R Crouse 2, Matthew A Allison 3
PMCID: PMC3268954  NIHMSID: NIHMS342588  PMID: 21503709

Abstract

Objective

To examine how chronic stress in major life domains [relationship, work, sympathetic-caregiving, financial] relates to CVD risk, operationalized using the inflammatory marker C-Reactive Protein (CRP), and whether gender differences exist.

Methods

Participants were 6583 individuals aged 45 to 84 years, recruited as part of the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA). Demographic and behavioral factors, health history, and chronic stress were self-reported. CRP was obtained through venous blood draw.

Results

In aggregate, gender by chronic stress interaction effects accounted for a significant, albeit small, amount of variance in CRP (p<.01). The sympathetic-caregiving stress by gender interaction was significant (p<.01); the work stress by gender effect approached significance (p=.05). Women with sympathetic-caregiving stress had higher CRP than those without, whereas no difference in CRP by stress group was observed for men.

Conclusions

Findings underscore the importance of considering gender as an effect modifier in analyses of stress – CVD risk relationships.

Keywords: Cardiovascular disease, C-reactive Protein, chronic stress, gender, inflammation, stress domains


Chronic psychosocial stress is a risk factor for the development and progression of cardiovascular disease [CVD; (Hamer, Molloy, & Stamatakis, 2008; Innes, Vincent, & Taylor, 2007; Kopp & Rethelyi, 2004; Ramachandruni, Handberg, & Sheps, 2004)]. Recent research has shown that stress may relate to CVD through its influence on inflammatory processes (Bierhaus, Humpert, & Nawroth, 2006; Steptoe, Hamer, & Chida, 2007). Although causality is debated (e.g., Danesh & Pepys, 2009; Rattazzi et al., 2003), chronic inflammation is believed to play a role in the formation of atherosclerotic plaques in part by promoting smooth muscle proliferation at the site of endothelial dysfunction, and initiating macrophage and lymphocyte accumulation, leading to plaque enlargement and fibrous cap formation (Ross, 1999). Inflammation may also instigate weakening of fibrous caps, resulting in plaque instability, and possibly, plaque rupture with subsequent thrombosis (Lind, 2003).

C-Reactive Protein (CRP), an acute phase marker of inflammation, has been consistently linked to CVD risk (Benzaquen, Yu, & Rifai, 2002; Casas, Shah, Hingorani, Danesh, & Pepys, 2008; Danesh, Collins, Appleby, & Peto, 1998; Danesh et al., 2000; Fortmann et al., 2004). Moreover, several prospective studies have found poorer prognosis for individuals who have experienced a prior CVD event and greater risk for developing CVD in those without CVD when CRP levels are elevated (Benzaquen et al., 2002; Danesh et al., 2000; Haverkate, Thompson, Pyke, Gallimore, & Pepys, 1997; Ridker, Cushman, Stampfer, Tracy, & Hennekens, 1998; Ridker, Hennekens, Buring, & Rifai, 2000; Tracy et al., 1997). In light of this evidence, the American Heart Association and Centers for Disease Control and Prevention have issued guidelines for monitoring CRP levels as a marker of CVD risk in primary care settings (Katrinchak & Fritz, 2007; Pearson et al., 2003).

Chronic Stress and C-Reactive Protein

Psychological stress is known to activate the sympathetic nervous system and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, resulting in the release of catecholamines and glucocorticoids (Miller & O'Callaghan, 2002; Rozanski & Kubzansky, 2005). The chronic secretion of these hormones may result in endothelial dysfunction and initiate an acute phase inflammatory response involving the release of cytokines, acute phase proteins, and other inflammatory mediators (Black & Garbutt, 2002). Several studies have documented relationships between acute stress and elevated CRP levels [e.g., (Hamer, Gibson, Vuononvirta, Williams, & Steptoe, 2006; Miller, Rohleder, Stetler, & Kirschbaum, 2005; Nijm, Kristenson, Olsson, & Jonasson, 2007)]. Fewer have assessed the association between chronic stress and CRP (Coussons-Read, Okun, & Nettles, 2007; Ranjit et al., 2007). For example, significant CRP elevations were found in individuals who reported severe chronic stress in the past two weeks compared to those who did not (Hapuarachchi, Chalmers, Winefield, & Blake-Mortimer, 2003). However, other studies have found no significant chronic stress-CRP relationship (Davis et al., 2008; Marin, Martin, Blackwell, Stetler, & Miller, 2007; McDade, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, 2006; O'Connor et al., 2009).

In addition, little research has examined how different types of chronic stress relate to markers of inflammation. Some studies have found positive associations between a single stressor (e.g., work, interpersonal, caregiving) and CRP levels (Fuligni et al., 2009; Hamer et al., 2006; Marin et al., 2007; Miller et al., 2008; von Kanel et al., 2006). To our knowledge, no studies have ascertained distinct associations between different types of stressors with CRP. This research direction could inform which stressors may be most harmful to health.

Gender as a Moderator of Physiological Responses to Stress

Inasmuch as the significance placed on stressors could vary according to archetypal gender-related values, men and women may differ in their responses to different types of stress. For example, researchers have posited that women, through socialization, define their female identity in large part by the interpersonal relationships they hold (Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton, 2001; Taylor et al., 2000). Consequently, females are more affiliation-oriented than men, placing more emphasis on relationships and interconnectedness. Although beneficial in potentially facilitating increased social support, this tendency is believed to make women more vulnerable to stressors associated with others (e.g., relationship stress). Conversely, men are socialized to associate masculine identity with work and accomplishment. They are therefore more likely to value goal-attainment, to be more achievement-oriented than women, and to be more vulnerable to stressors related to vocational or financial success (Helgeson, 1994).

Research concerning the degree to which men and women experience different types of chronic stressors in major life domains is conflicting. Studies appear to consistently show that females report more chronic stressors related to interpersonal relationships (e.g., friends, relatives, acquaintances in social network) than males (Barker, 2007; Davis, Matthews, & Twamley, 1999; Liu & Alloy, 2010; McIntyre, Korn, & Matsuo, 2008; Shih, Eberhart, Hammen, & Brennan, 2006). A few studies have demonstrated that males experience more work-related and financial chronic stressors than women (Loosemore & Waters, 2004; Young & Korszun, 2010). Results from the National Study of Daily Experiences, in which a nationally representative sample of 1483 adults completed eight consecutive nights of telephone-based interviews on daily stress-processes, indicated that females were more likely to report experiencing daily relationship or network-related stressors than males, and males were more likely to report work-related and financial stressors (Almeida, 2005). Other studies have reported no gender differences in stress levels across domains (Choi & Chen, 2006; de Smet et al., 2005; Gyllensten & Palmer, 2005; McDonough & Walters, 2001; Shih et al., 2006; Stafford, 2007). In addition, some studies indicate that females report more stress in both relationship and occupational domains than males (e.g., Liu, Spector, & Shi, 2008; McIntyre et al., 2008). A meta-analysis of 119 studies that found females were exposed to more overall stress, but that for interpersonal stress the effect size difference was larger and more homogeneous across studies, whereas for work stress it was smaller and more heterogeneous (Davis et al., 1999). Although findings are mixed overall, a recent review of the literature on gender differences in stress experiences concluded that findings from retrospective accounts of stress indicate that females experience more chronic stress regardless of domain, whereas findings from day-to-day or momentary accounts of stress point to individuals experiencing stressors consistent with their gender-linked social roles (Davis, Burleson, & Kruszewski, 2011). In addition, the authors noted that vulnerability to chronic stress in terms of affective responses to stressors (e.g., emotional distress) seems to follow gender-linked social role patterns, with males responding more strongly to traditionally masculine-role stressors and females to feminine-role stressors.

A few studies have examined gender differences in associations between relationship or achievement stressors and CVD risk. Some studies have found the effect of work stress to be stronger for men than women (Panagiotakos et al., 2003), whereas others have found the opposite (Barnett, Steptoe, & Gareis, 2005; Hallman, Burell, Setterlind, Oden, & Lisspers, 2001). Similarly, results for interpersonal stressors have been equivocal (Atienza, Henderson, Wilcox, & King, 2001; Barnett et al., 2005). Laboratory studies that manipulated the “gender-relevance” of a stressor by making it more masculine or feminine oriented have also produced mixed results, with some researchers finding relatively greater cardiovascular reactivity for gender-matched stressor tasks (Lash, Eisler, & Southard, 1995; Smith, Gallo, Goble, Ngu, & Stark, 1998; Smith, Limon, Gallo, & Ngu, 1996), and others finding no such effects (Matthews, Davis, Stoney, Owens, & Caggiula, 1991).

A recent review on the relationship between chronic stress and inflammation examined several different types of stressors including chronic work and caregiving stress (Hansel, Hong, Camara, & von Kanel, 2010). Work stress was defined in terms of the burnout syndrome (i.e., emotional exhaustion associated with work-related characteristics), and the few studies that were done involved mainly female samples. These studies found positive associations between inflammatory markers such as Tumor Necrosis Factor Alpha and CRP and symptoms of burnout; however, a study including both genders found that burnout was not related to the presence of increased pro-inflammatory cytokines, but was related to increased anti-inflammatory cytokines (Mommersteeg, Vermetten, Kavelaars, Geuze, & Heijnen, 2008). Other studies have shown that caregivers of chronically ill spouses have elevated levels of the proinflammatory cytokine interleukin-6, with one showing that spousal dementia caregivers had a four-fold increase in interleukin-6 levels over 6 years, when compared to non-caregivers (Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2003). Much of the work on the relationship between work stress and inflammation has involved female samples, so that gender differences have not been explored. One exception was a study of 81 spousal dementia caregivers and 41 non-caregivers that examined how gender and caregiving status was related to interleukin-6, but found no group differences by gender or caregiving status on interleukin-6 (Mills et al., 2009). Further, of those studies reporting significant relationships between caregiving stress and inflammation, moderation by gender was only assessed in one study, and found to be non-significant (i.e., Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2003).

There is some evidence to suggest that gender differences may exist in cardiovascular physiological reactivity to chronic stressors (e.g., CVD morbidity, cardiovascular reactivity). However, little research exists regarding gender differences in relation to inflammatory outcomes. Moreover, the research that does exist has focused on only a few stress domains (e.g., caregiving, work). The current study sought to address these limitations by examining two types of interpersonal stress [i.e., relationship, sympathetic-caregiving (relating to health problems in a loved one)] and two types of achievement-based stress (i.e., work, financial). These stress types have been identified in past research as common major life domains in which males and females experience ongoing, daily stress. We evaluated whether the relationship between different domains of stress (i.e., interpersonal versus achievement-based) and inflammation, as indicated by high sensitivity CRP levels, differs by gender. We hypothesized that 1) Interpersonal stressors would be associated with greater elevations in CRP for females than for males, and 2) Achievement-based stressors would be associated with greater elevations in CRP for males than for females.

Methods

Overview and Participants

Data from the baseline examination of the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA), collected between 2000–2002, were used for this analysis. MESA is a longitudinal study of the prevalence, risk factors for, and progression of subclinical CVD in those initially free of CVD. The sample consisted of 6,814 men and women aged 45 to 84 years, selected from 6 US sites. Each site recruited from local sources including lists of residents, dwellings, and telephone registries; supplemental sources were used to increase yield of minorities and elderly individuals when necessary. Details on sampling and data collection are described elsewhere (Bild et al., 2002). The MESA sample includes individuals from non-Hispanic White (38%), Chinese-American (12%), Black/African-American (28%), and Hispanic (22%) ethnic groups. The study was approved by Institutional Review Boards at all sites; all participants gave written informed consent. The current study included data for individuals who provided baseline values of CRP, chronic stressors, and basic demographics (N=6583).

Procedures

Participants underwent a fasting venous-blood draw the morning of the baseline examination. Standardized methods were used for the collection, processing, and transport of blood samples to a central laboratory. All demographic, behavioral, health history, and chronic stress information was obtained via self-report measures in English, Spanish, or Chinese during the baseline examination. Anthropometric measurements were conducted on site by trained assessors.

C-Reactive Protein

CRP was measured using the BNII nephelometer (N high sensitivity CRP; Dade Behring Inc., Deerfield, Illinois) at the Laboratory for Clinical Biochemistry Research (University of Vermont, Burlington, Vermont). Analytical intra-assay coefficients of variation ranged from 2.3% to 4.4% and inter-assay coefficients of variation ranged from 2.1% to 5.7%.

Chronic Stress

The Chronic Burden Scale measured ongoing stress in five domains: personal health problems, health problems of close others, relationship stress, work stress, and financial stress (Bromberger & Matthews, 1996). This measure was originally developed for use in the Study of Women’s Health Across the Nation, and subsequent studies have demonstrated its association with relevant psychological outcomes [e.g. depression, anxiety (Bromberger & Matthews, 1996)], cardiovascular parameters such as cardiovascular pathogen burden (Aiello et al., 2009) and average carotid intima-media thickness (Troxel, Matthews, Bromberger, & Sutton-Tyrrell, 2003), and inflammatory markers such as interleukin-6 (Ranjit et al., 2007). All domains were examined in the current study with the exception of personal health problems, given potential overlap with CRP elevation and lack of theoretical basis to presume gender differences. Each domain was measured with a single item that determined presence, chronicity, and severity of the stressor. Specifically, for each domain, participants were asked to indicate whether they experienced ongoing stress (i.e., stress related to “serious ongoing health problem in someone close to you”, “ongoing difficulties with your job or ability to work”, “ongoing financial strain”, “ongoing difficulties in a relationship with someone close to you”) and if yes, 1) whether this stress persisted for 6 months or longer and 2) whether the situation was very stressful (“1”), moderately stressful (“2”), or very stressful (“3”). Responses were recoded for analyses to contrast those who reported moderate or severe stress in a given domain that persisted for at least 6 months (“1”) with those who did not (“0”). Stress surrounding a close friend/family member’s health problems is referred to as “sympathetic-caregiving” stress. This domain is labeled as such because it is similar to caregiving stress, but could also apply to individuals who are not direct caregivers, yet feel stress as a result of sympathetic caring for their ill loved one.

Covariates

Demographic characteristics, aspects of health history, behavioral variables and body mass index (BMI) were utilized as covariates in analyses. Demographic variables included age, gender, race (dummy coded with non-Hispanic White as the comparison group). Marital status was categorized as “partnered” if the individual reported being married or living as married and “un-partnered” if not (e.g., widowed, divorced, separated, never married). Employment status was categorized as “employed” if the individual indicated he/she was employed full-time or part-time, and “unemployed” if not (e.g. unemployed, retired, homemaker). Education was self-reported in nine categories: no schooling; 1–8th grade; 9th–11th grade; high school or GED; some college; technical school; associates degree; bachelors degree; graduate or professional degree. Total family income in the last year was specified on 13 categories ranging from <$5,000 to $100,000 and above. Income was imputed using the expectation-maximization algorithm method (Dempster, Laird, & Rubin, 1977) for 273 participants with missing data using age and education as predictors.

Health history information included use of medications [e.g., non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs, anti-hypertensives], diabetes diagnosis (“yes” if fasting glucose >= 126 mg/dL or self-reported treatment with insulin/oral hypoglycemic agents) and hypertension diagnosis (“yes” if systolic blood pressure >= 140 or diastolic blood pressure >= 90 or self-report of anti-hypertensive medication). To account for the possible influence of acute conditions on CRP values, participants were also classified as having a recent infection if they endorsed fever, cold/flu, sinus, urinary or tooth infection, bleeding gums, seasonal allergies, arthritis flare-up, bronchitis or pneumonia within the past two weeks. Use of hormone replacement therapy was reported for women. Hormone replacement therapy use was imputed using the expectation-maximization algorithm method for 351 women with missing data using age, socioeconomic status (i.e., education, income), menopausal status and possession of health insurance as predictors. Health behavior information included current smoking, alcohol use (yes/no), and physical activity, assessed via an adapted version of the Cross-Cultural Activity Study (LaMonte, Durstine, Addy, Irwin, & Ainsworth, 2001), and represented as total intentional exercise in MET (Metabolic Equivalent of Task Units)-minutes per week. BMI was calculated from height and weight.

Statistical Analyses

Multiple linear regression was used to examine the main and interactive effects of gender and chronic stress domain on CRP. All non-categorical variables were centered about the sample mean. CRP was shown to be positively skewed and a square-root transformation was applied to normalize the distribution. Three models were tested. Model 1 adjusted for age, race, socioeconomic status (i.e., education and income), marital status and employment status. Model 2 included additional adjustments for potential confounding factors, including current use of medications (aspirin, non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs, anti-hypertensives, lipid-lowering medications, hormone replacement therapy), recent infection, and currently diagnosed diabetes and hypertension. Model 3 added common bio-behavioral factors often related to CRP, including BMI, smoking, alcohol use, and physical activity. For all models, covariates were entered at Step 1, followed by main effect variables (i.e., stress domains, gender) at Step 2, and stress by gender interaction effects at Step 3. All stress variables were entered simultaneously in order to isolate the unique effect of each domain, and to gauge the extent to which the overall gender by stress interactions added predictive utility to the model. Significant gender by chronic stress type interaction effects that were evident in the full sample were probed through simple slope analyses within gender, to determine how the nature of the stress-CRP relationship differed in males versus females. In order to further examine the clinical significance of the statistically significant interaction effects, CRP was dichotomized into groups: CRP levels of 3.0 and less (0; “low CRP”) and greater than 3.0 (1; “high CRP”). Previous research has employed this cutoff as a clinically significant demarcation of increased CVD risk, and the American Heart Association and Centers for Disease Control have identified CRP values of 3.0 and greater as indicating high-risk for heart disease (Jialal, Devaraj, & Venugopal, 2004; Pearson et al., 2003). Simple slope binomial logistic regressions for each model were then conducted by gender on CRP group. Missing data were excluded on a pairwise basis; consequently, sample sizes vary slightly across models.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Descriptive statistics for all variables for the total sample and grouped by gender are presented in Table 1. The mean age was 62.1 years (SD= 10.2), 52.5% of the sample was female, 38.8% was white, and 81.9% had high school or more education. There were no significant gender differences for age or ethnicity. However, men had higher levels of education overall than women. For example, more men (68.5%) than women (59.5%) had attended at least some college. A greater number of men were also married or partnered (72.6% versus 51.0% for women), and employed full or part-time (51.3% versus 43.1% for women).

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics for covariates, chronic stress domains, and C-Reactive Protein, for the total sample and each gender group.

Total Sample
(N= 6583)
Males
(N=3125)
Females
(N=3458)
t-test or χ2
for gender difference
Demographic Characteristics
  Age (years) [M (SD)] 62.1 (10.2) 62.2 (10.2) 62.1 (10.2) t (6581) = −0.21
  Ethnicity [N (%)] χ2(3,N=6583) = 5.21
    Caucasian 2553 (38.8) 1234 (39.5) 1319 (38.1)
    African-American 1778 (27.0) 803 (25.7) 975 (28.2)
    Hispanic 1454 (22.1) 703 (22.5) 751 (21.7)
    Chinese-American 798 (12.1) 385 (12.3) 413 (11.9)
  Educationa [N (%)] χ2(2,N=6583) = 59.47*
    Elementary/some high school 1217 (18.1) 510 (16.3) 689 (19.9)
    High school graduate 1223 (18.1) 474 (15.2) 712 (20.6)
    Some college/college graduate 4299 (63.8) 2141 (68.5) 2057 (59.5)
  Incomea [N (%)] χ2(13,N=6583) = 190.18*
    Less than $12,000 743 (11.3) 267 (8.6) 476 (13.7)
    $12,000–49,999 3303 (50.1) 1427 (45.7) 1876 (54.4)
    $50,000–74,999 1081 (16.3) 573 (18.3) 508 (14.7)
    Greater than $75,000 1456 (22.2) 858 (27.5) 598 (17.3)
  Married/Partnered [N (%)] 4033 (61.3) 2270 (72.6) 1763 (51.0) χ2(1,N=6583) = 324.43*
  Employed Full/Part-Time [N (%)] 3093(47.0) 1602 (51.3) 1491 (43.1) χ2(1,N=6583) = 43.73*
Medication Usage [N (%)]
  Aspirin 1571 (23.9) 857 (27.4) 714 (20.6) χ2(1,N=6541) = 42.88*
  NSAIDs 1148 (17.4) 407 (13.0) 741 (21.4) χ2(1,N=6581) = 80.73*
  Anti-hypertensive Medication 2188 (33.2) 964 (30.8) 1224 (35.4) χ2(1,N=6583) = 15.3*
  Lipid-lowering Medication 1081 (16.4) 496 (15.9) 585 (16.9) χ2(1,N=6583) = 1.31
  Hormone Replacement Therapy 993 (15.1) - 993 (28.7) -
Body Mass Index [M (SD)] 28.3 (5.5) 27.9 (4.4) 28.7 (6.2) t (6581) = 6.31*
Current diabetes diagnosis [N (%)] 839 (12.7) 441 (14.1) 398 (11.5) χ2(1,N=6574) = 10.16*
Current hypertension diagnosis [N (%)] 2943 (44.7) 1340 (42.9) 1603 (46.4) χ2(1,N=6583) = 8.02*
Recent infection [N (%)] 3335 (50.7) 1304 (41.7) 1942 (56.2) χ2(1,N=6581) = 136.23*
Behavioral Factors [N (%)]
  Current Smoker 848 (12.9) 449 (14.4) 399 (11.5) χ2(1,N=6583) = 11.71*
  Current Alcohol Use 3644 (55.4) 1956 (62.6) 1688 (48.8) χ2(1,N=6558) = 124.9*
  Physical Activity (MET-mins/week) [M (SD)] 1547 (2319) 1775 (2613) 1341 (1996) t (6579) = −7.62*
Chronic Stress [N (%)]
  Sympathetic-Caregiving 1798 (27.3) 677 (21.7) 1141 (32.3) χ2(1,N=6583) = 95.62*
  Relationship 1104 (16.8) 422 (13.5) 682 (19.7) χ2(1,N=6583) = 45.48*
  Work 713 (10.8) 297 (9.5) 416 (12.0) χ2(1,N=6583) = 10.85*
  Financial 1156 (17.6) 455 (14.6) 701 (20.3) χ2(1,N=6583) = 36.99*
C-Reactive Protein mg/L [M (SD)] 3.78 (5.92) 2.90 (5.56) 4.57 (6.13) t (6581) = 11.54*

Note. Means and standard deviations are included for continuous variables, and numbers and percents for categorical variables.

*

p< .05

a

Categories created for table (full distribution used in analysis)

Females were more likely to report non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drug (21.4% versus 13.0% for men) and anti-hypertensive medication use (35.4% versus 30.8% for men), and males reported greater aspirin use (27.4% versus 20.6% for women). No difference was found for lipid-lowering medications. Women had a higher mean BMI (28.7, SD= 6.2) than males (M= 27.9, SD= 4.4), and a greater percentage of hypertension (46.4% versus 42.9%); however, men had a higher rate of diabetes (14.1% versus 11.5% for women). A greater number of women (56.2%) than men (41.7%) also reported a recent infection.

Males engaged in more intentional exercise (M= 1775 MET-mins/wk, SD= 2613) than females (M= 1341 MET-mins/wk, SD= 1996), and a greater percentage of males (14.4%) than females (11.5%) reported smoking and alcohol use (62.6% versus 48.8%, respectively). Regardless of domain, females reported moderate or severe stress more frequently than men, with the largest difference observed for sympathetic-caregiving (32.3 versus 21.7) and the smallest for work stress (12.0 versus 9.5). Additionally, females had higher average CRP levels (M= 4.57, SD= 6.13) than males (M= 2.90, SD= 5.56).

Main Effects of Gender and Chronic Stress Domains

Table 2 displays results from analyses regressing CRP on gender, chronic stress domains, and their interactions. There was a significant main effect of gender across all models; females had higher CRP levels than males (for Model 3, β=.078, p<.01). In the model that controlled for demographic factors only (Model 1), sympathetic-caregiving stress was significantly associated with higher CRP (β=.029, p<.05). There were no other significant stressor main effects.

Table 2.

Multiple Regression Analyses Regressing CRP Square Root Transformed on Gender, Chronic Stress Domains, and their Interaction

Model 1aN = 6583 Model 2bN = 6529 Model 3cN = 6503



β(SE) B ΔR2 β(SE) B ΔR2 β(SE) B ΔR2

Step 1. Covariates .057** .114** .208**
Step 2. .032** .008** .005**
  Gender .177(.026)** .369 .102(.029)** .213 .078(.028)** .163
Chronic Stress Domain
  Sympathetic-Caregiving .029(.029)* .068 .023(.028)# .053 .014(.027) .034
  Relationship −.008(.035) −.021 −.011 (.034) −.031 −.015(.033) −.041
  Work .016(.043) .053 .008(.042) .027 .006(.040) .020
  Financial .005(.036) .014 −.002(.035) −.006 −.014(.033) −.041
Step 3. .002** .002* .002*
  Gender*Sympathetic-Caregiving .058(.058)** .161 .052(.057)* .144 .053(.054)** .148
  Gender* Relationship .019(.070) .066 .018(.070) .061 .010(.066) .033
  Gender*Work .035(.085)# .152 .036(.083)# .154 .036(.079)# .153
  Gender*Financial −.003(.071) −.011 −.013(.070) −.043 −.025(.066) −.086
a

Controlled for demographic factors (age, ethnicity, income, education, marital status, employment status)

b

Controlled for demographic factors, medication use, hypertension, diabetes, and recent infection

c

Controlled for demographic factors, medication use, hypertension, diabetes, recent infection and bio-behavioral risk factors (smoking, alcohol, exercise, and BMI)

*

p< .05,

**

p< .01,

#

p<.10

Does Gender Moderate the Relationship between Chronic Stress Domain and CRP?

In aggregate, the gender by stressor domain interaction effects accounted for a small, but statistically significant amount of variance in CRP (p<.01). Examination of individual effects showed that the gender by sympathetic-caregiving stress effect was statistically significant; the gender by work stress interaction effect approached significance (p=.05). The gender by relationship stress and gender by financial stress interaction terms were non-significant across all models. These results are summarized in Table 2.

Interpersonal stressors

The gender by sympathetic-caregiving stress interaction effect was statistically significant across all models (all p< .01). As hypothesized and shown in Table 3, sex-specific analyses revealed that females with chronic sympathetic-caregiving stress had higher CRP levels relative to those without (for Model 3, β=.037, p<.05). In men, however, there was no significant difference in CRP by chronic sympathetic-caregiving stress group. Consistently, as displayed in Table 3, analyses examining the clinically significant CRP cutoff of 3.0 mg/L indicated that females with chronic sympathetic-caregiving stress had 1.35 times greater odds of being in the high CRP group as those without stress in this domain (CI.95= 1.10, 1.65, p<.01); sympathetic-caregiving stress did not relate to CRP levels for men in this analysis. As noted, relationship stress and gender did not interact to predict CRP.

Table 3.

Results of Multiple Linear Regression Analyses Regressing CRP Square Root Transformed on Chronic Stress Domains within Gender 1, and Binomial Logistic Regression Analyses Regressing CRP group on Chronic Stress Domains within Gender 2

Males Females

β(SE)1 OR2 95% CI2 β(SE)1 OR2 95% CI2
Model 1a (N males=3125, females=3458)
      Sympathetic-Caregiving Stress −.013(.040) 0.94 0.76, 1.15 .054(.040)** 1.31** 1.13, 1.53
      Work Stress −.002(.060) 0.89 0.65, 1.21 .023(.059) 1.13 0.90, 1.42

Model 2b (N males=3090, females=3439)
      Sympathetic-Caregiving Stress −.013(.041) 0.94 0.76, 1.15 .045(.039)** 1.29** 1.11, 1.51
      Work Stress −.011(.060) 0.82 0.60, 1.12 .019(.058) 1.11 0.88, 1.40

Model 3c (N males=3082, females=3421)
      Sympathetic-Caregiving Stress −.020(.040) 0.88 0.70, 1.10 .037(.037)* 1.35** 1.10, 1.65
      Work Stress −.013(.059) 0.82 0.59, 1.14 .017(.054) 1.13 0.84, 1.53
a

Controlled for demographic factors (age, ethnicity, income, education, marital status, employment status)

b

Controlled for demographic factors, medication use, hypertension, diabetes, and recent infection

c

Controlled for demographic factors, medication use, hypertension, diabetes, recent infection and bio-behavioral risk factors (smoking, alcohol, exercise, and BMI)

*

p< .05,

**

p< .01,

#

p<. 10

Achievement-oriented stressors

As shown in Table 2, there was a gender by work stress interaction effect for CRP that approached significance (all p=.05) across all models. Contrary to predictions, gender-specific analyses indicated that women experiencing chronic work stress tended to show higher CRP levels, whereas men experiencing chronic work stress tended to show lower CRP levels. However, the gender-specific associations did not reach statistical significance. A significant interaction effect was not observed for financial stress.

Discussion

The current study sought to fill gaps in previous research by evaluating the unique association between different domains of stress and CRP, an inflammatory marker related to CVD morbidity and mortality in prior research. Additionally, based on theories regarding gender-role related beliefs and values, we explored whether the impact of different stressors varied for men and women.

Higher CRP levels in participants with sympathetic-caregiving stress were present in age-adjusted models, but no other chronic stressor main effects were observed. The current study also demonstrated that gender moderated the aggregate association of chronic stress with CRP. Moreover, the significance of the aggregate chronic stress by gender interaction effect did not change with the inclusion of potential confounding factors (e.g., demographics, medication use) or mediating pathways (e.g., BMI, exercise, smoking). Consequently, the aggregate chronic stress by gender effect does not appear to be solely a function of the impact of health or behavioral sequelae of stress on inflammation.

Our hypothesis that interpersonally-oriented stressors would have stronger associations with CRP in women was supported for sympathetic-caregiving stress only. Women who reported sympathetic-caregiving stress had higher CRP levels than those who did not. The absolute reduction in risk of having clinically elevated CRP (i.e., CRP > or = 3.0 mg/L) associated with transitioning from chronic to no chronic sympathetic-caregiving stress for women was 8.5% (CI.95= 5.01, 12.02). This absolute risk reduction is comparable in magnitude to the absolute risk reduction for smoking (10.3%) and alcohol (7.7%) in this sample. In men, there was no significant association of sympathetic caregiving stress with CRP. Results of a meta-analysis on caregiving health outcomes and gender found that female caregivers reported poorer self-rated physical and mental health than males, but that gender differences overall were small (Pinquart & Sorensen, 2006). Few studies have examined caregiving stress and objective physical health outcomes in relation to gender, but at least one found that male caregivers had higher levels of D-Dimer (a biomarker of blood coagulability related to increased CVD risk) than female caregivers and non-caregivers of both genders (Mills et al., 2009). Only one study that we are aware of has examined gender differences in regards to inflammation, and that study found no variation by gender in relationships between caregiving stress and interleukin-6 (Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2003). Results from the current study suggest that future research should probe the relationship between caregiving stress and CRP to determine whether it persists for potentially less severe forms of illness, as the current assessment, by asking about stress related to “any serious ongoing health problem” of a loved one, was broader in nature than previous studies of caregiving stress and CRP, which have focused on caregivers of individuals with debilitating, chronic illnesses (Miller et al., 2008; von Kanel et al., 2006). Additional research might also further examine whether detrimental health effects stem from supportive emotional or “sympathetic” caregiving as well as direct caregiving.

Contrary to predictions, gender did not alter the association of relationship stress with CRP. This may be due, in part, to the ambiguous nature of this domain, which could refer to any type of relationship, regardless of intimacy or importance. Individuals could have been reporting “ongoing” stress experienced with relatives or friends they encounter consistently but at lengthy intervals, or they may have been referring to daily stress in close relationships. Prior studies have found that specific interpersonal stressors, such as marital stress (Graham et al., 2009; Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 2005) or conflicts with close family members or friends (Fuligni et al., 2009), relate to health, but have not typically explored the implications of more general forms of social stress. Moreover, relationship stress could be more insidious, involving maladaptive patterns of interaction of which individuals may not even be consciously aware. Indeed, in prior research observer reports (Fuligni et al., 2009), but not self-reports of relationship stress (Marin et al., 2007) have been found to predict CRP levels. To reconcile these findings, future studies could examine multiple types of relationship stress (e.g., related to significant other, friend, family), and/or multiple sources of social stress ratings (e.g., self, close-other, observer).

Likewise, our hypothesis that achievement-oriented stressors would demonstrate a more powerful relationship with CRP levels in men than in women was not supported. While the overall chronic work stress by gender interaction approached significance (p=.05), when this effect was examined within each gender separately, the chronic work stress effect on CRP was not significant for either gender. Further exploratory analyses were conducted to determine whether the association within gender would differ if the sample were restricted to current full-time employed individuals only. Although the effect for men remained nonsignificant, there was a trend for a positive relationship between chronic work stress and CRP (β=.009, n.s.). For women, the association between chronic work stress and CRP was marginally significant and positive (β=.049, p=.08) in the model controlling for demographics only, but was attenuated and nonsignificant in other models (analyses not reported). Consequently, when the sample was restricted to individuals who were currently working full-time, trends were in a similar direction for both genders, such that higher work stress was associated with higher CRP levels, but appeared to be somewhat stronger in women, with a nonsignificant association in men, and a marginal association in women. Others have reported significant associations between chronic work stress and greater CRP in women, but not men (Toker, Shirom, Shapira, Berliner, & Melamed, 2005). In part, the lack of an effect for work stress in men in the current study may be a reflection of underreporting (Jick & Mitz, 1985). For example, although a greater percentage of men (44.2%) than women (33.1%) reported full-time employment, a smaller percentage of men (9.5%) than women (12.0%) reported chronic work stress. If present, underreporting could have led to a “muddying” of the “no work stress” group, resulting in falsely high CRP levels. In addition, men who report having work stress may be in higher-level positions, affording greater autonomy and control. Previous studies have found that men tend to perceive more control at work than women (de Smet et al., 2005; Li, Yang, & Cho, 2006; Lindfors et al., 2007), and that higher control relates to lower levels of inflammatory markers (Clays et al., 2005). As these factors were not measured in MESA, examining these possibilities is a suggested area for future research.

Although possessing several strengths, including a large sample size, representation of four major ethnic minority groups, and the assessment of various stress domains, the current study also has several limitations. Due to the cross-sectional design, inferences about directionality of relationships cannot be made. It is possible that CRP levels affect perceptions of chronic stress, as inflammation has also been shown to affect emotions (Bilbo et al., 2008; Dantzer, 2009); longitudinal designs with multiple measures over time could help delineate temporal patterns. Additionally, each chronic stress domain was assessed with the use of only one item involving subparts. The ability of one item to capture the complex nature of stressors in a particular domain reliably is limited, and intracategory variability can be large, with those individuals responding positively to stress in a particular domain using experiences with disparate valence, sources, or severities as exemplars (Dohrenwend, 2006). In addition, while a one-item measure can broadly capture subjective experiences of stress, more nuanced information regarding possible reasons for the gender differences found across stressor type (e.g., differences in actual severity of stressor, differences in perceived severity, differences in physiological affects with equal severity) could not be ascertained. The use of a more descriptive multiple-item measure will be important in future research. Although an attempt was made to include several demographic and biobehavioral factors that could contribute to the chronic stress-CRP relationship in models, in the interest of parsimony, other relevant factors such as environmental influences, coping behaviors, personality (e.g., neuroticism, optimism), and social factors (e.g., social network size) that could have served in a protective or risk capacity were not assessed. Finally, effect sizes overall accounted for a relatively small percentage of total variance in CRP, and consequently, are of unknown clinical significance. However, as noted previously, the attributable risk reduction for chronic sympathetic-caregiving stress in women was comparable to attributable risk reductions for smoking and drinking in this sample, providing some indication of clinical relevance. Overall, the pattern of moderating effects supports the tenet of gender differences in chronic stress reactivity in certain stress domains, with possible greater reactivity in females than males, depending on domain. However, replication in future research will be critical to more definitively ascertain clinical significance and to determine if the findings in the current study are reliable.

Conclusion

The interesting nature of results in the current study illustrates the difficulty of forming broad conclusions regarding the relationship of chronic stress to CVD risk. Consistent with underlying theoretical perspectives, our findings indicate gender differences in how stress impacts physiological parameters. The pattern, however, was not necessarily as predicted; rather, in those domains for which interactions were significant, women tended to show greater stress responsiveness, relative to men. Stress may in fact be more strongly linked to inflammatory markers in women than men. The acute stress literature suggests that that women respond with larger and more sustained increases in proinflammatory cytokine production following psychosocial stress than do men (Miller et al., 2005; Nijm et al., 2007). However, due to debate regarding whether CRP is causally linked to CVD (e.g., Nordestgaard & Zacho, 2009), whether these putative differences in inflammatory activation lead to differences in the progression of CVD is unclear.

Additional research is needed to explore possible explanations for the observed findings. However, overall the current study suggests that future efforts to examine stress in relation to CVD should consider gender as a possible effect modifier, rather than simply adjusting for its influence. With recent recommendations by a joint commission of the American Heart Association and Centers for Disease Control on the importance of CRP levels in relation to heart disease risk, identifying the complex ways in which stress relates to this marker of inflammation is both timely and pertinent.

Acknowledgments

The Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA) was supported by contracts N01-HC-95159 through N01-HC-95165 and N01-HC-95166 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. The authors thank the other investigators, staff, and participants of the MESA study for their valuable contributions. A full list of participating MESA investigators and institutions can be found at http://www.mesa-nhlbi.org. Smriti Shivpuri was supported by grant number T32HL079891-03 from the National Institutes of Health (during preparation of this manuscript).

Abbreviations

CVD

cardiovascular disease

CRP

C-reactive Protein

Footnotes

Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

References

  1. Aiello AE, Diez-Roux A, Noone AM, Ranjit N, Cushman M, Tsai MY. Socioeconomic and psychosocial gradients in cardiovascular pathogen burden and immune response: The multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2009;23:663–671. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2008.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Almeida DM. Resilience and Vulnerability to Daily Stressors Assessed via Diary Methods. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2005;14:64–68. [Google Scholar]
  3. Atienza AA, Henderson PC, Wilcox S, King AC. Gender differences in cardiovascular response to dementia caregiving. Gerontologist. 2001;41:490–498. doi: 10.1093/geront/41.4.490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barker DB. Antecedents of stressful experiences: Depressive symptoms, self-esteem, gender, and coping. International Journal of Stress Management. 2007;14:333–349. [Google Scholar]
  5. Barnett RC, Steptoe A, Gareis KC. Marital-role quality and stress-related psychobiological indicators. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2005;30:36–43. doi: 10.1207/s15324796abm3001_5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Benzaquen LR, Yu H, Rifai N. High sensitivity C-reactive protein: an emerging role in cardiovascular risk assessment. Critical Reviews In Clinical Laboratory Sciences. 2002;39:459–497. doi: 10.1080/10408360290795556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bierhaus A, Humpert PM, Nawroth PP. Linking stress to inflammation. Anesthesiology Clinics. 2006;24:325–340. doi: 10.1016/j.atc.2006.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bilbo SD, Yirmiya R, Amat J, Paul ED, Watkins LR, Maier SF. Bacterial infection early in life protects against stressor-induced depressive-like symptoms in adult rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33:261–269. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bild DE, Bluemke DA, Burke GL, Detrano R, Diez Roux AV, Folsom AR. Multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis: objectives and design. American Journal Of Epidemiology. 2002;156:871–881. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Black PH, Garbutt LD. Stress, inflammation and cardiovascular disease. Journal Of Psychosomatic Research. 2002;52:1–23. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(01)00302-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bromberger JT, Matthews KA. A longitudinal study of the effects of pessimism, trait anxiety, and life stress on depressive symptoms in middle-aged women. Psychology And Aging. 1996;11:207–213. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.11.2.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Casas JP, Shah T, Hingorani AD, Danesh J, Pepys MB. C-reactive protein and coronary heart disease: a critical review. Journal Of Internal Medicine. 2008;264:295–314. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2008.02015.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Choi J, Chen CC. Gender differences in perceived work demands, family demands, and life stress among married Chinese employees. Management and Organization Review. 2006;2:209–229. [Google Scholar]
  14. Clays E, De Bacquer D, Delanghe J, Kittel F, Van Renterghem L, De Backer G. Associations between dimensions of job stress and biomarkers of inflammation and infection. Journal Of Occupational And Environmental Medicine. 2005;47:878–883. doi: 10.1097/01.jom.0000171056.22917.ad. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Coussons-Read ME, Okun ML, Nettles CD. Psychosocial stress increases inflammatory markers and alters cytokine production across pregnancy. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2007;21:343–350. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2006.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Danesh J, Collins R, Appleby P, Peto R. Association of fibrinogen, C-reactive protein, albumin, or leukocyte count with coronary heart disease: meta-analyses of prospective studies. JAMA. 1998;279:1477–1482. doi: 10.1001/jama.279.18.1477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Danesh J, Pepys MB. C-reactive protein and coronary disease: is there a causal link? Circulation. 2009;120:2036–2039. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.109.907212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Danesh J, Whincup P, Walker M, Lennon L, Thomson A, Appleby P. Low grade inflammation and coronary heart disease: prospective study and updated meta-analyses. BMJ (Clinical Research Ed.) 2000;321:199–204. doi: 10.1136/bmj.321.7255.199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dantzer R. Cytokine, sickness behavior, and depression. Immunology And Allergy Clinics Of North America. 2009;29:247–264. doi: 10.1016/j.iac.2009.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Davis MC, Burleson MH, Kruszewski DM. Gender: Its relationship to stressor exposure, cognitive appraisal/coping processes, stress responses, and health outcomes. In: Contrada RJ, Baum A, editors. The handbook of stress science: Biology, psychology, and health. New York, NY US: Springer Publishing Co; 2011. pp. 247–261. [Google Scholar]
  21. Davis MC, Matthews KA, Twamley EW. Is life more difficult on Mars or Venus? A meta-analytic review of sex differences in major and minor life events. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 1999;21:83–97. doi: 10.1007/BF02895038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Davis MC, Zautra AJ, Younger J, Motivala SJ, Attrep J, Irwin MR. Chronic stress and regulation of cellular markers of inflammation in rheumatoid arthritis: implications for fatigue. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2008;22:24–32. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.06.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. de Smet P, Sans S, Dramaix M, Boulenguez C, de Backer G, Ferrario M. Gender and regional differences in perceived job stress across Europe. European Journal Of Public Health. 2005;15:536–545. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cki028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dempster AP, Laird NM, Rubin DB. Maximum Likelihood from Incomplete Data Via Em Algorithm. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B-Methodological. 1977;39:1–38. [Google Scholar]
  25. Dohrenwend BP. Inventorying stressful life events as risk factors for psychopathology: Toward resolution of the problem of intracategory variability. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132:477–495. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fortmann SP, Ford E, Criqui MH, Folsom AR, Harris TB, Hong Y. CDC/AHA Workshop on Markers of Inflammation and Cardiovascular Disease: Application to Clinical and Public Health Practice: report from the population science discussion group. Circulation. 2004;110:e554–e559. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000148982.95775.BF. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fuligni AJ, Telzer EH, Bower J, Cole SW, Kiang L, Irwin MR. A preliminary study of daily interpersonal stress and C-reactive protein levels among adolescents from Latin American and European backgrounds. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2009;71:329–333. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181921b1f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Graham JE, Glaser R, Loving TJ, Malarkey WB, Stowell JR, Kiecolt-Glaser JK. Cognitive word use during marital conflict and increases in proinflammatory cytokines. Health Psychology. 2009;28:621–630. doi: 10.1037/a0015208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gyllensten K, Palmer S. The Role of Gender in Workplace Stress: A Critical Literature Review. Health Education Journal. 2005;64:271–288. [Google Scholar]
  30. Hallman T, Burell G, Setterlind S, Oden A, Lisspers J. Psychosocial risk factors for coronary heart disease, their importance compared with other risk factors and gender differences in sensitivity. Journal Cardiovascular Risk. 2001;8:39–49. doi: 10.1177/174182670100800106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hamer M, Gibson EL, Vuononvirta R, Williams E, Steptoe A. Inflammatory and hemostatic responses to repeated mental stress: Individual stability and habituation over time. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2006;20:456–459. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2006.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hamer M, Molloy GJ, Stamatakis E. Psychological distress as a risk factor for cardiovascular events: pathophysiological and behavioral mechanisms. Journal Of The American College Of Cardiology. 2008;52:2156–2162. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2008.08.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hamer M, Williams E, Vuonovirta R, Giacobazzi P, Gibson EL, Steptoe A. The Effects of Effort-Reward Imbalance on Inflammatory and Cardiovascular Responses to Mental Stress. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2006;68:408–413. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000221227.02975.a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hansel A, Hong S, Camara RJA, von Kanel R. Inflammation as a psychophysiological biomarker in chronic psychosocial stress. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010;35:115–121. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hapuarachchi JR, Chalmers AH, Winefield AH, Blake-Mortimer JS. Changes in clinically relevant metabolites with psychological stress parameters. Behavioral Medicine. 2003;29:52–59. doi: 10.1080/08964280309596057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Haverkate F, Thompson SG, Pyke SD, Gallimore JR, Pepys MB. Production of C-reactive protein and risk of coronary events in stable and unstable angina. European Concerted Action on Thrombosis and Disabilities Angina Pectoris Study Group. Lancet. 1997;349:462–466. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(96)07591-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Helgeson VS. Relation of agency and communion to well-being: Evidence and potential explanations. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;116:412–428. [Google Scholar]
  38. Innes KE, Vincent HK, Taylor AG. Chronic stress and insulin resistance-related indices of cardiovascular disease risk, part I: neurophysiological responses and pathological sequelae. Alternative Therapies In Health And Medicine. 2007;13(4):46–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Jialal I, Devaraj S, Venugopal SK. C-reactive protein: risk marker or mediator in atherothrombosis? Hypertension. 2004;44:6–11. doi: 10.1161/01.HYP.0000130484.20501.df. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Jick TD, Mitz LF. Sex differences in work stress. Academy of Management Review. 1985;10:408–420. [Google Scholar]
  41. Katrinchak C, Fritz K. Clinical implications of C-reactive protein as a predictor of vascular risk. Journal Of The American Academy Of Nurse Practitioners. 2007;19:335–340. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-7599.2007.00234.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Loving TJ, Stowell JR, Malarkey WB, Lemeshow S, Dickinson SL. Hostile marital interactions, proinflammatory cytokine production, and wound healing. Archives Of General Psychiatry. 2005;62:1377–1384. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.12.1377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Newton TL. Marriage and health: his and hers. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:472–503. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Preacher KJ, MacCallum RC, Atkinson C, Malarkey WB, Glaser R. Chronic stress and age-related increases in the proinflammatory cytokine IL-6. Proceedings Of The National Academy Of Sciences Of The United States Of America. 2003;100:9090–9095. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1531903100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kopp MS, Rethelyi J. Where psychology meets physiology: chronic stress and premature mortality--the Central-Eastern European health paradox. Brain Research Bulletin. 2004;62:351–367. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. LaMonte MJ, Durstine JL, Addy CL, Irwin ML, Ainsworth BE. Physical activity, physical fitness, and Framingham 10-year risk score: the cross-cultural activity participation study. Journal Of Cardiopulmonary Rehabilitation. 2001;21:63–70. doi: 10.1097/00008483-200103000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lash SJ, Eisler RM, Southard DR. Sex differences in cardiovascular reactivity as a function of the appraised gender relevance of the stressor. Behavioral Medicine. 1995;21:86–94. doi: 10.1080/08964289.1995.9933747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Li J, Yang W, Cho SI. Gender differences in job strain, effort-reward imbalance, and health functioning among Chinese physicians. Social Science & Medicine. 2006;62:1066–1077. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lind L. Circulating markers of inflammation and atherosclerosis. Atherosclerosis. 2003;169:203–214. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9150(03)00012-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lindfors PM, Meretoja OA, Toyry RP, Luukkonen RA, Elovainio MJ, Leino TJ. Job satisfaction, work ability and life satisfaction among Finnish anaesthesiologists. Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica. 2007;51:815–822. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-6576.2007.01343.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Liu C, Spector PE, Shi L. Use of both qualitative and quantitative approaches to study job stress in different gender and occupational groups. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology. 2008;13:357–370. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.13.4.357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Liu RT, Alloy LB. Stress generation in depression: A systematic review of the empirical literature and recommendations for future study. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010;30:582–593. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Loosemore M, Waters T. Gender Differences in Occupational Stress Among Professionals in the Construction Industry. Journal of Management in Engineering. 2004;20:126–132. [Google Scholar]
  54. Marin TJ, Martin TM, Blackwell E, Stetler C, Miller GE. Differentiating the impact of episodic and chronic stressors on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis regulation in young women. Health Psychology. 2007;26:447–455. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.4.447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Matthews KA, Davis MC, Stoney CM, Owens JF, Caggiula AR. Does the gender relevance of the stressor influence sex differences in psychophysiological responses? Health Psychology. 1991;10:112–120. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.10.2.112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. McDade TW, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. Psychosocial and behavioral predictors of inflammation in middle-aged and older adults: the Chicago health, aging, and social relations study. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2006;68:376–381. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000221371.43607.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. McDonough P, Walters V. Gender and health: reassessing patterns and explanations. Social Science & Medicine. 2001;52:547–559. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00159-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. McIntyre KP, Korn JH, Matsuo H. Sweating the small stuff: How different types of hassles result in the experience of stress. Stress and Health: Journal of the International Society for the Investigation of Stress. 2008;24:383–392. [Google Scholar]
  59. Miller DB, O'Callaghan JP. Neuroendocrine aspects of the response to stress. Metabolism: Clinical And Experimental. 2002;51:5–10. doi: 10.1053/meta.2002.33184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Miller GE, Chen E, Sze J, Marin T, Arevalo JMG, Doll R. A functional genomic fingerprint of chronic stress in humans: blunted glucocorticoid and increased NF-kappaB signaling. Biological Psychiatry. 2008;64:266–272. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Miller GE, Rohleder N, Stetler C, Kirschbaum C. Clinical depression and regulation of the inflammatory response during acute stress. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2005;67:679–687. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000174172.82428.ce. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Mills PJ, Ancoli-Israel S, von K√§nel R, Mausbach BT, Aschbacher K, Patterson TL. Effects of gender and dementia severity on Alzheimer's disease caregivers' sleep and biomarkers of coagulation and inflammation. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2009;23:605–610. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2008.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Mommersteeg PMC, Vermetten E, Kavelaars A, Geuze E, Heijnen CJ. Hostility is related to clusters of T-cell cytokines and chemokines in healthy men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33:1041–1050. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Nijm J, Kristenson M, Olsson AG, Jonasson L. Impaired cortisol response to acute stressors in patients with coronary disease. Implications for inflammatory activity. Journal Of Internal Medicine. 2007;262:375–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2007.01817.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Nordestgaard BG, Zacho J. Lipids, atherosclerosis and CVD risk: is CRP an innocent bystander? Nutrition, Metabolism, And Cardiovascular Diseases: NMCD. 2009;19:521–524. doi: 10.1016/j.numecd.2009.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. O'Connor DB, Hendrickx H, Dadd T, Elliman TD, Willis TA, Talbot D. Cortisol awakening rise in middle-aged women in relation to psychological stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:1486–1494. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Panagiotakos DB, Chrysohoou C, Pitsavos C, Antoniou S, Vavouranakis E, Stravopodis P. The association between occupational stress and the risk of developing acute coronary syndromes: the CARDIO2000 Study. Central European Journal of Public Health. 2003;11:25–30. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Pearson TA, Mensah GA, Alexander RW, Anderson JL, Cannon RO, III, Criqui M. Markers of inflammation and cardiovascular disease: application to clinical and public health practice: A statement for healthcare professionals from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2003;107:499–511. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.0000052939.59093.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Pinquart M, Sorensen S. Gender differences in caregiver stressors, social resources, and health: an updated meta-analysis. The Journals Of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences And Social Sciences. 2006;61:P33–P45. doi: 10.1093/geronb/61.1.p33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Ramachandruni S, Handberg E, Sheps DS. Acute and chronic psychological stress in coronary disease. Current Opinion In Cardiology. 2004;19:494–499. doi: 10.1097/01.hco.0000132321.24004.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Ranjit N, Diez-Roux AV, Shea S, Cushman M, Seeman T, Jackson SA. Psychosocial factors and inflammation in the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Archives Of Internal Medicine. 2007;167:174–181. doi: 10.1001/archinte.167.2.174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Rattazzi M, Puato M, Faggin E, Bertipaglia B, Zambon A, Pauletto P. C-reactive protein and interleukin-6 in vascular disease: culprits or passive bystanders? Journal Of Hypertension. 2003;21:1787–1803. doi: 10.1097/00004872-200310000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Ridker PM, Cushman M, Stampfer MJ, Tracy RP, Hennekens CH. Plasma concentration of C-reactive protein and risk of developing peripheral vascular disease. Circulation. 1998;97:425–428. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.97.5.425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Ridker PM, Hennekens CH, Buring JE, Rifai N. C-reactive protein and other markers of inflammation in the prediction of cardiovascular disease in women. The New England Journal Of Medicine. 2000;342:836–843. doi: 10.1056/NEJM200003233421202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Ross R. Atherosclerosis--an inflammatory disease. The New England Journal Of Medicine. 1999;340(2):115–126. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199901143400207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Rozanski A, Kubzansky LD. Psychologic functioning and physical health: a paradigm of flexibility. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2005;67:S47–S53. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000164253.69550.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Shih JH, Eberhart NK, Hammen CL, Brennan PA. Differential exposure and reactivity to interpersonal stress predict sex differences in adolescent depression. Journal Of Clinical Child And Adolescent Psychology. 2006;35:103–115. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3501_9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Smith TW, Gallo LC, Goble L, Ngu LQ, Stark KA. Agency, communion, and cardiovascular reactivity during marital interaction. Health Psychology. 1998;17:537–545. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.17.6.537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Smith TW, Limon JP, Gallo LC, Ngu LQ. Interpersonal control and cardiovascular reactivity: goals, behavioral expression, and the moderating effects of sex. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology. 1996;70:1012–1024. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.70.5.1012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Stafford L. Interpersonal rejection sensitivity: toward exploration of a construct. Issues in Mental Health Nursing. 2007;28:359–372. doi: 10.1080/01612840701244250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Steptoe A, Hamer M, Chida Y. The effects of acute psychological stress on circulating inflammatory factors in humans: a review and meta-analysis. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2007;21:901–912. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Taylor SE, Klein LC, Lewis BP, Gruenewald TL, Gurung RA, Updegraff JA. Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review. 2000;107:411–429. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.107.3.411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Toker S, Shirom A, Shapira I, Berliner S, Melamed S. The Association Between Burnout, Depression, Anxiety, and Inflammation Biomarkers: C-Reactive Protein and Fibrinogen in Men and Women. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology. 2005;10:344–362. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.10.4.344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Tracy RP, Lemaitre RN, Psaty BM, Ives DG, Evans RW, Cushman M. Relationship of C-reactive protein to risk of cardiovascular disease in the elderly. Results from the Cardiovascular Health Study and the Rural Health Promotion Project. Arteriosclerosis, Thrombosis, And Vascular Biology. 1997;17:1121–1127. doi: 10.1161/01.atv.17.6.1121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Troxel WM, Matthews KA, Bromberger JT, Sutton-Tyrrell K. Chronic stress burden, discrimination, and subclinical carotid artery disease in African American and Caucasian women. Health Psychology. 2003;22:300–309. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.22.3.300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. von Kanel R, Dimsdale JE, Mills PJ, Ancoli-Israel S, Patterson TL, Mausbach BT. Effect of Alzheimer caregiving stress and age on frailty markers interleukin-6, C-reactive protein, and D-dimer. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2006;61:963–969. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.9.963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Young E, Korszun A. Sex, trauma, stress hormones and depression. Molecular Psychiatry. 2010;15:23–28. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES