Abstract
This study examined home environment conditions (housing quality, material resources, formal and informal learning materials) and their relations with the Human Development Index (HDI) in 28 developing countries. Home environment conditions in these countries varied widely. The quality of housing and availability of material resources at home were consistently tied to HDI; the availability of formal and informal learning materials little less so. Gross domestic product (GDP) tended to show a stronger independent relation with housing quality and material resources than life expectancy and education. Formal learning resources were independently related to the GDP and education indices, and informal learning resources were not independently related to any constituent indices of the overall HDI.
Promoting and protecting the well-being of young children is considered the sine qua non of responsible parenting. To assure well-being for children, parents must: (a) provide for their safety, (b) provide sustenance and other health promoting supports, (c) foster socio-emotional competence, (d) provide stimulation and supports for learning, (e) monitor their activities, (f) supervise their behavior, (g) provide routines, guidance, and directions that give structure to daily life and ongoing activities, and (h) provide social connections to key persons and institutions that facilitate the child’s adaptive functioning and long-term productivity (Bradley, 2006). Being the guardian of children’s welfare can be a formidable task in conditions of extreme poverty and environmental degradation. Poor housing quality and limited access to material resources can directly undermine the health and adaptive functioning of children and limit what parents can do to protect children and promote their development (Bartlett, 1999; Evans, 2006; Evans, Wells, & Moch, 2003; Leventhal & Newman, 2010).
The World Health Organization (WHO, 2008) estimated that six health conditions account for 73% of the 10.4 million deaths among children under 5 annually. Major risk factors for four of those (acute respiratory illnesses, diarrhea, malaria, and sepsis) include poor housing quality and inadequate material resources. Housing quality and lack of proper facilities for food preparation and storage are also risk factors for malnutrition, a condition implicated in approximately 30% of all deaths among young children (Arabi, Frongillo, Avula, & Mangasaryan, 2012). Likewise, lack of material resources undermines the development of competence and can make it more difficult to address issues of safety and health maintenance (Bradley & Corwyn, 2002). These same conditions contribute to poor parental health and adaptive functioning as well as increase allostatic load (i.e., physiological wear and tear on the body that results from ongoing adaptive efforts to maintain stability (homeostasis) in response to stressors; Evans, 2003). Altogether these poor physical conditions undermine parents’ capacities to engage in the kinds of activities needed to maximally support children’s development (Bornstein & Bradley, 2003). Differences in societal modernity, indexed by access to writing tablets, books, electricity, water, radios, TVs, and motor vehicles, are associated with children’s cognitive functioning and self-managed behavior during play for families living in Belize, Kenya, Nepal, and American Samoa (Gauvin & Munroe, 2009).
The main goals of this study are to report on home environment conditions in 28 developing countries and to examine relations between the Human Development Index (HDI) and four aspects of the home environment that have established associations with child well-being: (1) household quality, such as floor composition, access to water, and toilet facilities; (2) material resources for enriching experiences, such as radios, TVs, motor vehicles, electricity, and telephones; (3) formal learning resources, such as books and store-bought toys; and (4) informal learning resources, such as makeshift toys. A secondary purpose is to examine relations between three constituent indices of the HDI (i.e., life expectancy, education, and gross domestic product) and four aspects of the home environment controlling for covariates and the other two constituent indices of HDI. As discussed in the introduction to this Special Section (Bornstein et al., 2012), the United Nations decided to use HDI in preference to other ways of representing the general standard of living present in a country.
Quality of Housing
Most of what is known about housing quality and how it affects well-being comes from research done in North America and Europe (Evans, 2006). For 3 decades, efforts have been made to establish standards pertaining to housing quality, both as regards the structural materials used to build homes (e.g., mud, thatch, wood) and the internal facilities (e.g., access to toilets, piped in water, closed facilities for cooking, and refrigeration appliances). In one of the few analyses done of housing quality in developing countries, Muoghalu (1991) observed that about 80% of dwellings in Benin, Nigeria met minimum standards for external construction materials but fewer than 50% met minimum standards for internal facilities. A subsequent study done in Nigeria showed that 80% of residents lived in households having three rooms or fewer, nearly 40% of which were not in good condition (Aribigbola, 2008). Likewise, a study done in Ghana revealed that the majority of residents lived in crowded conditions in substandard housing, especially those living in rural areas (Fiadzo, 2004). Using data from 85 Demographic and Health Surveys conducted under the auspices of the United Nations, Montgomery and Hewett (2005) estimated that roughly one-half to two-thirds of urban households in Latin America, North Africa, Sub-Saharan Africa, Southeast Asia, West Asia, and South and Central Asia had sleeping rooms. The recent mass migration of families from rural to urban areas in developing countries has increased concerns about the quality of housing afforded young children (Meng & Hall, 2006).
Provisions for water
A hallmark of housing quality in poor countries is clean drinking water. WHO estimates that only 58% of people in sub-Saharan Africa have access to improved drinking water sources (i.e., either piped into the home or from other nearby sources such as public taps, tube wells, boreholes, protected dug wells, protected springs, or rainwater collections). About 70% of households in Lima, Peru had water in the home (Meng & Hall, 2006). Studies done in Egypt and Turkey indicate that substandard water sanitation facilities increase morbidity and mortality in young children (Abou-ali, 2003; Arik & Arik, 2009). WHO estimates that water-related deaths account for 4% of all deaths and nearly 6% of all disease burden for young children. Contaminated drinking water is a significant source of cholera and diarrhea in young children. When water does not come into the house, the storage of water becomes a major issue as regards contamination. Young children may dip their hands into water or drop water scoops on the floor which then becomes a major source of disease (Lindskog & Lundqvist, 1998). A study done in Malawi showed that diarrhea in children under 5 years old could be significantly reduced by something as simple as an improved bucket for carrying water (Roberts et al., 2001).
Sanitation facilities
The World Health Organization in 2008 estimated that 18% of the world population practices indiscriminate or open defecation, another 12% uses an unimproved sanitation facility that does not ensure hygienic separation of human excreta from human contact. Despite advances in access to improved sanitation facilities throughout the world, open defecation is still practiced by 48% of the population in Southern Asia and 28% in sub-Saharan Africa. In rural areas around the world less than half the population has access to improved sanitation facilities (WHO & UNICEF, 2008). Not having proper facilities to deal with waste contributes to childhood illness and mortality (Agha, 2000; Baltazar & Solon, 1989; Mertens, Fernando, Cousens, & Feacham, 1992; Podewils et al., 2004). Death rates remain alarmingly high in developing countries. For example, in some informal urban settlements in poor Asian, African, and South American cities, mortality rates among young children are estimated at 150–200 per 1000 (Bartlett, 2005; Prüss-Üstün, Kay, Fewtrell, & Bartram, 2004). The largest number of cases involve diarrhea, which is caused by a mélange of bacterial, viral, and parasitic pathogens connected to poor hygiene and sanitation (Podewils, Mintz, Natao, & Parashar, 2004). Diarrheal disease and intestinal parasites also impair food absorption, increasing the risk of malnutrition and stunting. These can lead to problems with behavior and cognitive functioning when children are school age (Grantham-McGregor & Fernald, 1997; Mendez & Adair, 1999). Lack of sanitation facilities has been consistently associated with diarrhea in children who live in poor countries. There is a higher incidence of intestinal parasites in children who share toilets or lack connection to the city sewer system (Ludwig, Fernando, Firmino, & Joao Tadeu 1999; Mahfouz, El-Morshedy, Fargaly, & Khalil, 1997).
Children’s vulnerability to pathogens from contaminated water and poor sanitation relates both to their exposure level (children actively explore their environments and are unaware of problems pertaining to sanitation) and their level of immunity. Not having adequate toilet facilities at home is particularly problematic for young children. Taking children any distance to defecate is impractical given their limited capacity to withhold bowel movements. Maintenance of shared toilets is a problem, and young children fear using pit latrines (Curtis et al., 1995; Lindskog & Lundqvist, 1998). They often use yards, increasing their exposure to pathogens.
Food storage/refrigeration
Demographic and Health Surveys done in developing countries show that fewer than 25% of urban households in Sub-Saharan Africa have a refrigerator (Montgomery & Hewett, 2005). Only about one-third of households in Southeast Asia and one-half of the households in Latin America have refrigerators. This creates a risk for food contamination and gastro-intestinal illness (Ehiri et al., 2001). Motarjemi, Käferstein, Moy, and Quevedo (1993) reported that contamination of foods used to wean the child from the breast is a leading cause of diarrheal disease and malnutrition in children under 5. Because young children can hold only limited quantities of food in their stomachs at any given time, they often need several small meals a day to obtain the necessary calories and nutrients. However, when homes contain inadequate facilities for food preparation and storage, it often means food is left out for later consumption, thus increasing the likelihood of contamination (Bartlett, 1999).
Cooking facilities
Having an open stove with no chimney in the household increases indoor pollutants that can pose health risks. Having open fires in the house or a poorly vented stove increases susceptibility to respiratory illness (Awashti, Glick, & Fletcher, 1996; Collins, Sithole, & Martin, 1990). In poor nations, acute respiratory illness associated with exposure to indoor air pollution is a leading cause of death among young children (Gauderman et al., 2004). Exposure to wood smoke can have other adverse health consequences as well. Prenatal exposure can lead to low birth weight (Siddiqui et al., 2008), and long-term exposure drives up blood pressure in mothers (McCracken, Smith, Díaz, Mittleman, & Schwartz, 2007). Having an open stove or fire in the home is also a significant risk factor for childhood burns. In a 10-year study in Manipal, India, 76% of burns happened to children under 5, and 8% were the result of contact with pressure stoves (a type of open stove used for cooking; Kumar, Chirayil, & Chittoria, 2000).
Construction materials
Few studies have examined the composition of materials used for the roof, floor, and walls in family residences in terms of how such materials may contribute to child well-being. Using data from 85 UN-sponsored Demographic and Health Surveys, Montgomery and Hewett (2005) estimated that roughly half to about 95% of urban households in Latin America, North Africa, Sub-Saharan Africa, Southeast Asia, West Africa, and South and Central Asia had finished floors. There is evidence that poor ventilation (often associated with inadequate flooring, wall composition, and roofing) is connected to poor indoor air quality and increased likelihood of respiratory illness (Awasthi et al., 1996; Collins et al., 1990). Dampness indoors is also associated with respiratory symptoms (Yang, Chiu, Chiu, & Kao, 1997). In a study done in Cameroon, Yongsi and Ntetu (2008) found that a composite index of housing quality that included composition of floors and walls, number of rooms, and availability of piped water and electricity together with material possessions in the residence such as TV, cookers, refrigerators, motor vehicles, and antennas was associated with the level of childhood diarrhea (19.9% prevalence rate in high standard homes versus 49.2% prevalence in low standard homes).
Studies done in countries ranked high on the HDI show that poor housing quality is associated with psychological distress and learned helplessness in school age children (Evans, Saltzman, & Cooperman, 2001). Low housing quality has been associated with mood disorders, psychosomatic complaints, and behavioral disregulation as well (Evans et al., 2003). The number of studies is small; and most do not include the kinds of samples and controls needed to make strong generalizations to countries ranked low on the HDI. Even so, the evidence suggests that poor housing quality poses a risk for children’s health and adaptive functioning.
Access to high quality housing materials, safe drinking water, improved sanitation, refrigeration, and a closed stove all increase the likelihood that young children will avoid diseases like diarrhea that could lead to malnutrition or even death. It also increases the likelihood children will avoid serious respiratory illnesses and injuries (Evans, 2006; Fisk, Lei-Gomez, & Mendell, 2007; Wu & Takaro, 2007). The MICS included each of these factors as indicators of housing quality for children under 5.
Material Resources
Most studies of household resources measure only monetary holdings or expenditures (e.g., Laaksonen, Rahkonen, Martikainen, & Lahelma, 2005). There is little published work on the prevalence of household material resources like a radio, TV, telephone, transportation, and electricity or how they relate to child well-being. One exception is the United Nations Demographic and Household Surveys. Those surveys reveal that, except in Sub-Saharan Africa, the majority of urban households have access to a TV, albeit the percentage is less than 70% in most of South Asia (Montgomery & Hewett, 2005). Access to radio tends to be higher except in West Asia. Access to electricity increases the likelihood children will have opportunities to learn from radio and TV and that there will be light for reading and other learning activities (Kanagawa & Nakata, 2008). Unfortunately, a household survey done in South Africa found that only 47% of traditional homesteads had such access (Statistics South Africa, 2008). This contrasts to about 90% in Lima, Peru (Meng & Hall, 2006) – reflective of the rural-urban divide. As part of its World Declaration on Education for All: Meeting Basic Learning Needs, the United Nations (1990) specified that radio and television play significant roles in educating both children and adults in poor countries. This requires having both the electronics and access to electricity. Having electricity in the household also reduces mortality in children under age 5 in developing countries (Ridder & Tunali, 1999; Wang, 2003). Having household transportation can be critical in dealing with certain injuries and illnesses. It also increases the likelihood that both adults and children will access resources that are farther from the dwelling, including attending school and going to events and facilities that afford opportunities for learning and increased income.
Having access to electricity and devices for communication and transportation markedly increases opportunities for learning and health maintenance, fosters social networks, and increases a family’s capacity to meet a variety of basic and emergency needs. Accordingly, we have included indicators of these material resources in this study.
Formal and Informal Learning Resources
Over the centuries there has been movement from the use of spontaneous learning episodes that involve natural objects and circumstances (i.e., informal learning activities) to more carefully structured experiences that involve the use of materials specifically designed to promote a particular type of learning (i.e., formal learning activities). The emphasis on providing stimulation and organized learning experiences for children escalated in technologically advanced societies. The practice fits with societal goals pertaining to higher order skills and independence. Research done in technologically advanced societies shows that children who have access to stimulating learning materials in the home and enriching experiences at home and elsewhere display higher levels of competence and adaptive functioning from late infancy through adolescence, albeit that varies somewhat by country (Bradley & Corwyn, 2006). In addition, there has been evidence from developing countries that exposure to stimulation in the home is associated with such other indicators of well-being as height and nutritional status (Chomitz, 1992; Church & Katigbak, 1991; Cravioto & DeLicardie, 1972; Lozoff, Jimenez, & Wolf, 1991). Unfortunately, few of the major international or national household surveys conducted in developing countries catalog such materials.
Many African households have very few material resources that children can use to stimulate learning (Aina, Agiobu-Kemmer, Etta, Zeitlin, & Setiloane, 1993; Drotar et al., 1999; Holding, 2003). Among the Yoruba, for example, only about 11% of children were observed engaging in technical play with toys and only 27% engaged in social-technical play with toys (Aina et al., 1993). Furthermore, Agiobu-Kemmer (1984) found that Nigerian mothers had to be told to play object games with their children to stimulate them. The paucity of toys and learning materials present in African homes reflects the high rates of poverty present in many African nations. The low level using objects to help children learn also reflects cultural practice and generally low levels of maternal education (Goldberg, 1977).
Children in rural China and Thailand tend to have few toys or books and relatively little exposure to parental teaching of literacy skills (Chi & Rao, 2003). Only 13.9% of Thai households had 10 or more books compared to 54.3% of U.S. households, only 16.7% had role-playing toys (vs. 99% U.S.), only 5.6% had musical toys (vs. 89.1% U.S.), only 11.1% had eye-hand coordination toys (vs. 49.5% U.S.). The reasons for low scores on these items might be because rural Thai mothers are introverted, low income, and have little formal education (Williams et al., 2003).
In countries rated high on the HDI, there tends to be a consistent relation between family socioeconomic status and the level of exposure children have to stimulating materials and experiences (Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & Garcia Coll, 2001; Lleras, 2008). Although fewer studies have been conducted in less wealthy countries, the findings are largely the same (Black, 2003; Church & Katigbak, 1991; de Oliveira, Barros, Anselmi, & Piccinini, 2006; Drotar et al., 1999; Duhan & Punia, 1998; Lozoff, Park, Radan, & Wolf, 1995; Masud, Luster, & Youatt, 1994; Misra & Tiwari, 1984; Richter & Grieve, 1991; Torralva & Cugnasco, 1996; Zeitlin et al., 1995; Zevalkink & Riksen-Walraven, 2001). Sahu and Devi (1982) observed a particularly striking SES difference in a study of the home environments of 3- to 6-year-old children from socially advantaged and socially disadvantaged families in an urban area of India. Others have also noted extremely low levels of materials and enriching experiences for children from lower castes in India (Das & Padhee, 1993; Kurtz, Borkowski, & Deshmukh, 1987).
Having access to a variety of both formal and informal materials for learning shows a consistent relation to children’s competence and achievement (Bradley & Corwyn, 2006). We focus on both types because formal learning resources (books and manufactured toys) generally require financial resources. By contrast, informal learning resources are items that are readily available in most home environments, but their construction and use may depend more on parental education and family assumptions about their children’s future (i.e., things that reflect different aspects of overall standards of living). Consequently, the implications of children not using formal and informal learning resources may differ.
Summary - A General Consideration of Poverty
There is a vast amount of evidence that the quality of the home environment is associated with children’s health, competence, and adaptive functioning. However, because of the social, political, and economic constraints present in many developing countries, there remains limited information on the extent to which children from developing nations are exposed to particular home conditions that either protect their health or increase the likelihood of social and intellectual competence. Evans (2003) argued that cumulative risk (sometimes in the form of environmental dangers and obstacles – often co-occurring with other conditions of chronic poverty) leads to allostatic load which reflects chronic wear and tear on body systems. The body’s efforts to adjust to ongoing environmental demands lead to constant readjustments of physiological systems which, in turn, can lead to a multiplicity of negative outcomes over the life course. The earlier the risk conditions manifest themselves and the more persistent they are through time, the more likely an individual will succumb to increasing allostatic load.
As discussed in the Introduction to this Special Section (Bornstein et al., 2012), housing quality and access to materials (along with parenting practices and access to health care) reflect not only the general standard of living present in a country but also conditions present at neighborhood and family levels. Many of the conditions examined in this study are co-factors of poverty (Bradley & Corwyn, 2002; Conger & Donnellan, 2007; Evans, 2006). Consequently, it can be difficult to isolate the impacts of specific conditions on particular aspects of parenting and child development. The challenge of determining how specific physical conditions influence parenting or child well-being is made greater given that parental behavior also reflects cultural and geographic circumstances. The general systems principles of equifinality (different conditions can produce the same outcome) and multifinality (the same condition can produce multiple outcomes) are operative. Granting this uncertainty, it is important for both scientific and policy reasons to more fully document how frequently certain family level conditions are present within a country and how these conditions connect to the broader conditions present in the country.
The data reported on in this study afford an opportunity to further elucidate how home conditions for young children are related to the Human Development Index in developing nations and to explore the relation of the constituent indices of the HDI to the quality of housing, access to material resources, and opportunities for formal and informal learning, enrichment, and comfort experienced by those children. Prior studies have shown that indicators of housing quality are related to several markers of health and health behavior in developing countries (Montgomery & Hewett, 2005). The total number of indicators of housing quality available in this study exceeds the number present in most studies, and the availability of child relevant material resources increases the relevance of findings for children.
Method
Participants
The total sample consisted of 127,347 households with children under age 5 in 28 developing countries (Table 1). Surveys done in all 28 countries included at least some of the home environment questions. The Jamaican survey did not include several questions about the household, and the Albanian survey did not ask whether the household had a radio or electricity. Surveys done in Belarus, Bangladesh, Guinea-Bissau, Iraq, and Somalia did not include questions about books and toys, and the Ghana survey did not ask questions about books. Across countries, the average number of children under 5 in the family was 1.30 (SD = .53; range = 1–6); for the purpose of this study, we randomly selected a target child under 5 from families with more than one child under 5. The randomly selected child under 5 averaged 29.16 months of age (SD = 16.82; range = 0–59), and 48.6% were female. Mothers averaged 29.37 years (SD = 8.13, range = 15–95), and the highest level of education mothers had completed was none or preschool for 29.5%, primary school for 27.5%, secondary school for 36.0%, and higher for 6.9%. Household crowding (computed as the number of household members divided by the number of bedrooms, and then recoded into 5 scaled categories; see Bornstein et al., 2012) averaged 3.23 (SD = 1.09; range = 1–5).
Table 1.
Descriptive Statistics and Effect Sizes for Deviation from the Grand Mean for Quality of Housing and Material Resources
n | Quality of Housing | Material Resources | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | ES | M | SD | ES | ||
High HDI | |||||||
Montenegro | 814 | .88 | .26 | 1.10 | 4.61 | .69 | 1.33 |
Serbia | 2868 | .82 | .30 | 1.01 | 4.41 | .90 | 1.18 |
Belarus | 2810 | .81 | .22 | .95 | 3.97 | .83 | .74 |
Macedonia | 3225 | .83 | .27 | 1.04 | 4.11 | 1.01 | .95 |
Albania | 944 | .84 | .27 | 1.00 | -- | -- | -- |
Kazakhstan | 3536 | .58 | .34 | .57 | 3.54 | .98 | .43 |
Bosnia and Herzegovina | 2704 | .91 | .18 | 1.10 | 4.62 | .63 | 1.27 |
Medium HDI | |||||||
Thailand | 8312 | .53 | .31 | .54 | 4.41 | .87 | 1.17 |
Ukraine | 2748 | .77 | .29 | .87 | 3.86 | .86 | .64 |
Belize | 585 | .40 | .50 | .34 | 3.58 | 1.35 | .53 |
Jamaica | 1168 | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- | -- |
Syrian Arab Republic | 7553 | .85 | .29 | 1.10 | 3.87 | .90 | .81 |
Mongolia | 3040 | .05 | .56 | .43 | 2.89 | 1.21 | .44 |
Viet Nam | 2302 | −.16 | .64 | −.57 | 2.98 | 1.29 | .02, ns |
Uzbekistan | 3876 | .46 | .41 | .42 | 3.30 | .98 | .28 |
Kyrgyzstan | 2353 | .54 | .41 | .57 | 3.28 | 1.01 | .29 |
Tajikistan | 3136 | .07 | .55 | −.16 | 2.98 | 1.03 | .08 |
Yemen | 2409 | .03 | .73 | −.18 | 2.50 | 1.67 | −.22 |
Ghana | 2624 | −.47 | .51 | −1.02 | 1.76 | 1.44 | −.90 |
Bangladesh | 26206 | −.63 | .44 | −1.29 | 1.34 | 1.43 | −1.24 |
Low HDI | |||||||
Togo | 3151 | −.67 | .43 | −1.38 | 1.62 | 1.46 | −1.06 |
Gambia | 4886 | −.18 | .51 | −.60 | 2.53 | 1.34 | −.34 |
Côte d’Ivoire | 6541 | −.33 | .57 | −.80 | 2.27 | 1.59 | −.50 |
Guinea-Bissau | 4485 | −.50 | .55 | −1.12 | 1.35 | 1.19 | −1.29 |
Central African Republic | 6565 | −.81 | .36 | −1.63 | .74 | .92 | −1.83 |
Sierra Leone | 4066 | −.88 | .45 | −1.71 | .60 | .85 | −1.90 |
HDI N/A | |||||||
Iraq | 10587 | .74 | .45 | .95 | 3.65 | 1.07 | .69 |
Somalia | 3853 | −.83 | .51 | −1.55 | .83 | 1.22 | −1.57 |
| |||||||
TOTAL | 127347 | .17 | .63 | NA | 2.91 | 1.25 | NA |
Note. All countries significantly differed from the grand mean at p < .001, except where noted.
Effect sizes (ES) are based on models with covariates. -- = Data were not collected. NA = Not applicable.
Procedures
MICS3 items
The Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey (MICS) is a nationally representative and internationally comparable household survey (UNICEF, 2006). It was implemented in many developing countries and provides a unique source of information to examine protective and risk factors for child health, nutrition, education, and adaptive functioning in different regions of the world. UNICEF developed MICS so low- and middle-income countries could collect comparable data to evaluate country-level progress on issues related to children and women. Three rounds of MICS have been implemented; we use data from the MICS3 which was conducted in 2005–2007. The MICS3 has three questionnaires: a Household Questionnaire, a Questionnaire for Individual Women (15 to 49 years old), and a Questionnaire for Children Under Five. Each questionnaire is composed of core, additional, and optional modules, which are sets of standardized questions grouped by topics. Each country was responsible for designing and selecting a sample. The survey sample was usually a probability sample in all stages of selection, national in coverage, and designed in as simple a way as possible so that its field implementation could be easily and faithfully carried out with minimum opportunity for deviation from an overall standard design. Multiple steps were taken to ensure data reliability. MICS3 respondents were normally the mother or primary caregiver of the child.
The home environment questions were taken from the water and sanitation and household characteristics modules of the MICS3 Household Questionnaire and the child development module of the Questionnaire for Children Under Five (questionnaires are available at http://www.childinfo.org/mics3_questionnaire.html). Unordered categorical items were recoded into ordinal categories. Following the WHO and UNICEF’s (2008) drinking water ladder, we coded drinking water into three categories: unimproved (1), improved (2), and piped (3). Unimproved drinking water sources were unprotected springs or wells, tanker-trucks or carts with a small tank/drum, surface water, or bottled water (bottled water is considered to be unimproved because it is not regulated and water from other unimproved sources is sometimes bottled and sold in developing countries). Improved drinking water sources were public taps or standpipes, tube wells or boreholes, protected wells or springs, or rainwater collection. Piped water sources were piped directly to the household dwelling, plot, or yard.
Following the sanitation ladder recommended by WHO and UNICEF (2008), we coded toilet facilities into four categories: open defecation (1), unimproved (2), shared improved (3), and unshared improved (4). Open defecation consisted of no facilities or toileting in the bush or field. Unimproved sanitation included pit latrines without a slab or platform, hanging latrines, and bucket latrines. Improved facilities included flush or pour-flush latrines, ventilated improved pit (VIP) latrines, pit latrines with slabs, and composting toilets.
The main material of the dwelling floor was recoded into the three existing superordinate categories of natural (1), rudimentary (2), and finished flooring (3).
Cooking was recoded to indicate whether household cooking was done on an open fire or stove (0) or in a closed stove (1).
Nine household items (radio, television, mobile telephone, non-mobile telephone, refrigerator, motorcycle or scooter, animal-drawn cart, car or truck, and boat with motor) were coded as no (0), yes (1). Mobile and non-mobile telephone were recoded into a single item to indicate the presence (1) or absence (0) of either type of telephone in the household. Finally, the four items about household transportation - motorcycle or scooter, animal-drawn cart, car or truck, and boat with a motor - were recoded into a single item to indicate the presence (1) or absence (0) of any kind of transportation not powered by humans.
The number of books and children’s books in the household were recoded into three categories to indicate no books (0), 1–9 books (1), 10 or more books (2).
Finally, 4 categories of children’s toys (household objects, outside objects, homemade toys, and store-bought toys) were coded as (0) child does not play with the item or (1) child plays with the item.
Home environment indices
We organized the items above into four indices relevant to child development: quality of housing, material resources, formal learning resources, and informal learning resources. Summary scores were computed for each index so long as fewer than 30% of the constituent items were missing. For example, a 5-item index allowed one item to be missing, and a 3-item index allowed no missing items. For summed indices with a missing item, the total was prorated based on the remaining items. We consider these aggregates to be indices and not scales because they are conceptually related as indicators of child development, but they are not necessarily statistically related (see Bradley, 2004; Streiner, 2003). For example, the likelihood of having a telephone in the household may be unrelated to the likelihood of having transportation, but both items indicate increased access to people and places outside the home.
Quality of housing included drinking water, toilet facilities, household flooring, cooking, and refrigeration. Higher scores on these 5 items were all indicative of a healthier and safer home environment for children under 5. These 5 items were standardized and averaged to create an index of quality of housing.
Material resources included the presence of a radio, television, telephone, non-human-powered transportation, and electricity in the household. Higher scores on these 5 items were all indicative of the availability of a broader range of experiences for children under 5. These 5 items were summed to form an index of material resources.
Formal learning resources consisted of books, children’s books, and store-bought toys. Higher scores on these items indicated the availability of items children under 5 might use to learn about letters, words, shapes, colors, and other object properties. These 3 items were standardized and averaged to form an index of formal learning resources.
Informal learning resources consisted of household objects, outside objects, and homemade objects used for play. Higher scores on these 3 items indicated that children under 5 used these commonly available items for play. Items were summed to form the index of informal learning resources.
Human Development Index
The Human Development Index (HDI) was developed by the United Nations as a measure of the social and economic status of a country (UNDP, nd). It was computed for 179 countries and territories in the world, serves as a proxy for standard of living, and is associated with the general level of purchasing power present within a country. The HDI ranges from 0 to 1 and has three major constituent indices: Life expectancy (in years), education (comprised of the adult literacy rate and combined gross enrollment in primary, secondary, and tertiary school), and gross domestic product (GDP; U.S. dollar purchasing power parity per capita). Countries with an HDI of .80 or greater are considered High, .50 to .79 Medium, and .00 to .49 Low. The countries in our study draw from high, medium, and low ranges of HDI. (Additional information about the HDI is available in Bornstein et al., 2012.)
Covariates
Household crowding was used as a covariate for analyses involving quality of housing and material resources. Households with more members are likely to have fewer resources that are divided among many. Crowding varied across countries, F(26, 124,000) = 649.72, p < .001, η2p = .12, and was significantly associated with quality of housing, r = −.09, p < .001, and material resources, r = −.14, p < .001. Therefore, household crowding was controlled in all analyses of quality of housing and material resources.
Child age and the number of children under age 5 in the family were used as covariates for formal and informal learning resources because having younger children and more children under 5 may relate to having fewer resources. Child age and number of children under 5 in the family varied across countries, F(27, 127,319) = 50.94, p < .001, η2p = .01, and F(27, 127,319) = 359.47, p < .001, η2p = .07, respectively, and were significantly associated with formal and informal learning resources, rs = .18 and .24, ps < .001, for child age, and rs = −.15 and .01, ps < .001, for the number of children under 5. Therefore, child age and the number of children under 5 were controlled in analyses of formal and informal learning resources.
Analytic Plan
First, the 4 indices of environmental resources were explored via ANCOVA, with country as a predictor and crowding as a covariate for quality of housing and material resources, and with child age and number of children under 5 in the family as covariates for formal and informal learning resources. Next, for each of the 4 resource indices we averaged the households in each country and treated each country average as an “observation”. Accordingly, instead of the observations being 120,000 (the total number of children in the survey), the number of observations ranged from 22 to 27 depending on the number of countries for whom data were available on particular resources. Because the households were averaged within countries, the power for these tests is low, and they should be interpreted accordingly. Country averages were then correlated with country HDI and its 3 constituent indices (life expectancy, education, and GDP), controlling for the covariates listed above, and, if significant, the other two indices of the HDI. The HDI is multi-dimensional and, although life expectancy, education, and GDP are related to one another, it is possible that the 3 indices relate in different ways to child development and parenting. Controlling the other 2 indices of the HDI allowed us to remove the shared variance among indices and give a more precise estimate of the effects of the index in question.
Results
Descriptive statistics for the 4 indices of environmental resources by country are presented in Tables 1 and 2. (Additional tables displaying the individual country results for individual items are available online at [URL].)
Table 2.
Descriptive Statistics and Effect Sizes for Deviation from the Grand Mean for Formal and Informal Learning Resources
Formal Learning Resources | Informal Learning Resources | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | ES | M | SD | ES | |
High HDI | ||||||
Montenegro | .77 | .60 | 1.23 | .61 | .81 | −1.08 |
Serbia | .47 | .76 | .71 | .86 | .96 | −.34 |
Macedonia | .07 | .76 | −.10 | .35 | .52 | −1.78 |
Albania | .05 | .65 | −.19 | .87 | .88 | −.39 |
Kazakhstan | .71 | .52 | 1.19 | .77 | .90 | −.54 |
Bosnia and Herzegovina | .59 | .64 | .90 | .76 | .90 | −.64 |
Medium HDI | ||||||
Thailand | .28 | .62 | .28 | 1.07 | 1.05 | .22 |
Ukraine | 1.01 | .36 | 1.74 | .72 | .90 | −.75 |
Belize | .54 | .63 | .88 | .97 | .95 | −.05, ns |
Jamaica | .61 | .55 | .97 | 1.44 | 1.09 | 1.16 |
Syrian Arab Republic | .07 | .67 | −.03, ns | .96 | .95 | −.03, ns |
Mongolia | .09 | .61 | −.06 | .50 | .68 | −1.20 |
Viet Nam | −.03 | .72 | −.34 | .43 | .66 | −1.43 |
Uzbekistan | .38 | .55 | .55 | 1.13 | .97 | .40 |
Kyrgyzstan | .36 | .56 | .49 | .95 | .92 | −.12 |
Tajikistan | −.04 | .66 | −.28 | .84 | .95 | −.37 |
Yemen | −.27 | .63 | −.68 | 1.07 | .97 | .31 |
Ghana | −.52 | .63 | −1.25 | 1.45 | 1.06 | 1.23 |
Low HDI | ||||||
Togo | −.68 | .52 | −1.59 | 1.05 | .96 | .27 |
Gambia | -- | -- | -- | 1.35 | 1.07 | 1.10 |
Côte d’Ivoire | −.64 | .52 | −1.49 | 1.08 | 1.11 | .35 |
Central African Republic | −.74 | .50 | −1.65 | 1.37 | .13 | 1.10 |
Sierra Leone | −.52 | .63 | −1.28 | 1.97 | 1.05 | 2.59 |
| ||||||
TOTAL | .12 | .51 | NA | .98 | .38 | NA |
Note. All countries significantly differed from the grand mean at p < .05, except where noted.
Effect sizes (ES) are based on models with covariates. -- = Data were not collected. NA = Not applicable.
Deviation from the Grand Mean
Quality of housing
Scores from the 27 countries for which we had data varied within a standard deviation in terms of the quality of housing (Table 1). Controlling for household crowding, the overall effect of country was significant, F(26, 123,816) = 10,851.72, p < .001, η2p = .70. Households in all of the high-HDI countries had quality of housing at least a half standard deviation higher than the grand mean of .17, and households in all the low-HDI countries had quality of housing at least a half standard deviation lower than the grand mean. Countries in the medium-HDI and HDI N/A groups were split above and below the grand mean.
Material resources
Scores from the 26 countries for which we had data indicated wide variability in material resources available in the home (Table 1). Controlling for household crowding, the overall effect of country was significant, F(25, 123,036) = 5,945.75, p < .001, η2p = .55. Households in every high-HDI country had more material resources than the grand mean of 2.91, and households in every low-HDI country had fewer material resources than the grand mean. Countries in medium-HDI and HDI N/A groups were split above and below the mean.
Formal learning resources
Scores from the 22 countries for which we had data varied within a standard deviation in the number of formal learning resources available to young children in the household (Table 2). Controlling for child age and the number of children under 5 in the family, the overall effect of country was significant, F(21, 74,331) = 2,346.68, p < .001, η2p = .40. Households in all of the low-HDI countries scored more than a standard deviation below the grand mean of .12. Countries in the high- and medium-HDI groups were split above and below the grand mean.
Informal learning resources
Scores from the 23 countries for which we had data varied as regards the informal learning resources accessible to children (Table 2). Controlling for child age and the number of children under 5 in the family, the overall effect of country was significant, F(22, 79,314) = 562.75, p < .001, η2p = .14. Households in all of the high-HDI countries used fewer informal learning resources than the grand mean of .98, and households in all of the low-HDI countries used more informal learning resources than the grand mean. Countries in the medium-HDI group were split above and below the grand mean.
Relations with the Human Development Index
Table 3 displays partial correlations of the HDI and its constituent indices with the home environment indicators, controlling for covariates. Because literacy and schooling could have different effects on child well-being, we also included these two indicators in the analyses.
Table 3.
Partial Correlations of HDI and its Constituent Indices with the Home Environment Indices
Quality of Housinga | Material Resourcesa | Formal Learningb | Informal Learningb | |
---|---|---|---|---|
HDI | .92*** | .94*** | .76*** | −.47* |
Life Expectancy Index | .80***/.10 | .85***/.35 | .50*/−.13 | −.44*/−.24 |
Education Index | .82***/.34 | .82***/.26 | .72***/.50* | −.50*/−.35 |
Literacy | .81***/.41 | .81***/.34 | .63**/.40 | −.54*/−.41 |
Schooling | .76***/.04 | .77**/−.03 | .82***/.63** | −.32 |
GDP Index | .90***/.64*** | .92***/.67*** | .71***/.48* | −.30 |
Note. N = 22–27 countries. Partial correlations after the / are controlling for covariates and the other 2 indices that compose the HDI.
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Controlling for crowding.
Controlling for number of children under 5 in the family and target child age.
Quality of housing
The HDI and its constituent indices were significantly correlated with the quality of housing. However, when the other indices of the HDI were controlled, only the GDP index was uniquely associated with quality of housing.
Material resources
The HDI and its constituent indices were significantly correlated with material resources. However, when the other indices of the HDI were controlled, only the GDP index was uniquely associated with material resources.
Formal learning resources
The HDI and its constituent indices were significantly correlated with formal learning resources. When controlling for the education and GDP indices, the life expectancy index attenuated to non-significance. When controlling for the life expectancy and GDP indices, the education index and schooling remained significant, but literacy attenuated. Finally, controlling the life expectancy and education indices, the GDP index remained significantly correlated with formal learning resources.
Informal learning resources
The HDI, life expectancy and the education indices, and literacy were negatively correlated with informal learning resources. However, controlling for the other 2 constituent indices of the HDI, no correlations remained significant.
Summary
Quality of housing, material resources, and formal learning resources were all uniquely related to the GDP index. Formal learning resources were also uniquely related to the education index and schooling. Informal learning resources were not uniquely related to any HDI index.
Discussion
We found a wide range of home environment conditions in the 28 developing countries from the MICS, consistent with findings obtained from other surveys done in developing countries (Montgomery & Hewett, 2005). The quality of housing and material resources were tied to HDI status in that they were higher in all high-HDI countries and lower in all low-HDI countries than the grand mean. However, there were some medium-HDI countries that scored higher than some high-HDI countries, and some low-HDI countries that scored higher than some medium-HDI countries. Formal and informal learning resources were somewhat less tied to HDI. Formal learning resources varied above and below the grand mean in both the high and the medium-HDI groups. Informal learning resources showed the opposite pattern, with all high-HDI countries scoring below the grand mean and all low-HDI countries scoring above the grand mean. This finding likely occurred because children with store-bought toys were more likely to choose to play with them than outside items, household items, and homemade toys. Those countries with high percentages of households with no store-bought toys also tended to have higher scores on informal learning resources. What was not determinable from the data is what use children actually made of the materials and particularly whether parents used the materials to assist their children’s learning. This latter activity may be more closely tied to country HDI.
Findings from this study show that, even within nations that have a low GDP, country level HDI is associated with aspects of the home environment known to support the well-being of young children. Specifically, homes in those countries that scored as relatively high in the HDI were far more likely to have piped water (46.5% to 90.7%) than households in countries that scored as low HDI (1.3% to 26.1%). Similar discrepancies were noted for having unshared improved toilet facilities (87.0% to 98.0% vs. 7.1% to 51.8%). Because GDP plays such a large role in calculating HDI, it was not surprising to find that households in countries with higher levels of HDI had significantly higher levels of access to resources such as radios, TVs, telephones, vehicles for transportation, and electricity. Most notable, perhaps, is the difference as regards electricity: high-HDI countries ranged from 98.8% to 100% of homes with electricity, whereas low-HDI countries ranged from 6.2% to 57.5%. Likewise, in homes from high-HDI countries, books for children were far more readily available: from 10% to 59.3% of households had 10 or more children’s books in high-HDI countries contrasted to no more than 2.0% in low-HDI countries. Store bought toys were available in from 62.4% to 91.6% of homes from high-HDI countries. This contrasts to 20.0% to 69.8% in homes from low-HDI countries. Differences in access to these materials and facilities would seem particularly significant given findings showing that country level differences in access to such resources are related to measures of children’s cognitive and behavioral functioning (Gauvin & Munroe, 2009).
Given that the HDI tracks GDP closely, it is not surprising that of the three constituent indices of the HDI, GDP shows the strongest association with quality of housing, access to opportunities for broadened experiences, and materials for learning available. Housing quality has been linked to family income in many studies (Evans, 2006); and family income has been associated with the availability of toys and learning materials as well (Bradley, Corwyn, MacAdoo, & Garcia Coll, 2001). Even so, the findings showed that the other constituent indices of the HDI contributed to the prediction of some material resources above and beyond what could be predicted from GDP alone. For example, maternal level of schooling showed a unique relation to the availability of books and store-bought toys for learning. Not surprisingly, parents with high levels of education tend to provide more materials and experiences for their children’s learning than do parents with low levels of educational attainment (Bornstein & Bradley, 2003).
According to the International Committee on Economic, Social, and Cultural Rights, every person has a right to adequate housing. The Committee argues (Comment 4, Section 8), “To ensure the health, security, comfort, and nutrition of its occupants, an adequate house should have sustainable access to natural and common resources, safe drinking water, energy for cooking, heating and lighting, sanitation and washing facilities, means of food storage, refuse disposal, site drainage and emergency services” (Office of the High Commissioner on Human Rights, Article 11(1): 12/13/1991). Living in poorly constructed homes with inadequate facilities for drinking, cooking, toileting, and learning not only pose direct threats to children’s health and competence, but they make the tasks of parenting more difficult. Parents themselves are likely to be more stressed and less healthy. Moreover, they have less time and fewer resources to provide the kinds of stimulation and nurturance children need to assure well being. Thus, poor housing both directly and indirectly undermines children’s development (Bradley, 2006; Evans, 2003).
One of the great difficulties with the high rate of low quality home environments in poor countries is that low quality home environments contribute to the intergenerational transmission of poverty and low quality of life. Because poor housing quality and lack of access to high quality materials at home contribute to poor health and lower competence, they reduce the likelihood of increased GDP at the community and societal levels (Qureshi & Mohyuddin, 2006). Failure to improve GDP then increases the likelihood that low quality of housing will persist into the second generation and beyond.
Finding that informal learning resources were not significantly related to GDP indicates that parents in all countries could be encouraging learning with readily available objects. For example, parents could teach colors using flowers and leaves, counting using rocks, and shapes using cups, sticks, and other household items. Children can learn about object properties without costly store-bought toys. It would seem worthwhile for UNICEF, WHO, the World Bank, NGOs, and governmental agencies to devise ways of both educating very low income parents on how to utilize everyday objects to teach their children concepts and skills pertinent for academic success, and encouraging them to employ those learning techniques as part of daily routines. The key is to devise ways that are both culturally appropriate and sensitive to the constraints parents (mostly mothers) face in daily life. This means brief, carefully targeted interventions and public health announcements. Such simple approaches have been found to be effective techniques for improving water quality in the home (Jalan & Somanathan, 2004).
The findings from the MICS3 are limited in terms of what they can say regarding relations between the HDI and the likelihood children will experience better housing conditions, more access to learning materials, and a greater likelihood of having experiences that afford opportunities for enrichment and comfort. Data were available from only a select number of countries, and not all countries included in the MICS3 completed all the key items. Moreover, the data from the MICS3, like the data from most other surveys that address household quality and material resources, do not contain an optimal set of indicators of these constructs. Nonetheless, the findings point to needs to increase the standards of living in low-HDI countries and to find ways of increasing family income and opportunities for education. The well-being of the current generation of children and the turnover to the next would almost certainly improve with greater access to income and education (Walker et al., 2007). Even so, the efforts to improve standards of living and quality of life are likely to be fraught with challenges as numerous other factors are implicated in quality of life, and a diverse array of cultural and political factors would help determine the likelihood that any effort would result in meaningful change at the individual household level (Southern African Regional Poverty Network, 2006). Moreover, a full understanding of how the HDI (and related measures of standard of living) connects to child development via housing quality and material availability awaits the implementation of longitudinal studies on carefully targeted samples.
There is increased interest in how assets of all sorts can be converted into practices and arrangements that foster children’s health and development (Chowa, Ansong, & Masa, 2010). For those interested in the well-being of children who live in low-HDI countries – and, for that matter, children who live in most countries – the data base on how housing quality and the materials available in homes affect developmental course and life prospects is especially weak. There is a growing literature on how poverty and chronic adversity affect parenting processes; but that literature is mostly informed by research on children from technologically advanced societies and research that is limited to a few key parenting practices. Research that is inclusive, both in terms of the people it investigates and the environmental conditions it considers, remains scarce (Leventhal & Newman, 2010). Moreover, the extant research rarely considers the indirect pathways through which physical arrangements and access to materials affect children’s well-being. For example, a study done in rural Ethiopia showed that a family’s access to sickles and plows increased the likelihood children would attend school regularly as the children were not needed as much to help with farming tasks (Cockburn & Dostie, 2007). In effect, for child development research to usefully inform housing and economic policy in most countries – policies that necessitate careful decision making and hard choices given limited economic resources – it must expand in terms of who is studied, what environmental conditions are considered, and what processes linking those conditions to key child outcomes are analyzed. This broader undertaking is also needed to advance the science of environment-development relations more generally. Too much of what we believe about how environments are implicated in children’s development derives from research that is insufficiently inclusive of the totality of factors in the home environment that likely matter and the diversity of living conditions present throughout the world. Most developmentalists would probably not have entertained the notion that a family’s access to a plow could increase the level of education a child might obtain (and whatever that might augur for employment or health downstream). But exploring such paths in a diverse array of places will almost certainly lead to expansions and corrections in our understanding of how environmental conditions function to impact children.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This research was partially funded by the intramural research program of the National Institutes of Health, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.
Contributor Information
Robert H. Bradley, Arizona State University
Diane L. Putnick, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
References
- Abou-Ali H. The effect of water and sanitation on child mortality in Egypt. Göteburg University; Sweden: 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Aina TA, Agiobu-Kemmer I, Etta EF, Zeitlin MF, Setiloane K. Early child care and nutrition in Lagos State, Nigeria. Tufts University School of Nutrition & Policy for UNICEF; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Agha S. The determinants of infant mortality in Pakistan. Social Science & Medicine. 2000;51:199–208. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(99)00460-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agiobu-Kemmer I. Cognitive and affective aspects of infant development. In: Curran HV, editor. Nigerian children: Developmental perspectives. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul; 1984. pp. 74–117. [Google Scholar]
- Arabi M, Frongillo EA, Avula R, Mangasaryan N. Infant and young child feeding in developing countries. Child Development. 2012 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01675.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aribihola A. Housing policy formulation in developing countries: Evidence from programme implementation from Akure, Ondo State, Nigeria. Journal of Human Ecology. 2008;23:125–134. [Google Scholar]
- Arik H, Arik M. Is it economic growth or socioeconomic development: A cross-sectional analysis of the determinants of infant mortality. The Journal of Developing Areas. 2009;43:31–55. [Google Scholar]
- Awasthi S, Glick HA, Fletcher RJH. Effects of cooking fuels on respiratory diseases in preschool children in Lucknow India. American Journal of Tropical Medicine & Hygiene. 1996;55:48–51. doi: 10.4269/ajtmh.1996.55.48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baltazar JC, Solon FS. Disposal of feces of children under two years old and diarrhea incidence: A case-control study. Int Journal of Epidemiology. 1989;18:S16–19. doi: 10.1093/ije/18.supplement_2.s16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartlett S. Children’s experience of the physical environment in poor urban settlements and the implications for policy and practice. Environment & Urbanization. 1999;11:63–73. [Google Scholar]
- Bartlett S. Water, sanitation and urban children: The need to go beyond “improved” provision. Children, Youth & Environments. 2005;15:115–137. [Google Scholar]
- Black M. Use of HOME as a measure of maternal responsivity and environments in Bangladesh and India. Presented at Assessing home environments across diverse populations; April, 2003; Ann Arbor, MI: Center for Human Growth and Development; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Bornstein MH, Bradley RH. Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Bornstein MH, Britto PB, Nonoyama-Tarumi Y, Ota Y, Petrovic O, Putnick DL. Child development in developing countries: Introduction and methods. Child Development. 2012 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01671.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH. Chaos, culture, and covariance structures: A dynamic systems view of children’s experiences at home. Parenting: Science and Practice. 2004;4:243–257. [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH. Home environment. In: Watts NF, Ayoub C, Bradley RH, Puma JE, editors. The crisis in youth mental health, vol. 4. Early intervention programs and policies. Westport, CN: Praeger; 2006. pp. 89–120. [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. SES and child development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2002;53:371–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Caring for children around the world: A view from HOME. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2005;26:468–478. [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RH, Corwyn RF, McAdoo HP, Garcia Coll C. The home environments of children in the United States. Part 1: Variations by age, ethnicity, and poverty status. Child Development. 2001;72:1844–1867. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chomitz VR. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Tufts University School of Nutrition; Medford, Mass: 1992. Diet of Javanese preschool children: Relationship to household environmental factors and stature. [Google Scholar]
- Chowa G, Ansong D, Masa R. Assets and child well-being in developing countries: A research review. Children and Youth Services Review. 2010;32:1508–1519. [Google Scholar]
- Church AT, Katigbak MS. Home environment, nutritional status, and maternal intelligence as determinants of intellectual development in rural Philippine preschool children. Intelligence. 1991;15:49–78. doi: 10.1016/0160-2896(91)90022-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chi J, Rao N. Parental beliefs about school learning and children’s educational attainment: Evidence from rural China. Ethos. 2003;31:330–356. [Google Scholar]
- Cockburn J, Dostie B. Child work and schooling: The role of household asset profiles and poverty in rural Ethiopia. Journal of African Economies. 2007;16:519–563. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Collins DA, Sithole SD, Martin KS. Indoor woodsmoke pollution causing lower respiratory disease in children. Tropical Doctor. 1990;20:151–155. doi: 10.1177/004947559002000403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conger RD, Donnellan MB. An interactionist perspective on the socioeconomic context of human development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:175–199. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cravioto J, DeLicardie E. Nutrition, the nervous system, and behavior, Proceedings of the seminar on malnutrition in early life and subsequent mental development. Scientific Publication No. 251. Washington, DC: Pan American Health Organization; 1972. Environmental correlates of severe clinical malnutrition in survivors from kwashiorkor or marasmus; pp. 73–94. [Google Scholar]
- Curtis V, Kanki B, Mertens T, Traore E, Diallo I, Tall F, Cousens S. Potties, pits and pipes: Explaining hygiene behaviour in Burkino Faso. Social Science & Medicine. 1995;40:251–254. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)00341-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Das S, Padhee B. Level II abilities of socially disadvantaged children: Effects of home environment, caste and age. Journal of Indian Psychology. 1993;11:38–43. [Google Scholar]
- de Oliveira EA, Barros FC, Anselmi LD, Piccinini CA. The quality of home environment in Brazil: A ecological model. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2006;15:633–644. [Google Scholar]
- Drotar D, Olness K, Wiznitzer M, Maruim L, Guay L, Hom D, Svilar G, Schatschneider C, Fagan J, Ndugwa C, Mayengo RK. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of Ugandan infants with HIV infection: An application of growth curve analysis. Health Psychology. 1999;18:114–121. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.18.2.114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duhan K, Punia S. Home environment as predictor of language development. Psycho-Lingua. 1998;28:45–48. [Google Scholar]
- Ehiri JE, Azubuike MC, Ubbaonu CN, Anyanwu EC, Ibe KM, Ogbonna MO. Critical control points of complimentary food preparation and handling in eastern Nigeria. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 2001;79:423–435. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans GW. A multimethodological analysis of cumulative risk and allostatic load among rural children. Developmental Psychology. 2003;39:924–933. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.5.924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans GW. Child development and the physical environment. Annual Review of Psychology. 2006;57:423–451. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans GW, Saltzman H, Cooperman JL. Housing quality and children’s socioemotional health. Environment & Behavior. 2001;33:389–399. [Google Scholar]
- Evans G, Wells N, Moch A. Housing and mental health: Review of the evidence and a methodological and conceptual critique. Journal of Social Issues. 2003;59:475–500. [Google Scholar]
- Fiadzo E. Joint Center for Housing Studies. Harvard University; 2004. Jun, Estimating the determinants of housing quality: The case of Ghana. Paper No. W04–6. [Google Scholar]
- Fisk WJ, Lei-Gomez Q, Mendell MJ. Meta-analysis of the associations of respiratory health effects with dampness and mold in homes. Indoor Air. 2007;17:284–296. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0668.2007.00475.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gauderman W, Avol E, Gilliland F, Vora H, Thomas D, Berhane K, McConnell R, Kuenzli N, Lurmann F, Rappaport E, Margolis H, Bates D, Peters J. The effect of air pollution on lung development from 10 to 18 years of age. New England Journal of Medicine. 2004;351:1057–1067. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa040610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gauvin M, Munroe RL. Contributions of societal modernity to cognitive development: A comparison of four cultures. Child Development. 2009;80:1628–1642. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01358.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg S. Infant development and mother-infant interaction in urban Zambia. In: Leiderman PH, Tulkin SR, Rosenfeld A, editors. Culture and infancy: Variations in the human experience. New York: Academic Press; 1977. pp. 211–244. [Google Scholar]
- Grantham-McGregor SM, Fernald LC. Nutritional deficiencies and subsequent effects on mental and behavioral development in children. Southeast Asian Journal of Tropical Medicine and Public Health. 1997;28(Suppl 2):2233S–2238S. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holding P. Unpublished manuscript. 2003. Adaptation and use of the middle childhood HOME Inventory in Kilifi, Kenya. [Google Scholar]
- Jalan J, Somanaathan E. SANDEE Working Paper 8–04. South Asian Network for Development and Environmental Economics; Kathmandu: 2004. The importance of being informed: Experimental evidence on the demand for environmental quality. [Google Scholar]
- Kanagawa M, Nakata T. Assessment of access to electricity and socioeconomic impacts in rural areas in developing countries. Energy Policy. 2008;36:2016–2029. [Google Scholar]
- Kumar P, Chirayil PT, Chittoria R. Ten years epidemiological study of paediatric burns in Manipal, India. Burns. 2000;26:261–264. doi: 10.1016/s0305-4179(99)00109-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurtz BE, Borkowski JG, Deshmukh K. Metamemory and learning in Maharashtrian children: Influences from home and school. Journal of Genetic Psychology. 1987;149:363–376. [Google Scholar]
- Laaksonen M, Rahkonen O, Martikainen P, Lahelma E. Socioeconomic position and self-rated health: The contribution of childhood socioeconomic circumstances, adult socioeconomic status, and material resources. Research and Practice. 2005;95:1403–1409. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.047969. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leventhal T, Newman S. Housing and child development. Children and Youth Services Review. 2010;32:1165–1174. [Google Scholar]
- Lindskog P, Lundqvist J. Why poor children stay sick: The human ecology of child health and welfare in rural Malawi. Uppsala: Scandinavian Institute of African Studies; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Lleras C. Employment, work conditions, and the home environment in single-mother families. Journal of Family Issues. 2008;29:1268–1297. [Google Scholar]
- Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Wolf A. Long-term developmental outcome of infants with iron deficiency. New England Journal of Medicine. 1991;325:687–694. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199109053251004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lozoff B, Park A, Radan A, Wolf A. Using the HOME Inventory with infants in Costa Rica. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1995;18:277–295. doi: 10.1177/016502549501800206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ludwig KM, Fernando F, Firmino AF, Joao Tadeu RP. Correlation between sanitation conditions and intestinal parasitosis in the population of Asses, State of Sao Paolo. Revista da Sociedade Brasileira de Meddicina Tropical. 1999;32:547–555. doi: 10.1590/s0037-86821999000500013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mahfouz AA, El-Morshedy H, Fargaly A, Khalil A. Ecological determinants of intestinal parasitestic infections among pre-school children in urban squatter settlement of Egypt. Journal of Tropical Pediatrics. 1997;43:341–344. doi: 10.1093/tropej/43.6.341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCracken JP, Smith KR, Díaz A, Mittleman MA, Schwartz J. Chimney stove intervention to reduce long-term wood smoke exposure lowers blood pressure among Guatemalan women. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2007;115:996–1001. doi: 10.1289/ehp.9888. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mendez MA, Adair LS. Severity and timing of stunting in the first two years of life affect performance on cognitive tests in late childhood. Journal of Nutrition. 1999;129:1555–1562. doi: 10.1093/jn/129.8.1555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meng G, Hall GB. Assessing housing quality in metropolitan Lima, Peru. Journal of Housing and the Built Environment. 2006;21:413–439. [Google Scholar]
- Mertens TE, Jaffar S, Fernando MA, Cousens SN, Feacham R. Excreta disposal behaviour and latrine ownership in relation to the risk of childhood diarrhoea in Sri Lanka. International Journal of Epidemiology. 1992;21:1157–1164. doi: 10.1093/ije/21.6.1157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Misra G, Tiwari BK. Environmental correlates of cognitive development. Indian Journal of Applied Psychology. 1984;24:41–52. [Google Scholar]
- Montgomery MR, Hewett PC. Urban poverty and health in developing countries: Household and neighborhood effects. Demography. 2005;42:397–425. doi: 10.1353/dem.2005.0020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Motarjemi Y, Käferstein F, Moy G, Quevedo F. Contaminated weaning food: A major risk factor for diarrhoea and associated malnutrition. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 1993;71:79–92. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muoghalu L. Measuring housing and environmental quality as an indicator of quality of urban life: A case of traditional city of Benin, Nigeria. Social Indicators Research. 1991;25:63–98. [Google Scholar]
- Prüss-Üstün A, Kay D, Fewtrell L, Bartram J. Unsafe water, sanitation and hygiene. In: Ezzati M, Lopez AD, Rodgers A, Murray CJ, editors. Comparative quantification of health risk. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO Press; 2004. pp. 1321–1352. [Google Scholar]
- Podewils L, Mintz E, Nataro J, Parashar U. Acute, infectious diarrhea among children in developing countries. Seminars in Pediatric Infectious Diseases. 2004;15:155–168. doi: 10.1053/j.spid.2004.05.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quereshi H, Mohyuddin H. Health status, diseases, and economic development: A cross-country analysis. The Journal of Developing Areas. 2006;39:121–128. [Google Scholar]
- Richter L, Grieve K. Home environment and cognitive development of black infants in impoverished South African families. Infant Mental Health Journal. 1991;12:88–102. [Google Scholar]
- Ridder G, Tunali I. Stratified partial likelihood estimation. Journal of Econometrics. 1999;92:193–232. doi: 10.1016/s0304-4076(98)00090-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts L, Chartier Y, Chartier O, Malenga G, Toole M, Rodka H. Keeping clean water clean in a Malawi refugee camp: A randomized clinical trial. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 2001;79:280–287. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sahu S, Devi B. Unpublished manuscript. 1982. Role of home environment in psycholinguistic abilities and intelligence of advantaged and disadvantaged preschool children. [Google Scholar]
- Siddiqui AR, Gold EB, Yang X, Lee K, Brown KH, Bhutta ZA. Prenatal exposure to wood fuel smoke and low birth weight. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2008;116:543–549. doi: 10.1289/ehp.10782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Southern African Regional Poverty Network. Poverty eradication and linking micro level impacts to macroeconomic policies and frameworks. 2006 Available online: http://www.sarpn.org.za/documents/d0002542/index.php.
- Statistics South Africa. General Household Survey 2007 Metadata. Cape Town, Pretoria: Statistics South Africa; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Streiner DL. Being inconsistent about consistency: When coefficient alpha does and doesn’t matter. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2003;80:217–222. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA8003_01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torralva T, Cugnasco I. Estudios epidemiologicicos sobre desarrollo infantile. In: O’Donnell A, Carmuega E, editors. Hoy y manana, Salud y calidad de vida de la ninez Argentina. Buenos Aires, Argentina: Centro de Estudios Sobre Nutricion Infantil; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- United Nations. World Declaration on Education For All: Meeting Basic Learning Needs. 1990 Available from: http://un-documents.net/jomtien.htm.
- United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF) Multiple indicator cluster survey manual 2005: Monitoring the situation of children and women. New York: UNICEF; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- United Nations Development Programme (UNDP) The human development index (HDI) n.d Retrieved July 10, 2009, from http://hdr.undp.org/en/statistics/indices/hd.
- US Department of Housing & Urban Development and US Bureau of Census. American Housing Survey for the United States: 2007 (H150/07) Washington DC: US Govt. Printing Office; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, Wasserman GA, Pollitt E, Carter JA the International Child Development Steering Group. Child development in developing countries 2. Child development: Risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369:145–157. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60076-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang L. Determinants of child mortality in LDCs: Empirical findings from demographic and health surveys. Health Policy. 2003;65:277–299. doi: 10.1016/s0168-8510(03)00039-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams P, Piamjariyakul U, Williams A, Hornboonherm P, Meena P, Channukool N, Singhala K, Hosakulthai M, Klunkarndee K, Mahasiripun T, Pusatum P, Tuebpo C, Pranprai P, Chamusri S. Thai mothers and children and the home observation for measurement of the environment (home inventory): pilot study. International Journal of Nursing Studies. 2003;40:249–258. doi: 10.1016/s0020-7489(02)00083-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organiztion. Global burden of disease, 2004 update. 2008 Available online: http://www.who.int/healthinfo/global_burden_disease/en/index.html.
- World Health Organization and United Nations Children’s Fund Joint Monitoring Programme for Water Supply and Sanitation. Progress on drinking water and sanitation: Special focus on sanitation. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO Press; 2008. Available online: http://www.who.int/water_sanitation_health/monitoring/jmp2008.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Wu F, Takaro T. Childhood asthma and environmental interventions. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2007;115:971–975. doi: 10.1289/ehp.8989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang CY, Chiu JF, Chiu HF, Kao WY. Damp housing conditions and respiratory symptoms in primary school children. Pediatric Pulmonology. 1997;24:73–77. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0496(199708)24:2<73::aid-ppul1>3.0.co;2-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yongsi HBN, Ntetu AL. Household standard of living and children’s health in Cameroon. The Journal of Developing Areas. 2008;42:251–259. [Google Scholar]
- Zeitlin MF, Megawagni R, Kramer EM, Coletta ND, Babatunde ED, Carman D. Strengthening the family: Implications for international development. Toyko, Japan: The United Nations University Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Zevalkink J, Riksen-Walraven MR. Parenting in Indonesia: Inter- and intracultural differences in mothers’ interactions with their young children. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2001;25:167–175. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.